Skip to main content
Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI logoLink to Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI
. 2018 Nov 27;8(12):222. doi: 10.3390/ani8120222

Who’s a Good Handler? Important Skills and Personality Profiles of Wildlife Detection Dog Handlers

La Toya J Jamieson 1,*, Greg S Baxter 1, Peter J Murray 1
PMCID: PMC6316394  PMID: 30486469

Abstract

Simple Summary

Professional working dog teams perform a range of functions faster and more accurately than other methods. Therefore, these teams are highly valuable to our society. Whilst some information is available on the skills that are important for dog handlers to possess, this isn’t always sourced from the handlers themselves. As a result, information may be missing, or the provided information may not be relevant. Through questionnaires, we collected information on the skills that wildlife detection dog handlers believe to be important for working success. Handler personality evaluations were also completed to determine whether specific personalities are better suited to this unique working field. Knowledge and understanding of dog body language and behaviour were rated highly. The handlers’ personality profiles had large ranges, indicating that no personality is attracted to, or perhaps best suited for, working with wildlife detection dogs. Dog handler dedication, training, and the dog–handler relationship are likely more influential factors.

Abstract

Wildlife detection dog teams are employed internationally for environmental surveys, and their success often depends on the dog handler. Minimal research is available on the skills that dog handlers believe are important, and no research has been published on the personality profiles of wildlife detection dog handlers. This may reveal the skills that people should acquire to be successful at, or suitable for, this work. An online questionnaire was distributed to Australian and New Zealand wildlife detection dog handlers. This questionnaire provided a list of skills to be rated based on importance, and a personality assessment measured their five main personality domains (Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness). A total of 35 questionnaires were collected, which represented over half of the estimated Australian wildlife detection dog handler population. The handlers had on average 7.2 years of dog handling experience, and 54% were female. More than half (57%) of the handlers stated that they were very emotionally attached to their dogs; however, 9% stated they were either not attached or mildly attached to their working dogs. The skill that was rated highest for importance was ‘ability to read dog body language’, and the lowest was ‘skilled in report writing’. On average, the handlers scored high in the Agreeableness domain, low in the Neuroticism domain, and average in the Extraversion, Conscientiousness, and Openness domains. However, all of the personality scores had large ranges. Therefore, a dog handler’s personality may not be as influential on their success as their training or their dog–handler bond. Further research would be beneficial regarding the direct impact that the dog–handler bond and the handler’s knowledge have on working team outcomes.

Keywords: dog handler, detection dog, personality, skills, dog–handler relationship

1. Introduction

The dog handler is potentially the most important factor affecting working dog performance [1,2,3]. The handler is responsible for more than simply following their dog in the field. They must be able to monitor and interpret their dog’s often subtle behaviours, and determine the most suitable areas and directions in which to work [4]. Underestimating the dog handler’s role can jeopardise not only the working dog’s performance, but also their welfare [1]. Whilst there is a large literature on ideal detection dog traits (reviewed by Jamieson et al. [5]), there is currently minimal information on the optimal profile of a dog handler, with handler and owner personality profiles only beginning to be researched [1,6,7,8]. Understanding what makes certain people more suited to dog handling, such as their knowledge, skills, and personality types, requires further research [8].

Wildlife detection dogs are dogs that are used for an environmental purpose, such as locating scat, plants, or live animals to determine species presence or distribution [9]. Wildlife detection dogs have been used globally for environmental surveys [10,11,12], and are particularly useful for species that occur at low densities [13,14]. Training a detection dog handler to operational standard requires significant time and dedication [14]. Therefore, it is crucial that the best candidates are selected prior to dedicating significant resources. Whilst the literature lists certain important traits for dog handlers to possess (e.g., Rebmann et al. [15], Hurt et al. [16], Minhinnick et al. [17]), there is no study that determines what wildlife detection dog handlers believe are important knowledge and skills. This is valuable information for new dog handlers or established handlers wanting to further their skills. Wildlife detection dog handlers will likely require unique skills in comparison with other detection fields due to the nature of wildlife detection work. Wildlife detection work is often required in highly remote areas, which poses multiple threats to the dog–handler team. The handler is also responsible for ensuring that their dogs pose no threat to wildlife, and may also be required to have a similar level of knowledge to wildlife ecologists to ensure survey success, which is specific to this field. Currently, there is little information published regarding the factors that influence dog handler selection [17]. Therefore, this field would benefit from an analysis of the skills and knowledge that are crucial for a detection dog team’s success [17].

