Abstract
Objective:
This study examined the longitudinal association between parental warmth and offspring flourishing in mid-life. We also considered associations between parental warmth and a number of mental health problems and adverse health behavioral outcomes.
Method:
Longitudinal data from the Midlife in the United States Study (N=3,929, mean baseline age=47.4 years) were analyzed using generalized estimating equations. Parental warmth in childhood was recalled at phase I (1995–1996), while flourishing and other outcomes were self reported at phase II (2004–2006). Following an approach developed by Keyes, flourishing was operationalized as a combined measure incorporating assessments of three aspects of well-being, including emotional, psychological, and social well-being.
Results:
The results suggest that parental warmth was positively associated with the continuous score of flourishing (B=0.21, 95% CI=0.18, 0.25). The association was not specific to any particular component (emotional, psychological, or social well-being) or subdomain of flourishing. Parental warmth was also inversely associated with several adverse health behavior outcomes such as drug use and smoking.
Conclusions:
Parental warmth in childhood may help promote offspring functioning across multiple domains of well-being in mid-life. The findings help to strengthen the call for a public health focus on the importance of parenting for outcomes beyond childhood and well into adulthood, and suggest the value of targeting parenting practices for prevention and intervention strategies to improve population health and well-being.
Keywords: parental warmth, flourishing, well-being, health assets, life course
Introduction
Empirical research has linked flourishing, measured with various approaches, to a broad range of better outcomes such as fewer conduct problems in adolescents and lower mortality in adults (Keyes, 2006; Keyes & Simoes, 2012; World Health Organization, 2009a). In comparison, antecedents of flourishing have remained relatively understudied (Huppert & So, 2013). While mental wellbeing and illness are correlated concepts and many risk factors for mental illness have been identified (Manderscheid et al., 2010), they are distinct concepts and may have different predictors (World Health Organization, 2009a). Understanding factors that promote well-being may require different research from what has been done to reduce illness, as reducing illness alone may not be sufficient for identifying strategies that enable individuals to attain and maintain well-being (Kobau et al., 2011; Seligman, 2008).
While there has not been a standard definition of flourishing given its multifaceted nature, it is sometimes understood as “a combination of feeling good and functioning effectively, and the experience that life is going well” (Huppert & So, 2013) or “a state in which all aspects of a person’s life are good” (VanderWeele, 2017a). The concept of human flourishing has received growing interest as an additional approach to measuring progress, improving policy decisions, and enhancing population well-being (Hone et al., 2014).
There is some general consensus across various conceptual models of flourishing that have been proposed (Diener et al., 2010; Huppert & So, 2013; Keyes, 2002; Seligman, 2012; VanderWeele, 2017a). That is, flourishing is a multi-dimensional construct combining multiple aspects of well-being, and it captures the high end of well-being spectrum (Hone et al., 2014). While there has been a concern that flourishing is no more than a reframing of work on psychopathology, growing evidence has suggested that the absence of illness does not necessarily imply the presence of positive health and well-being (Keyes, 2002). For instance, the association between psychological well-being and cardiovascular health often remained robust after adjusting for psychological ill-being (Boehm & Kubzansky, 2012; Soo et al., 2018). Such work provides evidence that well-being or flourishing is worth studying in its own right.
Having positive family relationships has been hypothesized as an essential asset in promoting flourishing (VanderWeele, 2017a). In particular, the parent-child relationship is crucial for shaping offspring trajectories of health and well-being over the life span (Ranson & Urichuk, 2008). For instance, negative parenting practices (e.g., cold, unsupportive, neglectful) may create or exacerbate children’s vulnerability to emotional dysregulation, psychological distress, social incompetency, disrupted neurobiological functioning, and the adoption of risky behaviors (e.g., smoking, drug use) as stress coping strategies. Such biopsychosocial risk profiles and adverse behavioral patterns may persist into adulthood and also increase individuals’ exposure to further adversity as they age, leading to long-term health consequences (Elstad, 2005; Felitti et al., 1998; Repetti et al., 2002). Conversely, positive parenting (e.g., secure attachment) provides children with a sense of emotional security and social integration. It shapes children’s trust and positive expectations of others, and boosts a sense of self-worth. Such experiences facilitate the formation of developmental and salutogenic assets such as psychosocial resources and behavioral patterns that would help increase resilience, maintain health, and promote multiple aspects of well-being in later life (Bretherton, 1992). One dimension of such positive parenting attributes is parental warmth. A number of empirical studies have linked parental warmth to better offspring health and well-being outcomes such as greater psychosocial well-being, and lower risk of adverse health behaviors, mental disorders and physical illness in later life (Huppert et al., 2010; Khaleque, 2013; Ranson & Urichuk, 2008; Shakya et al., 2012; Yap et al., 2014). However, prior work has often examined parental warmth in relation to a single dimension of well-being in separate studies. To our knowledge, the association between parental warmth and composite measures of flourishing (incorporating multiple aspects of well-being simultaneously) has not been studied. Flourishing, as broadly conceived, provides a more comprehensive picture of health, well-being, and wholeness (VanderWeele, 2017a). For instance, the composite measure of flourishing was a stronger predictor of all-cause mortality, as compared to considering single components (e.g., emotional, psychological, social) of well-being separately (Keyes & Simoes, 2012). Parental warmth likely exerts pervasive influences on multiple aspects of offspring well-being through various pathways (Bretherton, 1992; Ranson & Urichuk, 2008), and multi-component measures of flourishing may provide a more holistic evaluation of such effects. While interventions on improving parenting are available (World Health Organization, 2007, 2009b), the progress on evaluating and implementing such programs has been relatively slow (Chu et al., 2012). Evidence on the long-term association between parental warmth and composite flourishing may help strengthen the case for implementing parenting programs at the population level, and for targeting parenting practices to improve population health and well-being over the lifecourse.
