Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Addict Behav. 2018 Oct 16;90:35–39. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.10.020

Substance Use Disorders and PTSD: Examining Substance Use, PTSD Symptoms, and Dropout Following Imaginal Exposure

Amber M Jarnecke 1, Nicholas P Allan 2, Christal L Badour 3, Julianne C Flanagan 1, Therese K Killeen 1, Sudie E Back 1,4
PMCID: PMC6324999  NIHMSID: NIHMS993995  PMID: 30355535

Abstract

Integrated exposure-based interventions to treat substance use disorders (SUD) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) may not be widely utilized, in part, because of clinician concerns that such interventions will worsen symptomatology and lead to treatment dropout. In order to address this question, the current pilot study examined whether participants’ ratings of craving and distress following imaginal exposure predicted increased substance use, PTSD severity, and treatment dropout. Participants (N=46) were U.S. military Veterans who met criteria for current SUD and PTSD. Subjective ratings of craving and distress, and past-week substance use and PTSD symptom severity were assessed at each treatment session. Multilevel modeling tested whether lagged ratings of craving and distress predicted the following week’s frequency of substance use and PTSD severity. Discrete time survival analysis, using proportional odds Cox ratio, examined whether craving and distress ratings predicted treatment dropout. The findings revealed that neither craving nor distress following imaginal exposure were associated with the following week’s substance use or PTSD severity. However, participants with higher craving and distress were more likely to drop out before completing treatment. Future research is needed to develop strategies to increase treatment retention for individuals at-risk for treatment dropout and identify mechanisms that account for the association between in-session ratings of craving and distress and dropout.

Keywords: addiction, substance use, posttraumatic stress disorder, imaginal exposure, treatment retention


Substance use disorders (SUD) frequently co-occur with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Individuals seeking SUD treatment are at greater risk for meeting lifetime criteria for PTSD and many individuals diagnosed with PTSD report misusing substances to self-medicate PTSD symptoms (Debell et al., 2014; Grant et al., 2015; Leeies, Pagura, Sareen, & Bolton, 2010). Veterans are at particularly high risk for this psychiatric comorbidity; approximately 41.4% of Veterans diagnosed with PTSD have a co-occurring substance use disorder (Petrakis, Rosenheck, & Desai, 2011).

Comorbid SUD and PTSD is associated with numerous negative sequelae (e.g., medical illness, homelessness, violence victimization, suicidality) and a challenging treatment course (Anderson et al., 2017; Mills et al., 2014; Ouimette, Goodwin, & Brown, 2006; Young, Rosen, & Finney, 2005). In practice, behavioral treatment for comorbid SUD and PTSD typically follows a sequential model (i.e., patients are treated for SUD before PTSD) (Schumm & Gore, 2016); however, recent years have seen increased development of integrated treatments for comorbid SUD and PTSD (e.g., Back et al., 2014; McGovern, Lambert-Harris, Alterman, Xie, & Meier, 2011; Najavits, 2003). Integrated treatments seek to acknowledge the interplay between SUD and PTSD symptoms, targeting both disorders simultaneously with one clinician. The integrated model has pulled support from research finding that improvement in PTSD impacts subsequent improvement in substance use, though improvement in substance use has little impact on subsequent improvement in PTSD (Back, Brady, Jaanimägi, & Jackson, 2006; Badour et al., 2017; Hien et al., 2010).

Integrated treatments have demonstrated efficacy, safety, and feasibility, and many patients report a preference for the integrated approach (Flanagan, Korte, Killeen, & Back, 2016; Schumm & Gore, 2016). Nevertheless, integrated treatments are not commonly utilized in substance use treatment centers (Back, Waldrop, & Brady, 2009; Schumm & Gore, 2016). This may be attributed to a variety of causes, including clinician concerns that addressing the trauma may worsen cravings and negatively impact treatment dropout and efficacy (Gielen, Krumeich, Havermans, Smeets, & Jansen, 2014; Norman & Hamblen, 2017). Indeed, prior research using laboratory-based tasks suggests that subjective craving and distress can be elicited by exposure to a stress or trauma-cue (Coffey et al., 2010; Coffey, Stasiewicz, Hughes, & Brimo, 2006; Sinha et al., 2008). However, repeated exposure to a trauma cue is associated with decreased craving and distress over time (Coffey et al., 2006); in turn, habituation of distress and craving is associated with PTSD symptom improvement and less frequent substance use, respectively (Badour et al., 2017; Foa & McLean, 2016). Research has also shown that, conditions comorbid with PTSD, such as SUD, depression, anxiety, and psychotic disorders, commonly decrease in severity or do not change as a function of PTSD treatments such as Prolonged Exposure (PE; Foa, Hembree, & Rothbaum, 2007; van Minnen, Zoellner, Harned, & Mills, 2015).

