Abstract
Objective
In soccer, unintentional and intentional (heading) head impacts are associated with concussive symptoms and cognitive dysfunction. We examined whether personality traits were associated with these behaviors in soccer players.
Design
Cross-sectional study.
Setting and Participants
Participants completed study visits at the Albert Einstein College of Medicine. A total of 307 adult amateur soccer players, recruited from the New York City and surrounding area, completed 737 HeadCount-2w questionnaires.
Predictor Variables
Personality traits (Intellect/Imagination, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism) were assessed with the Mini-International Personality Item Pool questionnaire at the baseline study visit.
Main Outcome Measures
Participants completed an on-line questionnaire (HeadCount-2w) to ascertain frequency of intentional head impacts and occurrence of unintentional head impacts every 3–6 months. Generalized estimating equations repeated measures regressions determined if personality predicted unintentional and intentional impacts.
Results
Personality traits were not associated with unintentional head impact(s) or frequency of intentional head impacts.
Conclusions
These findings have important clinical implications, suggesting that personality is not driving the association between high levels of unintentional and intentional head impacts and worse neuropsychological functioning and concussive symptoms.
Keywords: personality, risk, soccer, heading, concussion, extraversion, sports
INTRODUCTION
Risk-taking behavior, which can be measured by assessing stable personality traits shown to predict personal harm (e.g., gambling, substance abuse, unsafe driving), may also be relevant to participation in and risk-taking behavior in sports like soccer1–3. In general, personality traits are predictive of participation in sports. People who engage in athletic activity tend to be more extraverted, less conscientious, and less neurotic than those who do not participate in athletic activity3–5, and these differences persist over time4. Personality traits also predict risk-taking behaviors among athletes, especially in extreme or high-risk sports where injury or death is inherent (e.g., skydiving, mountain biking). Among risky-sport participants, low conscientiousness and high extraversion have consistently predicted the greatest risk-taking behaviors6,7, while the findings for neuroticism are inconsistent6,8. Most studies have focused on extreme sports in which risk of injury is readily apparent. In soccer, risk of injury is less extreme and not as apparent in the short-term, yet important to study nonetheless. We examined whether these same traits were relevant to less extreme forms of risk-taking behavior in soccer.
Soccer, the most popular sport worldwide, may account for a significant share of sports-related head injury from both unintentional (i.e., direct head contact with other player’s body or head, the ground, and stationary objects (e.g., goal post) and ball hitting the back of the head) and intentional (i.e., heading the ball) events9–11. Intentional head impacts (heading) are largely regarded as a complex skill that requires precise timing, coordination, and strength12. However, even with proper technique, the intentional head impact itself can elicit brain injury and functional sequelae over time13. In light of the growing awareness of medical9 and neuropsychological sequelae14–16 of unintentional and intentional head impacts in soccer play, it is important to study the personality factors that may increase a player’s tendency to experience injury. If personality traits are predictive of risk-taking behaviors among soccer players, this could inform the development of more targeted public health interventions to reduce the rate of head injuries. Whereas, if personality traits are not predictive of risk-taking behaviors, this suggests that other interventions should be investigated. Thus, the goal of the current study was to examine if relationships exist between personality and unintentional head impacts and intentional head impacts (heading) in a well-characterized sample of amateur adult soccer players. Based on the personality traits associated with participation in sports and risk-taking behaviors among athletes, we hypothesized that high levels of extraversion and low levels of conscientiousness would be related to unintentional and intentional head impacts. Given mixed evidence for the association between neuroticism and risk-taking behaviors among athletes, the hypothesis for this personality trait was more exploratory in nature.