This study examined the longitudinal association between parental warmth and offspring flourishing in mid-life. Following an approach developed by Keyes (2002), flourishing was operationalized as a combined meaure incorporating assessments of three aspects of well-being, including emotional, psychological, and social well-being. Following prior work (Adam & Chase-Lansdale, 2002; Gunnoe et al., 1999; Slack et al., 2004; Slopen et al., 2017; Walsh et al., 2003), we adjusted for a wide range of childhood familial factors that have been linked to parenting and/or offspring health (e.g., family socioeconomic status, parental substance use, family religiosity) as covariates. There has been suggestive evidence that mental illness and some adverse health behaviors may be associated with lower likelihood to flourish (Keyes & Simoes, 2012). Therefore, in secondary analyses we took an outcome-wide analytic approach (VanderWeele, 2017b) to examine parental warmth in relation to multiple mental health problems (e.g., depression) and adverse health behavior outcomes (e.g., smoking, drug use). We also examined maternal and paternal warmth separately across outcomes. We hypothesized that parental warmth would be positively associated with the composite flourishing measures and the associations would not be specific to particular component or subdomain of flourishing, but rather parental warmth would influence all three primary flourishing components (i.e., emotional, phychological, and social well-being) as well as the 13 subdomains that comprise these three components (Keyes, 2002) (see methods and the supplementary materials for details). We also expected that parental warmth would be inversely associated with various mental health problems and adverse behavioral outcomes. In addition, we hypothesized that maternal and paternal warmth would both matter for offspring flourishing.
Method
Study Sample
This study used data from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study, initiated between 1995 and 1996 to investigate psychosocial factors, health, and well-being in mid-life. The first phase of the study (MIDUS I) recruited 7,108 non-institutionalized individuals (including 950 siblings and 957 twin pairs) between 25 and 74 years old from across the United States through random digit dialing. Over 90% of the respondents were White. Participants were invited to participate in a phone interview, and were then mailed a self-administered questionnaire. The second phase of the study (MIDUS II) was conducted around 2004 to 2006, which followed up 70% (N=4,963) of the original participants (Radler & Ryff, 2010).
Parental warmth and flourishing were only assessed in the self-administered questionnaire (SAQ). A total of 6,325 participants completed the SAQ at phase I, and 4,669 of them remained in the cohort at phase II (Table S1 compares baseline characteristics of the participants who remained in the cohort and those lost to follow-up). Among participants who remained in the cohort (N=4,669), 3,929 individuals completed the SAQ at phase II. The analytic sample for the current study was drawn from those who completed the SAQ at both phases (N=3,929; 569 of them either siblings or twins). Since this study used de-identified and publicly available data, it was exempted from review by the institutional review board (IRB) at Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health. The original MIDUS study was approved by the IRB at the University of Wisconsin-Madison. All participants provided written informed consent (Radler, 2014).
Exposure Assessment
Parental warmth.
Parental warmth during a respondent’s years of growing up was measured with a six-item Parental Support Scale (Rossi, 2001) at phase I (Table S2). Maternal and paternal warmth were assessed separately (e.g., “How much love and affection did your mother/father give you?”). Response categories ranged from one (a lot) to four (not at all). Responses were reverse coded so that higher scores indicate greater warmth. In our analytic sample, over 98% of the participants had data on all items of the maternal warmth scale, and another 1% had data on at least half of the scale items (i.e., three out of the six items). Simiarly, over 94% of the participants had data on all items of the paternal warmth scale, and another 1% had data on at least half of the scale items. For participants with data on at least half of the scale items, missing data were imputed with the mean value of the completed items (Graham, 2009); participants with missing data on more than half of the scale items were considered as missing on maternal or paternal warmth. Maternal and paternal warmth were calculated by summing responses across items on the maternal (α = 0.89) and paternal scales (α = 0.91) separately. An overall parental warmth score was created by taking the average of maternal and paternal warmth (Rothrauff et al., 2009). The parental warmth score was used as a continuous variable. As a sensitivity analysis, we also created tertiles of the parental warmth score based on its distribution in the sample, to assess the possibility of discontinuous effects (Boehm et al., 2017).
Primary Outcome Assessment
Following Keyes’ definition (Keyes, 2002), composite flourishing was assessed with the components of emotional, psychological, and social well-being. All components were measured at phase II as described below (Keyes, 2002; Keyes & Simoes, 2012).
Emotional well-being.
Two aspects of emotional well-being were measured (Keyes & Simoes, 2012). First, a six-item validated positive affect scale (Crawford & Henry, 2004; Mroczek & Kolarz, 1998) was used to assess positive feelings (i.e., cheerful, in good spirits, happy, calm and peaceful, satisfied, full of life) over the past 30 days. Response options ranged from one (all the time) to five (none of the time). When appropriate, responses were reverse coded so higher scores reflect greater positive feeling. In our analytic sample, 98% of the participants had valid data on all scale items, and another 1.68% had data on at least half of the scale items (i.e., three out of the six items). For participants with data on at least half of the scale items, missing data were imputed with the mean value of the completed items (Graham, 2009); those with missing data on more than half of the scale items were considered as missing on positive feelings. An overall positive affect score (α =0.72) was calculated by summing responses across items. Second, a single question (“ how would you rate your life overall these days”) was used to assess life satisfaction on a scale from zerp (worst possible life overall) to 10 (best possible life overall). An overall emotional well-being score (α =0.72) was calculated by summing the standardized positive affect and life satisfaction scores (i.e., the two scores were assessed with different scales, so they were both standardized at mean=0, standard deviation [SD]=1 before summing).
Psychological well-being.
Psychological well-being was assessed with Ryff’s validated 18-item scale (Ryff, 1989; Ryff & Keyes, 1995). The scale measured six dimensions of subjective well-being including self-acceptance (e.g., “I like most parts of my personality”), positive relations with others (e.g., “People would describe me as a giving person, willing to share my time with others”), personal growth (e.g., “For me, life has been a continuous process of learning, changing and growth”), purpose in life (e.g., “Some people wander aimlessly through life, but I am not one of them”), environmental mastery (e.g., “In general, I feel I am in charge of the situation in which I live”) and autonomy (e.g., “I judge myself by what I think is important, not by the values of what others think is important”). Response categories ranged from one (strongly agree) to seven (strongly disagree). When appropriate, responses were reverse coded such that higher scores reflect greater well-being. In our analytic sample, over 99% of the participants had data on all scale items, and another 0.4% of the participants had data on two out of the three items across scales. Following the MIDUS coding book, for participants with missing data on one scale item, missing data were imputed with the mean value of the completed items on that scale (Graham, 2009); those with missing data on more than one scale item were considered as missing on that scale. An overall psychological well-being score (α=0.80) was created by summing scores across all six subscales.