To directly examine whether imaginal exposure is associated with an increase in subsequent substance use, PTSD severity, or treatment dropout, the current pilot study took an exploratory approach to examine if ratings of craving and distress following imaginal exposures, delivered in the context of an integrated treatment (i.e., Concurrent Treatment of PTSD and Substance Use Disorders using Prolonged Exposure, or COPE; Back et al., 2014), are associated with 1) increased substance use during the following week, 2) increased PTSD symptom severity over the following week, or 3) treatment dropout.

Methods

Participants.

Participants were U.S. military Veterans enrolled in a clinical trial examining the efficacy of COPE. Eligible individuals: 1) were a Veteran, reservist, or member of the National Guard, 2) fluent in English, 3)18–65 years old, 4) met DSM-IV criteria for SUD based on the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Sheehan et al., 1998) 4), used substances within the past 90 days, 5) met DSM-IV criteria for PTSD 6) had a score of 50 or higher on the Clinician Administered PTSD Scale (Blake et al., 1995). Individuals were excluded if they: 1) reported current suicidal or homicidal ideation with intent, 2) had conditions requiring advanced levels of care such as psychotic or bipolar affective disorders, 3) endorsed a current eating disorder, 4) were receiving ongoing treatment for PTSD or SUD, or 5) demonstrated severe cognitive impairment as evidenced by the Mini Mental Status Exam (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). Participants stabilized on psychotropic medications at baseline were permitted to participate.

Only participants randomized to the COPE intervention were included in the current analyses (N=55; 90.9% male). Most participants identified as White/Caucasian (69.1%); the remainder identified as Black/African American (29.1%) or Multiracial/Other (1.8%). The average age of the sample was 39.49 (SD = 11.01). Most participants indicated alcohol was their primary substance (34.5%). One individual was found ineligible after completing the baseline assessment and 8 participants dropped out of treatment prior to session 4 (when imaginal exposures began). Thus, data was analyzed for 46 participants.

Procedures

Potential participants were screened and individuals meeting eligibility criteria were invited to partake in a more thorough baseline assessment. At the assessment visit, participants provided informed consent and completed semi-structured clinical interviews and self-report measures. Participants were then randomized to receive 12 weekly individual sessions of COPE or Relapse Prevention (Kadden et al., 1992). The COPE treatment integrates Relapse Prevention with PE. Abstinence was encouraged during treatment but not required of participants. Before each treatment session, participants completed assessments measuring substance use and PTSD symptoms. Consistent with the COPE manual, imaginal exposures occurred during sessions 4–11. As part of homework assignments, participants were instructed to listen to audio-recorded imaginal exposures daily.

Measures

Subjective ratings of distress and craving.

During sessions 4–11, participants rated their level of craving for their substance of choice on a 0–100 scale (0 = no craving to 100 = highest craving) immediately before and after in-session imaginal exposures. Before, after, and every 5 minutes throughout the imaginal exposure participants rated their level of distress on a scale of 0 (no distress) to 100 (the worst distress experienced).

Substance use.

The Timeline Followback (Sobell & Sobell, 1992), which uses a calendar based method for collecting self-reports, assessed drug and alcohol use. Percent days used (PDU) for alcohol or any substance was assessed two months prior to baseline and weekly, immediately prior to the therapy session, during treatment.

PTSD severity.

The PTSD Checklist–Military Version (Weathers, Litz, Herman, Huska, & Keane, 1994) assessed DSM-IV PTSD symptoms at baseline and weekly, immediately prior to, the therapy session. At each weekly assessment, participants were asked about their PTSD symptoms over the previous week. In the current sample, internal consistency ranged from good to excellent between sessions 4–12 (αs = 0.86 to 0.97).