METHODS
Participants
The Einstein Soccer Study is an ongoing, multi-faceted longitudinal study of heading during soccer play and its consequences in adult amateur soccer players. Details on the study population and data collection protocol are described in detail elsewhere9. In brief, amateur adult soccer players recruited by various outreach methods in New York City and surrounding areas were directed to an enrollment website for preliminary consent and screening. Inclusion criteria included: age 18–55; at least 5 years of active amateur soccer play; current active amateur soccer play; 6 months of amateur soccer play per year; and English language fluency. Exclusion criteria included: schizophrenia, bipolar disorder; known neurological disorder; prior moderate or severe TBI; pregnancy; and medical contraindication to MRI (relevant to a separate arm of the study). The study protocol was approved by The Albert Einstein College of Medicine Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent was obtained from qualifying individuals prior to enrollment at their initial in-person study visit. Participants visited the laboratory up to 5 times every 3 to 6 months over the course of a maximum study period of 2 years. The enrollment period for the data included in this study was 2013 to 2016. At these visits, participants were asked to complete a battery of neuropsychological tasks and psychological questionnaires. Immediately following each visit, participants were asked to complete a web-based two-week recall questionnaire (Head Count-2w) on soccer activity and unintentional and intentional head impacts.
Unintentional and Intentional Head Impacts: HeadCount-2w
Details on the development of HeadCount-2w, the two-week recall questionnaire, are described elsewhere along with the format and structure of the questionnaire9,17. In brief, HeadCount-2w was designed as a web-based questionnaire organized into four categories of play including outdoor practice, outdoor games, indoor practice, and indoor games and two categories of head impacts including intentional and unintentional. Days played was computed by adding number of indoor and outdoor games and practices the participants reported within the two-week HeadCount-2w recall period. For indoor and outdoor competition and practice, questions ascertained: 1) Number of competitive soccer games and number of practice sessions; 2) Positions played during games; and 3) Average number of headings during games or practice. For all soccer activities in the past two weeks, participants were asked how often they experienced unintentional head impacts from specific causes (e.g., ball hitting the back of the head, head to goal post, head to head, head to ground, head to body part (elbow, knee etc.), and head stepped on or kicked). Participants were asked to report these unintentional head impacts over the time period of the questionnaire.
Personality Questionnaire: Mini International Personality Item Pool (Mini-IPIP)
The Mini-IPIP18,19, administered at the first study visit only, is a 20-item short form questionnaire based on the 50-item IPIP five-factor model20,21 that measures the “Big Five” personality factors of Intellect/Imagination (also referred to as “Openness to experience”), Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism. When presented with a statement (e.g., “I am the life of the party”), participants are instructed to choose a response that most accurately describes themselves on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1=very inaccurate to 5=very accurate. There are four items for each of the five personality domains. Eleven of the items are reverse scored. A total score ranging from 4 – 20 is derived for each of the five personality domains. This measure has demonstrated high test-retest reliability, metric invariance, and criterion validity18,19. The primary variable of interest for this study was the total score for each of the five personality domains.
Statistical Analysis
IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows Version 24.0 (IBM Corp.; Armonk, NY) was used to determine if selected personality traits were directly associated with unintentional and intentional head impacts. Two-week recall of unintentional head impacts was defined as a dichotomous variable (0 versus 1+ unintentional head impacts). Given the non-linear distribution of intentional head impacts, heading exposure was represented as ten ordinal groups of equal size (deciles); this transformed intentional head impacts into zero exposure (median=0), and the nine subsequent groups were assigned the median value of the range of the group.
Separate generalized estimating equations (GEE) repeated measures regressions were used to determine the relation of unintentional head impacts and intentional head impacts (dependent variables) with each of the five personality traits (independent variables). For unintentional impacts a binary logistic regression was used. For the intentional impacts, a linear regression was used. The GEE models were fit with the following additional covariates: gender, age, number of days played in two weeks, concussion count, number of drinks of alcohol in a week, years of soccer play at a similar frequency, cigarette use (yes/no), the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT-4) Reading subtest estimated premorbid intelligence score22, and main position played. The final GEE models reported in Table 2 included only the covariates which remained significant at the p < 0.1 level.
Table 2.