Social well-being.
Social well-being was measured with Keyes’ validated 15-item scale (Keyes & Shapiro, 2004; Keyes, 1998). The scale assessed five dimensions of social functioning including social acceptance (e.g., “I believe that people are kind”), social actualization (e.g., “The world is becoming a better place for everyone”), social contribution (e.g., “I have something valuable to give to the world”), social coherence (e.g., “The world is too complex for me”, reverse coded) and social integration (e.g., “My community is a source of comfort”). Response categories ranged from one (strongly agree) to seven (strongly disagree). Responses were reverse coded when appropriate, such that higher scores reflect greater well-being. In our analytic sample, 98% of the participants had data on all scale items, and another 0.5% of the participants had data on at least half of the scale items (e.g., two out of the three items). Following the MIDUS coding book, for participants with missing data on less than half of the scale items, missing data were imputed with the mean value of the completed items on that scale (Graham, 2009); those with missing data on more than half of the items were considered as missing on that scale. An overall social well-being score (α =0.74) was calculated by summing scores across all five subscales.
Flourishing.
Our primary outcome was a continuous measure of flourishing.
Specifically, the emotional, psychological, and social well-being scores were first standardized (M=0, SD=1) because they were assessed with different scales. An overall flourishing score was then created by summing the standardized emotional, psychological, and social well-being scores (see Table S3 for the distribution of the flourishing score and its components and subdomains before standardization). As a sensitivity analysis, a dichotomized measure of flourishing was created following Keyes’ criteria (Keyes, 2002; Keyes & Simoes, 2012): to be considered as flourishing, participants need to show high levels (i.e., top tertile) of emotional well-being on at least one of the two subscales (i.e., positive affect or life satisfaction) as well as high levels (i.e., top tertile) of psychological and social well-being on at least six of the eleven subscales (e.g., self-acceptance, social integration). As a second sensitivity analysis, to better capture functioning across all subdomains, we also created a count measure of flourishing that summed the number of subscales on which the participant had a score in the top tertile, ranging from zero to 13. Considering potential limitations of using researcher-determined thresholds (e.g., top tertile split) in creating the dichotomized and the count measure (Hone et al., 2014; Keyes, 2007), the continuous measure of flourishing, was taken as the primary outcome, which may provide a broarder picture of well-being on a continuum.
Secondary Outcome Assessment
Mental health problems.
The Composite International Diagnostic Interview Short Form (CIDI-SF) (Kessler et al., 1998) was used to assess major depression and generalized anxiety disorder over the past year. The CIDI-SF was validated against physician diagnosis based on the full CIDI in prior work, and showed excellent diagnostic sensitivity and specificity. For instance, the overall concordance between CIDI-SF and the full CIDI is 93.2% on classifications of major depression and 99.6% on classifications of generalized anxiety disorder (Kessler et al., 1999).
Adverse health behaviors.
Participants reported whether they ever used marijuana or any other substance (cocaine, hallucinogens, or heroin) on their own over the past 12 months (yes, no) (Turiano et al., 2012). They also reported their current height (in inches) and weight (in pounds) based on which the body mass index (BMI, kg/m2) was calculated. A subgroup of respondents (N=1,255) participated in a biomarker project at phase II (Dienberg Love et al., 2010). The self-reported BMI was validated against measured BMI among the biomarker project participants, and showed high concordance (r=0.92). BMI ≥ 25 kg/m2 was defined as overweight or obese, and < 25 kg/m2 as normal weight (World Health Organization, 1995). Participants who reported smoking cigarettes regularly now or in the past were considered as current or former smoker (Boehm et al., 2016). Those who reported ever having five or more drinks on the same occasion over past month were considered as heavy drinkers (Cutler & Lleras-Muney, 2010).
Covariates Assessment
Demographic characteristics.
Demographic covariates include participant age (in years), gender (male, female), race (White, Black, others), nativity (born in the United States, born outside the United States), parents’ nativity (parents were born in the United States, born outside the United States), and the number of siblings (used as a continuous variable), all measured at phase I.
Childhood family environment factors.
All childhood familial characteristics were assessed at phase I unless otherwise specified. Participants’ childhood socioeconomic status (SES) was measured with threel indicators: the highest level of education attained by parents (less than high school, high school, some college, college or more), whether their family was on welfare for six months or more during their years of growing up (yes, no), and their self-rated financial levels in childhood compared to the average family (on a scale from one to seven). The three childhood SES indicators were included in models as separate variables. Participants also reported whether they lived with both biological parents until they were 16 years (yes, no). Residence area during childhood was also queried (rural, small town, medium-sized town, suburbs, city, moved around). A single question was used to assess childhood residential stability (“How many times during your childhood did you move to a totally new neighborhood or town?’). Those who moved less than three times were considered as having residential stability (yes, no) (Bures, 2003). Respondents also reported whether their mother and father were smokers (yes, no), and whether they lived with alcoholics when growing up (yes, no, recalled at phase II). In addition, a single question was used to assess childhood family religiousness: “How important was religion in your home when you were growing up?”. Those who reported the highest level of importance were considered as being raised in a religious family (yes, no).
Statistical Analyses
Statistical analyses were performed in SAS 9.4. We considered several descriptive analyses. Analysis of variance tests and chi-square tests were first used to examine the correlation between participant characteristics and tertiles of parental warmth. We also used a generalized estimating equation model (GEE, with Poission distribution) to regress the top tertile of parental warmth on all covariates simultaneously, adjusting for clustering by sibling status. We also explored the cross-sectional sociodemographic distribution of flourishing by regressing the dichotomized flourishing indicator on a number of adulthood characteristics simultanesouly (see supplemental materials for details).
