Abstract
Background
Heterogeneity in the effects of trait neuroticism on mortality has inspired recent theories of “healthy neuroticism,” or the possibility that neuroticism can lead people down either healthy or unhealthy behavioral pathways. The logical extension of this theory is that some construct—perhaps another trait, financial resource, or health-relevant situation—changes the relationship between neuroticism and health. The other possibility is that different components of neuroticism lead to different health behaviors and therefore different outcomes.
Purpose
The current study systematically examines the relationship between child and adult neuroticism and various health indicators including perceptions of health, behaviors, health outcomes, and biomarkers of health. Finally, we examine both potential moderators of the associations with neuroticism and examine its facet structure.
Methods
The current study utilizes data from the Hawaii Longitudinal Study of Personality and Health, which includes both adult (IPIP-NEO) and childhood (teacher-reported) measures of personality and socioeconomic status, as well as a variety of health outcomes, from self-reported health and health behavior to biological markers, such as cholesterol and blood glucose levels. Sample sizes range from 299 to 518.
Results
The relationship between neuroticism and health was not consistently moderated by any other variable, nor were facets of neuroticism differentially related to health.
Conclusions
Despite a systematic investigation of the potential “paths” which may differentiate the relationship of neuroticism to health, no evidence of healthy neuroticism was found.
Keywords: personality, health, neuroticism, NxC, moderation
The health effects of trait neuroticism, defined by being dispositionally anxious and upset, appear to be similar across different individual differences, like personality and health.
Introduction
One catalyst for the increasing recognition that personality traits influence health outcomes has been the greater awareness and adoption of common trait taxonomies, allowing researchers the ability to compare findings across samples. For instance, research founded within the Big Five taxonomy has allowed researchers to demonstrate consistent linkages between traits like conscientiousness and neuroticism and health outcomes, typically showcasing the benefits associated with the former and detriments for those individuals higher on the latter [1–4]. However, this summary of the literature ignores a substantial number of studies which find null [5–7] or protective effects of neuroticism on health [8–12]. Although commonly cast as a universally maladaptive trait for health, researchers have also suggested the possibility that neuroticism may prove beneficial in some circumstances, leading to the concept of “healthy neuroticism” [13]. The current study sought to comprehensively examine the evidence for healthy neuroticism using data from the long-running Hawaii Longitudinal Study of Personality and Health [14]. Specifically, we sought to examine four potential influences on whether neuroticism may prove adaptive for an individual’s health and health behavior: (i) the individual’s level of conscientiousness, (ii) whether the individual reports having a chronic disease, (iii) the individual’s socioeconomic status (SES), and (iv) if the associations with health differ based on the facet of neuroticism. Although the personality literature has often referred to the notion of “healthy” neuroticism [13], and thus we focus on that terminology here, it is worth noting that each of these tests will help indicate whether neuroticism is at least less maladaptive in some situations; in so doing, our intent was to conduct a comprehensive investigation into when and in which contexts the role of neuroticism on health may differ. Each of these possibilities reflect previous theoretical and empirical literature on the role of neuroticism on health outcomes, and each can potentially lead to healthy neuroticism effects.
Conscientious Neuroticism
A common tendency in research on personality and health is to focus on traits as independent factors influencing health. Such a perspective fails to properly consider that any one individual’s personality reflects a constellation of Big Five (and other) traits, and that association with any single trait may vary depending on a person’s standing on another trait. With respect to neuroticism, one common theory has focused on the interaction between neuroticism and conscientiousness, insofar that it may benefit individuals to be relatively high on both dimensions [15, 16]. The rationale behind conscientious neuroticism reflects the notion that conscientious individuals are prone toward healthy behavior [17], and this effect may be only further amplified when those individuals also are somewhat anxious or vigilant by disposition, which in turn will keep individuals focused on promoting health.
Evidence for this claim has been found for specific health outcomes and health behaviors. For instance, researchers using the Midlife in the USA data have found a conscientiousness-by-neuroticism interaction when predicting the inflammation biomarker Interleukin-6 [15]. In addition, this interaction held when predicting smoking behavior following the onset of disease [18]. However, even these studies have shown evidence against conscientious neuroticism; for instance, this interaction did not hold when predicting smoking for the general population without disease [18], suggesting the need to consider additional moderators.
The Moderating Role of Chronic Disease
One possibility is that the role neuroticism plays in health and health behavior is altered in the presence of chronic disease. Individuals may respond to the onset of chronic illness with an increase in health-related vigilance. Alternatively, if individuals have not experienced past disease onset, it is less important for them to focus on identifying symptoms or signs of disease progression. Neuroticism has been linked more broadly to higher symptom reporting [19, 20], in line with the central argument for why the trait could prove “healthy” in some instances [13], namely, that higher rates of symptom reporting could result from heightened health vigilance. Unfortunately, outside of that one study [18], research has failed to address whether neuroticism’s impact on health and health behavior differs based on chronic disease status.
The Moderating Role of SES
Central to the theory that neuroticism could be adaptive is that vigilant or anxious individuals are able to enact healthier behaviors when confronted with health concerns [13]. Neuroticism has been linked to greater health care costs [21] and more doctor visits [22], presumably related to the associations between the trait and greater vigilance and symptom reporting. However, it is unclear whether these associations would hold across socioeconomic strata, insofar that individuals from poorer backgrounds may not have the resources available for these frequent visits. Moreover, even if neuroticism could promote healthier behaviors such as exercise and activity engagement, these opportunities are limited for those without sufficient resources. Similarly, neuroticism may fail to promote health for less educated individuals, who may have limited knowledge of best health practices. Support for this claim comes from recent work predicting inflammatory makers [23]. In that study, neuroticism proved a risk factor for greater inflammation among low-SES participants, but was related to a reduced risk for inflammation in those higher in SES. In addition, higher neuroticism has been linked to greater longevity among women with higher SES [24]. The current study sought to replicate and extend this interaction by testing whether it holds across different health and health behavior measures.
Differing Facets of Neuroticism
A final possibility reflects another common issue resulting from the movement in health psychology toward broader trait dimensions, namely, that these dimensions reflect constellations of lower-order traits, or facets, that may not all work in tandem. Neuroticism is comprised of facets that reflect different aspects of emotional instability, including depression, anxiety, anger or hostility, immoderation, and self-consciousness [25]. However, as noted above, the notion of a “healthy neurotic” is largely reflective of the anxiety component of the trait domain [13], as anxiety may encourage vigilance to potential health conditions, and to situations that may prove health-averse. Contrarily, one would not expect depression or hostility to promote health and health behavior, and indeed, both depression [26] and hostility [27] have been linked instead to greater mortality risk. That said, work has shown that hostility and anger, facets typically seen as negative for health, may prove less maladaptive for some groups and in some contexts [28, 29]; accordingly, although we focus on anxiety as it proves central to the past discussion of healthy neuroticism [13], we investigate all facets to consider the potential for differential influences on health outcomes.
Indeed, this work is in line with a broader literature that has demonstrated the utility of “moving beyond” the Big Five when predicting health outcomes. For instance, regarding neuroticism, work suggests that the impulsivity/immoderation component may be the facet most associated with obesity [30, 31], likely due to inappropriate eating habits. However, multiple facets appear negatively linked to self-rated health [32], including anxiety, the facet most associated with “healthy neuroticism.” Although it appears important to consider neuroticism at the facet level, little support exists for the facet predictions aligned with the healthy neuroticism concept.
The Current Study
Although we are not the first study to seek evidence of healthy neuroticism, the current study may have the highest likelihood of identifying such evidence, if it exists, for two reasons. First, the current study contains both adulthood and childhood personality measures. Concurrent assessments of predictors and outcomes often have the largest effect sizes, making them easier to detect. However, in the case of personality and cumulative health outcomes, traits often only have small effects at any single time point present moment, with more easily observed effects accruing over time [4, 33]. For example, an adult smokes a single cigarette in response to high levels of neuroticism, having a small impact on one’s health. In contrast, if a teenager chooses to initiate a smoking habit in response to high levels of neuroticism, the resulting sustained behavior may have an enormous impact. In addition, many health habits are set in late adolescence and early adulthood, and these choices are likely influenced by personality at that age. By examining both adulthood and childhood personality, we leveraged both the possibilities of stronger associations due to concurrent assessment and stronger associations due to the accrual of behaviors over time.
Second, the current study utilized multiple measures of health. As with many studies of personality and health, we examined how traits may predict self-rated health and health behavior. Additionally, we predicted major chronic disease status. Finally—and unlike the majority of studies—we employed biomarker data as objective outcomes. By examining a large variety of health indicators, we are able to not only increase the likelihood of identifying healthy neuroticism, but we are also able to better understand the mechanisms involved. For example, if we identify evidence for healthy neuroticism with respect to the clinical biomarkers but not behaviors, it would provide evidence that researchers need to move beyond lifestyle behaviors when explaining the value of healthy neuroticism, such as considering stress reactivity. All model output summaries can be found in Supplementary File 1, and the code for all analyses can be found in Supplementary File 2.
Methods
Participants
Participants came from the Hawaii Personality and Health Cohort. This cohort was originally assessed between 1959 and 1967, when teachers in entire elementary-school classrooms on two Hawaiian Islands assessed children (N = 2,418) on their personality traits at the end of the school year, one time for each child. In 1998, efforts to find these children, now middle-aged adults, were begun. When each member of the original cohort was located (i.e., rolling recruitment), he or she was invited to join the study. Since joining, participants have completed one or more of seven questionnaires (Q1–Q7) and, from 2003 onward, have been invited to attend a half-day medical and psychological examination. In addition to the childhood personality assessment, measures used here were drawn from items in Q5 (adult personality; assessed in 2008), Q6 (self-rated health, smoking, alcohol use, and chronic disease status; assessed in 2013), and Q7 (exercise and flossing; assessed in 2015), and the clinic visit (physical health outcomes; assessed at age 50).
To be included in the subsample for this study, participants had to have completed either the Q5 adult personality questionnaire or the childhood personality questionnaire and at least one of these portions of the study: Q6, Q7, or the clinical assessments. This resulted in a subsample of 1,115 participants (51% female). Additional demographic information is presented in Supplementary Material 1, Supplementary Table 5. (Supplementary Materials can be found at osf.io/bdgs7.) However, the sample sizes for various analyses differ, given which participants completed which surveys. Specifically, data were collected in a longitudinal study, in which questionnaires were mailed to participants at nonregular intervals across decades. If a participant chose not to respond to a questionnaire—because they moved, were busy, and were tired of participating—the result was missingness on multiple questions in a systematic way. We chose to not to limit our sample to only participants with all data, as this would greatly limit our power; we also chose not to use a multiple imputation approach, as missingness was not completely at random. Moreover, we think it is likely that the data are missing not at random, and the use of multiple imputation could exaggerate existing bias and produce results that are not representative of this population. The largest sample size was for regressions of systolic blood pressure on the interaction of childhood neuroticism and conscientiousness (N = 802), and the smallest was for the regression of alcoholic behaviors on the facets of neuroticism (N = 544). We note that even the smallest sample sizes are well-powered, if we can assume the interaction of neuroticism and other variables to be large enough to predict substantial differences between studies. Sample sizes for each regression are listed in Supplementary Material 1, Supplementary Tables 7–22.
Attrition analyses suggested that participants who chose not to fill out certain questionnaires differed in important ways from those who did. Participants who did not complete the adult personality questionnaire were more likely to be female (54% vs. 47%, t(584) = −2.13, p = .034) and less educated (Mparticipated = 7.02, Mwithdrew = 6.52, t(408) = 3.66, p < .001). Participants without health data from Q6 (self-rated health, smoking, alcohol use, and chronic disease status) were less educated (Mparticipated = 7.09, Mwithdrew = 6.52, t(654) = 4.71, p < .001), less conscientious as children (Mparticipated =0.05, Mwithdrew = −0.08, t(830) = 2.00, p = .046), and had parents with lower SES (Mparticipated = 0.05, Mwithdrew = −0.12, t(417) = 2.13, p = .034). Participants without health data from Q7 (exercise and flossing) were less educated (Mparticipated = 6.99, Mwithdrew = 6.51, t(330) = 3.37, p = .001) and had parents with lower SES (Mparticipated = 0.05, Mwithdrew = −0.21, t(234) = 2.89, p = .004). Participants who did not have biomarker data did not differ on any variables of interest from those who did. At this time, researchers at the Oregon Research Institute have archived the child data from the Hawaii and Personality Cohort, and portions of the adult follow-up assessments [14]. Additional waves of data will be added to these as resources become available.
Measures
Adult neuroticism
In 2008, participants completed the 120-item IPIP-NEO from the International Personality Item Pool [34, 35], in which participants are asked to rate how well a statement describes them on a scale from 1 (Not at all) to 5 (Very much). Neuroticism was measured using 24 statements, including “I worry about things” and “I become overwhelmed by events” (α = .88). Trait levels of neuroticism were measured by averaging responses to these items (M = 2.57, SD = 0.55). Neuroticism was standardized for these analyses.
Facets of adult neuroticism
Six facets of neuroticism can be estimated using these items: anxiety, anger, depression, self-consciousness, immoderation, and vulnerability. Each of these was measured using four of the 24 items in the neuroticism scale. Facets were estimated using a bifactor approach, described below.
Childhood neuroticism
For the children in the present sample, teachers assessed their entire elementary school classrooms (Grades 1, 2, 5, or 6; M age 10 years) by rank-ordering children on each of a comprehensive set of 43–49 personality attributes, using a 7- or 9-step quasinormal distribution. Definitions developed by teachers for each attribute were provided. These were converted to standardized scores, and orthogonal factor scores for the Big Five were derived for within each subsample of children using all available items in each subsample [34]. The average reliability of the neuroticism factor scores across subsamples was estimated at α = .68 [35]. Previous analyses of the childhood and adulthood assessments of personality in this sample indicate that the neuroticism measures are unassociated with each other [35].
Self-rated health
Participants rated their own general health, compared with others of their same age and sex, on a scale from 1 (Poor) to 5 (Excellent; M = 3.36, SD = 1.00) [36].
Health behaviors
In 2013, participants reported whether they had (1) or had not (0) ever smoked cigarettes (48% said yes). Current smokers reported the number of cigarettes they typically smoked per day and nonsmokers were assigned a value of 0 for this variable (M = 0.34, SD = 0.91).
Participants also reported whether they had (1) or had not (0) consumed alcohol at any point in their lives (86% said yes). Current drinkers reported on how many days of the week they drank alcohol (M = 1.68, SD = 1.97), how many drinks they consumed when they drank (M = 1.29, SD = 1.62), and how many days a week they engaged in binge drinking (defined as five or more drinks in one sitting; M = 0.58, SD = 1.49), and nondrinkers were assigned a value of 0 for each of these variables.
In 2015, participants reported on how frequently they engaged in strenuous exercise (defined as feeling your heart beat rapidly), moderate exercise (defined as not exhausting), and mild exercise (defined as minimal effort) [37]. Responses were coded as zero times [1], one or two times [2], three or four times [3], five or six times [4], or seven or more times a week [5]. On average participants engaged in strenuous exercise 1.98 times a week (SD = 1.19), moderate exercise 2.69 times a week (SD = 1.28), and mild exercise 2.93 times a week (SD = 1.34). Participants also reported on how frequently they flossed: never (0), sometimes (1), or always (2; M = 1.59, SD = 0.80).
Chronic disease status
In 2013, participants reported whether they had ever been diagnosed with the following chronic diseases or health events: heart attack, heart disease, stroke, thyroid disease, migraine, cancer, chronic fatigue syndrome, type 1 diabetes, and type 2 diabetes. The total number of diseases reported was a primary outcome (M = 0.71, SD = 0.98). In addition, we examined whether a participant had been diagnosed with any heart condition (heart attack or heart disease; 13.4%). This condition was chosen for closer consideration due to its relatively large base rate, compared with the other conditions, and the substantial impact it has on American mortality rates.
Objectively assessed health
Starting in 2003 (Mage = 51.00, SD = 2.93), participants were invited to a half day clinic assessment which included the collection of urine and blood samples that were tested for various biomarkers [38]. The following biomarkers were used as individual outcomes: fasting blood glucose (M = 100.68, SD = 26.14), total cholesterol/HDL ratio (M = 4.29, SD = 1.53), systolic blood pressure (M = 122.39, SD = 15.72), and waist-to-hip ratio (M = 0.88, SD = 0.09). These outcomes were standardized. We control for participants’ age at the clinic visit in analyses.
Moderators
Adult conscientiousness was measured in 2008 using 24 statements from the IPIP-NEO, including “I complete tasks successfully” and “I am always prepared” (α = .88; M = 4.01, SD = 0.46). Neuroticism and adult conscientiousness were negatively correlated (r = −.51). Typically, it is advised that variables be uncorrelated in moderation analyses; however, this interaction was examined in prior studies of healthy neuroticism [15, 18] despite correlations between the variables. Childhood conscientiousness was assessed using the same procedures described above for childhood neuroticism (average α = .77) [35].
Educational attainment was used as a proxy for adult SES. Education was assessed on a scale from 1 (8th grade education or less) to 9 (professional or postgraduate degrees). The current sample was well educated (M = 6.89, SD = 1.81) All participants had completed at least junior high. Childhood SES was a linear combination of father’s education, mother’s education (both assessed on a scale from 1 [8th grade or less] to 9 [professional or postgraduate degree]), and whether the parents owned a home before the participant had entered school (1—Yes, 0—No). Each variable was standardized, and then the average of the three standardized variables was used as the participant’s score.
Chronic disease was a binary variable defined as whether a participant had been diagnosed with one of the chronic diseases listed above (47%). We acknowledge that this variable encompasses a wide range of health problems, including heart disease, diabetes, and chronic fatigue. However, we wanted to use a variable that broadly represented having major health problems—as those studies which find protective effects of neuroticism represent broad populations of older adults—and because we would expect any major health problem to incite anxiety in a person high in neuroticism. When using this moderator, we did not include number of chronic conditions or heart disease status as outcomes, given these are too theoretically and numerically similar to the moderator.
Analyses
All analyses were performed in R (version 3.4.2) [39]. We used the pscl package [40] to estimate the zero-inflated Poisson models and the lavaan package [41] to estimate the bifactor models. For all analyses, we included every participant who had provided the data on the relevant constructs. Because of the longitudinal nature of the study, not every participant had data at every wave. In some cases, such as for the adult personality assessment or self-report, some participants were simply unavailable during data collection. In the case of the clinic data, data collection was targeted at about age 55, as opposed to a specific year, so some participants were not yet eligible for participation. For transparency, we reported the sample size available for the models run (Supplementary Material 1, Supplementary Tables 7–22).
To move beyond the simple significant–nonsignificant binary and better describe the evidentiary value of these results, we have presented the exact p-values for all coefficients of interest in our regression results. In other words, we presented the exact p-values for the multiple regressions with neuroticism or childhood neuroticism as predictors, the exact p-values for the interaction terms in the moderation models, and the exact p-values for the regression results in the bifactor models. By presenting these p-values, a discerning reader will be able to adequately assess the evidentiary value of any significant results, as well as examine the distribution of results across all analyses.
Question 1: To what extent is neuroticism associated with health (i.e., self-rated health, health outcomes, health behaviors and clinical indicators of health)?
To assess the extent to which neuroticism is related to health, we examined both bivariate correlations and coefficient estimates from generalized linear models. For each outcome, we estimate a model with adult neuroticism—because concurrent measurements are likely to yield the largest effect sizes—and a separate model with childhood neuroticism—which may have cumulative influences on health accruing over long spans of time. Generalized linear models controlled for gender and age at the clinic assessment. For outcomes that were normally distributed (i.e., self-rated health, moderate and mild exercise, flossing, and clinical indicators), linear models were estimated using ordinary least squares. For binary outcomes (i.e., heart conditions, ever smoking, and ever drinking alcohol), binary logistic regression was used. For most count outcomes (number of cigarettes smoked, number of drinks per day, days drinking per week, and number of days of binge drinking per week), absence of behavior was over-represented, so we used zero-inflated Poisson regression models. As a reminder, these models estimated the relationship between the predictors and both the probability of the outcome (e.g., probability the outcome is greater than or equal to 1) and also the relationship of the predictors to the count for cases when the outcome is greater than 0. We presented the results of both models. For models with strenuous exercise as the outcome, we used a generalized linear model with a negative binomial distribution. For number of chronic diseases, we used a Poisson distribution, because models using the negative binomial failed to converge in a few instances.
Question 2: Does conscientiousness moderate the association of neuroticism with health?
Next, we examined the extent to which conscientiousness moderated the association of neuroticism on health. To do so, we added conscientiousness and the interaction of conscientiousness and neuroticism to each of the generalized linear models described above. When adult neuroticism was the predictor, adult conscientiousness was chosen as the moderator; when childhood neuroticism was the predictor, childhood conscientiousness was the moderator. We used a broad interpretation of healthy neuroticism: in these models, a significant interaction term in any of these models was interpreted as an example of “healthy neuroticism.” This encompassed both instances when neuroticism had no association with health and instances when neuroticism was protective.
Question 3: Does SES moderate the association of neuroticism with health?
We examined the extent to which SES moderated the association of neuroticism with health. Here, we added SES and the interaction of SES and neuroticism to the models described in Question 1. When adult neuroticism was the predictor, adult education was chosen as the moderator. When childhood neuroticism was the predictor, childhood SES was used as the moderator. If the interaction term in any of these models is significant, it would suggest that SES is a “healthy neuroticism” moderator.
Question 4: Does chronic disease moderate the relationship between neuroticism and health?
Here, we examined whether having been diagnosed with a chronic disease status moderates the association of neuroticism with health. We added the binary disease status variable and the interaction of this variable with adult neuroticism to the models described in Question 1. We also chose not to examine number of chronic diseases or likelihood of having a heart condition as outcomes, given they were substantially overlapping with the moderating variable. If the interaction term in any of these models is significant, it would suggest that chronic disease status is a “healthy neuroticism” moderator.
Question 5: Do facets of neuroticism predict health, and if so, do they predict health in opposing directions?
To determine whether facets of neuroticism had differing effects on health, we used a bifactor model approach. In this model, all items were set to load on general trait neuroticism. Simultaneously, each item was set to load on its corresponding facet. We constrained the covariance between general neuroticism and each of the facets to be 0. This approach has been used in other studies of personality facets and outcomes [38, 42, 43] and allowed us to specify facet constructs that are “pure” of the shared variance across facets, and in turn provided a better opportunity to test whether the facets hold unique associations with the outcomes of interest. Fit for this model was very good (RMSEA = .04, CFI = .952). However, some loadings of items onto their assigned facets were poor. For each outcome, we used this model and added a set of regression pathways by regression the health outcome onto latent general neuroticism and each latent facet simultaneously. If any of the pathways from personality (neuroticism and its facets) to health are positive (i.e., indicating greater levels of a facet or trait are linked to better health), it would suggest that some facets of neuroticism are “healthy.”
Results
Question 1: To what extent is neuroticism associated with health (i.e., self-rated health, health outcomes, health behaviors and clinical indicators of health)?
First, we examined the bivariate correlations of adult neuroticism and childhood neuroticism with health outcomes, behaviors, and indicators, as shown in Table 1. Sample sizes ranged from 558 (outcome: ever drink alcohol) to 802 (systolic blood pressure). Adult neuroticism was associated with worse self-rated health (r = −.37, p < .001), increased likelihood of having been diagnosed with a heart condition (r = .08, p = .033), more chronic diseases (r = .15, p < .001), more cigarettes smoked (r = .09, p =.016), less strenuous exercise (r = −.10, p = .014), and larger waist-to-hip ratios (r = .08, p =.038). Childhood neuroticism was not associated with any of the health variables. Next, we used linear models to estimate the relationship of adult neuroticism and childhood neuroticism to health, controlling for gender, birth year, and highest level of education. These results are shown on the right side of Table 1. Adult neuroticism was associated with self-rated health (b = −0.59, 95% CI [−0.73, −.46], p < .001), number of chronic diseases (b = 0.25 [0.07, 0.42], p = .005), more cigarettes smoked (b = 0.61 [0.07, 1.18], p = .030), greater likelihood of consuming alcohol (b = 0.44 [0.02, 0.89], p = .044), larger amounts of binge drinking (b = 0.53 [0.05, 1.02], p = .030), and waist-to-hip ratio (b = 0.17 [0.06, 0.28], p = .003).
Table 1.
Zero-order correlations (r estimate) | Linear models controlling for covariates (b estimate) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Adult neuroticism | Child neuroticism | Adult neuroticism | Child neuroticism | |||
Est [95% CI] | p-value | Est [95% CI] | p-value | |||
Self-rated health |
−.37*
[−.44, −.31] |
−.06 [−.11, .02] |
−0.59*
[−0.73, −0.46] |
<.001 |
−0.08*
[−0.16, −0.01] |
.026 |
Heart condition |
.08*
[.00, .16] |
−.03 [−.11, .03] |
0.21 [−0.25, 0.67] |
.362 | −0.01 [−0.24, 0.22] |
.954 |
Number chronic diseases |
.15*
[.07, .22] |
.02 [−.04, .08] |
0.25*
[0.07, 0.42] |
.005 | 0.07 [−0.03, 0.17] |
.110 |
Cigarettes ever | .07 [.00, .15] |
.02 [−.06, .10] |
0.25 [−0.05, 0.55] |
.100 | 0.07 [−0.08, 0.22] |
.350 |
Cigarettes (count model) |
.09*
[.02, .16] |
−.01 [−.07, .06] |
0.11 [−0.22, 0.45] |
.498 | −0.08 [-0.24, 0.08] |
.334 |
Cigarettes (zero model) |
– | – |
−0.61*
[−1.08, −0.13] |
.013 | −0.12 [−0.35, 0.11] |
.307 |
Alcohol ever | .05 [−.04, .13] |
−.07 [−.14, .00] |
0.45*
[0.02, 0.89] |
.044 | −0.10 [−0.31, 0.11] |
.333 |
Alcohol days/week (count model) |
−.03 [−.11, .05] |
−.02 [−.08, .06] |
0.03 [−0.08, 0.15] |
.570 | 0.01 [−0.05, 0.07] |
.825 |
Alcohol days/week (zero model) |
– | – | 0.13 [−0.02, 0.46] |
.442 | 0.05 [−0.12, 0.21] |
.585 |
Alcohol drinks/sitting (count model) |
.01 [−.08, .09] |
−.05 [−.11, .01] |
0.17*
[0.02, 0.32] |
.023 | 0.03 [−0.05, 0.11] |
.451 |
Alcohol drinks/sitting (zero model) |
– | – | 0.29 [−0.16, 0.74] |
.209 | 0.08 [−0.14, 0.29] |
.476 |
Alcohol binge days/week (count model) |
.05 [−.03, .12] |
−.02 [−.08, .04] |
0.24*
[0.02, 0.46] |
.032 | −0.05 [−0.16, 0.06] |
.370 |
Alcohol binge days/week (zero model) |
– | – | −0.24 [−0.67, 0.18] |
.257 | −0.11 [−0.32, 0.10] |
.300 |
Strenuous exercise |
−.10*
[−.17, −.03] |
−.04 [−.12, .04] |
−0.11 [−0.22, 0.00] |
.052 | −0.01 [−0.06, 0.04] |
.669 |
Moderate exercise | −.05 [−.11, .02] |
−.03 [−.11, .06] |
−0.09 [−0.24, 0.05] |
.206 | −0.03 [−0.10, 0.05] |
.487 |
Mild exercise | −.06 [−.12, .02] |
.00 [−.06, .07] |
−0.10 [−0.25, 0.05] |
.179 | 0.00 [−0.07, 0.07] |
.944 |
Flossing | .06 [−.01, .13] |
−.05 [−.12, .02] |
0.11 [−0.03, 0.25] |
.138 | −0.03 [−0.10, 0.05] |
.491 |
Blood glucose | .08 [−.01, .16] |
−.03 [−.09, .03] |
0.14 [0.00, 0.27] |
.053 | −0.02 [−0.09, 0.05] |
.565 |
Cholesterol/HDL ratio | .01 [−.05, .08] |
−.04 [−.1, .03] |
0.02 [−0.13, 0.17] |
.769 | −0.01 [−0.09, 0.06] |
.690 |
Systolic blood pressure | .01 [−.07, .09] |
.03 [−.03, .10] |
0.02 [−0.12, 0.16] |
.772 | 0.05 [−0.02, 0.12] |
.170 |
Waist-to-Hip ratio |
.08*
[.01, .16] |
−.07 [−.13, .00] |
0.17*
[0.06, 0.28] |
.003 | 0.01 [−0.04, 0.06] |
.743 |
Zero-order correlations are presented in the first two columns with bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals. Modeled associations use ordinary least squares, binary logistic regression, or negative binomial regression, depending upon the outcome. Unstandardized regression coefficients and 95% confidence intervals are presented. All models control for gender and age at the time of outcome measurement.
*Indicates p < .05. Exact p-values for the regression models are presented.
Question 2: Does conscientiousness moderate the association of neuroticism with health?
In only one of the 19 models did adult conscientiousness moderate the association of adult neuroticism with health. The results of all models are shown in Table 2. In this model, as adult conscientiousness increased, adult neuroticism had a stronger, positive relationship with binge drinking (b = 1.26 [0.18, 2.53], p = .022). In only one of the 19 models did childhood conscientiousness moderate the association of childhood neuroticism with adult health. In this model, higher childhood conscientiousness strengthened the relationship between higher childhood neuroticism and greater waist-to-hip ratios. (b = 0.01 [0.001, 0.012], p = .025). Sample sizes ranged from 558 (ever smoked cigarettes) to 802 (systolic blood pressure). To conserve space in the current paper, and to avoid confusing readers, we do not provide estimates of slopes at different levels of moderators here. However, we have plotted the simple slopes at one standard deviation above and below the means, and these plots are available in Supplementary Materials 1 and 3.
Table 2.
Outcome | Adulthood predictors | Childhood predictors | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neur | Con | Neur x Con | Neur | Con | Neur x Con | |||
Est [95% CI] | Est [95% CI] | Est [95% CI] | p | Est [95% CI] | Est [95% CI] | Est [95% CI] | p | |
Self-rated health | −0.54 [−0.70, −0.38] |
0.13 [−0.06, 0.32] |
0.11 [−0.17, 0.38] |
.451 | −0.08 [−0.16, −0.01] |
0.03 [−0.04, 0.11] |
−0.01 [−0.08, 0.06] |
.865 |
Heart condition | 0.23 [−0.31, 0.75] |
0.08 [−0.55, 0.72] |
0.80 [−0.22, 1.95] |
.149 | −0.01 [−0.24, 0.22] |
0.05 [−0.19, 0.30] |
0.10 [−0.12, 0.31] |
.353 |
Number chronic illnesses | 0.23 [0.02, 0.43] |
−0.05 [−0.29, 0.20] |
−0.02 [−0.37, 0.35] |
.927 | 0.07 [−0.02, 0.16] |
−0.07 [−0.16, 0.03] |
0.02 [−0.06, 0.11] |
.619 |
Ever smoked | 0.17 [−0.17, 0.52] |
−0.18 [−0.60, 0.23] |
0.04 [−0.57, 0.65] |
.899 | 0.08 [−0.07, 0.23] |
−0.32 [−0.49, −0.16] |
−0.02 [−0.17, 0.12] |
.751 |
Number cigarettes (count model) | 0.05 [−0.35, 0.46] |
−0.05 [−0.56, 0.45] |
0.57 [−0.31, 1.44] |
.203 | −0.10 [−0.27, 0.07] |
−0.02 [−0.17, 0.14] |
−0.08 [−0.23, 0.08] |
.323 |
Number cigarettes (zero model) |
−0.61 [−1.16, −0.05] |
0.06 [−0.62, 0.75] |
0.22 [−0.90, 1.34] |
.695 | −0.13 [−0.37, 0.11] |
0.23 [−0.01, 0.47] |
0.01 [−0.22, 0.24] |
.916 |
Ever drank | 0.43 [−0.07, 0.96] |
0.05 [−0.56, 0.66] |
0.42 [−0.45, 1.22] |
.328 | −0.09 [−0.30, 0.12] |
0.06 [−0.18, 0.28] |
−0.14 [−0.33, 0.05] |
.143 |
Alcohol (days per week) (count model) | −0.04 [−0.18, 010] |
−0.17 [−0.35, 0.01] |
0.20 [−0.07, 0.46] |
.147 | −0.01 [−0.05, 0.07] |
−0.03 [−0.09, 0.04] |
−0.04 [−0.10, 0.02] |
.212 |
Alcohol (days per week) (zero model) |
0.04 [−0.34, 0.43] |
−0.24 [−0.72, 0.23] |
0.38 [−0.36, 1.11] |
.315 | 0.04 [−0.13, 0.20] |
−0.01 [−0.19, 0.17] |
0.06 [−0.10, 0.22] |
.452 |
Alcohol (drinks per sitting) (count model) |
0.12 [−0.06, 0.30] |
−0.17 [−0.40, 0.06] |
0.26 [−0.09, 0.61] |
.141 | 0.03 [−0.05, 0.10] |
−0.07 [−0.16, 0.01] |
−0.06 [−0.14, 0.02] |
.115 |
Alcohol (drinks per sitting) (zero model) | −0.04 [−0.18, 0.10] |
−0.17 [−0.35, 0.01] |
0.20 [−0.07, 0.46] |
.119 | 0.01 [−0.05, 0.07] |
−0.03 [−0.09, 0.04] |
−0.04 [−0.10, 0.02] |
.815 |
Alcohol (binge per week) (count model) |
0.26 [0.02, 0.50] |
−0.28 [−0.61, 0.05] |
1.51*
[0.73, 2.29] |
< .001 | −0.09 [−0.20, 0.03] |
−0.11 [−0.22, 0.01] |
−0.11 [−0.22, 0.01] |
.072 |
Alcohol (binge per week) (zero model) |
−0.09 [−0.60, 0.42] |
0.15 [−0.45, 0.75] |
0.27 [−0.87, 1.41] |
.643 | −0.12 [−0.33, 0.09] |
0.12 [−0.10, 0.34] |
0.01 [−0.11, 0.31] |
.358 |
Exercise (strenuous) | −0.05 [−0.18, 0.07] |
0.11 [−0.04, 0.27] |
−0.04 [−0.23, 0.17] |
.700 | −0.01 [−0.06, 0.04] |
−0.02 [−0.07, 0.04] |
0.02 [−0.03, 0.07] |
.403 |
Exercise (moderate) | −0.09 [−0.27, 0.08] |
0.00 [−0.20, 0.21] |
0.12 [−0.15, 0.39] |
.389 | −0.03 [−0.10, 0.05] |
−0.05 [−0.13, 0.02] |
0.01 [−0.06, 0.07] |
.856 |
Exercise (mild) | −0.01 [−0.19, 0.16] |
0.19 [−0.02, 0.39] |
0.08 [−0.20, 0.35] |
.584 | 0.00 [−0.07, 0.07] |
−0.06 [−0.13, 0.02] |
−0.01 [−0.07, 0.06] |
.859 |
Flossing | 0.05 [−0.12, 0.22] |
−0.11 [−0.31, 0.09] |
−0.16 [−0.42, 0.11] |
.254 | −0.02 [−0.10, 0.05] |
−0.05 [−0.12, 0.03] |
−0.02 [−0.09, 0.04] |
.527 |
Blood glucose levels | 0.08 [−0.08, 0.24] |
−0.12 [−0.32, 0.07] |
−0.04 [−0.32, 0.24] |
.785 | −0.02 [−0.10, 0.05] |
−0.08 [−0.16, −0.01] |
0.00 [−0.07, 0.07] |
.919 |
Cholesterol/HDL ratio | 0.00 [−0.18, 0.17] |
−0.06 [−0.28, 0.15] |
0.09 [−0.23, 0.40] |
.587 | −0.02 [-0.09, 0.05] |
−0.11 [−0.18, −0.04] |
0.02 [−0.04, 0.09] |
.521 |
Systolic blood pressure | 0.10 [−0.06, 0.26] |
0.16 [−0.03, 0.36] |
0.24 [−0.04, 0.52] |
.087 | 0.05 [−0.02, 0.12] |
−0.03 [−0.10, 0.04] |
−0.01 [−0.07, 0.06] |
.778 |
Waist to hip ratio | 0.15 [0.01, 0.28] |
−0.05 [−0.21, 0.11] |
0.01 [−0.21, 0.24] |
.901 | 0.01 [−0.05, 0.06] |
−0.10 [−0.15, −0.04] |
0.06*
[0.01, 0.10] |
.025 |
Neuroticism, childhood neuroticism, and moderators are all standardized, allowing for easier interpretation of the simple slopes. Regression coefficients (standardized when the outcome is continuous and the model is estimated using ordinary least squares) and 95% confidence intervals are presented. All models control for gender and age at outcome measurement.
*Indicates p < .05. Stars are only used indicate significant interaction terms; we do not interpret significance of the simple slopes (e.g., the coefficients for neuroticism and conscientiousness) because they are dependent upon the level of the other predictor and only included to allow for better interpretation of the interaction. Exact p-values for the interaction terms are presented to allow for estimation of the evidentiary value of these findings.