Personality Traits | Unintentional Impacts | Heading Impacts | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
β (95% CI) | p-value | β (95% CI) | p-value | |
Intellect/ Imagination | 0.01 (−0.06,0.08) | 0.78 | 1.31 (−0.17, 2.78) | 0.08 |
| ||||
Conscientiousness | −0.03 (−0.09,0.03) | 0.32 | 0.13 (−0.99, 1.25) | 0.83 |
| ||||
Extraversion | 0.06 (0.01,0.12) | 0.06 | 0.57 (−0.66, 1.80) | 0.37 |
| ||||
Agreeableness | 0.06 (−0.02,0.14) | 0.15 | 0.62 (−0.87, 2.12) | 0.41 |
| ||||
Neuroticism | 0.03 (−0.04, 0.10) | 0.41 | 0.46 (−0.93, 1.84) | 0.52 |
RESULTS
Sample characteristics
Three-hundred and seven participants reported soccer activity (Table 1). Of these, 125 participants completed one HeadCount-2w questionnaire, 70 participants completed two HeadCount-2w questionnaires, and 112 participants completed three or more for a total of 737 HeadCount-2w questionnaires. Of the 737 questionnaires, 245 reported both intentional and unintentional head impacts, 432 reported intentional but no unintentional head impacts, 11 reported unintentional but no intentional head impacts, and 49 reported neither intentional nor unintentional head impacts.
Table 1.
Variables | Category | Percent (n=737) |
Heading/2-wks | Unintentional/2 -wks (%) |
Extraversion | Agreeableness | Conscientiousness | Neuroticism | Imagination | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||||
Mean | Median | |||||||||
Total | 100 | 35.3 | 14 | 36 | 13.4 | 15.7 | 15.1 | 9.2 | 15.1 | |
| ||||||||||
Gender | Male | 78 | 50.2 | 17 | 33.9 | 13.3 | 15.3 | 15 | 9.2 | 15.4 |
Female | 22 | 26.7 | 7.5 | 37.7 | 13.6 | 16.7 | 15.4 | 9.4 | 14.4 | |
| ||||||||||
Age | 18–20 | 23.8 | 63.6 | 24.5 | 40.9 | 13.3 | 14.9 | 14.6 | 9.6 | 15.2 |
21–23 | 25.1 | 58.1 | 24 | 37.8 | 13.9 | 15.9 | 14.8 | 9.4 | 15.8 | |
22–30 | 28.6 | 26.8 | 10 | 28.9 | 13.6 | 15.7 | 15.2 | 9.1 | 15.4 | |
31+ | 22.4 | 34 | 13 | 32.1 | 12.6 | 15.7 | 15.7 | 8.9 | 14.2 | |
| ||||||||||
Years Playing Soccer at similar frequency | 0–5.9 | 20.2 | 37.6 | 11 | 36.9 | 13.4 | 15.9 | 15 | 9.3 | 15.1 |
6–9.9 | 17.9 | 50.9 | 22 | 32.6 | 12.9 | 15.8 | 14.8 | 9 | 15.5 | |
10–15.9 | 32.7 | 58.6 | 21 | 41.5 | 13.2 | 14.9 | 15.2 | 9.9 | 14.9 | |
16+ | 29.2 | 31.4 | 12 | 27 | 13.7 | 15.9 | 15.3 | 8.6 | 15.2 | |
| ||||||||||
Number of Days Played | 0–1 | 10.2 | 5.1 | 2 | 21.3 | 13.8 | 15.9 | 15.1 | 9.3 | 15 |
2–3 | 27.4 | 12.6 | 7 | 25.7 | 13.2 | 15.7 | 15.1 | 9.1 | 15.2 | |
4–7 | 35.7 | 32.7 | 15 | 33.5 | 13.3 | 15.7 | 15.1 | 9.2 | 15.3 | |
8+ | 26.7 | 110 | 76 | 50.8 | 13.4 | 15.1 | 15.1 | 9.5 | 15 | |
| ||||||||||
Position played most often | Forward | 19.8 | 56.1 | 22 | 45.2 | 12.5 | 15.1 | 15.8 | 9.7 | 14.7 |
Midfield | 37.3 | 39.9 | 15 | 32.7 | 13.2 | 15.5 | 14.9 | 9.2 | 15.4 | |
Defense | 36 | 48.6 | 15 | 29.1 | 13.8 | 16 | 15 | 9.1 | 15.3 | |
Goal | 6.9 | 22.9 | 6 | 45.8 | 14.2 | 15.1 | 14.6 | 8.9 | 14.6 | |
| ||||||||||
Concussions (all causes): Lifetime | None | 79.6 | 50.7 | 16 | 34.2 | 13.5 | 15.5 | 15.1 | 9.5 | 15 |
1 | 12.2 | 19.6 | 9 | 33.3 | 12.6 | 15.8 | 15.1 | 8.8 | 16 | |
>2 | 8.1 | 28.3 | 13.5 | 41.7 | 13.6 | 16.2 | 15.5 | 7.3 | 15.3 | |
| ||||||||||
Drinks of Alcohol Per Week | Never | 30 | 80.3 | 36 | 40.7 | 11.9 | 14.5 | 15 | 9.8 | 15 |
1–2 | 36 | 33.7 | 15 | 32.5 | 15.1 | 16.3 | 15.7 | 9 | 15.6 | |
3–7 | 26.2 | 24.4 | 10 | 32.6 | 13.6 | 15.7 | 14.4 | 9 | 14.7 | |
8+ | 7.9 | 31.3 | 11.5 | 29.3 | 15.1 | 16.4 | 15.3 | 8.9 | 15.4 | |
| ||||||||||
Cigarette Use | 0 | 71.6 | 49 | 16 | 35 | 13.4 | 15.5 | 15.2 | 9.4 | 15.1 |
1 | 28.4 | 35.1 | 13 | 34 | 13.3 | 15.6 | 14.8 | 8.7 | 15.3 | |
| ||||||||||
WRAT-4 Reading | 0–96 | 23.3 | 71.6 | 31 | 37.2 | 12.6 | 14.5 | 18.8 | 10.5 | 14.6 |
97–104 | 25.4 | 42 | 17 | 35.3 | 14.2 | 15.5 | 15.2 | 9 | 15.1 | |
105–113 | 26.2 | 41.7 | 15 | 31.6 | 13 | 15.9 | 15.3 | 8.6 | 15.1 | |
114+ | 25.1 | 26.9 | 9 | 35.1 | 13.6 | 16.3 | 15.1 | 9 | 15.8 |
Unintentional head impacts and personality
Among all eligible HeadCount-2w questionnaires, 34.7% reported one or more unintentional head impacts, with 15.9% reporting two or more unintentional head impacts in the past two weeks. “Head hit elbow or knee” and “ball hit the back of the head” were the two most common unintentional head impact exposures. The mean unintentional head impact score was 0.66 with a median of zero.
The multivariable GEE binary logistic regression models for predicting unintentional head impacts adjusted for number of days played and main position. Number of days played was computed as described above. Main Position was a self-reported question. Separate GEE regression models were fit with each of the five personality domains as a predictor and unintentional head impact (0 versus 1+ unintentional head impacts) as the outcome. Personality traits were not significantly associated with unintentional head impacts (Table 2). The association between higher levels of extraversion and the occurrence of unintentional head impact(s) was nearly significant (p=0.06) with an Odds Ratio per 5 unit increase in extraversion score equal to exp (0.06 × 5) = 1.35 (95% CI: 1.05, 1.82).
Intentional head impacts (heading) and personality
Among all eligible HeadCount-2w questionnaires, 91.9% reported intentional head impacts (heading) in the past two weeks. The mean number of headers was 45.04 with a median of 15 headers per two weeks.
The linear GEE models for predicting intentional head impacts adjusted for number of alcoholic drinks per week, main position of the player, the number of days played within a two-week period, and the number of concussions reported. For the agreeableness personality domain, gender was included as an additional covariate. Alcoholic drinks per week was calculated based on the self-reported question, “How many alcoholic drinks do you drink per week?” with possible responses including 0, 1–2, 3–7 and 8–12 drinks per week. Concussion count was computed by asking the participants how many instances did they seek or were advised to seek medical attention after a head impact.
Separate GEE regression models were fit with each of the five personality domains as a predictor and the intentional head impact category (0–10) as the outcome. None of these models yielded significant findings (Table 2). However, a nearly significant relationship between intellect/imagination and frequency of intentional head impacts was found (p=0.08).
DISCUSSION
Overall, personality traits of amateur adult soccer players were not associated with risk of incurring unintentional head impact(s), such as impact of the head against a goal post, the ground, or another player’s body or head, the head being stepped on or kicked, and impact of ball unintentionally hitting the player’s head. Personality also did not predict frequency of intentional heading of the ball. The findings from this study were contrary to our hypothesis that some of the “Big Five” personality traits, namely extraversion, conscientiousness, and neuroticism, would be related to risk-taking behaviors among soccer players. We propose an explanation for the lack of an association between personality traits and unintentional and intentional head impacts in this study and explore the implications of such null-findings for interventions to ensure safe-soccer play.
Based on the small existing literature examining personality traits that predict level of engagement in sports and risk-taking behaviors among such athletes4,6,7, we predicted that the personality traits of high extraversion and low conscientiousness would be related to risk-taking behaviors among soccer athletes. We also predicted that the trait of neuroticism would be related to risk-taking behaviors, but did not specify directionality. However, as noted above, no statistically-significant association was found between personality traits and risk-taking behaviors in soccer players (i.e., unintentional and intentional head impacts). There is growing awareness that heading of the ball and unintentional head impacts incurred during soccer play place athletes at risk for injury, including poorer neuropsychological functioning11,15,16,23, concussive symptoms9, abnormal white matter microstructure11,24, structural brain changes25,26, and electrophysiological changes27. As mentioned above, the risk of injury from soccer, although important in its implications, is not as extreme or readily apparent to soccer athletes as the risk level of sports examined in prior studies in which potential for injury or death is clear to participants. The absence of an association of personality with exposure to risk in our study may be because intentional head impacts in soccer are not perceived by participants as sufficiently high-risk activities. In this vein, a recent study reported heading as an uncommon cause of concussion28, but failed to acknowledge the potential for adverse effects of cumulative heading on brain function over time29,30. This recent study28 highlights the importance of public health interventions to increase awareness of the potential risks of soccer-related intentional head impacts over the long-term.
In the only other study to date to examine the association between personality and intentional head impacts (heading) in soccer players, higher extraversion was related to an increased likelihood of heading the ball among 60 elite male players31. The present study did not replicate these findings, possibly due to differences between the samples. Importantly, the present study included (1) a much larger sample (737 questionnaires from 307 players), (2) a higher percentage of female soccer athletes relative to their representation in the general population of soccer players worldwide (22% females, compared to 10% females in the population of registered and unregistered soccer players according to the 2006 FIFA Big Count Survey32), (3) study of both unintentional and intentional head impacts, and (4) inclusion of amateur league, not elite or professional soccer players. Amateur and elite soccer players may differ in heading technique, which could contribute to risk of injury from such cumulative heading exposures. The current findings may better represent the worldwide population of soccer players, with amateur players far outnumbering professional players32.
The reasons for the lack of an association between personality traits and unintentional head impacts may differ from the reasons stated above for intentional head impacts (heading). The way in which personality traits could alter risk for the two types of head impacts likely differs. For example, unintentional head impacts are somewhat random and athletes may have less control over these types of exposures, while intentional head impacts, as stated in their name, are more intentional and calculated.
In the largest sample of amateur soccer players studied to date, personality traits were not associated with frequency of unintentional head impacts (heading) of the ball during soccer play. This finding adds to the growing study of heading and neuropsychological functioning by expanding our understanding of underlying mechanisms. One potential implication is that personality traits are not likely to drive the association between high levels of heading and worse neuropsychological functioning11,16,33, placing the focus on the repetitive exposure to head impacts rather than pre-existing traits.
There are several limitations to our study that should be noted. Our measure of heading and unintentional head impacts was obtained from a self-report questionnaire that has demonstrated reliability and validity across settings9,11,17,34, but caution should still be taken when interpreting our results as recall bias cannot be excluded. For unintentional head impacts, the choices we included for mechanism of injury were based on a series of focus groups held with the population from which we subsequently enrolled participants and included common types of unintentional head impacts experienced among this population (i.e., ball hitting the back of the head, head to goal post, head to head, head to ground, head to body part (elbow, knee etc.), and head stepped on or kicked). This range of possibilities may not be exhaustive in light of prior work35. Other possible mechanisms of injury include head to shoulder, head to lower leg, head to trunk, as well as specifying the location of contact on the head and should be included in future studies. Additionally, our sample comprises amateur adult soccer players. Although study enrollment criteria ensured a relatively high level of commitment to the game (at least 5 years of active amateur soccer play, current active amateur soccer play, and at least 6 months of amateur soccer play per year), the findings should be considered as reflective of this unique sample and may not necessarily apply to elite, youth, or amateur athletes with lower levels of routine soccer play. Additionally, as players self-selected to participate in the study, a self-selection bias cannot be ruled-out; caution should be taken when generalizing our study to soccer play among community samples, who may experience different patterns of exposure. However, the mean activity level of our study participants has been deemed comparable to that of amateur players worldwide36. Despite these limitations, these findings from our large, well-characterized sample may help inform the development of interventions aimed to increase awareness of the risks of both unintentional and intentional in the ever-increasingly popular, yet high-risk, sport of soccer.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (Grant #R01NS082432; PI: M.L.L) and The Dana Foundation.
Footnotes
The authors report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript.
References
- 1.Sutin AR, Terracciano A, Deiana B, et al. High neuroticism and low conscientiousness are associated with interleukin-6. Psychol Med. 2010;40(9):1485–1493. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709992029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Schwebel DC, Severson J, Ball KK, Rizzo M. Individual difference factors in risky driving: the roles of anger/hostility, conscientiousness, and sensation-seeking. Accid Anal Prev. 2006;38(4):801–810. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2006.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Piedmont RL, Hill DC, Blanco S. Predicting athletic performance using the five-factor model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences. 1999;27(4):769–777. [Google Scholar]
- 4.McKelvie SJLP, Stout D. Extraversion and Neuroticism in Contact Athletes, No Contact Athletes, and Non-Athletes: A Research Note. The Online Journal of Sport Psychology. 2003;5(3) [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tok S. The Big Five personality traits and risky sport participation. Social Behavior and Personality. 2011;39(8):1105–1112. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Castanier C, Le Scanff C, Woodman T. Who takes risks in high-risk sports? A typological personality approach. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2010;81(4):478–484. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2010.10599709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Merritt CJ, Tharp IJ. Personality, self-efficacy and risk-taking in parkour (free-running) Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2013;14(5):608–611. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Merritt VC, Rabinowitz AR, Arnett PA. Personality factors and symptom reporting at baseline in collegiate athletes. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2015;40(1):45–50. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2014.979928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Stewart WF, Kim N, Ifrah CS, et al. Symptoms from repeated intentional and unintentional head impact in soccer players. Neurology. 2017 doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003657. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Caccese JB, Kaminski TW. Minimizing Head Acceleration in Soccer: A Review of the Literature. Sports Med. 2016;46(11):1591–1604. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0544-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lipton ML, Kim N, Zimmerman ME, et al. Soccer heading is associated with white matter microstructural and cognitive abnormalities. Radiology. 2013;268(3):850–857. doi: 10.1148/radiol.13130545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Spiotta AM, Bartsch AJ, Benzel EC. Heading in soccer: dangerous play? Neurosurgery. 2012;70(1):1–11. doi: 10.1227/NEU.0b013e31823021b2. discussion 11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tagge CA, Fisher AM, Minaeva OV, et al. Concussion, microvascular injury, and early tauopathy in young athletes after impact head injury and an impact concussion mouse model. Brain. 2018;141(2):422–458. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Matser, Kessels, Lezak, Jordan, Troost Neuropsychological impairment in amateur soccer players. JAMA. 1999;282(10):971–973. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.10.971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Downs DS, Abwender D. Neuropsychological impairment in soccer athletes. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. 2002;42(1):103–107. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Witol AD, Webbe FM. Soccer heading frequency predicts neuropsychological deficits. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2003;18(4):397–417. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Catenaccio E, Caccese J, Wakschlag N, et al. Validation and calibration of HeadCount, a self-report measure for quantifying heading exposure in soccer players. Research in Sports Medicine. 2016:1–10. doi: 10.1080/15438627.2016.1234472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Baldasaro RE, Shanahan MJ, Bauer DJ. Psychometric properties of the mini-IPIP in a large, nationally representative sample of young adults. J Pers Assess. 2013;95(1):74–84. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2012.700466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Donnellan MB, Oswald FL, Baird BM, Lucas RE. The mini-IPIP scales: tiny-yet-effective measures of the Big Five factors of personality. Psychol Assess. 2006;18(2):192–203. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.2.192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Goldberg LR. A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-factor models. Personality psychology in Europe. 1999;7(1):7–28. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Goldberg LR, Johnson JA, Eber HW, et al. The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. Journal of Research in personality. 2006;40(1):84–96. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Orme DR, Johnstone B, Hanks R, Novack T. The WRAT-3 Reading Subtest as a Measure of Premorbid Intelligence Among Persons With Brain Injury. Rehabilitation Psychology. 2004;49(3):250–253. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Matser, Kessels, Jordan, Lezak, Troost Chronic traumatic brain injury in professional soccer players. Neurology. 1998;51(3):791–796. doi: 10.1212/wnl.51.3.791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Koerte IK, Ertl-Wagner B, Reiser M, Zafonte R, Shenton ME. White matter integrity in the brains of professional soccer players without a symptomatic concussion. JAMA. 2012;308(18):1859–1861. doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.13735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Adams J, Adler CM, Jarvis K, DelBello MP, Strakowski SM. Evidence of anterior temporal atrophy in college-level soccer players. Clin J Sport Med. 2007;17(4):304–306. doi: 10.1097/JSM.0b013e31803202c8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sortland O, Tysvaer AT. Brain damage in former association football players. An evaluation by cerebral computed tomography. Neuroradiology. 1989;31(1):44–48. doi: 10.1007/BF00342029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Di Virgilio TG, Hunter A, Wilson L, et al. Evidence for Acute Electrophysiological and Cognitive Changes Following Routine Soccer Heading. EBioMedicine. 2016;13:66–71. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2016.10.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Comstock RD, Currie DW, Pierpoint LA, Grubenhoff JA, Fields SK. An Evidence-Based Discussion of Heading the Ball and Concussions in High School Soccer. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169(9):830–837. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.1062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Belanger, Vanderploeg, Rodney, McAllister Subconcussive Blows to the Head: A Formative Review of Short-term Clinical Outcomes. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation. 2016;31(3):159–166. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kontos AP, Braithwaite R, Dakan S, Elbin RJ. Computerized neurocognitive testing within 1 week of sport-related concussion: meta-analytic review and analysis of moderating factors. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(3):324–332. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713001471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Webbe FM, Ochs SR. Personality traits relate to heading frequency in male soccer players. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology. 2007;1(4):379–389. [Google Scholar]
- 32.FIFA. FIFA Big Count 2006. FIFA Communications Division; 2007. www.fifa.com. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Levitch, Zimmerman, Lubin, et al. Recent and Long-Term Soccer Heading Exposure Is Differentially Associated With Neuropsychological Function in Amateur Players. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2017:1–9. doi: 10.1017/S1355617717000790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lipton ML, Ifrah C, Stewart WF, et al. Validation of HeadCount-2w for estimation of two-week heading: Comparison to daily reporting in adult amateur player. J Sci Med Sport. 2017 doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Delaney JS, Al-Kashmiri A, Correa JA. Mechanisms of injury for concussions in university football, ice hockey, and soccer. Clin J Sport Med. 2014;24(3):233–237. doi: 10.1097/JSM.0000000000000017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Lingsma H, Maas A. Heading in soccer: More than a subconcussive event? Neurology. 2017;88(9):822–823. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]