Abstract
Introduction
Intelligence is a core construct of individual differences in cognitive abilities and a strong predictor of important life outcomes. Within recent years, rates of cesarean section have substantially increased globally, though little is known about its effect on neurodevelopmental trajectories. Thus, we aimed to investigate the influence of delivery by cesarean section on the genetics of intelligence in children.
Methods
Participants were recruited through the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Intelligence was measured by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC). Genotyping was performed using the Illumina Human Hap 550 quad genome‐wide SNP genotyping platform and was followed by imputation using MACH software. Genome‐wide interaction analyses were conducted using linear regression.
Results
A total of 2,421 children and 2,141,747 SNPs were subjected to the genome‐wide interaction analyses. No variant reached genome‐wide significance. The strongest interaction was observed at rs17800861 in the GRIN2A gene (β = −3.43, 95% CI = −4.74 to −2.12, p = 2.98E−07). This variant is predicted to be located within active chromatin compartments in the hippocampus and may influence binding of the NF‐kappaB transcription factor.
Conclusions
Our results may indicate that mode of delivery might have a moderating effect on genetic disposition of intelligence in children. Studies of considerable sizes (>10,000) are likely required to more robustly detect variants governing such interaction. In summary, the presented findings prompt the need for further studies aimed at increasing our understanding of effects various modes of delivery may have on health outcomes in children.
Keywords: Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, glutamate, intelligence, mode of delivery
1. INTRODUCTION
Intelligence is a core construct of individual differences in cognitive abilities. Measured intelligence is a strong predictor of important life outcomes, such as mental and physical health and mortality (Deary, Weiss, & Batty, 2010). It is well established that intelligence is a highly heritable behavioral trait (Davies et al., 2012), with heritability increasing from childhood to adulthood, while maintaining genetic stability across a lifespan (Plomin & Deary, 2014). Nonetheless, the extent and nature of such genetic influence is still not clear (Visscher, Hill, & Wray, 2008) and warrants further investigation.
Intelligence captures a broad scope of cognitive abilities and can be enumerated by measures of vocabulary or verbal IQ (VIQ) and matrix reasoning or performance IQ (PIQ) that can be combined into a full scale IQ (FSIQ; David, 1991). Neurobiological studies on intelligence have linked it to brain structure and functioning (Gray & Thompson, 2004; Song et al., 2008). For example, it has been shown that VIQ and PIQ capture different aspects of intelligence—both behaviorally and biologically (Nisbett et al., 2012). VIQ includes knowledge accumulated throughout life (e.g., vocabulary), determined by education and experience (crystallized intelligence), while PIQ reflects problem solving and reasoning abilities (e.g., matrix reasoning) that has little reliance on stored knowledge or learning (fluid intelligence; Toga & Thompson, 2005).
The heritability of intelligence is hypothesized to be influenced by many genes of small effects, and it increases from approximately 20% in early childhood to 60% in adulthood (Haworth et al., 2010). However, despite this increase, the same genes are believed to influence intelligence during the life course (genetic stability; Plomin & Deary, 2014). To date, the catalogue of genome‐wide association (GWA) studies (https://www.ebi.ac.uk/gwas/home, accessed on May 24, 2018) reports nine studies, using “intelligence” as a search word (Benyamin et al., 2014; Butcher, Davis, Craig, & Plomin, 2008; Davies et al., 2012; Davis et al., 2010; Gialluisi et al., 2014; Hill et al., 2018; Kirkpatrick, Mcgue, Iacono, Miller, & Basu, 2014; Loo et al., 2012; Sniekers et al., 2017). The biggest study among those was performed on 248,482 individuals, using combined data for intelligence and correlated phenotypes (Hill et al., 2018). The authors reported 187 genome‐wide significant independent loci, showing that the combined effects of common single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) across the genome explain only 25.44% of phenotypic variation in intelligence (Hill et al., 2018).
Apart from genetics, the environment also plays a substantial role in the variability of intelligence. Genes can affect intelligence directly depending on the environment (gene–environment interaction) or indirectly through gene–environment correlation (Gray & Thompson, 2004). The increase in heritability of intelligence, assuming genetic stability, might occur due to contributions from genetic amplification, where genetic influences are amplified in a selected environment (Plomin & Deary, 2014). The examinations of environmental influences on the genetic underpinnings of human intelligence are, therefore, an important area of research.
Studies on the environmental effects on intelligence have more commonly been done within the framework of heritability estimates in twin studies (Deary, Johnson, & Houlihan, 2009), while molecular gene–environment interaction analyses are sparse. Interaction analyses may provide an estimate of the extent to which a specific environment affects the variability in intelligence, given a certain genetic predisposition. Such estimates may help our understanding of intelligence's etiology as well as provide insights into how intelligence may be affected by changes in our day‐to‐day lives, and to, eventually, inform healthcare practices.
Delivery by cesarean section (DCS) refers to giving birth through surgical incisions in the mother's abdomen and uterus as opposed to vaginal delivery (VD), occurring via contractions of uterine muscles. DCS can be a life‐saving procedure when either the mother or the baby is at high risk of adverse outcomes (Black, Bhattacharya, Philip, Norman, & McLernon, 2015). As cesarean sections performed on mother's demand have become increasingly common in recent years (Christilaw, 2006; D'Souza, 2013; Kwee, Cohlen, Kanhai, Bruinse, & Visser, 2004; Lavender, Hofmeyr, Neilson, Kingdon, & Gyte, 2014), the number of DCSs has substantially risen worldwide (Belizán, Althabe, & Cafferata, 2007; Villar et al., 2006), although the short‐ and long‐term effects of both DCS and VD on health outcomes in children remain unclear.
So far, epidemiological studies suggest that DCS may present increased risk for developing wheezing and asthma (Magnus et al., 2011; Thavagnanam, Fleming, Bromley, Shields, & Cardwell, 2008), childhood obesity (Kuhle, Tong, & Woolcott, 2015), and type 1 diabetes mellitus (Black et al., 2015; Cardwell et al., 2008). Experimental data also implicate the lack of VD in altered immune function, stress response, and epigenetic modifications in children (Ronca, Abel, Ronan, & Alberts, 2009; Round & Mazmanian, 2009; Salminen, Gibson, McCartney, & Isolauri, 2004; Schlinzig, Johansson, Gunnar, Ekström, & Norman, 2009).
The effect of delivery method has also been studied in relation to development of the nervous system and related phenotypes. It has been recently shown that VD can be implicated in gene expression involved in neuroprotection, synaptogenesis, and neuronal differentiation in the hippocampus—a brain region important for memory formation (Seli & Horvath, 2013; Simon‐Areces et al., 2012; Varela, Schwartz, & Horvath, 2016). Since intelligence has been linked to working memory (Kane & Engle, 2002; Kyllonen & Christal, 1990) and activity‐dependent synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus (Morris et al., 2003; Neves, Cooke, & Bliss, 2008), developmental disturbances of either could potentially affect intelligence, which in turn is associated with a number of neurodevelopmental disorders (Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist, & Sundquist, 2015; Kuntsi et al., 2004). DCS has also been implicated in long‐term changes in the dopaminergic system (Boksa, Zhang, & Bestawros, 2002; Brake, Noel, Boksa, & Gratton, 1997; El‐Khodor & Boksa, 1997; Vaillancourt & Boksa, 1998), believed to play role in the pathomechanisms of several neuropsychiatric conditions (Bassett, Chow, Waterworth, & Linda, 2008; Grace, 2012; Homberg et al., 2016; Martelle et al., 2016). Indeed, DCS has been linked to neurodevelopmental impairments (Wilson‐Costello et al., 2007), special educational needs in later life (Kapellou, 2011; Mackay, Smith, Dobbie, & Pell, 2010), autism (Curran et al., 2015; Emberti Gialloreti, Benvenuto, Benassi, & Curatolo, 2014; Gregory et al., 2009), attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Curran et al., 2016; Sucksdorff et al., 2018), and obsessive–compulsive disorder (Vasconcelos et al., 2007). Children delivered by cesarean section may also face more emotional disturbances, withdrawal, and sleep problems and display a high number of internalizing problems during preschool ages (Kelmanson, 2013).
Taken together, the method of delivery has been reported to be associated with a number of neurodevelopmental processes and disorders of the brain, all of which have also been linked to intelligence. Given the high heritability and substantial environmental effect on the variability of intelligence, we aimed to investigate a possible interplay between genetic factors and mode of delivery on the development of intelligence in children. We performed a genome‐wide examination of moderating effect of mode of delivery on the genetics of intelligence (gene × environment interaction) in a sample of 2,421 children aged 8.5 years. We then examined the robustness of the interaction by accounting for potential confounding perinatal factors, such as gestational age and Apgar score.
2. MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1. Participants
The participants of this study were recruited through the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC; RRID:SCR_007260), also known as “Children of the 90s” (Boyd et al., 2013). All pregnant women living in the Avon County with estimated delivery dates between April 1, 1991, and December 31, 1992, were eligible to participate. Of the original 14,541 pregnancies, 13,988 children were alive at one year of age. An additional 713 children were enrolled after age seven, resulting in a total sample of 14,701 children. The data were collected from 14,009 participants (self‐reported or provided by biological mother/primary caregiver). These mother–child pairs have been followed for over 20 years, generating an immense amount of data through biological samples, measurements, and questionnaires. The study website contains details of all the data that is available through a fully searchable data dictionary which can be obtained here: https://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access. Phenotype‐matched genotype data were available for up to 6,832 children, depending on the variables. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC's own Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees in the UK (Bristol and Weston Health Authority: E1808 Children of the Nineties: Avon Longitudinal Study of Pregnancy and Childhood (ALSPAC); Southmead Health Authority: 49/89 Children of the Nineties—“ALSPAC”; Frenchay Health Authority: 90/8 Children of the Nineties; United Bristol Healthcare Trust: E4445 ALSPAC Focus at Eight; Southmead (North Bristol Trust): Project 084/99 ALSPAC Assessments at Age Eight; Frenchay (North Bristol Trust): Project 99/42 Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Assessments at Age Eight (ALSPAC Focus at Eight); Weston Area Health Trust: E177 Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Assessments at Age Eight).
2.2. Genotyping and quality control (QC)
ALSPAC participants’ DNA was extracted from whole blood or buccal swab samples and prepared for genotyping using standard protocols. A total of 9,912 samples were genotyped using the Illumina Human Hap 550 quad genome‐wide SNP genotyping platform, and quality control was performed by ALSPAC as described at https://www.bristol.ac.uk/media-library/sites/alspac/migrated/documents/gwas-data-generation.pdf. In short, SNPs with a minor allele frequency (MAF) <1%, with a call rate <95%, and out of Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (p < 5.00E−07) were removed. The resulting data set consisted of 8,365 individuals.
Known autosomal variants were imputed by ALSPAC with MACH version 1.0.16 (RRID:SCR_007260) Markov Chain Haplotyping software (Li, Willer, Ding, Scheet, & Abecasis, 2010; Li, Willer, Sanna, & Abecasis, 2009), using the Centre d'Etude du Polymorphisme Humain (CEPH) individuals from phase 2 of the HapMap Project (HG18; RRID:SCR_002846) as a reference set (release 22). Imputation of X chromosomal SNPs was done with MiniMac version 4.43 (RRID:SCR_009292; Howie, Fuchsberger, Stephens, Marchini, & Abecasis, 2012). Only SNPs with imputation quality estimates above 0.3 were included.
We performed additional quality control in PLINK software (RRID:SCR_001757), version 1.09 (Purcell et al., 2007), in the subset of individuals with available IQ measures to ensure that no SNPs or individuals had poor genotyping rates (<98%), no SNPs were rare (MAF <5%) or SNPs were out of Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (p < 1.00E−06). In addition, we removed individuals revealing cryptic relatedness (PI_HAT>15%), excessive heterozygosity (outside the range of mean ± 3 standard deviations (SD)), incorrect sex assignment, and/or non‐European ancestry based on multidimensional scaling with HapMap data (phase 3).
2.3. Measure of intelligence and perinatal factors
Intelligence was assessed via the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC; Wechsler, Rust, & Golombok, 1992). Verbal intelligence quotient (VIQ) was measured based on five oral subtests: information, similarities, arithmetic, vocabulary, and comprehension. Performance IQ (PIQ) subtests included nonverbal problems: picture completion, coding, picture arrangement, block design, and object assembly. Full scale IQ (FSIQ) was calculated based on verbal and performance IQ scores. All IQ measures were subjected to outlier removal (outside the range of mean ± 3 SD), and normal distribution was ensured.
Perinatal factors have been reported to play a role in neuropsychiatric disorders where IQ is affected as well as to highly correlate with DCS (Guinchat et al., 2012; Halmøy, Klungsøyr, Skjærven, & Haavik, 2012; Sucksdorff et al., 2018). We, therefore, examined their possible confounding effect on the relationship between mode of delivery and the genetics of intelligence. Available perinatal factors included birth weight, birth length, Apgar score measured at 1 min, and gestational age. To avoid collinearity, we assessed correlation among all of the aforementioned factors as well as their correlation with mode of delivery. Factors revealing correlation above 30% were excluded from the analyses.
2.4. Genome‐wide interaction analyses
We explored the moderating effect of mode of delivery on the genetics of intelligence by performing linear regression analyses. We first constructed crude models with each of the IQ measurements as an outcome (three models in total), examining the main effects of SNPs, mode of delivery, and sex. We also included a two‐way interaction term between SNPs and mode of delivery. SNPs were tested assuming additive model in PLINK version 1.09 (Purcell et al., 2007).
As the effect of cesarean section may be confounded by other perinatal factors, the initial crude models were also examined adjusted for those factors. Since birth weight, birth length, and gestational age revealed high correlation (Supporting Information Table S1), only Apgar score and gestational age were included as covariates in the adjusted models to avoid collinearity. None of the models were adjusted for age as all participants were of the same age (8.5 years), but all models were adjusted for sex. Interaction analyses place a high parameter burden on samples of modest sizes, so cell counts of the most significant variants were examined. When those were below 5, the models were analyzed assuming a dominant genetic effect.
To correct for multiple testing, genome‐wide significance threshold was set at a p‐value of 5.00E−08 (Risch & Merikangas, 1996). Calculation of genomic control coefficient (lambda (λ)) and QQ plots were utilized to ensure integrity of the observed test statistics.
2.5. In silico exploration of the most significant SNPs
To assess possible functional relevance of our top findings, we performed in silico analyses of the most significant SNPs using HaploReg software (RRID:SCR_006796), version 4.1 (Ward & Kellis, 2012). HaploReg is a tool for exploring chromatin states and regulatory motif alterations among variants in the genome (https://archive.broadinstitute.org/mammals/haploreg/haploreg.php).
3. RESULTS
3.1. Subjects, genotyping, QC, and measure of intelligence and perinatal factors
Overall, 2,141,747 SNPs and 2,421 individuals passed all QC filters. The same number of individuals had all phenotypes available for the analyses. Table 1 summarizes the examined phenotypes, the number of participants, and the sex distribution by each IQ measure. The summary of the overall data processing, reflecting the number of participants depending on the availability of the IQ measure, genetic data, and performed quality controls is shown in Supporting Information Figure S1 (constructed in PRISMA (Liberati et al., 2009)). The distribution of IQ measures is presented in Supporting Information Figures S2–S4.