Multiple GEE models (with normal distribution) were used to model the continuous scores of flourishing and its three components (emotional, psychological, and social well-being) as the dependent variables and with the continuous score of parental warmth (standardized at M=0, SD=1 for easier interpretation) as the independent variable in separate models, while adjusting for clustering by sibling status. We standardized (M=0, SD=1) all continuous outcomes so that effect estimates could be compared across outcomes. We also reanalyzed the models with the dichotomized indicator of flourishing (with Poisson distribution) and the count measure of flourishing (with normal distribution, standardized) as the dependent variable separately. All models controlled for demographic characteristics and a wide range of childhood family environment factors. To examine whether the association between parental warmth and flourishing, if any, was specific to particular subdomain of flourishing, we reanalyzed the primary sets of models with each of the 13 subdomains comprising emotional, psychological, or social well-being as the dependent variable in separate models. In secondary analyses, we examined the associations between parental warmth and an array of mental health problems and adverse health behavior outcomes. A similar modeling strategy as the primary analysis was used to regress each outcome on parental warmth in separate models. Bonferroni correction was used to correct for multiple testing. We also reanalyzed the models using tertiles of the parental warmth score as the independent variable as a sensitivity analysis.
We examined maternal and paternal warmth separately across outcomes. Specifically, the primary sets of models were reanalyzed with maternal warmth and paternal warmth as the independent variable in separate models and also simultaneously in one model.
Missing Data
In the analytic sample (N=3,929), 10 participants had missing data on parental warmth, 250 had missing data on one or more covariates, and the number with missing data on outcome variables ranged from zero to 158 individuals, depending on the outcome. Complete-case analysis would result in a loss of 9.0% (n=353) of the respondents for analyses on flourishing (Table S4 compares baseline characteristics of participants with complete data for analyses on flourishing and participants with missing data), and up to 10.6% (n=418) loss for analyses on other outcomes. Therefore, we used a multivariate normal multiple imputation procedure to impute missing data on all variables. Multiple imputation often provides more accurate estimates than other methods of handling missing data (Sterne et al., 2009). Specifically, we included all variables used in this study (i.e., the exposure variable, outcome variables and all covariates) in the imputation procedure, and created five imputed datasets. We present results with multiple imputation for all analyses, and also report results of the complete-case analysis as a sensitivity analysis.
Results
Descriptive Analyses
Participants were mostly White and slightly higher percentage female, with a mean baseline age of 47.4 years (SD=12.4) (Table 1). Around 22.2% of participants met criteria for the dichotomized flourishing measure at phase II. Correlations between the three components of flourishing (emotion, psychosocial, social well-being) ranged from 0.42 to 0.61. Correlations between the 13 subdomains ranged from 0.12 to 0.57 (Table S5). Correlation of the dichotomized flourishing indicator with depression and anxiety was −0.13 and −0.06 (both p<0.001). Table 1 shows participant characteristics across tertiles of parental warmth. When parental warmth was regressed on all covariates simultaneously, greater childhood family religiousness was the strongest predictor of high parental warmth (Table S6).
Table 1.
Distribution of participant characteristics by recalled parental warmth (N=3,929).
Parental Warmth |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Full sample | Bottom tertile | Middle tertile | Top tertile | p-value | |
n = 3,929 | n = 1,288 | n =1,439 | n = 1,192 | ||
Age (range: 24–75), mean (SD) | 47.4 (12.4) | 47.0 (11.7) | 46.9 (12.2) | 48.4 (13.4) | 0.005 |
Male, % | 44.5 | 40.4 | 46.8 | 46.2 | 0.001 |
White, % | 93.5 | 93.7 | 94.9 | 91.6 | <0.001 |
Born in US, % | 95.8 | 95.3 | 96.2 | 96.0 | 0.43 |
Mother born in US, % | 90.6 | 90.5 | 90.8 | 90.9 | 0.93 |
Father born in US, % | 90.0 | 90.4 | 90.4 | 89.3 | 0.57 |
Lived with biological parents, % | 80.8 | 73.0 | 84.2 | 85.2 | <0.001 |
Number of siblings (range: 0–5), mean (SD) | 3.1 (1.6) | 3.2 (1.5) | 3.1 (1.5) | 2.9 (1.6) | <0.001 |
Highest parental education, % | <0.001 | ||||
Less than high school | 25.8 | 29.0 | 23.4 | 25.0 | |
High school | 36.1 | 37.9 | 35.1 | 35.4 | |
Some college | 15.7 | 14.9 | 15.5 | 16.9 | |
College or more | 22.4 | 18.3 | 26.0 | 22.7 | |
Family on welfare or ADC, % | 5.7 | 9.5 | 4.8 | 2.6 | <0.001 |
Subjective SES (range: 1–7), mean (SD) | 4.0 (1.3) | 3.7 (1.4) | 4.1 (1.2) | 4.3 (1.2) | <0.001 |
Area where raised, % | 0.007 | ||||
Rural | 24.6 | 20.9 | 24.7 | 28.3 | |
Small town | 25.9 | 26.0 | 26.0 | 25.5 | |
Medium-sized town | 11.8 | 12.9 | 11.5 | 11.0 | |
Suburbs | 15.6 | 16.2 | 16.6 | 13.9 | |
City | 18.0 | 19.0 | 17.0 | 18.0 | |
Moved around | 4.2 | 5.0 | 4.2 | 3.3 | |
Residential stability in childhood, % | 74.8 | 69.9 | 75.1 | 80.0 | <0.001 |
Mom was smoker | 30.4 | 33.6 | 30.5 | 26.8 | 0.001 |
Dad was smoker, % | 60.2 | 60.8 | 61.8 | 57.4 | 0.06 |
Lived with alcoholic during childhood, % | 19.9 | 30.9 | 17.5 | 16.8 | <0.001 |
Importance of religion in family, % | <0.001 | ||||
Very important | 45.2 | 32.6 | 45.3 | 58.5 | |
Somewhat important | 35.2 | 36.9 | 37.3 | 30.8 | |
Not very important | 15.4 | 22.8 | 14.1 | 9.1 | |
Not at all important | 4.3 | 7.8 | 3.3 | 1.7 |
Note: Percentages refer to the proportion of individuals within each parental warmth category with that characteristic. p values comes from χ2 or analysis of variance tests.