Data analytic plan

Multilevel modeling (MLM) was conducted in Mplus version 8 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2017) to examine whether subjective ratings of cravings and distress following in-session imaginal exposures predicted substance use frequency or PTSD severity the following week. Intercepts and slopes were allowed to vary. Time, for which the first session analyzed in the data (i.e., session 4) was coded 0, was entered as a fixed effect. Group-mean centered lagged ratings of craving and distress were modeled as fixed effects as well. Lagged rating by time interactions examined how subjective ratings predicted substance use frequency and PTSD severity across sessions 4–12. Prior week’s frequency of substance use and PTSD severity were group mean-centered and included as covariates in the respective MLMs1. Two-part modeling was used to examine substance use outcomes due to the presence of zero-inflated data (Olsen & Schafer, 2001). In this approach, data are modeled in separate parts within the same model. One part of the model captures the likelihood of using (dichotomous, 0 = no use, 1 = use) and the other part models severity of use, conditional upon substance use (PDU).

Survival analysis, using a Cox proportional hazard model, as described by (Singer & Willett, 1993), was fit to the data in Mplus to examine the effects of craving and distress on treatment dropout. This model was compared to a model with the proportional hazard assumption relaxed to ensure best fit. A significant likelihood ratio test (LRT) and a lower Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) would indicate better fit for the Cox proportional hazard model. Continuing in treatment was coded as 0, dropout as 1, and time after a participant discontinued treatment was coded as missing. Lagged ratings of craving and distress were group mean-centered and entered together as predictors of dropout.

Results

Descriptive statistics.

Mean ratings of craving and distress before and after each session’s imaginal exposures are represented in Table 1. Participants tended to indicate a relative increase in subjective ratings of craving and distress from pre- to post-imaginal exposure across sessions. Post-imaginal exposure ratings of craving and distress, averaged across sessions 4–11, were not significantly associated with baseline levels of PDU (craving: r = 0.20, p = 0.196; distress: r = −0.05, p = 0.721) or PTSD severity (craving: r = 0.05, p = 0.735; distress: r = 0.06, p = 0.717). Participants reported using substances 19.85% (SD = 26.68) of the days during an average week.

Table 1.

Mean Ratings of Pre- and Post-imaginal Craving and Distress by Session

Craving Distress
Session Pre-imaginal
M (SD)
Post-imaginal
M (SD)
Pre-imaginal
M (SD)
Post-imaginal
M (SD)
4 18.11 (25.99) 23.31 (32.04) 52.05 (24.03) 58.13 (27.10)
5 22.08 (30.36) 24.57 (31.61) 41.35 (28.12) 50.22 (26.51)
6 16.05 (25.63) 19.05 (25.73) 41.03 (26.88) 42.44 (25.54)
7 8.91 (15.95) 10.03 (19.94) 35.30 (24.97) 38.64 (24.73)
8 8.44 (16.34) 12.37 (22.87) 28.59 (23.29) 36.72 (26.32)
9 10.21 (17.93) 13.75 (25.41) 33.83 (24.94) 35.70 (27.65)
10 8.62 (14.69) 6.496 (19.50) 21.38 (19.77) 28.28 (24.50)
11 7.78 (16.25) 7.67 (17.33) 25.37 (22.31) 27.78 (19.18)

Note. Means calculated with missing data excluded.

MLM analyses.

Results of MLM are reported in Table 2. Prior week’s post-imaginal craving and the interaction of craving by time was not associated with probability of any substance use, PDU, or PTSD severity. Similarly, distress and the interaction of distress by time were not associated with substance use, PDU, or PTSD severity. There was a significant effect of time on PTSD such that as time progressed, PTSD severity decreased.

Table 2.

Lagged Ratings of Craving and Distress Predicting Substance Use and PTSD severity

Estimate B SE t p B SE t p
Substance Use Continuous Categorical
Fixed Effects
 Intercept/Threshold 3.40 0.18 18.72 <0.001 −3.20 2.99 −1.07 0.285
 Time −0.10 0.03 −0.40 0.688 −0.36 0.31 −1.16 0.246
 Prior Week Substance Use −0.003 0.002 −1.10 0.270 0.03 0.03 0.93 0.351
 Craving −0.001 0.01 −0.08 0.939 −0.02 0.04 −0.36 0.720
 Distress 0.002 0.004 0.37 0.711 −0.05 0.06 −0.88 0.377
 Time × Craving 0.00 0.002 −0.12 0.908 0.004 0.02 0.26 0.797
 Time × Distress 0.00 0.001 −0.05 0.957 0.02 0.01 1.22 0.224
Random Effects
 Intercept/Threshold 0.69 0.26 - - 44.24 49.35 - -
 Time 0.01 0.01 - - 0.67 1.31 - -
PTSD Severity B SE t p
Fixed Effects
 Intercept 47.89 2.76 17.36 <0.001
 Time −1.04 0.32 −3.25 0.001
 Prior Week’s PCL 0.09 0.08 1.15 0.250
 Craving −0.07 0.06 −1.16 0.246
 Distress 0.03 0.05 0.66 0.506
 Time × Craving 0.02 0.01 1.62 0.105
 Time × Distress −0.002 0.01 −0.19 0.854
Random Effects
 Intercept 234.16 54.36 - -
 Time 1.40 0.52 - -

Note. PCL = PTSD Checklist. Statistically significant at p < 0.05 level.

Survival analysis.

Prior to survival analysis, a life table was constructed to quantify the number of participants who dropped out at each session, with corresponding survival and hazard proportions (Table 3). Results of the life table indicated most participants who attended session 4 completed treatment (63%). Dropout rates did not fluctuate greatly (i.e., hazard probability ranged from .00 to .11 across sessions), although the highest hazard probability of dropping out (11%) occurred between sessions 9 and 10 and all participants who attended session 7 returned for session 8. The Cox proportional hazard model provided better fit than the unrestricted hazard model (LRT = 39.29, df = 14, p < .001; ΔBIC = 63.53). Results of the survival analysis indicate that changes in post-imaginal craving ratings predicted treatment dropout (B = 0.02, p = .004), with an odds ratio of 1.02 (95% confidence interval [CI; 1.01, 1.02]). Participants whose prior-week post-imaginal cravings increased from earlier sessions had greater odds of dropping out during their next session compared to participants whose prior-week craving ratings did not increase. Additionally, changes in post-imaginal distress ratings predicted treatment dropout (B = 0.03, p < .001), with an odds ratio of 1.03 (95% CI [1.02, 1.04]), indicating that participants whose prior-week distress ratings increased from earlier sessions had greater odds of dropping out during their next session compared to participants whose prior-week distress ratings did not increase.

Table 3.

Life Table Displaying Treatment Dropout and Corresponding Survival and Hazard Probabilities

Session In Treatment Dropped Out Survivor
Probability
Hazard
Probability
4 46 - 1.00 0.00
5 43 3 0.93 0.07
6 41 2 0.89 0.05
7 38 3 0.83 0.07
8 36 2 0.78 0.05
9 35 1 0.76 0.03
10 31 4 0.67 0.11
11 29 2 0.63 0.06
12 29 0 0.63 0.00

Note. Participants are considered to have dropped out of treatment if they failed to return for the indicated session. For example, three participants who showed for session 4 did not return for session 5.

Discussion

This pilot study is the first to examine whether in-session ratings of craving and distress following imaginal exposure are associated with subsquent substance use, PTSD symptom exacerbation, or drop out in a comorbid sample. The findings revealed ratings of craving and distress were not associated with the following week’s substance use or PTSD severity. This finding is consistent with research suggesting exposure-based PTSD treatments as well as nonexposure-based PTSD treatments do not increase SUD symptomatology (Kaysen et al., 2014; van Minnen et al., 2015). It is also consistent with findings that most indivdiuals treated for PTSD do not experience symptom exacerbation (Larsen, Wiltsey Stirman, Smith, & Resick, 2016). The results from the current study may be attributed to the fact that craving and distress habituate within and between therapy sessions, and thus SUD and PTSD symptomatology is reduced (Badour et al., 2017; Foa & McLean, 2016). It may also be that post-imaginal exposure ratings of craving and distress are not appropriate indicators of who will experience symptom exacerbations over the course of treatment.

As our group showed in a previous study using data from the same trial, the results from the current study suggest the largest number of dropouts in this sample occurred between sessions 9 and 10 (Szafranski et al., 2017). This differs from research on PE in Veterans that demonstrates most participants drop out of treatment prior to session four (Garcia, Kelley, Rentz, & Lee, 2011; Mott et al., 2014). This discrepancy may be because exposures were implemented later in the COPE treatment following implementation of coping skills training (i.e., at session 4 versus session 3). Thus, particpants presumably had rapport built with the therapist and the coping skills needed to better tolerate the imaginal exposures. Alternatively, participants may have dropped out later because symptoms had improved (Szafranski et al., 2017; Zandberg, Rosenfield, Alpert, McLean, & Foa, 2016).

The current pilot study builds upon this work and found that craving and distress following imaginal exposures are predictive of treatment dropout. That is, individuals who showed between-session increases in craving and distress following imaginal exposures were less likely to complete treatment compared to those who experienced between-session craving/distress improvement, as well as those who reported no change. Upon post-hoc analysis, it was revealed that a minority of participants reported increases in post-imaginal exposure ratings of craving (0.00 – 30.00% of participants between sessions 4–11) and distress (11.80 – 40.00% of participants between sessions 4–11) relative to the week prior. Although findings from the surivival analysis were statistically signficant they were small in magnitude.

Although post-imaginal ratings of craving and distress were not predictive of subsequent substance use or PTSD severity, it is possible that some of the individuals who dropped out of the study did so due to substance use relapse or an increase in PTSD symptom severity after their last completed session. Because we could not collect data from individuals who dropped out of the study, the effect of craving and distress on symptomatology may not have been fully captured in the results. Mechanisms other than SUD and PTSD symptomatology could account for the associations between craving, distress, and treatment retention as well. For instance, individuals who consistently rate higher levels of craving and distress may be more likely to present with characteristics or circumstances that make it difficult to adhere to treatment (e.g., poor distress tolerance, homelessness). Future research should identify specific mechanisms that explain the link between post-imaginal exposure ratings of craving and distress and treatment dropout.

Limitations.

This pilot study included a small sample of primarily male Veterans; participation did not require abstinence from substances. Thus, results may not generalize to other treatment-seeking samples and findings need to be replicated using larger samples. Ratings of post-imaginal craving and distress were only collected in the COPE arm of the study and participants in the Relapse Prevention condition did not provide ratings of in-session craving and distress. It is possible that associations between craving, distress, and dropout are not unique to exposure-based therapies, and would have been observed in participants receiving substance use only treatment. Future research should collect these data in the context of substance use only treatment in order to assess the impact of in-session craving and distress on therapy outcome across treatments. We were unable to obtain substance use and PTSD severity data from partipants who dropped out of the study, thus findings from the MLM should be interpreted with this in mind. Finally, our data collection methods may have also impacted our findings and interpretation of the data. For instance, collecting self-report ratings of substance use and PTSD symptoms daily or multiple times daily through the use of ecological monetary assessment (EMA) may have generated different results.

Conclusions.

Craving and distress did not impact the subsequent week’s substance use or PTSD symptom severity in this sample; however, individuals reporting higher levels of post-imaginal craving and distress were at increased risk for treatment dropout. Research is needed to help identify mechanisms that account for the association between in-session ratings of craving and distress and subsequent dropout. In addition, future research is needed to develop strategies to enhance treatment retention for individuals with comorbid SUD and PTSD.

Acknowledgements

This manuscript is the result of work supported, in part, by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (T32AA747430 and K23AA023845), the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01DA030143 and K02DA039229), and the Office of Research on Women’s Health (K12DA035150). NIAAA, NIDA, and ORWH had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Footnotes

1

MLMs also included pre-imaginal ratings of craving and distress as covariates. These covariates were not significant in either model nor did they impact the interpretation of the findings described below. Thus, they were subsequently removed.

References

  1. Anderson RE, Bonar EE, Walton MA, Goldstick JE, Rauch SAM, Epstein-Ngo QM, & Chermack ST (2017). A Latent Profile Analysis of Aggression and Victimization Across Relationship Types Among Veterans Who Use Substances. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 78(4), 597–607. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2017.78.597 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Back SE, Brady KT, Jaanimägi U, & Jackson JL (2006). Cocaine dependence and PTSD: A pilot study of symptom interplay and treatment preferences. Addictive Behaviors, 31(2), 351–354. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Back SE, Foa EB, T., K., Mills K, Teesson M, Carroll K, . . . Brady KT (2014). Concurrent Treatment of PTSD and Substance Use Disorders using Prolonged Exposure (COPE). Therapist Manual. New York, New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  4. Back SE, Waldrop AE, & Brady KT (2009). Treatment Challenges Associated with Comorbid Substance Use and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Clinicians’ Perspectives. The American journal on addictions / American Academy of Psychiatrists in Alcoholism and Addictions, 18(1), 15–20. doi: 10.1080/10550490802545141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Badour CL, Flanagan JC, Gros DF, Killeen T, Pericot-Valverde I, Korte KJ, . . . Back SE (2017). Habituation of distress and craving during treatment as predictors of change in PTSD symptoms and substance use severity. J Consult Clin Psychol, 85(3), 274–281. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Blake D, Weathers F, Nagy L, Kaloupek D, Charney D, & Keane T (1995). Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-IV (CAPS-DX) National Center for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, Behavioral Science Division, Boston VA Medical Center, Boston, MA. [Google Scholar]
  7. Coffey SF, Schumacher JA, Stasiewicz PR, Henslee AM, Baillie LE, & Landy N (2010). Craving and physiological reactivity to trauma and alcohol cues in posttraumatic stress disorder and alcohol dependence. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 18(4), 340–349. doi: 10.1037/a0019790 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Coffey SF, Stasiewicz PR, Hughes PM, & Brimo ML (2006). Trauma-focused imaginal exposure for individuals with comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder and alcohol dependence: Revealing mechanisms of alcohol craving in a cue reactivity paradigm. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20(4), 425–435. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.20.4.425 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Debell F, Fear NT, Head M, Batt-Rawden S, Greenberg N, Wessely S, & Goodwin L (2014). A systematic review of the comorbidity between PTSD and alcohol misuse. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 49(9), 1401–1425. doi: 10.1007/s00127-014-0855-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Flanagan JC, Korte KJ, Killeen TK, & Back SE (2016). Concurrent Treatment of Substance Use and PTSD. Current Psychiatry Reports, 18(8), 70. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0709-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Foa EB, Hembree EA, & Rothbaum BO (2007). Prolonged exposure therapy for PTSD: Emotional processing of traumatic experiences therapist guide: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  12. Foa EB, & McLean CP (2016). The Efficacy of Exposure Therapy for Anxiety-Related Disorders and Its Underlying Mechanisms: The Case of OCD and PTSD. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12(1), 1–28. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, & McHugh PR (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Garcia HA, Kelley LP, Rentz TO, & Lee S (2011). Pretreatment predictors of dropout from cognitive behavioral therapy for PTSD in Iraq and Afghanistan war veterans. Psychological Services, 8(1), 1. [Google Scholar]
  15. Gielen N, Krumeich A, Havermans RC, Smeets F, & Jansen A (2014). Why clinicians do not implement integrated treatment for comorbid substance use disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: A qualitative study. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 5. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v5.22821 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Grant BF, Goldstein RB, Saha TD, Chou SP, Jung J, Zhang H, . . . Hasin DS (2015). Epidemiology of DSM-5 Alcohol Use Disorder: Results From the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions III. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(8), 757–766. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0584 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hien DA, Jiang H, Campbell ANC, Hu M-C, Miele GM, Cohen LR, . . . Nunes EV (2010). Do treatment improvements in PTSD severity affect substance use outcomes? A secondary analysis from a randomized clinical trial in NIDA’s Clinical Trials Network. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(1), 95–101. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09091261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kadden R, Carroll K, Donovan D, Cooney N, Monti P, Abrams D, . . . Hester R (1992). Cognitive-behavioral coping skills therapy manual. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. [Google Scholar]