When examining a person’s personality, their five main personality domains are typically measured—Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness [18]. Previous research has determined that dog handlers have unique qualities, as shown by their five main personality domains, when compared to the personality profiles of the general population [19]. In some cases, handler personality traits have also been correlated with their dog-handling practises [8]. For example, male Polish police dog handlers were reported to have very high Conscientiousness scores, slightly above average Agreeableness and Extraversion scores, slightly below average Openness scores, and very low Neuroticism scores [19]. However, no research has determined if wildlife detection dog handlers share similar traits. As previously highlighted, wildlife detection dog handlers have unique working requirements, so determining this information may assist with dog handler selection. As a handler’s personality impacts their dog training and handling practises [8], it may also allow for dog-handler training programs to be better constructed for individuals.

This research had three main aims: (1) collect general information from Australian and New Zealand wildlife detection dog handlers to improve our understanding of their working roles; (2) determine the characteristics and knowledge that dog handlers believe are important to be successful in this specialised field; and (3) use a well-tested psychological assessment to determine if wildlife detection dog handlers share similar personality profiles. This information may assist with future wildlife detection dog handler selection and highlight skills that need to be attained to be successful in this niche field. This is especially needed in Australia, where wildlife detection dogs are still a relatively new survey method. The research hypotheses were: (1) handlers will rate the skills specific to dog handling and training highly (>4.5); and (2) handlers will have high Conscientiousness scores and low Neuroticism scores.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Questionnaire

We constructed a questionnaire to collect information regarding the important skills and personality profiles of wildlife detection dog handlers. The questionnaire was in three sections (see Appendix A). The first listed the traits and characteristics that the published literature has stated as being important dog handler qualities. These qualities were rated from one to five (with one being not important, and five being very important). The second section was a personality assessment, which was comprised of 120 questions that the participants used to describe themselves. The personality assessment was the International Personality Item Pool-NEO (IPIP-NEO-120; [18]). The IPIP-NEO-120 is a free, web-based personality assessment that has been demonstrated to be highly reliable with the results showing high similarity to commercial personality assessments [18,20]. The IPIP-NEO-120 assessed the handlers’ five main personality domains: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness [18]. The final section collected personal information such as the handlers’ age, sex, and years working as a detection dog handler.

Questionnaire Distribution

The questionnaire was distributed via email to dog handlers actively working with wildlife detection dogs in Australia and New Zealand. At the time of the survey, there was no formal wildlife detection dog community in Australia; therefore, it was difficult to determine the potential sample size. Based on working groups, individual dog handlers’ internet presence, and inclusion in the published literature, we estimated that there were approximately 50 individuals working within this field in Australia. New Zealand’s detection dog community is governed by the Department of Conversation (DOC). Australian wildlife detection dog teams require no formal selection or assessment. In contrast, New Zealand detection dog teams are selected by the New Zealand Department of Conservation (DOC), and teams must complete both an interim and full certificate assessment [21]. DOC estimated that there were approximately 70 dog handlers working in this field [21]. This department distributed our questionnaire to the New Zealand dog handlers. Professional dog trainers in Australia, who were also responsible for training certain handlers, were also contacted via email to distribute the questionnaire to their students (handlers). A reminder email was sent to all of the parties two weeks after initial contact in an attempt to increase sample size. All of the contacted parties were encouraged to share this questionnaire with other dog handlers who were known to them. All of the research that was completed had the University of Queensland’s Human Ethics approval (approval number: 2016001089).

2.2. Data Analyses

Personality profiles were calculated online using the scoring process developed by Johnson [18]. The personality profiles that were created revealed where each dog handler ranked on the scale within each personality domain. General linear models were constructed to determine the influence of the dog handler’s age, gender, country of origin, employment (professional or volunteer), experience level, or target species (native or pest) on their personality scoring or rating of skills. Pearson correlations were then measured between the dog handler’s age and years of dog handling experience, and their personality scoring. Minitab 18 was used for all of the statistical analyses. Significance was set at p < 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. General Handler Information

A total of 35 completed questionnaires were collected from Australian and New Zealand dog handlers. Thirty-two individual dog handlers and eight working dog organisations were sent the questionnaire. As some working dog organisations distributed these questionnaires independently, it was not possible to calculate the exact response rate. Of the returned questionnaires, 31 were from Australian handlers and four were from New Zealand. The dog handlers had a mean age of 43.9 ± 9.1 years, with 54% being female. The majority of the handlers were professionals (80%), with a mean of 7.2 years of dog handling experience. Based on their detection dogs’ target species, 57% handled native species detection dogs, 37% handled pest species detection dogs, and 6% handled both native and pest species detection dogs (Table 1).