Question 3: Does SES moderate the association of neuroticism with health?
In none of nineteen models did adult SES moderate the association of adult neuroticism with health. In only two of 19 models did childhood SES moderate the association of childhood neuroticism with adult health. Specifically, this interaction was associated with the binary portion of the alcoholic beverages per sitting model (b = 0.24, OR = 1.27, 95% CI [1.01, 1.60], p = .040) and the count model of number of times per month the participant engaged in binge drinking (b = 0.13, 95% CI [0.00, 0.26], p = .045). We note that both of these p-values are between .04 and .05 and thus hold little evidentiary value. In both cases, the relationship between childhood neuroticism and alcohol consumption was stronger at higher levels of childhood SES. Sample sizes for the adult predictor models ranged from 562 (alcoholic drinks per sitting) to 593 (flossing). Sample sizes for the childhood predictor models ranged from 636 (cholesterol/HDL ratio) to 693 (flossing). Results for all models are shown in Table 3.
Table 3.
Outcome | Adulthood predictors | Childhood predictors | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neuroticism | SES | N x SES | Neuroticism | SES | N x SES | |||
Est [95% CI] |
Est [95% CI] |
Est [95% CI] |
p | Est [95% CI] |
Est [95% CI] |
Est [95% CI] |
p | |
Self-rated health | −0.55 [−0.69, −0.42] |
0.22 [0.13, 0.3] |
0.00 [−0.15, 0.14] |
.958 | −0.09 [−0.16, −0.02] |
0.29 [0.19, 0.38] |
−0.05 [−0.13, 0.04] |
.306 |
Heart condition | 0.23 [−0.25, 0.71] |
−0.01 [−0.28, 0.29] |
−0.05 [−0.55, 0.47] |
.860 | 0.00 [−0.24, 0.23] |
−0.31 [−0.61, −0.01] |
−0.08 [−0.37, 0.21] |
.593 |
Number chronic illnesses | 0.24 [0.06, 0.42] |
−0.13 [−0.23, −0.02] |
−0.17 [−0.35, 0.01] |
.110 | 0.09 [−0.01, 0.18] |
−0.16 [−0.28, −0.05] |
0.04 [−0.07, 0.15] |
.489 |
Ever smoked | 0.19 [−0.13, 0.51] |
−0.54 [−0.74, −0.34] |
−0.26 [−0.64, 0.10] |
.167 | 0.08 [−0.07, 0.24] |
−0.23 [−0.43, −0.04] |
0.16 [−0.02, 0.36] |
.090 |
Number cigarettes (count model) |
0.10 [−0.27, 0.47] |
−0.12 [−0.35, 0.12] |
0.10 [−0.31, 0.50] |
.637 | −0.08 [−0.25, 0.09] |
0.04 [−0.16, 0.24] |
0.03 [−0.12, 0.17] |
.720 |
Number cigarettes (zero model) |
−0.44 [−0.96, 0.07] |
0.58 [0.29, 0.87] |
0.36 [−0.17, 0.89] |
.181 | −0.22 [−0.46, 0.03] |
0.51 [0.24, 0.78] |
−0.23 [−0.48, 0.02] |
.068 |
Ever drank | 0.40 [−0.05, 0.87] |
−0.21 [−0.51, 0.06] |
−0.04 [−0.57, 0.46] |
.881 | −0.09 [−0.31, 0.12] |
−0.15 [−0.42, 0.12] |
−0.08 [−0.34, 0.18] |
.537 |
Alcohol (days per week) (count model) |
0.06 [−0.07, 0.20] |
0.11 [0.02, 0.19] |
0.00 [−0.17, 0.16] |
.968 | 0.01 [−0.05, .08] |
0.11 [0.04, 0.18] |
0.00 [−0.06, 0.07] |
.946 |
Alcohol (days per week) (zero model) |
0.08 [−0.26, 0.42] |
−0.14 [−0.35, 0.07] |
0.05 [−0.34, 0.43] |
.809 | 0.06 [−0.11, 0.23] |
−0.19 [−0.37, −0.01] |
0.15 [−0.03, 0.33] |
.092 |
Alcohol (drinks per sitting) (count model) |
0.13 [−0.03, 0.29] |
−0.18 [−0.28, −0.09] |
−0.09 [−0.27, 0.08] |
.293 | 0.01 [−0.07, 0.08] |
−0.10 [−0.19, −0.01] |
0.02 [−005, 0.12] |
.443 |
Alcohol (drinks per sitting) (zero model) |
0.16 [−0.31, 0.62] |
−0.37 [−0.63, −0.11] |
−0.10 [−0.57, 0.37] |
.671 | 0.09 [−0.13, 0.31] |
−0.37 [−0.63, −0.11] |
0.24*
[0.01, 0.47] |
.040 |
Alcohol (binge per week) (count model) |
0.26 [0.04, 0.49] |
0.03 [−0.11, 0.16] |
0.01 [−0.23, 0.26] |
.924 | −0.04 [−0.15, 0.08] |
0.01 [−0.12, 0.13] |
0.13*
[0.00, 0.26] |
.045 |
Alcohol (binge per week) (zero model) |
−0.12 [−0.55, 0.32] |
0.38 [0.13, 0.62] |
−0.01 [−0.46, 0.44] |
.963 | −0.09 [−0.30, 0.12] |
0.12 [−0.11, 0.35] |
0.13 [−0.10, 0.35] |
.269 |
Exercise (strenuous) | −0.11 [−0.22, 0.00] |
0.00 [−0.06, 0.07] |
−0.01 [−0.13, 0.11] |
.892 | 0.00 [−0.05, 0.05] |
0.00 [−0.06, 0.07] |
−0.06 [−0.12, 0.00] |
.070 |
Exercise (moderate) | −0.11 [−0.27, 0.04] |
0.01 [−0.08, 0.09] |
0.13 [−0.04, 0.29] |
.127 | −0.01 [−0.09, 0.06] |
0.07 [−0.02, 0.16] |
−0.06 [−0.15, 0.03] |
.180 |
Exercise (mild) | −0.12 [−0.28, 0.03] |
0.00 [−0.08, 0.09] |
0.13 [−0.03, 0.29] |
.119 | 0.01 [−0.06, 0.08] |
0.08 [−0.01, 0.17] |
−0.04 [−0.13, 0.05] |
.388 |
Flossing | 0.09 [−0.06, 0.23] |
−0.10* [−0.18, −0.01] |
−0.04 [−0.20, 0.11] |
.573 | −0.02 [−0.09, 0.05] |
−0.13* [−0.22, −0.04] |
0.07 [−0.02, 0.15] |
.147 |
Blood glucose levels | 0.10 [−0.04, 0.25] |
−0.13* [−0.21, −0.05] |
−0.05 [−0.19, 0.10] |
.539 | −0.02 [−0.10, 0.05] |
−0.15* [−0.24, −0.06] |
0.04 [−0.06, 0.13] |
.450 |
Cholesterol/HDL ratio | −0.02 [−0.18, 0.14] |
−0.13* [−0.22, −0.04] |
0.04 [−0.12, 0.21] |
.597 | −0.02 [−0.10, 0.06] |
−0.05 [−0.14, 0.04] |
0.00 [−0.09, 0.09] |
.993 |
Systolic blood pressure | −0.02 [−0.17, 0.12] |
−0.12* [−0.20, −0.04] |
0.13 [−0.02, 0.28] |
.101 | 0.03 [−0.04, 0.11] |
−0.09* [−0.17, 0.00] |
0.06 [−0.03, 0.14] |
.207 |
Waist to hip ratio | 0.13* [0.01, 0.25] |
−0.14* [−0.21, −0.08] |
0.05 [−0.07, 0.17] |
.393 | 0.00 [−0.06, 0.06] |
−0.11* [−0.18, −0.05] |
0.04 [−−0.03, 0.11] |
.229 |
Neuroticism, childhood neuroticism, and moderators are all standardized, allowing for easier interpretation of the simple slopes. Regression coefficients (standardized when the outcome is continuous and the model is estimated using ordinary least squares) and 95% confidence intervals are presented. All models control for gender and age at outcome measurement.
*Indicates p < .05. Stars are only used indicate significant interaction terms; we do not interpret significance of the simple slopes (e.g., the coefficients for neuroticism and SES) because they are dependent upon the level of the other predictor and only included to allow for better interpretation of the interaction. Exact p-values for the interaction terms are presented to allow for estimation of the evidentiary value of these findings.
Question 4: Does chronic disease moderate the relationship between neuroticism and health?
Chronic disease status was indicated by a binary variable, 0 (no chronic diseases) or 1 (one or more chronic diseases). Results are shown in Table 4. We did not examine the health outcomes of heart condition or number of chronic diseases, as these were confounded with the moderator. In only two of the 16 models did adult chronic disease status moderate the association of adult neuroticism with health. This interaction was associated with the likelihood of drinking alcohol ever in the lifespan (b = 1.27, OR = 3.56 [1.46, 8.89], p = .005) and the count model of number of times per month the participant engaged in binge drinking (b = 0.70, 95% CI [0.25, 1.15], p = .002). In both cases, the interaction suggested that while neuroticism was unrelated to alcohol consumption in the absence of a chronic condition; however, when a chronic condition was present, neuroticism was positively associated with alcohol consumption. Sample sizes ranged from 484 (cholesterol/HDL ratio) to 588 (number of cigarettes smoked).
Table 4.
Outcomes | Neuroticism | Chronic disease | N x chronic disease | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Est [95% CI] |
Est [95% CI] |
Est [95% CI] |
p | |
Self-rated health | −0.47 [−0.66, −0.29] | −0.47 [−0.61, −0.32] | −0.15 [−0.41, 0.12] | .270 |
Ever smoked | 0.11 [−0.30, 0.53] | 0.15 [−0.18, 0.48] | 0.26 [−0.34, 0.86] | .405 |
Number cigarettes (count model) |
−0.12 [−0.60, 0.37] | 0.03 [−0.38, 0.44] | 0.40 [−0.27, 1.06] | .243 |
Number cigarettes (zero model) |
−0.46 [−1.18, 0.27] | −0.39 [−0.95, 0.17] | −0.21 [−1.18, 0.77] | .680 |
Ever drank | −0.16 [−0.78, 0.47] | −0.41 [−0.90, 0.08] | 1.27* [0.38, 2.19] | .005 |
Alcohol (days per week) (count model) |
0.08 [−0.08, 0.23] | −0.14 [−0.29, 0.00] | −0.07 [−0.31, 0.18] | .588 |
Alcohol (days per week) (zero model) |
0.27 [−0.20, 0.74] | 0.49 [0.12, 0.86] | −0.39 [−1.06, 0.28] | .251 |
Alcohol (drinks per sitting) (count model) |
0.10 [−0.09, 0.30] | −0.12 [−0.30, 0.06] | 0.18 [−0.13, 0.49] | .252 |
Alcohol (drinks per sitting) (zero model) |
0.30 [−0.37, 0.96] | 0.57 [0.03, 1.10] | −0.12 [−1.06, 0.82] | .805 |
Alcohol (binge per week) (count model) |
−0.02 [−0.31, 0.26] | −0.25 [−0.52, 0.03] | 0.70* [0.25, 1.15] | .002 |
Alcohol (binge per week) (zero model) |
−0.43 [−0.99, 0.13] | 0.21 [−0.26, 0.69] | 0.42 [−0.46, 1.29] | .351 |
Exercise (strenuous) | −0.07 [−0.22, 0.08] | −0.07 [−0.20, 0.05] | −0.05 [−0.28, 0.18] | .676 |
Exercise (moderate) | −0.03 [−0.24, 0.17] | −0.17 [−0.34, 0.00] | −0.07 [−0.38, 0.24] | .655 |
Exercise (mild) | 0.04 [−0.17, 0.24] | 0.04 [−0.13, 0.21] | −0.28 [−0.59, 0.03] | .080 |
Flossing | 0.11 [−0.09, 0.30] | 0.15 [−0.01, 0.31] | 0.02 [−0.27, 0.32] | .875 |
Blood glucose levels | 0.01 [−0.20, 0.23] | 0.46 [0.29, 0.63] | 0.19 [−0.11, 0.49] | .224 |
Cholesterol/HDL ratio | 0.04 [−0.20, 0.28] | 0.13 [−0.05, 0.32] | 0.05 [−0.28, 0.38] | .783 |
Systolic blood pressure | −0.02 [−0.24, 0.19] | 0.32 [0.16, 0.48] | 0.06 [−0.23, 0.36] | .676 |
Waist to hip ratio | 0.07 [−0.10, 0.24] | 0.41 [0.28, 0.54] | 0.14 [−0.09, 0.38] | .237 |
Neuroticism is standardized and chronic disease status is binary (0 = no chronic disease, 1 = one or more chronic diseases). Regression coefficients (standardized when the outcome is continuous and the model is estimated using ordinary least squares) and 95% confidence intervals are presented. All models control for gender and age at outcome measurement.
*Indicates p < .05. Stars are only used indicate significant interaction terms; we do not interpret significance of the simple slopes (e.g., the coefficients for neuroticism and health) because they are dependent upon the level of the other predictor and only included to allow for better interpretation of the interaction. Exact p-values for the interaction terms are presented to allow for estimation of the evidentiary value of these findings.
Question 5: Do facets of neuroticism predict health, and if so, do they predict health in opposing directions?
To examine the associations with specific neuroticism facets, we turned to a bifactor model, which defines unique latent constructs for general neuroticism and specific facets, allowing for an investigation of whether facets are associated with health outcomes after accounting for their shared variance (the general factor). Both the loadings of variables on to latent factors and the results of the regressions with the bifactor variables can be found in Table 5. Finally, we found no evidence that facets of adult neuroticism showed opposing associations associated with health and health behaviors. Instead, general neuroticism and two facets, immoderation and vulnerability, were both associated with worse health. General neuroticism was associated with worse self-rated health (b = −0.55 [−0.72, −0.38], p < .001), smoking a greater number of cigarettes (b = 0.18 [0.04, 0.32], p = .013), more frequent binge drinking (b = 0.24 [0.00, 0.47], p = .046) and a greater waist-to-hip ratio (b = 0.34 [0.09, 0.59], p = .008). Immoderation was associated with worse health behaviors and worse health. Specifically, immoderation was associated with less moderate exercise (b = −0.42 [−0.82, −0.03], p = .035), less mild exercise (b = −0.33 [−0.67, −0.00], p = .048). Vulnerability was associated with worse self-rated health (b = −0.68 [−1.17, −0.20], p = .005), a greater number of chronic illnesses (b = 0.48 [0.15, 0.80], p = .004), less mild exercise (b = −0.41 [−0.79, −0.03], p = .036), high cholesterol to HDL ratios (b = 0.60 [0.08, 1.11], p = .025), and a higher waist-to-hip ratio (b = 0.99 [0.43, 1.55], p = .001). Vulnerability was also associated with greater flossing (b = 0.37 [0.01, 0.73], p = .042; although, here we again note the p-value is between .04 and .05). All other facets were either negatively associated with health and health behaviors, or showed no association. Sample sizes ranged from 544 (ever drink alcohol) to 590 (flossing).
Table 5.
Factor loadings from bifactor structural equation model | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Item | General Neuroticism | Anxiety | Anger | Depression | Self-consciousness | Immoderation | Vulnerability |
Worry about things | .32 | .50 | |||||
Fear for the worst | .49 | .41 | |||||
Am afraid of many things | .55 | .31 | |||||
Get stressed out easily | .53 | .59 | |||||
Get angry easily | .33 | .77 | |||||
Get irritated easily | .45 | .63 | |||||
Lose my temper | .42 | .69 | |||||
Am not easily annoyed | .26 | .26 | |||||
Often feel blue | .65 | .40 | |||||
Dislike myself | .73 | .05 | |||||
Am often “down in the dumps” | .70 | .54 | |||||
Feel comfortable with myself (-) | .51 | -0.02 | |||||
Find it difficult to approach others | .46 | .51 | |||||
Am afraid to draw attention to myself | .29 | .41 | |||||
Only feel comfortable with friends | .37 | .53 | |||||
Am not bothered by difficult social situations (-) | .16 | .12 | |||||
Go on binges (overeating, gambling, etc.) | .36 | .35 | |||||
Rarely overindulge (-) | .14 | .45 | |||||
Easily resist temptations (-) | .06 | .47 | |||||
Am able to control my cravings (-) | .29 | .47 | |||||
Panic easily | .48 | .38 | |||||
Get overwhelmed by events | .49 | .58 | |||||
Feel that I can’t deal with things | .61 | .15 | |||||
Remain calm under pressure (-) | .35 | .40 | |||||
Coefficient estimates from regression models | |||||||
Self-rated health |
−0.55*
[−0.72, −0.38] p < .001 |
−0.58 [−1.92, 0.77] p = .401 |
0.01 [−0.25, 0.27] p = .944 |
0.70 [−0.46, 1.86] p = .239 |
−0.25 [−0.57, 0.07] p = .129 |
−0.68*
[−1.17, −0.20] p = .005 |
4.63 [−4.93, 14.19] p = .342 |
Heart condition | −0.010 [−0.06, 0.05] p = .827 |
0.02 [−0.28, 0.32] p = .895 |
0.00 [−0.07, 0.06] p = .890 |
0.10 [−0.13, 0.33] p = .400 |
0.05 [−0.03, 0.14] p = .201 |
0.02 [−0.09, 0.12] p = .753 |
0.64 [−1.64, 2.92] p = .580 |
Number chronic illnesses | 0.10 [−0.05, 0.24] p = .203 |
0.3 [−0.52, 1.12] p = .475 |
0.02 [−0.16, 0.20] p = .819 |
−0.16 [−0.78, 0.46] p = .616 |
0.16 [−0.09, 0.40] p = .204 |
0.48*
[0.15, 0.80] p = .004 |
−1.52 [−7.05, 4.02] p = .591 |
Ever smoked | 0.06 [−0.02, 0.14] p = .145 |
−0.1 [−0.51, 0.31] p = .632 |
0.03 [−0.07, 0.12] p = .581 |
−0.13 [−0.42, 0.16] p = .369 |
−0.02 [−0.14, 0.11] p = .811 |
0.14 [−0.02, 0.30] p = .082 |
−1.24 [−5.02, 2.54] p = .520 |
Number cigarettes |
0.18*
[0.04, 0.32] p = .013 |
−0.25 [−0.97, 0.47] p = .495 |
0.08 [−0.07, 0.24] p = .307 |
0.04 [−0.49, 0.58] p = .870 |
−0.05 [−0.26, 0.16] p = .614 |
−0.01 [−0.28, 0.26] p = .923 |
−1.02 [−6.49, 4.45] p = .715 |
Ever drank | 0.31 [−0.34, 0.96] p = .351 |
2.59 [−4.05, 9.23] p = .445 |
0.02 [−0.41, 0.46] p = .914 |
0.72 [−0.72, 2.16] p = .328 |
1.44 [−1.65, 4.53] p =.360 |
2.11 [−2.49, 6.7] p = .368 |
−4.12 [−13.64, 5.40] p = .396 |
Alcohol (days per week) | −0.08 [−0.4, 0.24] p = .609 |
−0.41 [−2.34, 1.52] p = .678 |
0.18 [−0.26, 0.63] p = .423 |
0.42 [−1.18, 2.02] p = .607 |
0.05 [−0.44, 0.54] p = .834 |
0.05 [−0.66, 0.75] p = .898 |
3.99 [−9.01, 16.99] p = .547 |
Alcohol (drinks per sitting) | 0.14 [−0.11, 0.38] p = .286 |
−1.05 [−2.9, 0.80] p = .267 |
0.17 [−0.21, 0.54] p = .385 |
0.49 [−1.05, 2.03] p = .533 |
−0.18 [−0.61, 0.25] p = .412 |
0.28 [−0.35, 0.91] p = .388 |
4.84 [−8.07, 17.76] p = .462 |
Alcohol (binge per week) |
0.24
[0.00, 0.47] p = .046 |
−1.30 [−3.41, 0.82] p = .229 |
0.17 [−0.25, 0.58] p = .433 |
0.66 [−1.16, 2.48] p = .478 |
−0.16 [−0.60, 0.29] p = .496 |
0.37 [−0.30, 1.04] p = .281 |
6.61 [−8.91, 22.13] p = .404 |
Exercise (strenuous) | −0.28 [−0.61, 0.05] p = .098 |
−0.33 [−2.03, 1.37] p = .704 |
0.09 [−0.14, 0.33] p = .425 |
−0.33 [−0.73, 0.06] p = .100 |
−0.25 [−0.54, 0.05] p = .098 |
−0.36 [−0.8, 0.08] p = .107 |
0.63 [−1.39, 2.64] p = .542 |
Exercise (moderate) | −0.02 [−0.3, 0.26] p = .874 |
0.80 [−1.17, 2.78] p = .426 |
0.01 [−0.23, 0.25] p = .925 |
−0.42*
[−0.82, −0.03] p = .035 |
−0.05 [−0.33, 0.24] p = .739 |
−0.44 [−0.89, 0.01] p = .057 |
−0.90 [−3.28, 1.48] p = .457 |
Exercise (mild) | −0.10 [−0.38, 0.18] p = .488 |
0.41 [−1.00, 1.83] p = .567 |
0.01 [−0.19, 0.21] p = .926 |
−0.33 [−0.67, 0.00] p = .048 |
−0.11 [−0.35, 0.13] p = .390 |
−0.41*
[−0.79, −0.03] p = .036 |
−0.28 [−1.95, 1.40] p = .747 |
Flossing | 0.05 [−0.23, 0.33] p = .720 |
−0.15 [−1.55, 1.26] p = .839 |
−0.08 [−0.28, 0.11] p = .403 |
0.29 [−0.05, 0.63] p = .098 |
0.05 [−0.19, 0.29] p = .685 |
0.37*
[0.01, 0.73] p = .042 |
0.29 [−1.4, 1.97] p = .739 |
Blood glucose levels | 0.26 [−0.03, 0.54] p = .080 |
1.20 [−0.81, 3.22] p = .242 |
−0.03 [−0.24, 0.17] p =.745 |
−0.71 [−1.80, 0.38] p = .199 |
0.00 [−0.36, 0.36] p = .985 |
0.36 [−0.15, 0.87] p = .163 |
−1.55 [−3.97, 0.88] p = .211 |
Cholesterol/HDL ratio | −0.03 [−0.32, 0.26] p = .833 |
1.03 [−0.67, 2.73] p = .236 |
−0.05 [−0.25, 0.15] p = .631 |
−0.26 [−1.12, 0.61] p = .559 |
0.00 [−0.30, 0.31] p = .988 |
0.60*
[0.08, 1.11] p = .025 |
−1.36 [−3.36, 0.64] p = .182 |
Systolic blood pressure | 0.05 [−0.20, 0.30] p = .707 |
0.20 [−0.84, 1.25] p = .702 |
0.08 [−0.06, 0.22] p = .273 |
−0.40 [−1.02, 0.22] p = .210 |
0.01 [−0.23, 0.24] p = .957 |
0.32 [−0.04, 0.68] p = .957 |
−0.64 [−1.98, 0.70] p = .082 |
Waist to hip ratio |
0.34*
[0.09, 0.59] p = .008 |
−0.01 [−0.97, 0.96] p = .990 |
−0.01 [−0.17, 0.15] p = .927 |
0.35 [−0.20, 0.90] p = .209 |
0.24 [−0.05, 0.53] p =.105 |
0.99*
[0.43, 1.55] p = .001 |
−0.13 [−1.33, 1.07] p =.834 |
Regression coefficients and 95% confidence intervals are presented. All models control for gender and age.
*Indicates p < .05.
Discussion
The current study sought to identify indicators of healthy neuroticism by examining multiple potential moderators of neuroticism and health and the role of facets of neuroticism, which may have competing associations with health. Even when examining both adulthood and childhood personality and using a variety of health indicators, there was no evidence that the relationship between neuroticism and health depends on levels of conscientiousness, SES, or chronic disease status. Moreover, there is no evidence that facets of neuroticism are associated with health in differing directions.
We examined 19 interactions for each combination of neuroticism and moderators, with only a few significant effects found. We chose not to interpret these significant effects because we do not believe they represent true population effects but are instead the result of sampling variability and chance. This point is underscored by the fact that some of these effects are in the direction opposite of what would have been predicted. For example, greater childhood conscientiousness is associated with stronger relationships between child neuroticism and adult waist-to-hip ratio, suggesting neuroticism is unhealthier at higher levels of conscientiousness, rather than less. This is in contradiction to other studies of neuroticism and conscientiousness [15, 18]. Irregularities across these studies may be due to differences in inclusion criteria; the current study removed a greater number of participants to maintain consistency across analyses. If this is the case, contradictions in results underscores the current study: these interactions may be nonexistent or too small to reliably detect. In summation, the current study provided no substantial evidence to support the healthy neuroticism theory.
Statistical analyses cannot disprove a theory, and we acknowledge that there is still much to explore regarding the role of neuroticism on health. For example, many descriptions of healthy neuroticism refer to the adaptability of vigilance [13, 15, 18, 44, 45], yet little research on trait vigilance and its relationship to personality can be found [46]. Similarly, neuroticism is widely accepted to be a trait that refers to fluctuating patterns of emotion and reactivity [47]. Examinations of emotional response and the ways in which emotion may be adaptive could also illuminate pathways from neuroticism to longevity. The potential benefits of neuroticism may also require delving deeper into a given context; for instance, it may be important to consider whether neuroticism predicts diabetes-specific adherence behavior (i.e., checking insulin counts) for individuals with diabetes. Such tests may provide better fidelity for considering the effects of neuroticism for individuals with a chronic condition. Finally, researchers may wish to consider whether traits such as anxiety hold nonlinear associations with health, as shown for personality and performance [48], and whether trait moderations of neuroticism may occur at the facet level (e.g., organization, industriousness, and self-control instead of conscientiousness). Indeed, personality and health researchers may need to think outside the box and beyond the self-report questionnaire to untangle these relationships.
However, we do caution the creative researcher. Theories of healthy neuroticism arose from the need to address heterogeneity in findings from a literature defined by large panel studies. This heterogeneity may be due to an important psychological variable or process that is inconsistently sampled across studies, or this heterogeneity may be due to random noise. We will never be able to say with certainty that it is the latter, so we may need to decide on standards of credibility for the former. Along these lines, we advocate for emphasis on base rates of potential moderators or processes. The examination of the high-conscientiousness, high-neuroticism combination is unlikely to yield many significant results, given that these traits tend to be negatively correlated with each other [15]. Panel studies may be needed to garner sample sizes sufficiently large to examine this combination, but this should influence our interpretation of the phenomenon as a whole. That is, if these large studies are needed to isolate these individuals, then this combination is an unlikely candidate for explaining heterogeneity across studies.
It is unclear why other studies have found interactions between neuroticism and variables of interest while the current study did not. The current study is limited by its relatively small sample size, compared with other studies of neuroticism and mortality, and we may lack the power to detect these interactions. It may be that published studies suffer from file drawer problems. More specifically, by focusing on single outcomes—such as mortality or smoking—instead of providing a comprehensive investigation, these studies fail to see the larger pattern of null results. Indeed, in the current study, one of our 19 interactions between child neuroticism and conscientiousness was statistically significant. If we had chosen to examine only that model, the current study might provide more favorable evidence for a moderator-based theory. In addition, it would be valuable to have an earlier self-assessment of neuroticism that occurred prior to the accumulation of the health conditions, and some reliabilities for the childhood measure evidenced less-than-ideal reliability. Finally, we were unable to examine some of the outcomes previously studied, such as interleukin-6 or mortality [13, 15]. Perhaps any effects of healthy neuroticism are more limited to these outcomes.
Overall, the current study searches for and ultimately fails to find any indication of healthy neuroticism, even when examining the most likely moderators of neuroticism and health, neuroticism across the lifespan, and the facets of neuroticism. Future research should consider other pathways through which neuroticism may influence health, including state-level processes.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This research was supported, in part, by the National Institute on Aging grants R01AG020048, awarded to S. E. Hampson, Principal Investigator; P01-AG043362, awarded to Scott M. Hofer, Principal Investigator, and D. K. Mroczek, Co-Investigator and Project Leader; and R01-AG018436, awarded to D. K. Mroczek, Principal Investigator. The authors gratefully acknowledge the contributions of members of the clinical assessment team at Kaiser Permanente Center for Health Research, Hawaii.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Authors’ Disp-Quote of Conflict of Interest and Adherence to Ethical Standards Authors Sara J. Weston, Patrick L. Hill, Grant W. Edmonds, Daniel K. Mroczek, and Sarah E. Hampson declare that they have no conflict of interest. All procedures, including the informed consent process, were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. Authors have full control over primary data and agree to allow the journal to review their data if requested.
Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Authors' Contributions Sara J. Weston performed all statistical analyses and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. Patrick L. Hill and Daniel K. Mroczek contributed to the writing of the manuscript. Grant W. Hill and Sarah E. Hampson collected all data.
References
- 1. Goodwin RD, Friedman HS. Health status and the five-factor personality traits in a nationally representative sample. J Health Psychol. 2006;11(5):643–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Roberts BW, Kuncel NR, Shiner R, Caspi A, Goldberg LR. The power of personality: the comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007;2(4):313–345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Weston SJ, Hill PL, Jackson JJ. Personality traits predict the onset of disease. Soc Psychol Pers Sci. 2015;6(3):309–317. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Hampson SE, Friedman HS.. Personality and Health: A Lifespan Perspective. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 5. Almada SJ, Zonderman AB, Shekelle RB, et al. . Neuroticism and cynicism and risk of death in middle-aged men: The Western Electric Study. Psychosom Med. 1991;53:165–175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Huppert FA, Whittington JE. Symptoms of psychological distress predict 7-year mortality. Psychol Med. 2009;25(05):1073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Iwasa H, Masui Y, Gondo Y, Inagaki H, Kawaai C, Suzuki T. Personality and all-cause mortality among older adults dwelling in a Japanese community: a five-year population-based prospective cohort study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16(5):399–405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Korten AE, Jorm AF, Jiao Z, et al. . Health, cognitive, and psychosocial factors as predictors of mortality in an elderly community sample. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1999;53(2):83–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Lang FR, Weiss D, Gerstorf D, Wagner GG. Forecasting life satisfaction across adulthood: benefits of seeing a dark future?Psychol Aging. 2012;28:249–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Weiss A, Costa PT Jr. Domain and facet personality predictors of all-cause mortality among Medicare patients aged 65 to 100. Psychosom Med. 2005;67(5):724–733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Brickman AL, Yount SE, Blaney NT, Rothberg ST, De-Nour AK. Personality traits and long-term health status. The influence of neuroticism and conscientiousness on renal deterioration in type-1 diabetes. Psychosomatics. 1996;37(5):459–468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Ragland DR, Brand RJ. Type A behavior and mortality from coronary heart disease. N Engl J Med. 1988;318(2):65–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Friedman HS. Long-term relations of personality and health: dynamisms, mechanisms, tropisms. J Pers. 2000;68(6):1089–1107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Edmonds GW, Hampson S, Goldbweg L, Digman J, Dubanoski J, Oshiro C.. The Hawaii Personality and Health Cohort, 1959–1967: Childhood Personality Data. 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 15. Turiano NA, Mroczek DK, Moynihan J, Chapman BP. Big 5 personality traits and interleukin-6: evidence for “healthy Neuroticism” in a US population sample. Brain Behav Immun. 2013;28:83–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Turiano NA, Whiteman SD, Hampson SE, Roberts BW, Mroczek DK. Personality and substance use in midlife: conscientiousness as a moderator and the effects of trait change. J Res Pers. 2012;46(3):295–305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Bogg T, Roberts BW. Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: a meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychol Bull. 2004;130(6):887–919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Weston SJ, Jackson JJ. Identification of the healthy neurotic: personality traits predict smoking after disease onset. J Res Pers. 2015;54:61–69. [Google Scholar]
- 19. Costa PT Jr, McCrae RR. Neuroticism, somatic complaints, and disease: is the bark worse than the bite?J Pers. 1987;55(2):299–316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Smith TW, O’Keeffe JL, Allred KD. Neuroticism, symptom reports, and type A behavior: interpretive cautions for the Framingham scale. J Behav Med. 1989;12(1):1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Cuijpers P, Smit F, Penninx BW, de Graaf R, ten Have M, Beekman AT. Economic costs of neuroticism: a population-based study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(10):1086–1093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Jerram KL, Coleman PG. The big five personality traits and reporting of health problems and health behaviour in old age. Br J Health Psychol. 1999;4:181–192. [Google Scholar]
- 23. Elliot AJ, Turiano NA, Chapman BP. Socioeconomic status interacts with conscientiousness and neuroticism to predict circulating concentrations of inflammatory markers. Ann Behav Med. March 2017;51:1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Hagger-Johnson G, Roberts B, Boniface D, et al. . Neuroticism and cardiovascular disease mortality: socioeconomic status modifies the risk in women (UK Health and Lifestyle Survey). Psychosom Med. 2012;74(6):596–603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Costa PT, Jr, McCrae RR. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R™) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 26. Schulz R, Beach SR, Ives DG, Martire LM, Ariyo AA, Kop WJ. Association between depression and mortality in older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study. Arch Intern Med. 2000;160(12):1761–1768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Barefoot JC, Dahlstrom WG, Williams RB Jr. Hostility, CHD incidence, and total mortality: a 25-year follow-up study of 255 physicians. Psychosom Med. 1983;45(1):59–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Assari S. Psychiatric disorders differently correlate with physical self-rated health across ethnic groups. J Pers med. 2017;7(4):6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Kitayama S, Park J, Boylan JM, et al. . Expression of anger and ill health in two cultures: an examination of inflammation and cardiovascular risk. Psychol Sci. 2015;26(2):211–220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Sutin AR, Ferrucci L, Zonderman AB, Terracciano A. Personality and obesity across the adult life span. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011;101(3):579–592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Terracciano A, Sutin AR, McCrae RR, et al. . Facets of personality linked to underweight and overweight. Psychosom Med. 2009;71(6):682–689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Löckenhoff CE, Sutin AR, Ferrucci L, Costa PT Jr. Personality traits and subjective health in the later years: the association between NEO-PI-R and SF-36 in advanced age is influenced by health status. J Res Pers. 2008;42(5):1334–1346. [Google Scholar]
- 33. Hampson SE. Personality processes: mechanisms by which personality traits “get outside the skin”. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63(1):315–339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Goldberg LR. Analyses of Digman’s child-personality data: derivation of Big-Five factor scores from each of six samples. J Pers. 2001;69:709–743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Edmonds GW, Goldberg LR, Hampson SE, Barckley M. Personality stability from childhood to midlife: relating teachers’ assessments in elementary school to observer- and self-ratings 40 years later. J Res Pers. 2013;47(5):505–513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Hampson SE, Edmonds GW, Barckley M, Goldberg LR, Dubanoski JP, Hillier TA. A Big Five approach to self-regulation: personality traits and health trajectories in the Hawaii longitudinal study of personality and health. Psychol Health Med. 2016;21(2):152–162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Hill PL, Edmonds GW, Hampson SE. A purposeful lifestyle is a healthful lifestyle: linking sense of purpose to self-rated health through multiple health behaviors. J Health Psychol. 2017. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/1359105317708251 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 38. Hampson SE, Goldberg LR, Vogt TM, Hillier TA, Dubanoski JP. Using physiological dysregulation to assess global health status: associations with self-rated health and health behaviors. J Health Psychol. 2009;14(2):232–241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
- 40.Jackman S.2017. github.com/atahk/pscl pscl: Classes and Methods for R Developed in the Political Science Computational Laboratory.
- 41. Rosseel Y. lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2):1–36. [Google Scholar]
- 42. Chen FF, Hayes A, Carver CS, Laurenceau JP, Zhang Z. Modeling general and specific variance in multifaceted constructs: a comparison of the bifactor model to other approaches. J Pers. 2012;80(1):219–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. McAbee ST, Oswald FL, Connelly BS. Bifactor models of personality and college student performance: a broad versus narrow View. Eur J Pers. 2014;28(6):604–619. [Google Scholar]
- 44. Hill PL, Roberts BW. Personality and health. In: Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. Elsevier; 2016:205–218 Edited by K Warner Schaie and Sherry L Willis. [Google Scholar]
- 45. Mroczek DK, Spiro A, Turiano N. Do health behaviors explain the effect of neuroticism on mortality? Longitudinal findings from the VA normative aging study. J Res Pers. 2009;43(4):653–659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Weston SJ, Jackson JJ. The role of vigilance in the relationship between neuroticism and health: a registered report. J Res Pers. 2018;73:27–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Goldberg LR. The structure of phenotypic personality traits. Am Psychol. 1993;48(1):26–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Le H, Oh IS, Robbins SB, Ilies R, Holland E, Westrick P. Too much of a good thing: curvilinear relationships between personality traits and job performance. J Appl Psychol. 2011;96(1):113–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.