  19. Kaysen D, Schumm J, Pedersen ER, Seim RW, Bedard-Gilligan M, & Chard K (2014). Cognitive processing therapy for veterans with comorbid PTSD and alcohol use disorders. Addict Behav, 39(2), 420–427. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.08.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Larsen SE, Wiltsey Stirman S, Smith BN, & Resick PA (2016). Symptom exacerbations in trauma-focused treatments: Associations with treatment outcome and non-completion. Behav Res Ther, 77, 68–77. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Leeies M, Pagura J, Sareen J, & Bolton JM (2010). The use of alcohol and drugs to self‐medicate symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 27(8), 731–736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. McGovern MP, Lambert-Harris C, Alterman AI, Xie H, & Meier A (2011). A Randomized Controlled Trial Comparing Integrated Cognitive Behavioral Therapy Versus Individual Addiction Counseling for Co-occurring Substance Use and Posttraumatic Stress Disorders. J Dual Diagn, 7(4), 207–227. doi: 10.1080/15504263.2011.620425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Mills KL, Ewer P, Dore G, Teesson M, Baker A, Kay-Lambkin F, & Sannibale C (2014). The feasibility and acceptability of a brief intervention for clients of substance use services experiencing symptoms of post traumatic stress disorder. Addictive Behaviors, 39(6), 1094–1099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Mott JM, Mondragon S, Hundt NE, Beason‐Smith M, Grady RH, & Teng EJ (2014). Characteristics of US veterans who begin and complete prolonged exposure and cognitive processing therapy for PTSD. J Trauma Stress, 27(3), 265–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (1998–2017). Mplus User’s Guide (8th Edition ed.). Lost Angeles, CA. [Google Scholar]
  26. Najavits LM (2003). Seeking safety: A new psychotherapy for posttraumatic stress disorder and substance use disorder In Ouimette P, Brown PJ, Ouimette P, & Brown PJ (Eds.), Trauma and substance abuse: Causes, consequences, and treatment of comorbid disorders. (pp. 147–169). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  27. Norman SB, & Hamblen JL (2017). Promising Directions for Treating Comorbid PTSD and Substance Use Disorder. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 41(4), 708–710. doi: 10.1111/acer.13349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ouimette P, Goodwin E, & Brown PJ (2006). Health and well being of substance use disorder patients with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Addict Behav, 31(8), 1415–1423. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.11.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Petrakis IL, Rosenheck R, & Desai R (2011). Substance use comorbidity among veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder and other psychiatric illness. Am J Addict, 20(3), 185–189. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2011.00126.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Schumm JA, & Gore WL (2016). Simultaneous treatment of co-occurring posttraumatic stress disorder and substance use disorder. Current Treatment Options in Psychiatry, 3(1), 28–36. [Google Scholar]
  31. Sheehan D, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan H, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, . . . Dunbar GC (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IVand ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry, 59(Suppl 20), 2233. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Singer JD, & Willett JB (1993). It’s About Time: Using Discrete-Time Survival Analysis to Study Duration and the Timing of Events. Journal of Educational Statistics, 18(2), 155–195. doi: 10.3102/10769986018002155 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Sinha R, Fox HC, Hong KA, Bergquist K, Bhagwagar Z, & Siedlarz KM (2008). Enhanced Negative Emotion and Alcohol Craving, and Altered Physiological Responses Following Stress and Cue Exposure in Alcohol Dependent Individuals. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34, 1198. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.78 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sobell LC, & Sobell MB (1992). Timeline follow-back Measuring alcohol consumption (pp. 41–72): Springer. [Google Scholar]
  35. Szafranski DD, Snead A, Allan NP, Gros DF, Killeen T, Flanagan J, . . . Back SE (2017). Integrated, exposure-based treatment for PTSD and comorbid substance use disorders: Predictors of treatment dropout. Addictive Behaviors, 73, 30–35. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.04.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. van Minnen A, Zoellner LA, Harned MS, & Mills K (2015). Changes in Comorbid Conditions After Prolonged Exposure for PTSD: a Literature Review. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17(3), 17. doi: 10.1007/s11920-015-0549-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Weathers F, Litz B, Herman D, Huska J, & Keane t. (1994). PTSD Checklist - Military version for DSM-IV. Boston, MA: National Center for PTSD, Behavioral Sciences Division. [Google Scholar]
  38. Young HE, Rosen CS, & Finney JW (2005). A survey of PTSD screening and referral practices in VA addiction treatment programs. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 28(4), 313–319. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Zandberg LJ, Rosenfield D, Alpert E, McLean CP, & Foa EB (2016). Predictors of dropout in concurrent treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder and alcohol dependence: Rate of improvement matters. Behav Res Ther, 80, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES