Abstract
Creative problem solving involves search processes, and it is known to be hard to motivate. Reward cues have been found to enhance performance across a range of tasks, even when cues are presented subliminally, without being consciously detected. It is uncertain whether motivational processes, such as reward, can influence problem solving. We tested the effect of supraliminal and subliminal reward on participant performance on problem solving that can be solved by deliberate analysis or by insight. Forty-one participants attempted to solve 100 compound remote associate problems. At the beginning of each problem, a potential reward cue (1 or 25 cents) was displayed, either subliminally (17 ms) or supraliminally (100 ms). Participants earned the displayed reward if they solved the problem correctly. Results showed that the higher subliminal reward increased the percentage of problems solved correctly overall. Second, we explored if subliminal rewards preferentially influenced solutions that were achieved via a sudden insight (mostly processed below awareness) or via a deliberate analysis. Participants solved more problems via insight following high subliminal reward when compared with low subliminal reward, and compared with high supraliminal reward, with no corresponding effect on analytic solving. Striatal dopamine (DA) is thought to influence motivation, reinforce behavior, and facilitate cognition. We speculate that subliminal rewards activate the striatal DA system, enhancing the kinds of automatic integrative processes that lead to more creative strategies for problem solving, without increasing the selectivity of attention, which could impede insight.
Keywords: Subliminal, Monetary rewards, Insight, Problem solving
Creative problem solving is essential for advances in human knowledge, but it is also one of the most challenging cognitive processes to motivate. Reward provides a powerful leverage to motivate human behavior: it facilitates learning, modulates attention and other cognitive-control processes, and reinforces cognitive flexibility (Amabile, Hennessey, & Grossman, 1986; D’Ardenne, McClure, Nystrom, & Cohen, 2008; Krebs, Boehler, Roberts, Song, & Woldorff, 2012; Wittmann et al., 2005). Attempts to facilitate creative problem solving via supraliminal (i.e., conscious perception of) external rewards have so far been unsuccessful and controversial. In fact, some studies have demonstrated that supraliminal external rewards are detrimental, and others have indicated that these explicit rewards increase creativity (e.g., Amabile et al., 1986; Csikszentmihalyi, 1984). However, a few studies have demonstrated that subliminal (i.e., below the level of awareness) information influences problem solving. For example, Maier (1931) observed that a hint surreptitiously given to people can help them solve the two-string (pendulum) problem (Maier, 1931). Schunn and Dunbar (1996) also showed that experience with one problem facilitates performance on another problem without subjects being aware that one problem has helped them solve the other problem (Schunn & Dunbar, 1996).
Generally, subliminal rewards enhance cognitive processes (Capa, Bouquet, Dreher, & Dufour, 2013; Pessiglione et al., 2008; Pessiglione et al., 2007). These studies demonstrated similar effects when a reward is presented subliminally or supraliminally (Kouider & Dehaene, 2007; Morris, Ohman, & Dolan, 1998; Pessiglione et al., 2007). For example, Pessiglione and collaborators demonstrated that subliminal monetary rewards increased participants’ motivation in a similar fashion to supraliminal rewards in a task where participants had to squeeze a power grip (Pessiglione et al., 2007). Subliminal rewards have been linked to striatal learning mechanisms that are not consciously accessible but nonetheless influence decision-making and have a direct impact on cognitive performance (Pessiglione et al., 2008). In addition, monetary reward presented supraliminally or subliminally can improve cognitive processes such as working memory (Bijleveld, Custers, & Aarts, 2010; Bustin, Quoidbach, Hansenne, & Capa, 2012; Capa et al., 2013). The idea is that awareness of a reward enables people to explicitly modulate the strategies they employ to obtain that reward. Speed-accuracy trade-offs tend to be affected only when individuals are aware of the reward (Bijleveld et al., 2010). These premises led to the idea that the size and/or awareness of reward cues could modulate problem solving. Specifically, we hypothesized that subliminal reward cues (indicating a potential reward for solving the problem that follows).
Some creative problem solving is deliberate and analytic, but other times people solves problems with a sudden insight, also known as a “Eureka!” or “Aha!” moment. Insight solutions are often perceived as an epiphany, suddenly emerging from the recesses of our mind. People are unable to predict when the idea will arrive, or explain the reasoning that led to the emergence of it. Sudden insights are generally considered to reflect an instance of creative thinking, although it is also possible for people to achieve a solution deemed creative through analytic processing, such as using a conscious strategy, methodical step-by-step progress, or through trial and error (Bowden & Jung-Beeman, 2003a; Jung-Beeman et al., 2004). Solving problems via insight relies on a unique pattern of neural activity when compared with problems solved by deliberate analysis (Jung-Beeman et al., 2004; Kounios & Beeman, 2014), leads to more accurate solutions, and it is impaired under stress (e.g., when running out of time; Salvi, Bricolo, Kounios, Bowden, & Beeman, 2016). The solutions via insight and via analysis also differ in their patterns of attention deployment. Our previous studies demonstrated that, as compared with analysis, insight problem solving is associated with turning attention inward and away from external inputs (e.g., Jung-Beeman et al., 2004; Salvi, Bricolo, Franconeri, Kounios, & Beeman, 2015), and with making attention less focused or selective (Rowe, Hirsh, & Anderson, 2007; Wegbreit, Suzuki, Grabowecky, Kounios, & Beeman, 2012). Internal states of attention are particularly conducive to insight problem solving, whereas external states of attention are more conducive to analytic problem solving (Kounios et al., 2006; Salvi et al., 2015).
Although much progress has been made toward identifying the cognitive and neural subcomponents of problem solving, few studies have succeeded in its enhancement (Rowe et al., 2007; Wegbreit et al., 2012). For example, in a recent study, Hattori and collaborators demonstrated that subliminal hints facilitate the solution of “classic insight” problems (Hattori, Sloman, & Orita, 2013). The authors suggested that subliminal cues could specifically affect solutions solved by insight, and they underlined the importance of studying these processes using subliminal priming techniques rather than verbal self-reports that involve conscious processes.
Therefore, in a second step, we hypothesized that subliminal reward might enhance insight problem solving more than analytical problem solving, and we examined the intersecting roles of awareness and reward cues in motivating or improving it.
In sum, we explore whether the size and/or awareness of reward cues could modulate creative problem solving. We hypothesized that subliminal reward cues (indicating a potential reward for solving the problem that follows) would facilitate creative problem solving, and specifically those solutions achieved via insight. We did not expect the same effect for supraliminal rewards, since supraliminal reward could interfere with problem solving by shifting and focusing participants’ attention to external information and strong associations, to the detriment of insight. Alternatively, supraliminal reward cues could induce greater anxiety/stress that could interfere with insight problem solving via increased focus and selectivity.
Method
Participants
Forty-one Northwestern University students (32 women, average age 20.2 ±1.4 years) were recruited for this study. This sample size has the power to detect small to medium size effects.
The study was approved by the Northwestern University Institutional Review Board. All participants gave written informed consent and received the money they earned in the experiment, based on the sum of rewards associated with correctly solved problems.
Stimuli
Participants attempted solving 100 compound remote associate (CRA) problems (Bowden & Jung-Beeman, 2003a). These hybrid-type problems can be solved through insight or through analytic processes, with participants on each successful trial required to report which of the two most contributed to problem solving. Self-reports differentiating between insight and analytic problem solving are reliable, and behavioral and neuroimaging markers have consistently demonstrated that the reports reflect distinct cognitive processes and neural substrates (Bowden & Jung-Beeman, 2003a; Jung-Beeman et al., 2004; Kounios et al., 2008).
Each problem consisted of the simultaneous presention of three words, each of which could form a compound word or phrase with the solution word (e.g., for way/board/sleep the solution word is WALK. For the full list of the problems and solution rates, see Bowden & Jung-Beeman, 2003a). The three CRA words were presented in black font on a white background in normal horizontal orientation above, at, and below the center of the monitor (see Fig. 1). The experimental procedure was presented using E-Prime 2.10 (Schneider, Eschman, & Zuccolotto, 2002) on a 22-in. LCD screen at a viewing distance of 60 cm. We utilized a 2 (reward presentation duration: 100 ms vs, 17 ms) × 2 (reward cue: 1 cent vs. 25 cents) × 2 (insight vs. analysis) within-participants trial design.
CRA trials were randomly distributed in the task, removing any unexpected association between CRA difficulty and subliminal/supraliminal presentation. In addition, CRA difficulty was balanced across conditions according to normative data previously published (Bowden & Jung-Beeman, 2003b).
Experimental procedure
Participants first read the task instructions and also received verbal instructions before starting the experimental procedure. We explained the difference between insight versus analytic problem solving, and we made sure that subjects understood the difference between the two different problem-solving strategies before beginning the experiment and again afterward when completing the debriefing questionnaire. Participants were additionally familiarized with the masks and stimuli in a practice task (trials N = 10).
CRA task
Each trial started with a 100-ms mask followed by the presentation of the coin (at 17 or at 100 ms) followed by a second 100 ms mask. Participants were told that the coin would sometimes be difficult to perceive. In accordance with Pessiglione et al. (2007), half ofthe trials displayed the coin supraliminally (i.e., 100 ms) and the other half presented the coin subliminally (i.e., 17 ms). Afterwards, the three CRAs problem words were presented simultaneously on the screen. Participants had 15 seconds to solve the problem, and they were instructed to press the space bar as soon as they attained the solution. Following their verbally reported solution, or the end of the time limit (15 s), the problem words were erased and, if solved, participants had to report how they solved the problem: via insight or via analysis. Participants were instructed that there was no optimal problem-solving style or right or wrong answers in reporting the way they used to solve each problem. Further explanation of the task was given if necessary. If the solution provided was correct, a screen appeared, saying, “Congratulations—you won 1 cent (or 25 cents)!”; coins were physically given to participants at the end of each trial. No feedback was given if the participant ran out of time or provided the wrong solution. At the end of the experiment, participants were paid the amount of money gained during the task, plus a fixed amount of $10. Participants earned an average of $ 16.50 ± 1.61.
Following the set of 100 CRA problems, participants performed a forced-choice task of 70 trials of reward cues detection, in which they had to report which coin they saw on the the screen. The coins were the same as those presented before each CRA (1 or 25 cents) at two different durations (17 and 100 ms). Each trial consisted of masks and reward cues stimuli, followed by the two choices presented simultaneously (1 or 25 cents). Participants pressed the 1 key if they saw “1 cent,” 2 if they saw “25 cents.” The participants were told that accuracy, not speed, was important. The choices remained on the screen until a response was made (see Fig. 1). The experiment was precceded by 10 practice trials. In total, the experiment took approximately 1 hour. After the CRA task, participants performed a forced-choice task, to ensure that the participants had not perceived the displayed coin in the subliminal condition.
Statistical analysis
Behavioral data analysis was performed using SPSS 22.0 (www.spss.com) and the significance level was set to p < .05. Data were tested for Gaussian distribution (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test) and homogeneity of variance (Levene’s test). Data were normally distributed, and assumptions for analysis of variance were not violated. Only problems correctly solved were used for the analyses.
Results
Forced-choice task
We first examined whether participants might have perceived the displayed coin in the subliminal condition. We debriefed participants before the forced-choice task, and not one reported being able to detect whether 1 cent or 25 cent coins were presented subliminally. We next examined whether participants might have perceived the displayed coin in the subliminal and supraliminal conditions.
A total of six participants performed more that 2.5 standard deviations (corresponding to 80.13%) above average in the subliminal reward condition or performed 2.5 standard deviations (corresponding to 69.11%) below average in the supraliminal reward condition. They were excluded from the analysis. Then we also calculated a d’ measure (i.e., the difference between normalized rates of hits; correct “different” response) and false alarms (incorrect responses), and this measure was not significantly different from zero at the group level, t(1, 34) = −0.778, p > .05. Therefore, we analyzed the means of correct responses, defined as the participant having seen or guessed the coins. In the subliminal condition, the mean percentage of correct responses was 55.81 (SD = 19.78) for 1 cent, which did not differ significantly from chance level t(34) = 1.73, p > .05, and 57.68 (SD = 24.27) for 25 cents, which did not differ significantly from chance level t(34) = 1.87, p > .05.
CRA task
Participants responded correctly to 35.11% of problems (SD = 9%); the rest of the problems were discarded from the analysis. Ignoring cue awareness, problems with low reward cues were solved correctly at 38.14% (SD = 10.82%); problems with high reward cues were solved correctly at 43.69% (SD = 11.78%). Problems associated with subliminal reward cues were solved correctly at 44.49% (SD = 12.38%); whereas problems associated with supraliminal reward cues were solved at 38.34% (SD = 11.78%).
In a logistic regression, awareness, b = 0.274, p < .01, Exp(b) = 1.315, and reward, b = 2.41, p < .01, Exp(b) = 0.786, were found to be significant predictors of problem solving accuracy. Subliminal rewards were 1.315 times more likely to produce correct solutions, compared with supraliminal reward, and lower rewards were 0.786 times less likely to produce correct solutions, compared with higher rewards. The logistic regression model was statistically significant, χ2(2) = 28.126, p < .0001.
We also computed a 2 × 2 ANOVA with reward cues (1 cent vs. 25 cents) and awareness (subliminal vs. supraliminal) as factors on the percentage of problems solved correctly. This analysis revealed that participants solved significantly more problems following high-reward than low-reward cues, F(1, 34) = 12.17, p < .001, ηp2 = 0.264. They also solved more problems following subliminal than supraliminal reward cues; main effect F(1, 34) = 9.09, p < .01, ηp2 = 0.211, with a significant interaction between awareness and size of reward cues, F(1, 34) = 4.2, p < .05, ηp2 = 0.112 (see Fig. 2).
Effect of reward and solution type on problem solving
Of the correctly solved problems, participants solved 50.89% (SD = 23.85%) of them via insight, and 49.11% (SD = 23.85%) via analysis.
In a logistic regression, awareness, b = 0.159, p < .05, Exp(b) = 1.172, and reward, b = .194, p < .05, Exp(b) = 0.823, were found to be significant predictors of problem-solving style. Subliminal rewards were 1.172 times more likely to produce an insight solution compared with supraliminal reward, and lower rewards were 0.823 times less likely to produce insight solutions compared with higher rewards. The logistic regression model was statistically significant, χ2(2)= 10.692, p < .005.
Effect of reaction times (RTs) on problem solving
Finally, we also assessed the effect of solution style by analyzing reaction times (RTs) and percentage of correct responses by 2 × 2 (2 durations of reward presentation x 2 reward values) and 2×2×2 ANOVA (2 durations of reward presentation × 2 reward values × 2 solution types). The only significant main effect was of awareness on overall problem-solving RT, F(1, 34) = 11.06, p > .05, ηp2 = 0.246. No other effect was found (all ps > .05).
Discussion
We investigated whether subliminal versus supraliminal reward cues facilitate problem solving. Our results demonstrated that people solved more problems when motivated by monetary reward presented subliminally. The results also indicate that the higher percentage of problems solved in the high reward cue condition was in part driven by insight-based solutions. In contrast to previous reports on the detrimental effects of monetary supraliminal reward on insight problem solving and functional fixedness (for a review, see McGraw, 1978), here we find that insight problem solving benefits from higher reward cues, but primarily due to the subliminal reward condition. There remains a debate regarding the conditions under which subliminal stimuli gain access consciousness of the stimuli. In our study, we referred to subliminal stimuli as stimuli not accessible to awareness. Nevertheless, the subliminal state can be influenced along a continuum depending on masking properties, top-down attention, and task instructions (Dehaene, Changeux, Naccache, Sackur, & Sergent, 2006).
Previous studies showed how positive mood both facilitates insight problem solving and enhances dopamine (DA) activity in the anterior cingulate cortex (Subramaniam, Kounios, Parrish, & Jung-Beeman, 2009). We speculate that a potential subliminal reward, by inducing positive affect, benefits insight problem solving and stimulates the DA/ reward system—results that are not likely to be obtained if the reward is presented supraliminally. In the latter case, conscious reward could increase “approach motivation,” possibly increase anxiety, and in either case increase focus/selectivity—which impairs problem solving (Kounios et al., 2006; Salvi et al., 2015; Salvi et al., 2016).
Accumulating evidence shows that creative acts do not reflect the operation of just one process, brain area, or faculty but rather require the interplay of multiple cognitive processes and neural networks (Eysenck, 1993; Heilman, 2005). One modulating influence might be the neurotransmitter DA that affects various neural networks (Chermahini & Hommel, 2010). Scores on creative tests are negatively related with dopamine (D2) receptor densities in the thalamus (de Manzano, Cervenka, Karabanov, Farde, & Ullen, 2010). Genetics studies showed that individuals with the DA D2 receptor gene (locus: DRD2 TAQ IA) had significantly better performance on creativity tasks (Reuter, Roth, Holve, & Hennig, 2006). Higher DA levels are associated with less inhibition of alternative thoughts and greater cognitive flexibility, which is important for generating new ideas (de Manzano et al., 2010; Flaherty, 2005; Takeuchi et al., 2010). Well-established biomarkers of DA activation in the striatum can be detected by eye movements. In particular, spontaneous eye-blink rate has been consistently linked to DA release (Colzato, van den Wildenberg, van Wouwe, Pannebakker, & Hommel, 2009; Colzato, van Wouwe, & Hommel, 2007; Kleven & Koek, 1996) and is often used as an index of striatal DA production (Karson, 1983; Taylor et al., 1999). Recently, several studies demonstrated that spontaneous blinks are involved during internally directed cognition and attentional disengagement from external inputs (Benedek, Stoiser, Walcher, & Korner, 2017; Nakano, 2015; Walcher, Korner, & Benedek, 2017). In the same study, Nakano (2015) found that spontaneous blink-related brain activation in areas that are part of the reward circuit (i.e., the ventral striatum) and brain regions activated by problem-solving tasks, such as the anterior cingulate cortex and the right superior temporal gyrus. An increasing number of studies indicate that higher eye-blink rates and eye-blink duration are associated with idea generation, problem solving (from anagrams to creative sentence generation), and better performance in divergent thinking tasks (Benedek et al., 2017; Chermahini & Hommel, 2010; Ueda, Tominaga, Kajimura, & Nomura, 2015; Walcher et al., 2017). In one of our recent studies, we recorded eye-blink rate when people were solving CRA and found that people have a higher eye-blink rate when solving problems via insight compared with analysis (Salvi et al., 2015).
Given the likely involvement of the DA system in problem solving, it would not be surprising that reinforcement-related DA release could be involved in creativity as well as insight problem solving. DA activity is increased when participants receive monetary rewards (Pas, Custers, Bijleveld, & Vink, 2014), and reward cues also increase the investment of effort in tasks, even when cues are presented subliminally, which makes them harder to be consciously detected (Pessiglione et al., 2007). Subliminal reward cues are assumed to rely on the mesolimbic and striatal DA system (Pas et al., 2014). Two experiments have investigated whether effortful responses vary with individual differences in markers of striatal DA functioning (Pas et al., 2014). The first experiment investigated whether there was an association between physical effort and resting-state eye-blink rate; the second one examined cognitive effort in relation to individually averaged error-related negativity. In both experiments, effort correlated with the markers only for subliminal, but not for supraliminal, monetary reward cues. These findings support the idea that responses to subliminal reward cues are linked to the striatal DA system, while responses to supraliminal reward cues depend on higher level cortical functions. Bijleveld and colleagues proposed that subliminal stimulus presentations occur outside of conscious awareness and rely on the mesolimbic and striatal DA system (Bijleveld, Custers, & Aarts, 2012). Supraliminal rewards, because they are presented long enough to be processed, become available to and rely upon higher order cortical functions that are linked to conscious deliberation, enabling strategic reward-related decision-making processes while reflecting on the reward that is at stake.
We acknowledge that to better understand at a cognitive/computational level, the phenomenon we observed, further experimentation is necessary, including conducting studies with a larger sample size and adding neurophysiological measures or neuroimaging. This might help clarify the cognitive and neural mechanisms that mediate subliminal effects upon problem solving.
In summary, our results indicated that subliminal reward cues might facilitate problem solving. Insight-based solutions may be particularly susceptible to subliminal cues, and we suspect that this effect may act via subcortical mechanisms that are influenced by the DA system. If substantiated, it would argue that subliminal rewards acting on insight processes could facilitate the processing of distantly or weakly related stimuli. One important implication of this idea is that subliminal reward cues could be used to facilitate desired behaviors without distracting people from ongoing activities—which could, for example, benefit patients participating in therapeutic interventions designed to improve performance in various task domains.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by NIH under Grant No. T32 NS047987 to C.S., by the United States Air Force Research Laboratory FA8650–15-2–5518, and by the Therapeutic Cognitive Neuroscience Fund to J.G.
References
- Amabile TM, Hennessey BA, & Grossman BS (1986). Social influences on creativity: The effects of contracted-for reward. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(1), 14–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benedek M, Stoiser R, Walcher S, & Korner C (2017). Eye behavior associated with internally versus externally directed cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bijleveld E, Custers R, & Aarts H (2010). Unconscious reward cues increase invested effort, but do not change speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Cognition, 115(2), 330–335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bijleveld E, Custers R, & Aarts H (2012). Adaptive reward pursuit: How effort requirements affect unconscious reward responses and conscious reward decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 728–742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowden EM, & Jung-Beeman M (2003a). Aha! Insight experience correlates with solution activation in the right hemisphere. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 730–737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowden EM, & Jung-Beeman M (2003b). Normative data for 144 compound remote associate problems. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(4), 634–639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bustin GM, Quoidbach J, Hansenne M, & Capa RL (2012). Personality modulation of (un)conscious processing: Novelty seeking and performance following supraliminal and subliminal reward cues. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(2), 947–952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capa RL, Bouquet CA, Dreher JC, & Dufour A (2013). Long-lasting effects of performance-contingent unconscious and conscious reward incentives during cued task-switching. Cortex, 49(7), 1943–1954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chermahini SA, & Hommel B (2010). The (b)link between creativity and dopamine: Spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking. Cognition, 115(3), 458–465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colzato LS, van den Wildenberg WP, van Wouwe NC, Pannebakker MM, & Hommel B (2009). Dopamine and inhibitory action control: Evidence from spontaneous eye blink rates. Experimental Brain Research, 196(3), 467–474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colzato LS, van Wouwe NC, & Hommel B (2007). Spontaneous eyeblink rate predicts the strength of visuomotor binding. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2387–2392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csikszentmihalyi M (1984). Creativity: The social psychology of creativity. Science, 225(4665), 918–919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Ardenne K, McClure SM, Nystrom LE, & Cohen JD (2008). BOLD responses reflecting dopaminergic signals in the human ventral tegmental area. Science, 319(5867), 1264–1267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Manzano O, Cervenka S, Karabanov A, Farde L, & Ullen F (2010). Thinking outside a less intact box: Thalamic dopamine D2 receptor densities are negatively related to psychometric creativity in healthy individuals. PLoS One, 5(5), e10670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Changeux JP, Naccache L, Sackur J, & Sergent C (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck HJ (1993). Creativity and personality: Suggestions for a theory. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 147–178. [Google Scholar]
- Flaherty AW (2005). Frontotemporal and dopaminergic control of idea generation and creative drive. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493(1), 147–153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hattori M, Sloman SA, & Orita R (2013). Effects of subliminal hints on insight problem solving. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(4), 790–797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heilman KM (2005). Creativity and the brain. New York, NY: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jung-Beeman M, Bowden EM, Haberman J, Frymiare JL, Arambel-Liu S, Greenblatt R, … Kounios J (2004). Neural activity when people solve verbal problems with insight. PLOS Biology, 2(4), E97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karson CN (1983). Spontaneous eye-blink rates and dopaminergic systems. Brain, 106(Pt. 3), 643–653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleven MS, & Koek W (1996). Differential effects of direct and indirect dopamine agonists on eye blink rate in cynomolgus mon-keys. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 279(3), 1211–1219. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kouider S, & Dehaene S (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: A critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 857–875. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kounios J, & Beeman M (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of insight. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 71–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kounios J, Fleck JI, Green DL, Payne L, Stevenson JL, Bowden EM, & Jung-Beeman M (2008). The origins of insight in resting-state brain activity. Neuropsychologia, 46(1), 281–291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kounios J, Frymiare JL, Bowden EM, Fleck JI, Subramaniam K, Parrish TB, & Jung-Beeman M (2006). The prepared mind: Neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight. Psychological Science, 17(10), 882–890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krebs RM, Boehler CN, Roberts KC, Song AW, & Woldorff MG (2012). The involvement of the dopaminergic midbrain and cortico-striatal-thalamic circuits in the integration of reward prospect and attentional task demands. Cerebral Cortex, 22(3), 607–615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier NRF (1931). Reasoning in humans: II. The solution of a problem and its appearance in consciousness. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 12(2), 181–194. [Google Scholar]
- McGraw KO (1978). The detrimental effects of reward on performance: A literature review and a prediction model. Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Morris JS, Ohman A, & Dolan RJ (1998). Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393(6684), 467–470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakano T (2015). Blink-related dynamic switching between internal and external orienting networks while viewing videos. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 96, 54–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pas P, Custers R, Bijleveld E, & Vink M (2014). Effort responses to suboptimal reward cues are related to striatal dopaminergic functioning. Motivation and Emotion, 38(6), 759–770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pessiglione M, Petrovic P, Daunizeau J, Palminteri S, Dolan RJ, & Frith CD (2008). Subliminal instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain. Neuron, 59(4), 561–567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pessiglione M, Schmidt L, Draganski B, Kalisch R, Lau H, Dolan RJ, & Frith CD (2007). How the brain translates money into force: A neuroimaging study of subliminal motivation. Science, 316(5826), 904–906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reuter M, Roth S, Holve K, & Hennig J (2006). Identification of first candidate genes for creativity: A pilot study. Brain Research, 1069(1), 190–197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowe G, Hirsh JB, & Anderson AK (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(1), 383–388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salvi C, Bricolo E, Franconeri SL, Kounios J, & Beeman M (2015). Sudden insight is associated with shutting out visual inputs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–6. 10.3758/s13423-015-0845-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salvi C, Bricolo E, Kounios J, Bowden E, & Beeman M (2016). Aha is right: Insight solutions are more often correct than analytic solutions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 69(6), 1064–1072.26810954 [Google Scholar]
- Schneider W, Eschman A, & Zuccolotto A (2002). E-Prime: User’s guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Incorporated. [Google Scholar]
- Schunn CD, & Dunbar K (1996). Priming, analogy, and awareness in complex reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 24(3), 271–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Subramaniam K, Kounios J, Parrish TB, & Jung-Beeman M (2009). A brain mechanism for facilitation of insight by positive affect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 415–432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Sassa Y, Hashizume H, Sekiguchi A, Fukushima A, & Kawashima R (2010). Regional gray matter volume of dopaminergic system associated with creativity: Evidence from voxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage, 51(2), 578–585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor JR, Elsworth JD, Lawrence MS, Sladek JR Jr., Roth RH, & Redmond DE Jr. (1999). Spontaneous blink rates correlate with dopamine levels in the caudate nucleus of MPTP-treated monkeys. Experimental Neurology, 158(1), 214–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ueda Y, Tominaga A, Kajimura S, & Nomura M (2015). Spontaneous eye blinks during creative task correlate with divergent processing. Psychological Research, 80(4), 652–9. 10.1007/s00426-015-0665-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walcher S, Korner C, & Benedek M (2017). Looking for ideas: Eye behavior during goal-directed internally focused cognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 53, 165–175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wegbreit E, Suzuki S, Grabowecky M, Kounios J, & Beeman M (2012). Visual attention modulates insight versus analytic solving of verbal problems. The Journal of Problem Solving, 4(2), 94–115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wittmann BC, Schott BH, Guderian S, Frey JU, Heinze HJ, & Duzel E (2005). Reward-related FMRI activation of dopaminergic midbrain is associated with enhanced hippocampus-dependent long-term memory formation. Neuron, 45(3), 459–467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]