Table 1.
Spontaneous delivery N = 1,902 |
Delivery by cesarean section N = 519 |
Difference p‐Value |
|
---|---|---|---|
Full scale IQ (mean (SD)) | 102.82 (19.54) | 103.95 (20.34) | 0.25a |
Sex (female %) | 52.05% | 49.32% | 0.29b |
Apgar score at 1 min (mean (SD)) | 8.51 (1.14) | 7.70 (2.10) | 6.08E−12c |
Gestational age (mean (SD)) | 39.46 (1.82) | 38.69 (2.48) | 6.71E−15a |
One‐way ANOVA.
Chi‐squared test.
Wilcoxon nonparametric test.
3.2. Genome‐wide interaction analyses
We did not observe any genome‐wide significant main effect at any SNP in any of our models. Furthermore, our interaction analyses did not reveal any genome‐wide significant findings (p < 5.00E−08) for any of the intelligence measures in the models examined.
Among the crude models, the most significant signals were observed at rs705670 for PIQ, rs17800861 for FSIQ, and rs1276529 for VIQ (Table 2). After adjusting for possible confounders, the strongest interaction was observed at rs17800861 for FSIQ in the intron of the GRIN2A (glutamate receptor, ionotropic N‐methyl‐d‐aspartate 2A gene (β = −3.43, 95% CI = −4.74 to −2.12, p = 2.98E−07; Table 2 and Figure 1). The second strongest interaction signal was observed for PIQ at rs705670 (β = 2.31, 95% CI = 1.43 to 3.19, p = 3.09E−07; Table 2 and Supporting Information Figures S5), an intronic variant within a long intergenic non‐protein‐coding RNA gene (LINC01502) located on chromosome 9. For VIQ, the strongest signal was noted at rs1276529 (β = −2.09, 95% CI = −2.91 to −1.26, p = 7.13E−07; Table 2 and Supporting Information Figures S6), an intergenic polymorphism on chromosome 6.
Table 2.
SNP | CHR | BP | Effect allele | Gene | MAF | Crude model | Adjusted model | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | CI |
Interaction p‐value |
β | CI |
Interaction p‐value |
||||||
Verbal IQ | |||||||||||
rs1276529 | 6 | 112,921,192 | G | RFPL4B | 0.32 | −2.04 | −2.86 to −1.22 | 1.10E−06 | −2.09 | −2.91 to −1.26 | 7.13E−07 |
rs1706066 | 6 | 112,923,618 | G | RFPL4B | 0.32 | −2.04 | −2.86 to −1.22 | 1.10E−06 | −2.09 | −2.91 to −1.26 | 7.13E−07 |
rs1276583 | 6 | 112,917,585 | G | RFPL4B | 0.35 | −1.98 | −2.79 to −1.18 | 1.33E−06 | −1.99 | −2.79 to −1.18 | 1.42E−06 |
Performance IQ | |||||||||||
rs705670 | 9 | 137,608,405 | G | LINC01502 | 0.28 | 2.30 | 1.42 to 3.18 | 3.07E−07 | 2.31 | 1.43 to 3.19 | 3.09E−07 |
rs12552228 | 9 | 27,163,693 | T | TEK | 0.09 | 2.49 | 1.33 to 3.65 | 2.70E−05 | 2.79 | 1.61 to 3.97 | 3.87E−06 |
rs12554799 | 9 | 27,163,704 | G | TEK | 0.09 | 2.49 | 1.33 to 3.65 | 2.70E−05 | 2.79 | 1.61 to 3.97 | 3.87E−06 |
Full scale IQ | |||||||||||
rs17800861a | 16 | 9,861,173 | A | GRIN2A | 0.11 | −3.32 | −4.63 to −2.01 | 7.22E−07 | −3.43 | −4.74 to −2.12 | 2.98E−07 |
rs12552228 | 9 | 27,163,693 | T | TEK | 0.09 | 2.44 | 1.33 to 3.55 | 1.81E−05 | 2.84 | 1.71 to 3.96 | 8.51E−07 |
rs12554799 | 9 | 27,163,704 | G | TEK | 0.09 | 2.44 | 1.33 to 3.55 | 1.81E−05 | 2.84 | 1.71 to 3.96 | 8.51E−07 |
Dominant model.
The presence of at least one copy of the A allele at rs17800861 revealed association with lower FSIQ score in children delivered by cesarean section, but not in those delivered vaginally (Figure 2). Similar to rs17800861, the presence of at least one copy of the minor allele at rs1276529 was associated with lower VIQ scores only in children delivered by cesarean section (Supporting Information Figures S7). For rs705670, the presence of at least one copy of the minor allele was associated with higher PIQ scores in children delivered by cesarean section, but not in those delivered by VD (Supporting Information Figures S8). These results remained similar after accounting for other possible confounding perinatal factors (Table 2).
Supporting Information Tables S2–S4 present top interaction signals (p < 1.00E−04) observed in this study. The QQ plots are depicted in Supporting Information Figures S9–S11 and Manhattan plots in Supporting Information Figures S12–S14. λ is close to one in all three models.
3.3. In silico assessment of the most significant SNPs
Examination of the strongest variant, rs17800861, in HaploReg revealed that this SNP is predicted to be located within active chromatin compartments, having transcription enhancer properties in the hippocampus—an important brain region for memory (Morris et al., 2003). The neurons of hippocampus are also capable of neurogenesis, which may be affected by the method of delivery (Seli & Horvath, 2013). This SNP is also reported to alter the NF‐kappaB transcription factor binding motif (https://archive.broadinstitute.org/mammals/haploreg/haploreg.php). NF‐kappaB is a ubiquitous transcription factor, acting as a master switch for the expression of a number of genes involved in immune and inflammatory responses (Sieben, Franzoso, & Brown, 1994). No other SNPs displayed functional activity in the hippocampus, and no SNPs revealed expression quantitative trait locus (eQTL) activity.
4. DISCUSSION
The central hypothesis explored in this study was whether the method of delivery may moderate the genetic disposition of intelligence in children. While we did not observe any genome‐wide significant interactions (p < 5.00E−08), we did note several potentially relevant loci displaying a moderating effect of delivery mode on childhood intelligence (Table 2).
The top finding of this study is the interaction signal at rs17800861 in the intron of GRIN2A gene, identified in the models examining FSIQ (Table 2, Figure 1). GRIN2A encodes a subunit of the glutamate‐gated ion channel protein family member, N‐methyl‐d‐aspartate receptors (NMDARs), which plays a central role in the nervous system by regulating synaptic function (Cull‐Candy, Brickley, & Farrant, 2001). NMDARs are required for spatial learning and memory (Nakazawa, McHugh, Wilson, & Tonegawa, 2004) and are associated with fluid intelligence (problem‐solving ability; Kane & Engle, 2002). GRIN2A has also been linked to a form of benign epilepsy, co‐occurring with intellectual disability (Endele et al., 2010; Lesca et al., 2013; Reutlinger et al., 2010).
Our in silico examination of rs17800861 in HaploReg suggests that this SNP is located within active chromatin in the hippocampus, where neurogenesis might be affected by the mode of delivery (Seli & Horvath, 2013). Moreover, HaploReg also predicted that this SNP may alter the binding of NF‐kappaB factor that has been implicated in several cognition‐related phenotypes, including autism (Naik et al., 2011), schizophrenia (Song, Lv, Li, Hao, & Zhao, 2009), aggression (Brevik et al., 2016), and intellectual disability (Philippe et al., 2009). Interestingly, VD also correlates with increased activity of NF‐kappaB (Lee et al., 2003) and may modulate its expression (Cindrova‐Davies et al., 2007; Li & Karin, 1999).
Other notable interaction signals observed in this study are rs705670 on chromosome 9 for PIQ and rs1276529 on chromosome 6 for VIQ (Table 2). The first SNP (rs705670) is an intronic variant within a long non‐protein‐coding RNA (lncRNA) gene, LINC01502, while rs1276529 is an intergenic SNP located 141,817 bp to RFPL4B (Ret Finger Protein‐Like 4B) gene. lncRNAs have recently been implicated in a number of neuropsychiatric disorders where IQ is affected, such as schizophrenia, fragile X syndrome, and attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Ripke et al., 2014; Vondervoort et al., 2013; Wapinski & Chang, 2011; Zayats et al., 2015), while RFPL4B gene has been linked to language delay (Szafranski et al., 2015) and neuropsychiatric conditions where IQ is also affected (Hudson et al., 2014; Sun, Cheng, Zhang, & Xu, 2015).
Both mother and child experience certain physiological changes during VD that are absent during DCS (Kuguoglu, Yildiz, Tanir, & Demirbag, 2012), including the release of the hormone oxytocin that stimulates uterine muscular contractions (Carter, 2014). Oxytocin also protects the neonatal brain from delivery‐induced hypoxia, exerts an analgesic effect (Cavanagh et al., 2006; Mazzuca et al., 2011), and mediates an excitatory‐to‐inhibitory shift of GABAergic neurons following birth (Cavanagh et al., 2006; Cherubini, Gaiarsa, & Ben‐Ari, 1991). It has also been implicated in a number of neuropsychiatric disorders (Cochran, Fallon, Hill, & Frazier, 2013). The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) has been reported to be associated with IQ, cognition, and daily living skills (Lerer et al., 2008). Interestingly, NMDARs (encoded by the gene where one of our top hits is located) have been proposed to regulate synchronized activity of oxytocin neurons (Moos, Rossi, & Richard, 1997). Oxytocin injections to induce labor have previously been examined as a risk factor for neurodevelopmental disorders, revealing mixed results (Emberti Gialloreti et al., 2014; Glasson, 2013; Juul‐Dam, Townsend, & Courchesne, 2001; Oberg et al., 2016). Together, these studies highlight the need to directly examine a potential causal role of oxytocin on neurodevelopmental outcomes.
Other potential mediators between method of delivery and neurodevelopmental phenotypes include epigenetic changes, microbiome composition, and oxidative stress. Epigenetic changes play an important role in synaptic plasticity and memory formation (Grissom, Lubin, Gupta, & Parrish, 2011) as well as in a number of neuropsychiatric conditions where IQ is affected (Chaste & Leboyer, 2012; Hoffmann, Ziller, & Spengler, 2016; Siniscalco, Cirillo, Bradstreet, & Antonucci, 2013; Walton et al., 2016). Likewise, changes in methylation profiles have been linked to DCS (Schlinzig et al., 2009), though another study did not detect an effect between method of delivery and global methylation of DNA in the blood (Virani et al., 2012).
Since newborns are not exposed to the maternal gut flora during DCS, their microbiome may be different from that of babies delivered vaginally (Black et al., 2015; Salminen et al., 2004). Method of delivery may also affect gut colonization and immunological development of a child (Malamitsi‐Puchner et al., 2005; Vogl et al., 2006). Studies have shown that microbe content may influence behavior and affect brain function (Bercik et al., 2011; Smith, 2015; Yano et al., 2015) as well as the development of neurodevelopmental disorders where IQ is affected (Haavik, Halmøy, Hegvik, & Johansson, 2011).
Neonatal hypoxia may play a role in neurodevelopmental phenotypes, including deficits in memory and learning (Allin et al., 2004; Baoyuan, Salmaso, Komitova, Simonini, & Silbereis, 2011; Fagel et al., 2006; Ment et al., 2003). Our main hit has been linked to NF‐kappaB, whose expression has been reported to be altered by hypoxia during VD (Cindrova‐Davies et al., 2007; Li & Karin, 1999). Hypoxia‐related impairments may be reversed later in life, with recent findings suggesting that such recovery may be dependent on the expression of the genes affected by the method of delivery (Seli & Horvath, 2013). However, further studies are needed to explore whether this disturbance could have long‐lasting effect on the brain function.
The importance of VD has been highlighted in a recent large‐scale epidemiological study, where it was noted that children born by planned cesarean delivery had worse health outcomes than those born by VD, but not than those born by unscheduled cesarean section (Black et al., 2015).
Our study should be viewed in light of its limitations. As the genetics of intelligence are known to be polygenic (Plomin & Deary, 2014), our modest sample size limits our ability to detect interactions of expected small effect sizes. Thus, our results should be replicated and interpreted with caution. Nonetheless, our findings provide insight into the possible effect of method of delivery on the health outcomes in children.
Due to the small sample size and the lack of complete medical history, we could not distinguish between different types of cesarean sections (e.g., scheduled or emergency) that could potentially play an important role in the interpretation of our results as indicated by a recent large‐scale study (Black et al., 2015). The physiology of high‐risk babies delivered by planned cesarean section may also be considered as a confounding effect.
Further studies are needed to provide a conclusive answer to whether VD or DSC has fundamentally different impacts on the intelligence given individual genetic attributes. As the rates of DCSs continue to rise, the need for a better understanding of the physiology behind this method of delivery also continues to grow. Such studies would provide valuable insight for developing guidelines, informed decision‐making, and setting thresholds for the medical necessity of DCS.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We are grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council and the Wellcome Trust (Grant Ref: 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. This publication is the work of the authors, and Dinka Smajlagić, Kaya Kvarme Jacobsen, Craig Myrum, Jan Haavik, Stefan Johansson, and Tetyana Zayats will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper.
This work was supported by Stiftelsen K.G. Jebsen, University of Bergen, the Western Norwegian Health Authorities (Helse Vest), and the European Community's Horizon 2020 Programme (H2020/2014–2020) under the grant agreement no 643051 (MiND).
Smajlagić D, Kvarme Jacobsen K, Myrum C, Haavik J, Johansson S, Zayats T. Moderating effect of mode of delivery on the genetics of intelligence: Explorative genome‐wide analyses in ALSPAC. Brain Behav. 2018;8:e01144 10.1002/brb3.1144
Contributor Information
Dinka Smajlagić, Email: dinka.smajlagic@uib.no.
Tetyana Zayats, Email: tetyana.zayats@uib.no.
REFERENCES
- Allin, M. , Henderson, M. , Suckling, J. , Nosarti, C. , Rushe, T. , Fearon, P. , … Murray, R. (2004). Effects of very low birthweight on brain structure in adulthood. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 46(1), 46–53. 10.1017/S0012162204000088 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baoyuan, B. , Salmaso, N. , Komitova, M. , Simonini, M. V. , & Silbereis, J. (2011). Cortical glial fibrillary acidic protein‐positive cells generate neurons after perinatal hypoxic injury. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(25), 9205–9221. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0518-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bassett, A. S. , Chow, E. W. C. , Waterworth, D. M. , & Linda, B.(2008). Genetic insights into Schizophrenia. Disability and Rehabilitation, 49(2), 743–750. 10.1167/iovs.07-1072 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Belizán, J. M. , Althabe, F. , & Cafferata, M. L. (2007). Health consequences of the increasing caesarean section rates. Epidemiology, 18(4), 485–486. 10.1097/EDE.0b013e318068646a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benyamin, B. , Pourcain, B. , Davis, O. S. , Davies, G. , Hansell, N. K. , Brion, M.‐J. , … Visscher, P. M. (2014). Childhood intelligence is heritable, highly polygenic and associated with FNBP1L. Molecular Psychiatry, 19(2), 253–258. 10.1038/mp.2012.184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bercik, P. , Denou, E. , Collins, J. , Jackson, W. , Lu, J. , Jury, J. , … Collins, S. M. (2011). The intestinal microbiota affect central levels of brain‐derived neurotropic factor and behavior in mice. Gastroenterology, 141(2), 599–609. 10.1053/j.gastro.2011.04.052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Black, M. , Bhattacharya, S. , Philip, S. , Norman, J. E. , & McLernon, D. J. (2015). Planned cesarean delivery at term and adverse outcomes in childhood health. Journal of the American Medical Association, 314(21), 2271 10.1001/jama.2015.16176 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boksa, P. , Zhang, Y. , & Bestawros, A. (2002). Dopamine D1 receptor changes due to caesarean section birth: Effects of anesthesia, developmental time course, and functional consequences. Experimental Neurology, 175(2), 388–397. 10.1006/exnr.2002.7896 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyd, A. , Golding, J. , Macleod, J. , Lawlor, D. A. , Fraser, A. , Henderson, J. , … Davey Smith, G. (2013). Cohort Profile: The ‘children of the 90s’–the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(1), 111–127. 10.1093/ije/dys064 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brake, W. G. , Noel, M. B. , Boksa, P. , & Gratton, A. (1997). Influence of perinatal factors on the nucleus accumbens dopamine response to repeated stress during adulthood: An electrochemical study in the rat. Neuroscience, 77(4), 1067–1076. 10.1016/S0306-4522(96)00543-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brevik, E. J. , van Donkelaar, M. M. J. , Weber, H. , Sánchez‐Mora, C. , Jacob, C. , … Zayats, T. (2016). Genome‐wide analyses of aggressiveness in attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 171(5), 733–747. 10.1002/ajmg.b.32434 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butcher, L. M. , Davis, O. S. P. , Craig, I. W. , & Plomin, R. (2008). Genome‐wide quantitative trait locus association scan of general cognitive ability using pooled DNA and 500K single nucleotide polymorphism microarrays. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 7(4), 435–446. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00368.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardwell, C. R. , Stene, L. C. , Joner, G. , Cinek, O. , Svensson, J. , Goldacre, M. J. , … Patterson, C. C. (2008). Caesarean section is associated with an increased risk of childhood‐onset type 1 diabetes mellitus: A meta‐analysis of observational studies. Diabetologia, 51(5), 726–735. 10.1007/s00125-008-0941-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter, C. S. (2014). Oxytocin pathways and the evolution of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 17–39. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanagh, P. , Intriligator, J. , Nanez, J. E. , Sasaki, Y. , Rensink, R. A. , Clark, J. J. , & Rock, I. (2006). Maternal oxytocin triggers a transient inhibitory switch in GABA signaling in the fetal brain during delivery. Science, 314(5806), 1788–1792. 10.1126/science.1133212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaste, P. , & Leboyer, M. (2012). Autism risk factors: Genes, environment, and gene‐environment interactions. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 14(3), 281–292. 10.2217/epi.11.19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cherubini, E. , Gaiarsa, J. L. , & Ben‐Ari, Y. (1991). GABA: An excitatory transmitter in early postnatal life. Trends in Neurosciences, 14(12), 515–519. 10.1016/0166-2236(91)90003-D [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christilaw, J. E. (2006). Cesarean section by choice: Constructing a reproductive rights framework for the debate. International Journal of Gynecology and Obstetrics, 94(3), 262–268. 10.1016/j.ijgo.2006.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cindrova‐Davies, T. , Yung, H.‐W. , Johns, J. , Spasic‐Boskovic, O. , Korolchuk, S. , Jauniaux, E. , Charnock‐Jones, D. S. (2007). Oxidative stress, gene expression, and protein changes induced in the human placenta during labor. The American Journal of Pathology, 171(4), 1168–1179. 10.2353/ajpath.2007.070528 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cochran, D. M. , Fallon, D. , Hill, M. , & Frazier, J. A. (2013). The role of oxytocin in psychiatric disorders: A review of biological and therapeutic research findings. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 21(5), 219–247. 10.1097/HRP.0b013e3182a75b7d [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cull‐Candy, S. , Brickley, S. , & Farrant, M. (2001). NMDA receptor subunits: Diversity, development and disease. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11(3), 327–335. 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00215-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran, E. A. , Dalman, C. , Kearney, P. M. , Kenny, L. C. , Cryan, J. F. , Dinan, T. G. , & Khashan, A. S. (2015). Association between obstetric mode of delivery and autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(9), 935 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0846 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran, E. A. , Khashan, A. S. , Dalman, C. , Kenny, L. C. , Cryan, J. F. , Dinan, T. G. , & Kearney, P. M. (2016). Obstetric mode of delivery and attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A sibling‐matched study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 45(2), 532–542. 10.1093/ije/dyw001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D'Souza, R. (2013). Caesarean section on maternal request for non‐medical reasons: Putting the UK National Institute of Health and Clinical Excellence guidelines in perspective. Best Practice and Research: Clinical Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 27(2), 165–177. 10.1016/j.bpobgyn.2012.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- David, W. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children 3rd edition manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Davies, G. , Tenesa, A. , Payton, A. , Yang, J. , Sarah, E. , Liewald, D. , … Lopez, L. (2012). UKPMC funders group genome‐wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and polygenic. Molecular Psychiatry, 16(10), 996–1005. 10.1038/mp.2011.85 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis, O. S. P. , Butcher, L. M. , Docherty, S. J. , Meaburn, E. L. , Curtis, C. J. C. , Simpson, M. A. , … Plomin, R. (2010). A three‐stage genome‐wide association study of general cognitive ability: Hunting the small effects. Behavior Genetics, 40(6), 759–767. 10.1007/s10519-010-9350-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deary, I. J. , Johnson, W. , & Houlihan, L. M. (2009). Genetic foundations of human intelligence. Human Genetics, 126(1), 215–232. 10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deary, I. J. , Weiss, A., Batty, G. D. (2010). Intelligence and personality as predictors of illness and death: How researchers in differential psychology and chronic disease epidemiology are collaborating to understand and address health inequalities. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 11(2), 53–79. 10.1177/1529100610387081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El‐Khodor, B. F. , & Boksa, P. (1997). Long‐term reciprocal changes in dopamine levels in prefrontal cortex versus nucleus accumbens in rats born by Caesarean section compared to vaginal birth. Experimental Neurology, 145(1), 118–129. 10.1006/exnr.1997.6437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emberti Gialloreti, L. , Benvenuto, A. , Benassi, F. , & Curatolo, P. (2014). Are caesarean sections, induced labor and oxytocin regulation linked to Autism Spectrum Disorders? Medical Hypotheses, 82(6), 713–718. 10.1016/j.mehy.2014.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Endele, S. , Rosenberger, G. , Geider, K. , Popp, B. , Tamer, C. , Stefanova, I. , … Kutsche, K. (2010). Mutations in GRIN2A and GRIN2B encoding regulatory subunits of NMDA receptors cause variable neurodevelopmental phenotypes. Nature Genetics, 42(11), 1021–1026. 10.1038/ng.677 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fagel, D. M. , Ganat, Y. , Silbereis, J. , Ebbitt, T. , Stewart, W. , Zhang, H. , … Vaccarino, F. M. (2006). Cortical neurogenesis enhanced by chronic perinatal hypoxia. Experimental Neurology, 199(1), 77–91. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2005.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gialluisi, A. , Newbury, D. F. , Wilcutt, E. G. , Olson, R. K. , DeFries, J. C. , Brandler, W. M. , … Fisher, S. E. (2014). Genome‐wide screening for DNA variants associated with reading and language traits. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 13(7), 686–701. 10.1111/gbb.12158 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasson, E. J. (2013). Perinatal factors and the development of autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(6), 618–627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grace, A. A. (2012). Dopamine system dysregulation by the hippocampus: Implications for the pathophysiology and treatment of schizophrenia. Neuropharmacology, 62(3), 1342–1348. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.05.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray, J. R. , & Thompson, P. M. (2004). Neurobiology of intelligence: Science and ethics. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(6), 471–482. 10.1038/nrn1405 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregory, S. G. , Connelly, J. J. , Towers, A. J. , Johnson, J. , Biscocho, D. , Markunas, C. A. , … Pericak‐Vance, M. A. (2009). Genomic and epigenetic evidence for oxytocin receptor deficiency in autism. BMC Medicine, 7, 62 10.1186/1741-7015-7-62 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grissom, N. , Lubin, F. D. , Gupta, S. , & Parrish, R. R. (2011). Epigenetic mechanisms: Critical contributors to long‐term memory formation epigenetic mechanisms: Critical contributors to long‐term memory formation. Neuroscience, 17(6), 616–632. 10.1177/1073858411386967 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guinchat, V. , Thorsen, P. , Laurent, C. , Cans, C. , Bodeau, N. , & Cohen, D. (2012). Pre‐, peri‐ and neonatal risk factors for autism. Acta Obstetricia Et Gynecologica Scandinavica, 91(3), 287–300. 10.1111/j.1600-0412.2011.01325.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haavik, J. , Halmøy, A. , Hegvik, T.‐A. , & Johansson, S. (2011). Maternal genotypes as predictors of offspring mental health: The next frontier of genomic medicine? Future Neurology, 6(6), 731–743. 10.2217/fnl.11.54 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Halmøy, A. , Klungsøyr, K. , Skjærven, R. , & Haavik, J. (2012). Pre‐ and perinatal risk factors in adults with attention‐deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 71(5), 474–481. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.11.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haworth, C. M. , Wright, M. J. , Luciano, M. , Martin, N. G. , de Geus, E. J. C. , van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M. , … Plomin, R. (2010). The heritability of general cognitive ability increases linearly from childhood to young adulthood. Molecular Psychiatry, 15(11), 1112–1120. 10.1038/mp.2009.55 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill, W. D. , Marioni, R. E. , Maghzian, O. , Ritchie, S. J. , Hagenaars, S. P. , McIntosh, A. M. , … Deary, I. J. (2018). A combined analysis of genetically correlated traits identifies 187 loci and a role for neurogenesis and myelination in intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry, 1–13, 10.1038/s41380-017-0001-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffmann, A. , Ziller, M. , & Spengler, D. (2016). The future is the past: Methylation QTLs in schizophrenia. Genes, 7(12), 104 10.3390/genes7120104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Homberg, J. R. , Olivier, J. D. A. , VandenBroeke, M. , Youn, J. , Ellenbroek, A. K. , Karel, P. , … Ellenbroek, B. A. (2016). The role of the dopamine D1 receptor in social cognition: Studies using a novel genetic rat model. Disease Models & Mechanisms, 9(10), 1147–1158. 10.1242/dmm.024752 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howie, B. , Fuchsberger, C. , Stephens, M. , Marchini, J. , & Abecasis, G. R. (2012). Fast and accurate genotype imputation in genome‐wide association studies through pre‐phasing. Nature Genetics, 44(8), 955–959. 10.1038/ng.2354 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hudson, C. , Schwanke, C. , Johnson, J. P. , Elias, A. F. , Phillips, S. , Schwalbe, T. , … Xu, D. (2014). Confirmation of 6q21‐6q22.1 deletion in Acro‐cardio‐facial syndrome and further delineation of this contiguous gene deletion syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part A, 164(8), 2109–2113. 10.1002/ajmg.a.36548 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juul‐Dam, N. , Townsend, J. , & Courchesne, E. (2001). Prenatal, perinatal, and neonatal factors in autism, pervasive developmental disorder‐not otherwise specified, and the general population. Pediatrics, 107(4), E63 10.1542/peds.107.4.e63 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane, M. J. , & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working‐memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual‐differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 637–671. 10.3758/BF03196323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapellou, O. (2011). Effect of caesarean section on brain maturation. Acta Paediatrica, International Journal of Paediatrics, 100(11), 1416–1422. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02427.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelmanson, I. A. (2013). Emotional and behavioural features of preschool children born by Caesarean deliveries at maternal request. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10(6), 676–690. 10.1080/17405629.2013.787024 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler, K. S. , Ohlsson, H. , Sundquist, J. , & Sundquist, K. (2015). IQ and schizophrenia in a Swedish national sample: Their causal relationship and the interaction of IQ with genetic risk. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(3), 259–265. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040516 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkpatrick, R. M. , Mcgue, M. , Iacono, W. G. , Miller, M. B. , & Basu, S. (2014). Results of a “‘GWAS plus:’” General cognitive ability is substantially heritable and massively polygenic. PLoS ONE, 9(11), e112390 10.1371/journal.pone.0112390 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuguoglu, S. , Yildiz, H. , Tanir, M. K. , & Demirbag, B. C. (2012). Breastfeeding after a cesarean delivery, cesarean delivery Raed Salim, IntechOpen. Retrieved from https://www.intechopen.com/books/cesarean‐delivery/breastfeeding‐after‐a‐cesarean‐delivery
- Kuhle, S. , Tong, O. S. , & Woolcott, C. G. (2015). Association between caesarean section and childhood obesity: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Obesity Reviews, 16(4), 295–303. 10.1111/obr.12267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuntsi, J. , Eley, T. C. , Taylor, A. , Hughes, C. , Asherson, P. , Caspi, A. , & Moffitt, T. E. (2004). Co‐occurrence of ADHD and low IQ has genetic origins. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 124B(1), 41–47. 10.1002/ajmg.b.20076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kwee, A. , Cohlen, B. J. , Kanhai, H. H. H. , Bruinse, H. W. , & Visser, G. H. A. (2004). Caesarean section on request: A survey in the Netherlands. European Journal of Obstetrics Gynecology and Reproductive Biology, 113(2), 186–190. 10.1016/j.ejogrb.2003.09.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kyllonen, P. C. , & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working‐memory capacity?! Intelligence, 14(4), 389–433. 10.1016/S0160-2896(05)80012-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lavender, T. , Hofmeyr, G. J. , Neilson, J. P. , Kingdon, C. , & Gyte, M. I. (2014). Caesarean section for non‐medical reasons at term. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, (3), CD004660 10.1002/14651858.CD004660.pub3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee, Y. , Allport, V. , Sykes, A. , Lindstrom, T. , Slater, D. , & Bennett, P. (2003). The effects of labour and of interleukin 1 beta upon the expression of nuclear factor kappa B related proteins in human amnion. Molecular Human Reproduction, 9(4), 213–218. 10.1093/molehr/gag026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lerer, E. , Levi, S. , Salomon, S. , Darvasi, A. , Yirmiya, N. , & Ebstein, R. P. (2008). Association between the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and autism: Relationship to vineland adaptive behavior scales and cognition. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(10), 980–988. 10.1038/sj.mp.4002087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesca, G. , Rudolf, G. , Bruneau, N. , Lozovaya, N. , Labalme, A. , Boutry‐Kryza, N. , … Szepetowski, P. (2013). GRIN2A mutations in acquired epileptic aphasia and related childhood focal epilepsies and encephalopathies with speech and language dysfunction. Nature Genetics, 45(9), 1061–1066. 10.1038/ng.2726 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, N. , & Karin, M. (1999). Is NF‐kappaB the sensor of oxidative stress? The FASEB Journal, 13(10), 1137–1143. 10.1096/fasebj.13.10.1137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, Y. , Willer, C. J. , Ding, J. , Scheet, P. , & Abecasis, G. R. (2010). MaCH: Using sequence and genotype data to estimate haplotypes and unobserved genotypes. Genetic Epidemiology, 34(8), 816–834. 10.1002/gepi.20533 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, Y. , Willer, C. , Sanna, S. , & Abecasis, G. (2009). Genotype imputation. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 10, 387–406. 10.1146/annurev.genom.9.081307.164242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liberati, A. , Altman, D. G. , Tetzlaff, J. , Mulrow, C. , Gøtzsche, P. C. , Ioannidis, J. P. A. , … Moher, D. (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta‐analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med, 6(7), e1000100 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loo, S. K. , Shtir, C. , Doyle, A. E. , Mick, E. , McGough, J. J. , McCracken, J. , … Nelson, S. F. (2012). Genome‐wide association study of intelligence: Additive effects of novel brain expressed genes. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(4), 432–440.e2. 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackay, D. F. , Smith, G. C. S. , Dobbie, R. , & Pell, J. P. (2010). Gestational age at delivery and special educational need: Retrospective cohort study of 407,503 schoolchildren. PLoS Medicine, 7(6), 1–10. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnus, M. C. , Haaberg, S. E. , Stigum, H. , Nafstad, P. , London, S. J. , Vangen, S. , & Nystad, W. (2011). Delivery by cesarean section and early childhood respiratory symptoms and disorders: The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 174(11), 1275–1285. 10.1093/aje/kwr242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malamitsi‐Puchner, A. , Protonotariou, E. , Boutsikou, T. , Makrakis, E. , Sarandakou, A. , & Creatsas, G. (2005). The influence of the mode of delivery on circulating cytokine concentrations in the perinatal period. Early Human Development, 81(4), 387–392. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.10.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martelle, S. E. , Raffield, L. M. , Palmer, N. D. , Cox, A. J. , Freedman, B. I. , Hugenschmidt, C. E. , … Bowden, D. W. (2016). Dopamine pathway gene variants may modulate cognitive performance in the DHS ‐ Mind Study. Brain and Behavior, 6(4), 1–12. 10.1002/brb3.446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazzuca, M. , Minlebaev, M. , Shakirzyanova, A. , Tyzio, R. , Taccola, G. , Janackova, S. , … Khazipov, R. (2011). Newborn analgesia mediated by oxytocin during delivery. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 5, 3 10.3389/fncel.2011.00003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ment, L. R. , Vohr, B. , Allan, W. , Katz, K. H. , Schneider, K. C. , Westerveld, M. , … Makuch, R. W. (2003). Change in cognitive function over time in very low‐birth‐weight infants. Journal of the American Medical Association, 289(6), 705–711. 10.1001/jama.289.6.705 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moos, F. C. , Rossi, K. , & Richard, P. (1997). Activation of N‐methyl‐D‐aspartate receptors regulates basal electrical activity of oxytocin and vasopressin neurons in lactating rats. Neuroscience, 77(4), 993–1002. 10.1016/S0306-4522(96)00536-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris, R. G. M. , Moser, E. I. , Riedel, G. , Martin, S. J. , Sandin, J. , Day, M. , & O'Carroll, C. (2003). Elements of a neurobiological theory of the hippocampus: The role of activity‐dependent synaptic plasticity in memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358(1432), 773–786. 10.1098/rstb.2002.1264 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naik, U. S. , Gangadharan, C. , Abbagani, K. , Nagalla, B. , Dasari, N. , & Manna, S. K. (2011). A study of nuclear transcription factor‐kappa B in childhood autism. PLoS ONE, 6(5), 1–6. 10.1371/journal.pone.0019488 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakazawa, K. , McHugh, T. J. , Wilson, M. A. , & Tonegawa, S. (2004). NMDA receptors, place cells and hippocampal spatial memory. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 5(5), 361–372. 10.1038/nrn1385 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neves, G. , Cooke, S. F. , & Bliss, T. V. P. (2008). Synaptic plasticity, memory and the hippocampus ‐ a neural network approach to causality. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 65–75. 10.1038/nrn2303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nisbett, R. E. , Aronson, J. , Blair, C. , Dickens, W. , Flynn, J. , Halpern, D. F. , & Turkheimer, E. (2012). Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67(2), 130–159. 10.1037/a0026699 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberg, A. S. , D'Onofrio, B. M. , Rickert, M. E. , Hernandez‐Diaz, S. , Ecker, J. L. , Almqvist, C. , … Bateman, B. T. (2016). Association of labor induction with offspring risk of autism spectrum disorders. JAMA Pediatrics, 61(3), e160965 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.0965 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Philippe, O. , Rio, M. , Carioux, A. , Plaza, J. M. , Guigue, P. , Molinari, F. , … Colleaux, L. (2009). Combination of linkage mapping and microarray‐expression analysis identifies NF‐kappaB signaling defect as a cause of autosomal‐recessive mental retardation. American Journal of Human Genetics, 85(6), 903–908. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.11.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plomin, R. , & Deary, I. (2014). Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings. Molecular Psychiatry, 20, 98–108. 10.1038/mp.2014.105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purcell, S. , Neale, B. , Todd‐Brown, K. , Thomas, L. , Ferreira, M. A. , Bender, D. , … Sham, P. C. (2007). PLINK: A tool set for whole‐genome association and population‐based linkage analyses. American Journal of Human Genetics, 81(3), 559–575. 10.1086/519795 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reutlinger, C. , Helbig, I. , Gawelczyk, B. , Subero, J. I. M. , Tönnies, H. , … Caliebe, A. (2010). Deletions in 16p13 including GRIN2A in patients with intellectual disability, various dysmorphic features, and seizure disorders of the rolandic region. Epilepsia, 51(9), 1870–1873. 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02555.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ripke, S. , Neale, B. M. , Corvin, A. , Walters, J. T. R. , Farh, K.‐H. , Holmans, P. A. , … O'Donovan, M. C. (2014). Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia‐associated genetic loci. Nature, 511, 421–427. 10.1038/nature13595 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Risch, N. , & Merikangas, K. (1996). The future of genetic studies of complex human diseases. Science, 273, 1516–1517. 10.1126/science.273.5281.1516 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ronca, A. E. , Abel, R. A. , Ronan, P. J., Alberts, J. R. (2009). Effects of labor contractions on catecholamine release and breathing frequency in newborn rats. Obstetrics & Gynecology, 120(6), 1308–1314. 10.1037/0735-7044.120.6.1308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Round, J. L. , & Mazmanian, S. K. (2009). The gut microbiome shapes intestinal immune responses during health and disease. Nature Reviews. Immunology, 9(5), 313–323. 10.1038/nri2515 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salminen, S. , Gibson, G. R. , & McCartney, A. L., Isolauri, E. (2004). Influence of mode of delivery on gut microbiota composition in seven year old children. Gut, 53(9), 1388–1389. 10.1136/gut.2004.041640 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlinzig, T. , Johansson, S. , Gunnar, A. , Ekström, T. , & Norman, M. (2009). Epigenetic modulation at birth – Altered DNA‐methylation in white blood cells after Caesarean section. Acta Paediatrica, International Journal of Paediatrics, 98(7), 1096–1099. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2009.01371.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seli, E. , & Horvath, T. L. (2013). Natural birth‐induced UCP2 in brain development. Reviews in Endocrine and Metabolic Disorders, 14(4), 347–350. 10.1007/s11154-013-9262-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sieben, U. , Franzoso, G. , & Brown, K. (1994). Structure, regulation and function of NF‐kappa B. Annual Review of Cell Biology, 10, 405–455. 10.1146/annurev.cb.10.110194.002201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon‐Areces, J. , Dietrich, M. O. , Hermes, G. , Garcia‐Segura, L. M. , Arevalo, M. A. , & Horvath, T. L. (2012). Ucp2 induced by natural birth regulates neuronal differentiation of the hippocampus and related adult behavior. PLoS ONE, 7(8), 2–9. 10.1371/journal.pone.0042911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siniscalco, D. , Cirillo, A. , Bradstreet, J. J. , & Antonucci, N. (2013). Epigenetic findings in autism: New perspectives for therapy. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 10(9), 4261–4273. 10.3390/ijerph10094261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, P. A. (2015). Brain, meet gut. Nature, 526(7573), 312 10.1038/526312a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sniekers, S. , Stringer, S. , Watanabe, K. , Jansen, P. R. , Coleman, J. R. I. , Krapohl, E. , … Posthuma, D. (2017). Genome‐wide association meta‐ Analysis of 78,308 individuals identifies new loci and genes influencing human intelligence. Nature Genetics, 49(7), 1107–1112. 10.1038/ng.3869 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song, X.‐Q. , Lv, L.‐X. , Li, W.‐Q. , Hao, Y.‐H. , & Zhao, J.‐P. (2009). The interaction of nuclear factor‐kappa B and cytokines is associated with schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 65(6), 481–488. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song, M. , Zhou, Y. , Li, J. , Liu, Y. , Tian, L. , Yu, C. , & Jiang, T. (2008). Brain spontaneous functional connectivity and intelligence. NeuroImage, 41(3), 1168–1176. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sucksdorff, M. , Lehtonen, L. , Chudala, R. , Suominena, A. , Gisslerd, M. , & Sourandera, A. (2018). Lower Apgar scores and Caesarean section are related to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Paediatrica, International Journal of Paediatrics, 107, 1750–1758. 10.1111/apa.14349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun, L. , Cheng, Z. , Zhang, F. , & Xu, Y. (2015). Gene expression profiling in peripheral blood mononuclear cells of early‐onset schizophrenia. Genomics Data, 5, 169–170. 10.1016/j.gdata.2015.04.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szafranski, P. , Von Allmen, G. K. , Graham, B. H. , Wilfong, A. A. , Kang, S.‐H.‐L. , Ferreira, J. A. , … Lalani, S. R. (2015). 6Q22.1 Microdeletion and susceptibility to pediatric epilepsy. European Journal of Human Genetics, 23(2), 173–179. 10.1038/ejhg.2014.75 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thavagnanam, S. , Fleming, J. , Bromley, A. , Shields, M. D. , & Cardwell, C. R. (2008). A meta‐analysis of the association between Caesarean section and childhood asthma. Clinical and Experimental Allergy, 38(4), 629–633. 10.1111/j.1365-2222.2007.02780.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toga, A. W. , & Thompson, P. M. (2005). Genetics of brain structure and intelligence. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 1–23. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135655 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaillancourt, C. , & Boksa, P. (1998). Caesarean section birth with general anesthesia increases dopamine‐mediated behavior in the adult rat. Neuroreport, 9(13), 2953–2959. 10.1097/00001756-199809140-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van de Vondervoort, I. , Gordebeke, P. M. , Khoshab, N. , Tiesinga, P. H. , Buitelaar, J. K. , Kozicz, T. , … Glennon, J. C. (2013). Long non‐coding RNAs in neurodevelopmental disorders. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience, 6, 53 10.3389/fnmol.2013.00053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varela, L. , Schwartz, M. L. , & Horvath, T. L. (2016). Mitochondria controlled by UCP2 determine hypoxia‐induced synaptic remodeling in the cortex and hippocampus. Neurobiology of Disease, 90, 68–74. 10.1016/j.nbd.2016.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vasconcelos, M. S. , Sampaio, A. S. , Hounie, A. G. , Akkerman, F. , Curi, M. , Lopes, A. C. , & Miguel, E. C. (2007). Prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal risk factors in obsessive‐compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 61(3), 301–307. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villar, J. , Valladares, E. , Wojdyla, D. , Zavaleta, N. , Carroli, G. , Velazco, A. , … Acosta, A. (2006). Caesarean delivery rates and pregnancy outcomes: The 2005 WHO global survey on maternal and perinatal health in Latin America. Lancet, 367(9525), 1819–1829. 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68704-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Virani, S. , Dolinoy, D. C. , Halubai, S. , Jones, T. R. , Domino, S. E. , Rozek, L. S. , … Padmanabhan, V. (2012). Delivery type not associated with global methylation at birth. Clinical Epigenetics, 4(1), 8 10.1186/1868-7083-4-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Visscher, P. M. , Hill, W. G. , & Wray, N. R. (2008). Heritability in the genomics era – Concepts and misconceptions. Nature Reviews Genetics, 9(4), 255–266. 10.1038/nrg2322 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogl, S. E. , Worda, C. , Egarter, C. , Bieglmayer, C. , Szekeres, T. , Huber, J. , & Husslein, P. (2006). Mode of delivery is associated with maternal and fetal endocrine stress response. BJOG: an International Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 113(4), 441–445. 10.1111/j.1471-0528.2006.00865.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walton, E. , Pingault, J.‐B. , Cecil, C. A. M. , Gaunt, T. R. , Relton, C. L. , Mill, J. , & Barker, E. D. (2016). Epigenetic profiling of ADHD symptoms trajectories: A prospective, methylome‐wide study. Molecular Psychiatry, 44, 1–7. 10.1038/mp.2016.85 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wapinski, O. , & Chang, H. Y. (2011). Long noncoding RNAs and human disease. Trends in Cell Biology, 21(6), 354–361. 10.1016/j.tcb.2011.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward, L. D. , & Kellis, M. (2012). HaploReg: A resource for exploring chromatin states, conservation, and regulatory motif alterations within sets of genetically linked variants. Nucleic Acids Research, 40(D1), 1–5. 10.1093/nar/gkr917 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. , Rust, J. , & Golombok, S. (1992). WISC‐III UK Wechsler intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.). Sidcup, UK: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson‐Costello, D. , Friedman, H. , Minich, N. , Siner, B. , Taylor, G. , Schluchter, M. , & Hack, M. (2007). Improved neurodevelopmental outcomes for extremely low birth weight infants in 2000–2002. Pediatrics, 119(1), 37–45. 10.1542/peds.2006-1416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yano, J. M. , Yu, K. , Donaldson, G. P. , Shastri, G. G. , Ann, P. , Ma, L. , … Hsiao, E. Y. (2015). c bacteria from the gut microbiota regulate host serotonin biosynthesis. Cell, 161(2), 264–276. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.02.047 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zayats, T. , Athanasiu, L. , Sonderby, I. , Djurovic, S. , Westlye, L. T. , Tamnes, C. K. , … Haavik, J. (2015). Genome‐wide analysis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Norway. PLoS One, 10(4), e0122501 10.1371/journal.pone.0122501 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.