Table 1.

Native and pest species, from Australia and New Zealand, listed as target species for the wildlife detection dogs. Total handlers currently handling these species-specific detection dogs are also listed.

Country Native Total Pest Total
Australia Bell’s turtle
(Myuchelys bellii)
1 Electric ants
(Wasmannia auropunctata)
1
Black-tailed antechinus
(Antechinus arktos)
1 Feral cat
(Felis catus)
7
Eastern bristlebird
(Dasyornis brachypterus)
1 Fireweed
(Chamerion angustifolium)
1
Emu
(Dromaius novaehollandiae)
2 Fire ants
(Solenopsis geminate)
1
Koala
(Phascolarctos cinereus)
10 Fox
(Vulpes vulpes)
6
Pygmy Blue-tongue lizard
(Tiliqua adelaidensis)
1 Hawkweed
(Hieracium spp.)
1
Northern and Tiger quolls
(Dasyurus hallucatus and D. maculatus)
8 Introduced rodents
(e.g., Rattus rattus)
2
New Zealand Blue duck
(Hymenolaimus malacorhynchos)
2 Feral cat
(Felis catus)
1
Brown teal
(Anas chlorotis)
1 Introduced rodents
(e.g., Rattus rattus)
1
Kakapo
(Strigops habroptila)
2
Kea
(Nestor notabilis)
1
Kiwi
(Apteryx sp.)
1
South Island Takahe
(Porphyrio hochstetteri)
1

The detection dogs used by the respondents belonged to a variety of breeds, with the majority coming from herding, gun dog, or terrier breeds (including cross-breeds). The dogs that are most commonly and currently used in Australia are Border Collies (29%), Labrador Retrievers (17%), and English Springer Spaniels (17%). Of the dog breeds listed, 26% of handlers recorded currently working with cross-breeds. Whilst the handlers varied in how emotionally attached they were to their detection dogs, all of the handlers agreed that their behaviours and stress levels impacted their dogs’ behaviours. Of the 35 handlers, 57% stated they were very emotionally attached to their dogs, while 34% were moderately emotionally attached, and 9% were mildly or not emotionally attached.

3.2. Important Handler Characteristics and Knowledge

The dog handlers were asked to rate a series of important dog handler skills and knowledge, which were listed in the literature. Qualities rated as most important were ‘ability to read dog body language’, ‘ability to trust in a dog’s indications’, ‘strong working ethic’, and ‘knowledgeable on dog behaviour’ (Table 2). The qualities that were rated least important were ‘skilled in report writing’, ‘strong leader’, and ‘theoretical background in ecology’.

Table 2.

Scoring of skills and knowledge based on their importance and relevance for wildlife detection dog handlers (1: strongly disagree it is important, 5: strongly agree it is important; SD: standard deviation).

Rank Skill Mean Score SD
1 Ability to read dog body language 4.7 0.5
Ability to trust in a dog’s indications 4.7 0.5
2 Strong work ethic 4.6 0.6
3 Knowledgeable on dog behaviour 4.5 0.6
4 Skilled in dog handling 4.4 0.8
Ability to read wind direction 4.4 0.7
5 Navigational skills 4.3 0.8
Sound knowledge of target species 4.3 0.8
6 High level of physical fitness/stamina 4.0 0.9
7 Practical ecological experience 3.9 1.0
8 Team player 3.7 1.1
9 Knowledgeable of canine olfactory physiology 3.6 0.9
Experienced in dog training 3.6 1.0
10 Theoretical background in ecology 3.5 1.0
Strong leader 3.5 1.0
11 Skilled in report writing 3.0 1.0

General linear models determined no significant differences between how the handlers rated these traits based on the handlers’ age, sex, country, employment (professional or volunteer), experience level (years dog handling), or target species (native or pest species). The only exception was trait three (skilled in dog handling), which was significantly impacted by the handlers’ employment, with volunteers rating it as significantly more important (mean = 4.9) than the professional handlers (mean = 4.3; p = 0.011). Of the handlers who listed additional skills or qualities that they believed were important, 45% stated patience, and 35% stated experience or knowledge of their working environment.

3.3. Personality Profiles

The handlers’ personality domains that were the most different to the average scores were the Agreeableness and Neuroticism domains. This scoring system is based on the classifications of ‘low’, ‘average’, or ‘high’ that are provided when the personality scores were calculated online, which was developed by Johnson [18]. This online program classified a person’s results based on whether their score was in the lowest 30%, middle 40%, or highest 30% based on age and sex. For the purpose of this research, due to the differences in participants’ ages and sex, scores between 35–65 were classified as ‘average’. Scores above 65 were classified as ‘high’, and scores below 35 were classified as ‘low’. Whilst handlers’ mean personality scores differed from the average, there was a large range between the individual handlers’ scores (Table 3).