Parental Warmth and Flourishing in Mid-life
Parental warmth was positively associated with the continuous score of composite flourishing (B=0.21, 95% CI=0.18, 0.25, Table 2). Analyses with the dichotomized indicator of flourishing (RR=1.34, 95% CI=1.25, 1.45) and the count measure (B=0.18, 95% CI=0.15, 0.22) of flourishing yielded similar results. A similar pattern was found when individual components of flourishing, namely emotional, psychological, and social well-being, and the subdomains that comprise these components were considered separately (Table 2). Notably, the association with parental warmth did not seem specific to any particular component of flourishing or any subdomain that comprises the components. Only for purpose in life and social coherence did the associations with parental warmth not pass the p<0.05 threshold after correction for multiple testing. Sensitivity analyses with tertiled parental warmth (Table S7) and the complete-case analysis (Table S8) yielded similar results. When maternal and paternal warmth were examined separately, each was positively associated with the composite flourishing score and the individual components of flourishing (Tables S9 to S11).
Table 2.
Parental warmth and flourishing in mid-life (N=3,929).
Parental warmth (standardized) B (95% CI) | |
---|---|
Composite flourishing score (continuous) | 0.21 (0.18, 0.25)*** |
Emotional well-being | 0.21 (0.17, 0.24)*** |
Positive affect | 0.19 (0.15, 0.23)*** |
Life satisfaction | 0.18 (0.14, 0.21)*** |
Psychological well-being | 0.19 (0.16, 0.23)*** |
Self-acceptance | 0.21 (0.17, 0.24)*** |
Positive relations | 0.23 (0.20, 0.27)*** |
Personal growth | 0.09 (0.06, 0.13)*** |
Purpose in life | 0.04 (0.01, 0.08)* |
Environmental mastery | 0.14 (0.10, 0.17)*** |
Autonomy | 0.07 (0.04, 0.11)*** |
Social well-being | 0.13 (0.10, 0.17)*** |
Social acceptance | 0.10 (0.06, 0.13)*** |
Social actualization | 0.08 (0.04, 0.11)*** |
Social contribution | 0.10 (0.06, 0.14)*** |
Social coherence | 0.03 (−0.01, 0.06) |
Social integration | 0.16 (0.12, 0.20)*** |
Note. The composite flourishing score and each of the flourishing components and subdomain outcomes were examined in separate models. Generalized estimating equations (with normal distribution and identity link) were used to estimate the mean change (B) in the standardized scores of flourishing (and its components and subdomains) with the change in parental warmth, adjusting for clustering by sibling status. All outcome variables were standardized (M=0, SD=1; all outcomes were continuous variables) to facilitate comparison of effect estimates across outcomes. All models adjusted for age, sex, race, nativity status, parents’ nativity status, number of siblings, and other childhood family factors (including childhood family structure, family SES, area of residence, residential stability, parents’ smoking status, lived with alcoholics and family religiousness). B= standardized beta.
p<0.05 before but not after Bonferroni correction;
p<0.01 before but not after Bonferroni correction;
p<0.05 after Bonferroni correction (the p value cutoff for Bonferroni correction =0.05/24 outcomes=0.002).
Parental Warmth, Mental Health Problems, and Adverse Health Behaviors in Mid-life
Parental warmth was also inversely associated with the likelihood of drug use (other than marijuana) and current or former smoking (Table 3). The sensitivity analysis with tertiled parental warmth (Table S7) and the complete-case analysis (Table S8) yielded similar results, except that the association with depression remained p<.05 even after correction for multiple testing. When maternal and paternal warmth were considered separately, maternal warmth had stronger inverse associations with depression and smoking compared to paternal warmth, whereas paternal warmth was more strongly associated with substance use compared to maternal warmth (Tables S9 to S11).
Table 3.
Parental warmth, mental health problems, and health behaviors in mid-life (N=3,929).
Parental warmth (standardized) OR/RR (95% CI) | |
---|---|
Mental health problems | |
Depression | 0.82 (0.72, 0.94)** |
Anxiety | 0.86 (0.65, 1.14) |
Health behaviors | |
Marijuana use | 0.78 (0.66, 0.93)** |
Any other drug use | 0.83 (0.74, 0.93)* |
Overweight or obese | 0.99 (0.96, 1.01) |
Current or former smoking | 0.94 (0.90, 0.97)*** |
Heavy drinking | 0.99 (0.91, 1.09) |
Note. Each mental health and behavioral outcomes were examined in separate models. Generalized estimating equations were used to estimate OR for rare binary outcomes (with binomial distribution, rare outcome defined as prevalence<10%) or RR for non-rare binary outcomes (with Poisson distribution, nonrare outcome defined as prevalence>=10%). All models adjusted for age, sex, race, nativity status, parents’ nativity status, number of siblings, and other childhood family factors (including childhood family structure, family SES, area of residence, residential stability, parents’ smoking status, alcoholics in the family and family religiousness). OR=odds ratio; RR=risk ratio.
p<0.05 before but not after Bonferroni correction;
p<0.01 before but not after Bonferroni correction;
p<0.05 after Bonferroni correction (the p value cutoff for Bonferroni correction =0.05/24 outcomes=0.002).
Considering the cross-sectional sociodemographic distribution of flourishing, greater adulthood SES (greater educational attainment, household income, and higher occupational class), frequent religious service attendance, older age, female gender, Black race, and being married was each correlated with higher likelihood of flourishing (See Tables S12 and S13, and supplemental materials for further discussion).
Discussion
This study suggests that parental warmth may contribute to promoting and maintaining offspring well-being as characterized by multi-component measures of flourishing in mid-life. The association did not seem specific to any particular component of flourishing or any subdomain comprising the components, suggesting parental warmth may contribute to effective functioning across various aspects of well-being and also protect against several adverse mental and behavioral outcomes.
Findings of this study are consistent with prior evidence suggesting parental warmth is positively associated with individual aspects of subjective well-being and is inversely associated with mental health problems (Chang et al., 2003; Chen et al., 2000; Enns et al., 2002; Huppert et al., 2010; McLeod et al., 2007). This study expanded prior work by showing parental warmth has relatively strong longitudinal associations both with composite measures of flourishing and across the various flourishing subdomains in mid-life. The associations remained robust after adjusting for a wide range of childhood familial characteristics including some aspects of adverse childhood experiences (e.g., parental abuse, substance misuse within household), which adds to the evidence that positive family relationships are more than the mere absence of family malfunctioning. Contrary to our expectation, there was little association of parental warmth with some behavioral outcomes such as heavy drinking, which contrasts with prior results in younger samples (Labrie & Sessoms, 2012; Ryan et al., 2010). The difference in results might be attributed to differences in the measurements, or it may relate to more precise estimates due to larger sample sizes, or the effects may only be evident earlier in life. It is also possible that early life serves as a sensitive period for affective and cognitive development, whereas behavioral patterns are more likely reshaped by later life experiences such as education and newly established social relationships (e.g., peers, partners)(Karvonen et al., 1999; Steinberg, 2005).
Parental warmth may have an even broader influence on well-being that was not fully captured by the measures of flourishing in the present study. Parental warmth, or love, is arguably a multi-dimensional construct itself that includes not only affective, but also cognitive and behavioral components (Clough, 2006). For instance, the measure of parental warmth in this study not only assessed parental affection, but also nurturance (e.g., “how much did your parents teach you about life”) and involvement (e.g., “how much time and attention did your mother/father give you when you needed it”). We may expect parental warmth, as a powerful force of love that takes many forms, to create a life of enhanced well-being on a variety of positive outcomes such as greater subjective well-being, deepened relationships, self-actualization, and also character development and even self-transcendence (Benson & Scales, 2011; Clough, 2006; Lee et al., 2013; Ranson & Urichuk, 2008). A virtuous cycle may be set in motion by a warm and nurturing upbringing (though the children’s developmental trajectory is still open to modification by experiences in later life), and some of the positive effects might even accumulate throughout life (Bretherton, 1992). The moral and spiritual aspects of wellbeing are arguably important components of flourishing, and perhaps even more so as individuals age (Bengtson et al., 2015; VanderWeele, 2017a). However, such aspects of wellbeing and the influences of parental warmth on them might not be captured in this study.
Prior work on childhood antecedents of mental health has mostly taken a deficit-oriented approach. That is, much prior work has sought to identify childhood risk factors for mental health problems such as parental abuse and neglect (Hughes et al., 2017; Kessler et al., 2010). However, reducing risk factors of mental illness in high risk populations is different from promoting assets that lead to better mental health in the general population (Kobau et al., 2011), partly because the absence of mental illness does not necessarily imply the presence of mental health (Keyes, 2002) and that determinants of mental well-being and ill-being may be different (World Health Organization, 2009a). It might be hard to reverse mental health problems once they have developed, as evidenced by ongoing high rates of mental illness (Reeves et al., 2011). It may, sometimes, be more efficient to promote and maintain positive mental health before problems develop (Kobau et al., 2011). It may therefore be as important to study assets for promoting mental health, as it is to examine approaches to reducing mental illness. Results from this study suggest that parental warmth may be one such asset for promoting positive mental health and flourishing. Moreover, there is also evidence from prior studies suggesting parental warmth may serve as resilience resources to buffer against the toxic effects of adverse childhood experiences (Schofield et al., 2016). This study may also suggest the determinants traditionally considered in epidemiology as shaping health outcomes may not provide a complete picture of the factors that are highly influential. Parenting practice is certainly not often considered in most public health discussions, and yet its effects on numerous dimensions of well-being seem strong (Ranson & Urichuk, 2008). A more comprehensive approach to public health would arguably place greater emphasis on parental warmth, or love, as a powerful social determinant of health. Indeed theological traditions often consider love as a central part of well-being (Aquinas, 1274/1948; Catholic Church, 2000). It is perhaps then not surprising that the effects observed in this study are substantial.
Limitations
This study is subject to certain limitations. First, parental warmth and other childhood familial characteristics were retrospectively reported in participants’ mid-life, which may be subject to recall bias. However, there is evidence suggesting that concerns about recalled childhood experiences may not be as substantial as previously thought (Reuben et al., 2016).
Moreover, in the present study the null association between parental warmth and some behavioral outcomes, taken as negative-controls, suggests that not all of the observed association with the flourishing outcomes are due to recall bias. Second, although this study controlled for a number of childhood factors as potential confounders, there may still be residual confounding by factors for which information was not available such as parents’ mental health. Finally, the MIDUS cohort was primarily White and did not comprise a nationally representative sample. Therefore, results of this study may not be generalizable to other populations.
Programs to improve parenting practices are available (Sanders, 2002, 2008). Such interventions have sought to increase positive parent-child interaction and reduce coersive parenting practices. There has also been suggestive evidence that such programs may be effective in reducing child emotional, mental and behavioral problems, and has demonstrated its potential to be scaled up to the population level (Prinz & Sanders, 2007; Sanders, 2002). In fact, there have been calls from the World Health Organization to implement large-scale parenting programs (World Health Organization, 2007, 2009b). This study suggests that such programs may also be useful for promoting positive mental health and well-being, and effects may persist into later life. Poor mental health is the single largest contributor of economic burden from noncommunicable diseases, and improving mental health is a key part of the global health agenda (Bloom et al., 2011). The asset-based paradigm may provide an additional approach to understanding the full spectrum of well-being and its determinants, may provide a less stigmatizing approach to reducing mental illness, and may promote use of psychological screening and prevention services (Kobau et al., 2011).
Conclusions
This study adds to the evidence that parental warmth may be one health asset leading to better offspring functioning across multiple aspects of well-being. It also helps to strengthen the call for a public health focus on the importance of parenting for outcomes beyond childhood and well into adulthood, and may suggest the value of targeting parenting practices for prevention and intervention strategies to improve population health and well-being.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Parental warmth is positively associated with flourishing in mid-life.
Association is not specific to any particular component or subdomain of flourishing.
Parental warmth is also inversely related to drug use and smoking.
This study suggests the value of targeting parenting to improve population health.
Acknowledgements:
Support for this research was provided by the Templeton Foundation (grant #52125) and the National Institutes of Health (grant ES017876). The original Midlife in the United States study was supported by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Midlife Development. Follow up data collection was supported by the National Institute on Aging (P01-AG020166). Funding agencies had no role in the data collection, analysis, or interpretation; nor were they involved in the writing or submission of this publication. We thank the staff of the clinical research centers at Georgetown University, University of Wisconsin-Madison, and University of California, Los Angeles for their support in conducting this study.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest: none
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Adam EK, & Chase-Lansdale PL (2002). Home sweet home(s): parental separations, residential moves, and adjustment problems in low-income adolescent girls. Dev Psychol, 38, 792–805. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aquinas T (1274/1948). Summa Theologica. Complete English Translation in Five Volumes. Notre Dame, IN: Ave Maria Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bengtson VL, Silverstein M, Putney NM, & Harris SC (2015). Does Religiousness Increase with Age? Age Changes and Generational Differences Over 35 Years. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 54, 363–379. [Google Scholar]
- Benson PL, & Scales PC (2011). Developmental Assets In Levesque RJR (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Adolescence pp. 667–683). New York, NY: Springer New York. [Google Scholar]
- Bloom DE, Cafiero ET, Jané-Llopis E, Abrahams-Gessel S, Bloom LR, Fathima S.Feigl,,, et al. (2011). The Global Economic Burden of Noncommunicable Diseases. Geneva: World Economic Forum. [Google Scholar]
- Boehm JK, Chen Y, Williams DR, Ryff CD, & Kubzansky LD (2016). Subjective wellbeing and cardiometabolic health: An 8–11year study of midlife adults. J Psychosom Res, 85, 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boehm JK, & Kubzansky LD (2012). The heart’s content: the association between positive psychological well-being and cardiovascular health. Psychological bulletin, 138, 655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boehm JK, Soo J, Chen Y, Zevon ES, Hernandez R, Lloyd-Jones D, et al. (2017). Psychological Well-being’s Link with Cardiovascular Health in Older Adults. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 53, 791–798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bretherton I (1992). The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth. Developmental psychology, 28, 759. [Google Scholar]
- Bures RM (2003). Childhood Residential Stability and Health at Midlife. American Journal of Public Health, 93, 1144–1148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Church Catholic (2000). Catechism of the Catholic Church. Vatican City, Chicago: Libreria Editrice Vaticana Loyola University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Chang L, McBride-Chang C, Stewart S, & Au E (2003). Life satisfaction, self-concept, and family relations in Chinese adolescents and children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 182–189. [Google Scholar]
- Chen X, Liu M, & Li D (2000). Parental warmth, control, and indulgence and their relations to adjustment in Chinese children: a longitudinal study. J Fam Psychol, 14, 401–419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chu JT, Farruggia SP, Sanders MR, & Ralph A (2012). Towards a public health approach to parenting programmes for parents of adolescents. J Public Health (Oxf), 34 Suppl 1, i41–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clough WR (2006). To be Loved and to Love. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 34, 23–31. [Google Scholar]
- Crawford JR, & Henry JD (2004). The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS): Construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 245–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cutler DM, & Lleras-Muney A (2010). Understanding differences in health behaviors by education. Journal of Health Economics, 29, 1–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dienberg Love G, Seeman TE, Weinstein M, & Ryff CD (2010). Bioindicators in the MIDUS national study: protocol, measures, sample, and comparative context. J Aging Health, 22, 1059–1080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diener E, Wirtz D, Tov W, Kim-Prieto C, Choi D. w., Oishi S, et al. (2010). New wellbeing measures: Short scales to assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Social Indicators Research, 97, 143–156. [Google Scholar]
- Elstad JI (2005). Childhood adversities and health variations among middle-aged men: a retrospective lifecourse study. European Journal of Public Health, 15, 51–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enns MW, Cox BJ, & Clara I (2002). Parental bonding and adult psychopathology: results from the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine, 32, 997–1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, et al. (1998). Relationship of Childhood Abuse and Household Dysfunction to Many of the Leading Causes of Death in Adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14, 245–258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham JW (2009). Missing Data Analysis: Making It Work in the Real World. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 549–576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunnoe ML, Hetherington EM, & Reiss D (1999). Parental Religiosity, Parenting Style, and Adolescent Social Responsibility. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 199–225. [Google Scholar]
- Hone LC, Jarden A, Schofield GM, & Duncan S (2014). Measuring flourishing: The impact of operational definitions on the prevalence of high levels of wellbeing. International Journal of Wellbeing, 4. [Google Scholar]
- Hughes K, Bellis MA, Hardcastle KA, Sethi D, Butchart A, Mikton C, et al. (2017). The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Public Health, 2, e356–e366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huppert FA, Abbott RA, Ploubidis GB, Richards M, & Kuh D (2010). Parental practices predict psychological well-being in midlife: life-course associations among women in the 1946 British birth cohort. Psychol Med, 40, 1507–1518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huppert FA, & So TT (2013). Flourishing Across Europe: Application of a New Conceptual Framework for Defining Well-Being. Soc Indic Res, 110, 837–861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karvonen S, Rimpela AH, & Rimpela MK (1999). Social mobility and health related behaviours in young people. Journal of epidemiology and community health, 53, 211217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Andrews G, Mroczek D, Ustun B, & Wittchen H-U (1998). The World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview short-form (CIDI-SF). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 7, 171–185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, DuPont RL, Berglund P, & Wittchen HU (1999). Impairment in pure and comorbid generalized anxiety disorder and major depression at 12 months in two national surveys. The American journal of psychiatry, 156, 1915–1923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 197, 378–385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keyes CL (2002). The mental health continuum: from languishing to flourishing in life. J Health Soc Behav, 43, 207–222. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keyes CL (2006). Mental health in adolescence: is America’s youth flourishing? Am J Orthopsychiatry, 76, 395–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keyes CL (2007). Promoting and protecting mental health as flourishing: a complementary strategy for improving national mental health. Am Psychol, 62, 95–108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keyes CL, & Shapiro AD (2004). Social well-being in the United States: A descriptive epidemiology In Brim OG, Ryff CD, & Kessler RC (Eds.), How Healthy Are We?: A National Study of Well-being at Midlife pp. 350–372). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Keyes CL, & Simoes EJ (2012). To flourish or not: positive mental health and all-cause mortality. Am J Public Health, 102, 2164–2172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keyes CLM (1998). Social well-being. Social Psychology Quarterly, 61, 121–140. [Google Scholar]
- Khaleque A (2013). Perceived Parental Warmth, and Children’s Psychological Adjustment, and Personality Dispositions: A Meta-analysis. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22, 297306. [Google Scholar]
- Kobau R, Seligman MEP, Peterson C, Diener E, Zack MM, Chapman D, et al. (2011). Mental Health Promotion in Public Health: Perspectives and Strategies From Positive Psychology. American Journal of Public Health, 101, e1–e9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Labrie JW, & Sessoms AE (2012). Parents still matter: The role of parental attachment in risky drinking among college students. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 21, 91–104. [Google Scholar]
- Lee MT, Poloma MM, & Post SG (2013). The heart of religion: Spiritual empowerment, benevolence, and the experience of God’s love: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Manderscheid RW, Ryff CD, Freeman EJ, McKnight-Eily LR, Dhingra S, & Strine TW (2010). Evolving Definitions of Mental Illness and Wellness. Preventing Chronic Disease, 7, A19. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLeod BD, Wood JJ, & Weisz JR (2007). Examining the association between parenting and childhood anxiety: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev, 27, 155–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mroczek DK, & Kolarz CM (1998). The effect of age on positive and negative affect: a developmental perspective on happiness. J Pers Soc Psychol, 75, 1333–1349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prinz RJ, & Sanders MR (2007). Adopting a population-level approach to parenting and family support interventions. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 739–749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radler BT (2014). The Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) Series: A National Longitudinal Study of Health and Well-being. Open Health Data, 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radler BT, & Ryff CD (2010). Who participates? Accounting for longitudinal retention in the MIDUS national study of health and well-being. J Aging Health, 22, 307–331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranson KE, & Urichuk LJ (2008). The Effect of Parent-Child Attachment Relationships on Child Biopsychosocial Outcomes: A Review. Early Child Development and Care, 178, 129–152. [Google Scholar]
- Reeves WC, Strine TW, Pratt LA, Thompson W, Ahluwalia I, Dhingra SS, et al. (2011). Mental illness surveillance among adults in the United States. MMWR Surveill Summ, 60, 1–29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Repetti RL, Taylor SE, & Seeman TE (2002). Risky families: family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychol Bull, 128, 330–366. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reuben A, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Belsky DW, Harrington H, Schroeder F, et al. (2016). Lest we forget: comparing retrospective and prospective assessments of adverse childhood experiences in the prediction of adult health. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 1103–1112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossi AS (2001). Caring and doing for others: Social responsibility in the domains of family, work, and community: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Rothrauff TC, Cooney TM, & An JS (2009). Remembered parenting styles and adjustment in middle and late adulthood. The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences, 64, 137–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan SM, Jorm AF, & Lubman DI (2010). Parenting factors associated with reduced adolescent alcohol use: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 44, 774–783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryff CD (1989). Happiness Is Everything, or Is It? Explorations on the Meaning of Psychological Well-Being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081. [Google Scholar]
- Ryff CD, & Keyes CLM (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. Journal of personality and social psychology, 69, 719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders MR (2002). Parenting interventions and the prevention of serious mental health problems in children. Med J Aust, 177 Suppl, S87–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders MR (2008). Triple P-Positive Parenting Program as a public health approach to strengthening parenting. J Fam Psychol, 22, 506–517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schofield TJ, Conger RD, Gonzales JE, & Merrick MT (2016). Harsh parenting, physical health, and the protective role of positive parent-adolescent relationships. Social Science & Medicine, 157, 18–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seligman ME (2008). Positive health. Applied psychology, 57, 3–18. [Google Scholar]
- Seligman ME (2012). Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-being: Simon and Schuster. [Google Scholar]
- Shakya HB, Christakis NA, & Fowler JH (2012). Parental influence on substance use in adolescent social networks. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine, 166, 11321139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slack KS, Holl JL, McDaniel M, Yoo J, & Bolger K (2004). Understanding the risks of child neglect: an exploration of poverty and parenting characteristics. Child Maltreat, 9, 395–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slopen N, Chen Y, Guida JL, Albert MA, & Williams DR (2017). Positive childhood experiences and ideal cardiovascular health in midlife: Associations and mediators. Prev Med, 97, 72–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soo J, Kubzansky LD, Chen Y, Zevon ES, & Boehm JK (2018). Psychological wellbeing and restorative biological processes: HDL-C in older English adults. Social Science & Medicine, 209, 59–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9, 69–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterne JA, White IR, Carlin JB, Spratt M, Royston P, Kenward MG, et al. (2009). Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. BMJ, 338, b2393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turiano NA, Whiteman SD, Hampson SE, Roberts BW, & Mroczek DK (2012). Personality and Substance Use in Midlife: Conscientiousness as a Moderator and the Effects of Trait Change. J Res Pers, 46, 295–305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- VanderWeele TJ (2017a). On the promotion of human flourishing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 8148–8156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- VanderWeele TJ (2017b). Outcome-wide epidemiology. Epidemiology, 28, 399–402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh C, MacMillan HL, & Jamieson E (2003). The relationship between parental substance abuse and child maltreatment: findings from the Ontario Health Supplement. Child Abuse Negl, 27, 1409–1425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. (1995). Physical status: the use and interpretation of anthropometry Report of a WHO Expert Committee. World Health Organization technical report series; pp. 1–452 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. (2007). Helping parents in developing countries improve adolescents’ health. Geneva: World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. (2009a). Mental health, resilience and inequalities. Geneva: World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. (2009b). Preventing violence through the development of safe, stable and nurturing relationships between children and their parents and caregivers Series of briefings on violence prevention: the evidence. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
- Yap MBH, Pilkington PD, Ryan SM, & Jorm AF (2014). Parental factors associated with depression and anxiety in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 156, 8–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.