Table 3.

The dog handlers’ personality scores in the five main personality domains (in bold), and the traits (shown below each domain) within these domains. The standard deviation and range is also provided.

Domain Mean SD Range
Extraversion 57.6 25.7 13–99
Friendliness 58.1 27.3 7–98
Gregariousness 44.9 29.2 2–99
Assertiveness 49.4 25.8 4–95
Activity Level 71.9 18.6 36–99
Excitement seeking 51.0 25.1 12–95
Cheerfulness 56.5 22.6 5–88
Agreeableness 67.5 24.0 13–99
Trust 62.9 21.7 8–95
Morality 63.2 23.3 4–89
Altruism 55.7 25.4 13–95
Co-operation 61.3 23.9 7–89
Modesty 68.4 26.5 16–99
Sympathy 55.9 26.2 2–99
Conscientiousness 57.7 25.3 2–96
Self-efficacy 48.6 24.4 1–97
Orderliness 46.9 22.5 16–90
Dutifulness 53.3 23.1 5–95
Achievement-striving 62.3 24.8 2–91
Self-discipline 59.4 27.6 6–99
Cautiousness 60.9 24.5 18–97
Neuroticism 34.9 27.7 1–82
Anxiety 36.6 29.0 1–97
Anger 31.5 27.4 1–76
Depression 34.7 25.0 3–77
Self-consciousness 44.9 23.3 1–90
Immoderation 43.8 24.4 1–85
Vulnerability 43.1 28.7 1–99
Openness 52.1 27.51 2–95
Imagination 42.1 27.4 1–83
Artistic interests 49.7 27.0 1–89
Emotionality 41.7 30.5 1–97
Adventurousness 66.4 24.7 15–99
Intellect 47.5 28.9 1–91
Liberalism 62.0 17.5 25–89

General linear models determined that the handlers’ personality domains were not affected by age, except for the Conscientiousness domain (p = 0.04). Age was negatively correlated to Conscientiousness (Pearson correlation = −0.348, p = 0.04). Similarly, the dog handlers’ personality domains were not impacted by which target species they detected (native, pest, or both), except for the Openness domain (p = 0.018). The dog handlers’ gender, country of residence (i.e., Australia or New Zealand), employment (professional or volunteer handler), or attachment to their dog did not impact their personality scores. The only exception was Neuroticism, which was impacted by sex (p = 0.049), with males having higher mean scores than females (44 and 27.3, respectively). However, there was no significant difference between the different sexes’ personality domains. Further, the dog handler’s personality domains weren’t impacted by their level of emotional attachment to their dogs.

4. Discussion

The returned questionnaires provided information on current wildlife detection dog handlers in Australia and New Zealand, skills they believed important, and their personality profiles. There are extensive lists of skills, knowledge, and traits that the literature states are important for detection dog handlers to possess (e.g., Rebmann et al. [15], Hurt et al. [16], Minhinnick et al. [17]). These lists aren’t always created by dog handlers. Therefore, important skills may be overlooked.

Whilst all the skills listed in the distributed questionnaire were sourced from the literature, the dog handlers clearly rated certain skills to be more important than others. The skills that were listed the highest were focused on the detection dog itself. The only traits that were directly related to the detection dogs and were rated low were knowledge of canine olfaction and experience in dog training. This was unexpected, not only because knowledge on canine olfaction has been listed as important dog handler knowledge [17,22], but also due to its relationship with environmental conditions. Dog handlers who have little experience in dog training or knowledge on dog learning theory are anticipated to struggle handling dogs in novel situations [17]. Dog handlers with dog training knowledge have been reported to be more self-confident, more aware of their dog’s working abilities, and use significantly less aversive handling methods [23]. Ensuring handlers have theoretical knowledge of dog training principles allows them to implement training practises that are efficient and humane. Therefore, future research would be beneficial to determine the performance variation between dog handlers who are also dog trainers, or have trained their current dogs, and handlers who are not experienced in dog training.

Whilst the dog handlers rated a sound knowledge of the target species relatively highly, both practical and theoretical ecological knowledge was not rated highly. This was unexpected, due to the impact that it may have on field survey success. Poor ecological knowledge may reduce the effectiveness of surveys and minimise information recorded on non-target species and habitat structure [16]. If wildlife detection dog handlers are working in close partnership with ecologists or land managers, this knowledge may not be as important. However, this knowledge is likely to aid dog handlers and potentially improve their future work [16].

There was a large variation in the handlers’ responses to how emotionally attached they were to their current detection dog/s. However, this level of attachment did not impact their personality scores. Based on the published literature, which highlights the importance and impact of the dog–handler relationship [8,24,25], it was anticipated that all of the handlers would be very emotionally attached to their dogs. This was not demonstrated in our findings. Previous research has determined that dog–handler teams with higher quality relationships also had higher performance levels and better dog–handler communication [25,26]. It could be argued that the higher the dog–handler relationship, the higher the dog’s dependence on their handler, thereby reducing the dog’s independence and performance [27]. However, a larger proportion of the literature to date has supported the hypothesis that the dog–handler relationship is positively correlated to the dogs’ problem-solving abilities and working performances [25,26,28,29,30,31]. During this project, the dog handler’s working performance was not evaluated. However, this would be beneficial in future research, with the dog teams’ performances being compared against their attachment levels. Further research is also needed on other factors that may impact dog–handler attachment and working performance, such as detection dogs’ living arrangements and conditions, daily time spent with the dog, and where the dog was sourced (e.g., adopted, purchased, or purposefully bred).

The dog handler’s personality influences the dog–handler interface and therefore the team’s performance [24]. As a result, previous research has investigated how these differences in a handler’s personality impact a working dog’s performance [8]. Within our study, there were differences between the handlers’ mean personality scores and the ‘average’ scores; however, there was a large range. This was most strongly demonstrated in the Agreeableness and Neuroticism domains. The handlers scored highly for Agreeableness. Agreeableness has previously been positively correlated with team co-operation and avoidance of conflicts, and negatively associated with dog owner-directed aggression [8,32]. Handlers scoring high for Agreeableness also use less verbal corrections, potentially creating a more positive dog–handler working relationship [8].

The dog handlers collectively scored low for the Neuroticism domain. Neuroticism in humans is related to anxious tendencies [33]. Dog handlers scoring high for Neuroticism are reported to have dogs who perform less efficiently at working tasks and take longer to respond to commands [8,33,34]. Having handlers with low Neuroticism scores may also improve a dog’s stress levels, with dogs having lower cortisol levels when handled by calm handlers [35].

Collectively, the dog handlers scored in the higher percentile of the ‘average’ score for the Conscientiousness and Extraversion domains. It has been postulated that high Conscientiousness scores will be positively associated with the dog–handler relationship and their working performance [8]. However, this association requires further research, and was not demonstrated in our results where Conscientiousness scores were not correlated with dog–handler emotional attachment. High scores for Extraversion are related to more confident individuals [36]. Whilst dog handlers should be confident in their team’s abilities, it is important that these scores don’t become too high, or else over-confidence could negatively impact their performance. Further research on the relationship between a handler’s Extraversion scores and their working performance would be beneficial. The dog handlers had an average score for the Openness domain. This was surprising, as Openness has previously been related to adaptability within a working environment [8]. However, there is currently minimal research on the relationship between a dog handler’s Openness score and their work.

5. Conclusions

Wildlife detection dogs and their handlers must have a compatible partnership. To achieve this, we need to begin to focus on the handlers—their selection, skills, and training—as much as we do on the dogs. Improving how we select and train detection dogs alone is not enough, nor is placing a high performing dog with an unsuitable or unknowledgeable handler going to result in good outcomes. Our research has highlighted the skills and knowledge that dog handlers believe to be important for working success. Whilst these skills were focused on wildlife detection work, certain skills are likely transferable to other detection fields. As these results weren’t always consistent with the published literature, it would be beneficial to determine how different skill sets and knowledge may impact a detection dog team’s success and performance. Further research is also required to determine the correlation between mean personality scores and dog handlers’ performance. On average, the handlers scored high in the Agreeableness domain and low in the Neuroticism domain. Assessing handlers’ personality scores may assist with highlighting individuals who have personalities suitable for this work, but this should not be the sole screening process. Whilst the mean scores of the handlers were mainly consistent with the literature, there was a very large range. Therefore, personality may not be the overall determinant of a person’s suitability for wildlife detection dog work. Rather, a potential handler’s suitability is related to how someone applies themselves to their training and work, and their dog–handler bond. Future research should begin to focus on improving how detection dog handlers are selected and trained, and ensuring that handlers are provided with the best available information to allow them to shape their own dog’s training and working plans. Optimising this dog–handler pairing will likely improve working success and efficiency [37]. Therefore, improving dog handler selection and training may improve field success and working dog welfare, which should be priorities for this highly applicable methodology.

Acknowledgments

Thank you to all the dog handlers who participated in this project, and to all the organisations who helped distribute the questionnaire. The first author would also like to acknowledge she received an Australian Government Research Training Scholarship to complete this research.

Appendix A

Below is the questionnaire which was distributed to Australian and New Zealand wildlife detection dog handlers.

Appendix A.1. Important Dog-Handler Traits

  • (1)
    Please indicate which of the following qualities and characteristics you agree are important for a wildlife detection dog-handler to possess, along with the qualities you agree you personally possessed when you first began wildlife detection dog work. The ratings are classed as follows:
    Strongly Disagree Disagree Neither Disagree nor Agree Agree Strongly Agree
    1 2 3 4 5
    Qualities/Characteristics of Dog-Handler Important Dog-Handler Traits (Please Rate 1–5) Qualities You Possessed (Please Click the Boxes)
    High level of physical fitness/stamina
    Knowledgeable of canine olfactory physiology
    Skilled in dog handling
    Experienced in dog training
    Knowledgeable on dog behaviour
    Ability to read dog body language
    Theoretical background in ecology
    Practical ecological experience
    Sound knowledge of target species
    Ability to read wind direction
    Navigational skills
    Team player
    Strong leader
    Ability to trust in a dog’s indications
    Strong working ethic
    Skilled in report writing
  • (2)
    Do you believe there are any other important characteristics or traits for a detection dog-handler to possess?
    • Personality:
    • Previous experiences/knowledge:

Appendix A.2. Personality Assessment

The following are a list of questions which are relevant to evaluating your personality type. Within the rows are statements about one’s personality, with all statements beginning with “I…”. Please indicate, by an ‘X’ in the appropriate box, how the statement describes you.

EXAMPLE ONLY Strongly Disagree Moderately Disagree Neither Agree nor Disagree Moderately Agree Strongly Agree
Worry about things X

Please respond to the below statements with how you currently see yourself and not how you would like to see yourself. Please ensure you respond to every statement.

Personal statements Strongly DISAGREE Moderately DISAGREE Neither agree nor disagree Moderately AGREE Strongly AGREE
Worry about things
Make friends easily
Have a vivid imagination
Trust others
Complete tasks successfully
Get angry easily
Love large parties
Believe in the importance of art
Use others for my own ends
Like to tidy up
Often feel blue
Take charge
Experience my emotions intensely
Love to help others
Keep my promises
Find it difficult to approach others
Am always busy
Prefer variety to routine
Love a good fight
Work hard
Go on binges
Love excitement
Love to read challenging material
Believe that I am better than others
Am always prepared
Panic easily
Radiate joy
Tend to vote for socially liberal political candidates
Sympathize with the homeless
Jump into things without thinking
Fear for the worst
Feel comfortable around people
Enjoy fantasy
Believe that others have good intentions
Excel in what I do
Get irritated easily
Talk to a lot of different people at parties
See beauty in things that others might not notice
Cheat to get ahead
Often forget to put things back in their proper place
Dislike myself
Try to lead others
Feel others’ emotions
Am concerned about others
Tell the truth
Am afraid to draw attention to myself
Am always on the go
Prefer to stick with things that I know
Yell at people
Do more than what’s expected of me
Rarely overindulge
Seek adventure
Avoid philosophical discussions
Think highly of myself
Carry out my plans
Become overwhelmed by events
Have a lot of fun
Believe that there is no absolute right or wrong
Feel sympathy for those who are worse off than me
Make rash decisions
Am afraid of many things
Avoid contact with others
Love to daydream
Trust what people say
Handle tasks smoothly
Lose my temper
Prefer to be alone
Do not like poetry
Take advantage of others
Leave a mess in my room
Am often down in the dumps
Take control of things
Rarely notice my emotions
Am indifferent to the feelings of others
Break rules
Only feel comfortable with friends
Do a lot in my spare time
Dislike changes
Insult people
Do just enough work to get by
Easily resist temptations
Enjoy being reckless
Have difficulty understanding abstract ideas
Have a high opinion of myself
Waste my time
Feel that I’m unable to deal with things
Love life
Tend to vote for socially conservative political candidates
Am not interested in other people’s problems
Rush into things
Get stressed out easily
Keep others at a distance
Like to get lost in thought
Distrust people
Know how to get things done
Am not easily annoyed
Avoid crowds
Do not enjoy going to art museums
Obstruct others’ plans
Leave my belongings around
Feel comfortable with myself
Wait for others to lead the way
Don’t understand people who get emotional
Take no time for others
Break my promises
Am not bothered by difficult social situations
Like to take it easy
Am attached to conventional ways
Get back at others
Put little time and effort into my work
Am able to control my cravings
Act wild and crazy
Am not interested in theoretical discussions
Boast about my virtues
Have difficulty starting tasks
Remain calm under pressure
Look at the bright side of life
Believe that we should be tough on crime
Try not to think about the needy
Act without thinking

Appendix A.3. Personal Information

  • (a)

    Age:

  • (b)

    Gender: Male   Female

  • (c)

    Country of residence: Australia   New Zealand

  • (d)

    Are you a professionally employed dog-handler or a volunteer

  • (e)

    Years working as a dog-handler:

  • (f)

    What dog breed do you most frequently use for conservation detection work:

  • (g)

    What target species do you handle/train the most dogs to detect:

  • (h)
    How emotionally attached are you to the dogs you’re currently handling (please click one below):
    • NOT EMOTIONALLY ATTACHED
    • MILDLY EMOTIONALLY ATTACHED
    • MODERATELY EMOTIONALLY ATTACHED
    • VERY EMOTIONALLY ATTACHED
  • (i)

    Do you believe your stress levels influence your dog’s behaviour?   YES   NO

END OF QUESTIONNAIRE.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.T.J.J.; Data curation, L.T.J.J.; Investigation, L.T.J.J.; Methodology, L.T.J.J.; Supervision, G.S.B. and P.J.M.; Writing—original draft, L.T.J.J., Writing—review & editing, G.S.B. and P.J.M.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Cobb M., Branson N., McGreevy P., Lill A., Bennett P. The advent of canine performance science: Offering a sustainable future for working dogs. Behav. Process. 2015;110:96–104. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.McGarrity M.E., Sinn D.L., Thomas S.G., Marti C.N., Gosling S.D. Comparing the predictive validity of behavioural codings and behavioural ratings in a working-dog breeding program. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016;179:82–94. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.03.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Jamieson L.J., Baxter G.S., Murray P.J. You are not my handler! Impact of changing handlers on dogs’ behaviours and detection performance. Animals. 2018;8:176. doi: 10.3390/ani8100176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bird R.C. An examination of the training and reliability of the narcotics detection dog. Ky. Law J. 1997;85:405–433. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jamieson L.J., Baxter G.S., Murray P.J. Identifying suitable detection dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017;195:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.06.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Schoeberl I., Wedl M., Bauer B., Day J., Moestl E., Kotrschal K. Effects of owner-dog relationship and owner personality on cortisol modulation in human-dog dyads. Anthrozoös. 2012;25:199–214. doi: 10.2752/175303712X13316289505422. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Curb L., Abramson C., Grice J., Kennison S. The Relationship between Personality Match and Pet Satisfaction among Dog Owners. Anthrozoös. 2013;26:395–404. doi: 10.2752/175303713X13697429463673. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Payne E.M., Arnott E.R., Early J.B., Bennett P.C., McGreevy P.D. Dogmanship on the farm: Analysis of personality dimensions and training styles of stock dog handlers in Australia. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2015;10:271–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.08.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hurt A., Smith D.A. In: Canine Ergonomics: The Science of Working Dogs. Helton W.S., editor. CRC Press; Boca Raton, FL, USA: 2009. pp. 175–194. Chapter 9. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kapfer J.M., Munoz D.J., Tomasek T. Use of wildlife detector dogs to study Eastern box turtle (Terrapene carolina carolina) populations. Herpetol. Conserv. Biol. 2012;7:169–175. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Browne C.M., Stafford K.J., Fordham R.A. The detection and identification of tuatara and gecko scents by dogs. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2015;10:496–503. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.08.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cristescu R.H., Foley E., Markula A., Jackson G., Jones D., Frère C. Accuracy and efficiency of detection dogs: A powerful new tool for koala conservation and management. Sci. Rep. 2015;5:8349–8355. doi: 10.1038/srep08349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.DeMatteo K., Rinas M., Sede M., Davenport B., Argüelles C., Lovett K., Parker P. Detection Dogs: An Effective Technique for Bush Dog Surveys. J. Wildl. Manag. 2009;73:1436–1440. doi: 10.2193/2008-545. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Orkin J.D., Yang Y., Yang C., Yu D.W., Jiang X. Cost-effective scat-detection dogs: Unleashing a powerful new tool for international mammalian conservation biology. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:34758. doi: 10.1038/srep34758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Rebmann A., David E., Sorg M.H.H. Cadaver Dog Handbook: Forensic Training and Tactics for the Recovery of Human Remains. CRC Press; Boca Raton, FL, USA: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hurt A., Woollett D.A., Parker M. In: Canine Olfaction Science and Law. Ensminger J., Jezierski T., Papet L.E., editors. CRC Press; London, UK: 2016. pp. 139–153. Chapter 11. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Minhinnick S., Papet L.E., Stephenson C.M., Stephenson M.R. In: Canine Olfaction Science and Law. Ensminger J., Jezierski T., Papet L.E., editors. CRC Press; London, UK: 2016. pp. 155–171. Chapter 12. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Johnson J.A. Measuring thirty facets of the Five Factor Model with a 120-item public domain inventory: Development of the IPIP-NEO-120. J. Res. Personal. 2014;51:78–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.05.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kaleta T., Pisula W., Fiszdon K., Kondrakiewicz P. Individual differences in police dog handlers. Pol. Psychol. Bull. 2011;42:52–55. doi: 10.2478/v10059-011-0008-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Maples J.L., Guan L., Carter N.T., Miller J.D. A test of the International Personality Item Pool Representation of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory and Development of a 120-Item IPIP-Based Measure of the Five-Factor Model. Psychol. Assess. 2014;26:1070–1084. doi: 10.1037/pas0000004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Thomas S. (Department of Conservation, New Zealand). Personal communication. 2018.
  • 22.Goldblatt A., Gazit I., Terkel J. In: Canine Ergonomics: The Science of Working Dogs. Helton W.S., editor. CRC Press; Boca Raton, FL, USA: 2009. pp. 136–174. Chapter 8. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Haverbeke A., Messaoudi F., Depiereux E., Stevens M., Giffroy J., Diederich C. Efficiency of working dogs undergoing a new Human Familiarization and training program. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2010;5:112–119. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2009.08.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Zubedat S., Aga-Mizrachi S., Cymerblit-Sabba A., Shwartz J., Fiko Leon J., Rozen S., Varkovitzky I., Eshed Y., Grinstein D., Avital A. Human-animal interface: The effects of handler’s stress on the performance of canines in an explosive detection task. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014;158:69–75. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.05.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Hoummady S., Péron F., Grandjean D., Cléro D., Bernard B., Titeux E., Desquilbet L., Gilbert C. Relationships between personality of human-dog dyads and performance in working tasks. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016;177:42–45. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2016.01.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Diverio S., Menchetti L., Riggio G., Azzari C., Laboni M., Zasso R., Di Mari W., Matteo Santoro M. Dogs’ coping styles and dog-handler relationships influence avalanche search team performance. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017;191:67–77. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.02.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Udell M.A.R. When dogs look back: Inhibition of independent problem-solving behaviour in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) compared with wolves (Canis lupus) Biol. Lett. 2015;11:20150489. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lefebvre D., Diederich C., Delcourt M., Giffory J. The quality of the relation between handler and military dogs influences efficiency and welfare of dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007;104:49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Horváth Z., Dóka A., Miklósi A. Affiliative and disciplinary behavior of human handlers during play with their dog affects cortisol concentrations in opposite directions. Horm. Behav. 2008;54:107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Horn L., Range F., Huber L. Dogs’ attention towards humans depends on their relationship, not only on social familiarity. Anim. Cognit. 2013;16:435–443. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0584-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Arnott E.R., Early J.B., Wade C.M., McGreevy P.D. Environmental Factors Associated with Success Rates of Australian Stock Herding Dogs. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e104457. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0104457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Herron M.E., Shofer F.S., Reisner I.R. Survey of the use and outcome of confrontational and non-confrontational training methods in client-owned dogs showing undesired behaviors. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009;117:47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.12.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kis A., Turcsán B., Miklósi Á., Gácsi M. The effect of the owner’s personality on the behaviour of owner-dog dyads. Interact. Stud. 2012;13:373–385. doi: 10.1075/is.13.3.03kis. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kotrschal K., Schoeberl I., Bauer B., Thibeaut A.-M., Wedl M. Dyadic relationships and operational performance of male and female owners and their male dogs. Behav. Process. 2009;81:383–391. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Slotta-Bachmayr L., Schwarzenberger F. Faecal cortisol metabolites as indicators of stress during training and search missions in avalanche dogs. Vet. Med. Austria. 2007;94:110–117. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Schaefer P.S., Williams C.C., Goodie A.S., Campbell W.K. Overconfidence and the Big Five. J. Res. Personal. 2004;38:473–480. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2003.09.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kydd E., McGreevy P. Sex differences in the herding styles of working sheepdogs and their handlers. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0184072. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0184072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES