Skip to main content
Conservation Physiology logoLink to Conservation Physiology
. 2019 Jan 15;7(1):coy067. doi: 10.1093/conphys/coy067

Bears habituate to the repeated exposure of a novel stimulus, unmanned aircraft systems

Mark A Ditmer 1,, Leland K Werden 2, Jessie C Tanner 3, John B Vincent 4, Peggy Callahan 5, Paul A Iaizzo 6, Timothy G Laske 6, David L Garshelis 1,7
Editor: Steven Cooke
PMCID: PMC6331175  PMID: 30680216

Drones are powerful new tools used in several biological sciences. Previous work indicated that animals behave fearfully or show a stress response near drone flights. Using heart monitors to gauge stress, we found that bears habituated to drones after ~20 flights over a 3–4-week period and remained habituated.

Keywords: Drone, cardiac biologger, habituation, stress, bears, unmanned aerial system

Abstract

Unmanned aircraft systems (UAS; i.e. ‘drones’) provide new opportunities for data collection in ecology, wildlife biology and conservation. Yet, several studies have documented behavioral or physiological responses to close-proximity UAS flights. We experimentally tested whether American black bears (Ursus americanus) habituate to repeated UAS exposure and whether tolerance levels persist during an extended period without UAS flights. Using implanted cardiac biologgers, we measured heart rate (HR) of five captive bears before and after the first of five flights each day. Spikes in HR, a measure of stress, diminished across the five flights within each day and over the course of 4 weeks of twice-weekly exposure. We halted flights for 118 days, and when we resumed, HR responses were similar to that at the end of the previous trials. Our findings highlight the capacity of a large mammal to become and remain habituated to a novel anthropogenic stimulus in a relatively short time (3–4 weeks). However, such habituation to mechanical noises may reduce their wariness of other human threats. Also, whereas cardiac effects diminished, frequent UAS disturbances may have other chronic physiological effects that were not measured. We caution that the rate of habituation may differ between wild and captive animals: while the captive bears displayed large initial spikes in HR change (albeit not as large as wild bears), these animals were accustomed to regular exposure to humans and mechanical noises that may have hastened habituation to the UAS.

Introduction

The human footprint continues to expand, reducing available habitat and increasing the frequency of interactions between wildlife and human activities (Venter et al., 2016). Investigations of anthropogenic effects on wildlife often rely on interpreting animal behavioral responses. Recent advances in biologging technology offer the capability to sense the physiological responses of wildlife that may not be apparent from behavioral responses alone (Ditmer et al., 2015).

The popularity of unmanned aircraft systems (UAS; i.e. ‘drones’) among recreationalists, researchers and conservationists has increased tremendously in recent years (Anderson and Gaston, 2013) and represents a new potential stress to wildlife (Ditmer et al., 2015; Mulero-Pázmány et al., 2017). As hurdles to use of UAS are eased (Vincent et al., 2015; Werden et al., 2015), UAS technology is seeing more use in population surveys (Seymour et al., 2017; Witczuk et al., 2017; Hodgson et al., 2018), collection of biological samples (Wolinsky, 2017; Domínguez‐Sánchez et al., 2018) and fine-scale habitat data (Olsoy Peter et al., 2018), observations of behaviors (Schofield et al., 2017; Barnas et al., 2018b) and curbing poaching (Mulero-Pázmány et al., 2015; O’Donoghue and Rutz, 2016). In the near future, UAS will be able to track animals using thermal signatures or VHF tags (Bayram et al., 2016, 2017; Cliff et al., 2015, 2018) and UAS swarms will be used to automatically identify multiple individuals or search larger areas for the presence of wildlife efficiently (Allan et al., 2018).

However, UAS may disturb animals more than other aerial survey methods due to the very nature of what makes these devices useful: the ability to fly and hover at low altitudes. Indeed, numerous studies have observed responses of wildlife to UAS (Pomeroy et al., 2015; Vas et al., 2015; Brisson-Curadeau et al., 2017; Mulero-Pázmány et al., 2017; Barnas et al., 2018a) and the noise may impact non-target species (Scobie and Hugenholtz, 2016). Work by our group (Ditmer et al., 2015) documented acute stress responses in wild American black bears (Ursus americanus) to low-altitude UAS flights. Bears in the study often did not display behavioral signs of fear (e.g. fleeing) but in extreme cases, their heart rates nearly quadrupled (162 beats per minute) compared to pre-flight baseline data (41 beats per minute). Here, we conducted experiments to address three follow-up questions that arose from our original study: (1) do bears habituate to the presence of UAS, (2) if so, over what timescale does tolerance develop and (3) does tolerance persist in the absence of exposure to the stimulus? Chronic impacts of the use of UAS are of biological and ethical concern, especially if UAS are used in wildlife surveys designed to minimally disturb animals.

Methods

Field methods

We flew an Iris+ model quadcopter UAS (3D Robotics, Berkeley, CA, USA) over five adult captive American black bears (three females and two males) fenced together in a 372-m2 facility maintained by the Wildlife Science Center (WSC) in east-central Minnesota. The WSC is an educational, non-profit organization that provides husbandry and regular veterinary care to bears used for educational and research purposes. The study subjects had varied backgrounds prior to being housed at the WSC; three came from other captive facilities, two from the wild. The WSC hosts tours, and staff often work around the enclosure, so the bears were accustomed to both human presence and mechanical noises, such as lawnmowers.

We measured responses of bears using an implanted cardiac biologger developed for human use (Medtronic plc, Reveal XT—Generation2 with BearWare, Minnesota, USA; see Laske et al. 2018 for device details). We are cognizant that both the implantation of these devices and the experimental UAS flights constituted a disturbance to these bears. Wildlife research often causes some amount of disturbance or discomfort to study subjects that needs to be weighed against scientific gains (Putman, 1995; McMahon et al., 2012). Our research team has successfully implanted and deployed hundreds of these cardiac biologgers in wild American black bears to gain insights about bear physiology, responses to environmental stressors, and advancements in human medicine (Laske et al., 2018). The miniaturization of the cardiac biologging devices (Wilson et al., 2015; Laske et al., 2018), along with their tested use in humans, combined with the extraordinary healing abilities of bears (Iaizzo et al., 2012), enabled the animals to quickly and fully recover from device implantation. We are consistently looking to improve our methods to reduce the potential harm to individuals. Moreover, we have tracked a number of individual wild bears with implanted cardiac biologgers over successive years and checked their health annually, finding that they showed no differences compared to non-implanted bears in terms of body condition and reproduction (Laske et al., 2017, 2018). Given the rapidly expanding use of UAS as a tool to study a variety of aspects of wildlife ecology, we believed that the use of the biologgers was warranted because they enabled us to investigate physiological responses that otherwise would not have been possible, and our findings can directly help to inform best practices that could reduce animal disturbance in the long run (McMahon et al., 2012).

In March 2016, bears were anesthetized as part of an animal handling course at the University of Minnesota with a mixture of Ketamine–Xylazine and a biologger was implanted subcutaneously in a peristernal location using aseptic techniques. The devices recorded each heartbeat and provided 2-min averages of heart rate (HR) in beats per minute (bpm). Data from the devices were downloaded telemetrically when the bears were anesthetized a year later for transport to a new WSC facility. Later, we matched the timing of UAS flights with date/time stamps on the HR data.

We conducted experimental UAS flights during two seasons, April–May 2016 (spring) and September–October 2016 (fall). A Federal Aviation Administration-certified pilot flew the UAS over the bears’ enclosure twice-weekly (3 or 4 days apart) for four consecutive weeks in each season, in accordance with our Certificate of Authorization (2015-CSA-150-COA). Each flight day consisted of five, 5-min flights, with 10-min pauses between flights (4 weeks each season × 2 days per week × 5 flights per day = 40 flights per season). The flight plan for all 80 flights included three distinct locations over the enclosure where the UAS hovered in place (‘loitering’) for 30 s. We designed the flight path to maximize the areal coverage of the enclosure. On each flight, the UAS completed this circuit three times at an altitude of 15 m before returning to the launch site, which was not visible to the bears. From the edge of the bear enclosure, we recorded the maximum sound pressure levels (dB SPL; re 20 μPa, Root Mean Square (RMS), A-weighted) during 4 flights using an Extech model 407 750 sound level meter. The average maximum value measured for all loitering and transition maneuvers was 60.3 dB (range 54.3–70.4 dB).

We operated our UAS at a low altitude, specifically aiming to elicit a cardiac response from which we could monitor a process of habituation (attenuated response) over time following repeated exposure. Our primary concern was not to determine the distance or flight approach that causes a physiological response. While we acknowledge that UAS operators rarely fly this close to target species, we chose to do so because we anticipated that the captive bears, living in an environment with regular human disturbances, might require a greater stimulus to achieve the same physiological reaction as wild bears in our previous study (Ditmer et al., 2015), which was the baseline we were attempting to emulate. Because the HR data were not retrieved until the study was over, we could not adapt the study design to responses of the bears and hence needed to ensure that the initial flights over the bears would elicit a response.

Furthermore, we note that in selecting flight altitudes, UAS operators must balance the tradeoff between sought-after image resolution and avoiding disturbance to wildlife species, but this is complicated by the fact that disturbance distance is unknown for many species due to varying aural capacities and hearing thresholds (Scobie and Hugenholtz, 2016). Additionally, the noise of the UAS operations on any given flight can be impacted by ambient conditions and the approach path of the UAS (Vas et al., 2015; Mulero-Pázmány et al., 2017; Rümmler et al., 2018) and species may respond physiologically but with no apparent behavioral change (Ditmer et al., 2015).

Methods were approved by the University of Minnesota’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (Protocol # 1002A77516) and were conducted in accordance with all relevant guidelines and regulations. Animal husbandry practices at the WSC were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (UMN-005).

Statistical methods

We calculated the 95% confidence interval of HR for each bear during the 30-min period prior to the first flight of each flight day and used the upper values as the pre-disturbance baseline. We calculated differences between baselines and the maximum HRs while the UAS was in flight for each of the five flights per day (MaxHRDiff). We created two linear mixed models using R package nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2017; R Core Team, 2018) with MaxHRDiff as the response variable in each. In Model 1, we tested whether the cardiac responses to each initial flight on a given flight day changed through time both within and between seasons. We used the first flight of each day because bears showed the strongest cardiac response to that flight. We regressed the MaxHRDiff of each first flight with Julian date and the interaction (Julian date × season) while accounting for individual differences using a random intercept based on bear identity and a random slope for Julian date. Our second model tested whether HRs changed within flight days or between the two seasons. We regressed MaxHRDiff with repetition number and the interaction of repetition number × season. We included a random intercept for bear identity. Two of the study bears were in a side enclosure during our first day of flights, so we excluded this day of data on these two bears from the analysis. We considered the bears to be habituated if the mean MaxHRDiff of the five bears was ≤10 bpm for consecutive flight days and did not subsequently increase for more than a day.

Results

All bears showed at least one strong HR elevation in response to the presence of the UAS overhead (max. increase above baseline [MaxHRDiff] observed during any flight exposures: X¯ = 52.7, range = 41−73 bpm). Responses to the initial flights each day diminished through the spring season (Fig. 1; Julian: βˆ = −1.38, SE = 0.28, P value <0.001) and bears were considered habituated by the third week of flights (flight days 5 and 6; Fig. 1). Although it was not until the fourth week (flight days 7 and 8) when most individuals exhibited HR increases <10 bpm (Fig. 1). Anecdotal behavioral observations corroborate these findings (Supplementary Video S1). When flights resumed in the fall, HR responses to first flights of each day were similar to those at the end of the spring season (Fig. 1; season [fall]: βˆ = −155.84, SE = 81.3, P-value 0.059;). Exposure days through the fall had much smaller effects than in spring (Julian × season [fall]: βˆ = 1.25, SE = 0.38, P value 0.002).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Differences between pre-flight baseline (95% upper CI of HR 30-min prior to first UAS flight of each day) and maximum HRs of five captive black bears (each point is a single bear; horizontal bars represent means of the bears) during exposure to the first UAS flight on each flight day (i.e. the flight eliciting the greatest response; see Fig. 2). We flew a quadcopter UAS 15 m over the bears’ enclosure 2 days per week, 4 weeks per season during spring and fall (16 flight-days). We considered habituation to have occurred when the mean of maximum elevations in HR remained below +10 bpm (dashed line). This occurred on flight day 5 (third week).

Bear HR responses moderated from the first to fifth flight within each flight day during the spring (Fig. 2; βˆ = Rep.n = 2.5 = −4.40, −9.07, −10.37, −9.67, SE = 3.54, P value Rep.n = 2.5 = 0.21, 0.011, 0.004, 0.007). Fall flights (not just first flights) elicited smaller responses than spring flights (season [fall]: βˆ = −7.91, SE = 3.49, P-value 0.024), and the minor changes in HRs during fall flights did not differ among repetitions within days (βˆ = season [fall] × Rep.n = 2.5 = 3.11, 5.46, 5.82, 5.02, SE = 4.94, P value Rep. 2.5 = 0.53, 0.27, 0.24, 0.31).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Example of diminishing cardiac responses (difference from baseline [95% upper CI of HR 30-min prior to first UAS flight of each day]) of one of five captive American black bears to repeated UAS flights. Bears were exposed to 80 flights: five times per day on eight flight days in both spring and fall. Each point is the HR response to one of these eight flights (bars represent means) grouped by the first to fifth flight of each day for each season separately.

Discussion

Black bears showed clear signs of increased tolerance to the UAS flights (Question 1), both short-term (within individual days comprising five flights, spanning 75 min; Fig. 2) and long-term (>20 accumulated flights over 3–4 weeks; Fig. 1; Question 2). Additionally, their tolerance to the flights was maintained after a hiatus of >3 months (Question 3), providing strong evidence of habituation to this previously foreign stressor (Bejder et al., 2009).

The use of biologging technology has increased and enabled researchers to address increasingly more complex questions about the physiological responses of animals subjected to diverse anthropogenic stimuli (Wilmers et al., 2015; Wilson et al., 2015; Madliger et al., 2018). Our experimental approach of pairing cardiac biologger technology with captive individuals enabled us to address novel questions by collecting physiological data at very fine time-scales (2-min averages) and repeatedly exposing individuals to the UAS stimulus at a regular schedule, which would not have been possible to execute with bears in the wild. The integration of physiological data into management and conservation is promising but nascent (Wilson et al., 2015; McGowan et al., 2017); here, we aimed to integrate these unique sources of data for improving the best practices of UAS use in wildlife conservation and research.

Given that wildlife are already repeatedly exposed to UAS for both research and conservation purposes, it is useful to know that bears could become habituated to frequent flights within a period of just a few weeks. Further UAS technological advancements will soon enable autonomous obstacle avoidance under forest canopies, regular tracking of VHF-tagged individuals (Bayram et al., 2017; Cliff et al., 2015, 2018) and multiple UAS working simultaneously to search out individuals (Allan et al., 2018), all of which suggest increased disturbance to wildlife. Whereas our results indicated that the initial stress response attenuated, meaning the stimulus became less disturbing, this could entail other maladaptive consequences. For example, bears may key on road noise to alert them to the danger of crossing roads (Ditmer et al., 2018); their waning response to other mechanical sounds may reduce their wariness and expose them to increased risks.

The rate of habituation is likely to be species dependent. Bears in general habituate to frequent contact with people, showing less fear after repeated exposure (Beckmann and Berger, 2003; Wheat and Wilmers, 2016); accordingly, bears may be predisposed to becoming tolerant of novel disturbances. Furthermore, our study subjects likely already had high tolerances for human activities, due to regular exposure to mechanical equipment, human interactions, and nearby vehicular traffic. Indeed, these captive bears initially responded less to UAS exposure than did wild bears—although the difference was not as large as might be expected given the background noises in their captive setting (max HR increase range among wild bears = 47–123 bpm [Ditmer et al., 2015]; max HR increase range among captive bears = 44–78 bpm). Also, the rate of habituation in our study was likely more rapid than in the wild, where interaction with UAS may be less frequent. However, we note that the frequency of flights used in our study may not be extreme in comparison to situations where UAS are used as anti-poaching tools (Mulero-Pázmány et al., 2014; O’Donoghue and Rutz, 2016), or where individuals are regularly surveyed in confined areas (e.g. haulouts [Pomeroy et al., 2015], nests [Weissensteiner et al., 2015], colonies [Hodgson et al., 2018; Ratcliffe et al., 2015] and island species [Ballaria et al., 2016]). Additionally, as UAS flight times and communication among UAS improves with technological advances, UAS will commonly be used to repeatedly record behavioral observations (Allan et al., 2018).

While our approach utilizing biologgers enabled us to examine fine temporal-scale cardiac changes, we echo the cautions of Bejder et al. (2009) against extrapolating this single metric of habituation to potential physiological responses that were not measured. Given the unknown chronic effects of continual disturbance (Wright et al., 2007), we strongly recommend that UAS users follow proper ethical guidelines (Hodgson and Koh, 2016) when operating these aircraft near wildlife. We also call on researchers to increase efforts to mitigate behavioral and stress responses, for example, by using quieter, fixed-wing craft when possible and conducting data collection at a sensor’s maximum useful distance.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Data

Acknowledgment

The authors thank the Wildlife Science Center and all their employees and volunteers for their support.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Conservation Physiology online.

Funding

University of Minnesota’s Institute on the Environment provided funding for the field work and unmanned aerial vehicles. Medtronic Plc donated the cardiac biologgers used in this study where T.G.L. is an employee and P.A.I. is a consultant.

References

  1. Allan BM, Nimmo DG, Ierodiaconou D, VanDerWal J, Koh LP, Ritchie EG (2018) Futurecasting ecological research: the rise of technoecology. Ecosphere 9: e02163. [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson K, Gaston KJ (2013) Lightweight unmanned aerial vehicles will revolutionize spatial ecology. Front Ecol Environ 11: 138–146. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ballaria D, Orellana D, Acostaa E, Espinoza A, Morocho V (2016) UAV monitoring for environmental management in Galapagos Islands. In The International Archives of Photogrammetry, Remote Sensing and Spatial Information Sciences. Gottingen. Copernicus GmbH, Gottingen, Germany, Gottingen, pp 1105–1111.
  4. Barnas AF, Felege CJ, Rockwell RF, Ellis-Felege SN (2018. b) A pilot(less) study on the use of an unmanned aircraft system for studying polar bears (Ursus maritimus). Polar Biol 41: 1055–1062. [Google Scholar]
  5. Barnas A, Newman R, Felege CJ, Corcoran MP, Hervey SD, Stechmann TJ, Rockwell RF, Ellis-Felege SN (2018. a) Evaluating behavioral responses of nesting lesser snow geese to unmanned aircraft surveys. Ecol Evol 8: 1328–1338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bayram H, Doddapaneni K, Stefas N, Isler V (2016) Active localization of VHF collared animals with aerial robots. In 2016 IEEE International Conference on Automation Science and Engineering (CASE), pp 934–939.
  7. Bayram H, Stefas N, Engin KS, Isler V (2017) Tracking wildlife with multiple UAVs: System design, safety and field experiments. In 2017 International Symposium on Multi-Robot and Multi-Agent Systems (MRS), pp 97–103.
  8. Beckmann JP, Berger J (2003) Rapid ecological and behavioural changes in carnivores: the responses of black bears (Ursus americanus) to altered food. J Zool 261: 207–212. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bejder L, Samuels A, Whitehead H, Finn H, Allen S (2009) Impact assessment research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 395: 177–185. [Google Scholar]
  10. Brisson-Curadeau É, Bird D, Burke C, Fifield DA, Pace P, Sherley RB, Elliott KH (2017) Seabird species vary in behavioural response to drone census. Sci Rep 7: 17884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cliff OM, Fitch R, Sukkarieh S, Saunders DL, Heinsohn R (2015) Online localization of radio-tagged wildlife with an autonomous aerial robot system. In Proceedings of Robotics: Science and Systems. Rome, Italy; (2015). [Google Scholar]
  12. Cliff OM, Saunders DL, Fitch R (2018) Robotic ecology: tracking small dynamic animals with an autonomous aerial vehicle. Sci Robot 3: eaat8409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ditmer MA, Rettler SJ, Fieberg JR, Iaizzo PA, Laske TG, Noyce KV, Garshelis DL (2018) American black bears perceive the risks of crossing roads. Behav Ecol 29: 667–675. [Google Scholar]
  14. Ditmer MA, Vincent JB, Werden LK, Tanner JC, Laske TG, Iaizzo PA, Garshelis DL, Fieberg JR (2015) Bears show a physiological but limited behavioral response to unmanned aerial vehicles. Curr Biol 25: 2278–2283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Domínguez‐Sánchez CA, Acevedo‐Whitehouse KA, Gendron D (2018) Effect of drone‐based blow sampling on blue whale (Balaenoptera musculus) behavior. Mar Mammal Sci. 10.1111/mms.12482. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hodgson JC, Koh LP (2016) Best practice for minimising unmanned aerial vehicle disturbance to wildlife in biological field research. Curr Biol 26: R404–R405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hodgson JC, Mott R, Baylis SM, Pham TT, Wotherspoon S, Kilpatrick AD, Raja Segaran R, Reid I, Terauds A, Koh LP (2018) Drones count wildlife more accurately and precisely than humans. Methods Ecol Evol. . [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Iaizzo PA, Laske TG, Harlow HJ, McCLAY CB, Garshelis DL (2012) Wound healing during hibernation by black bears (Ursus americanus) in the wild: elicitation of reduced scar formation. Integr Zool 7: 48–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Laske TG, Evans AL, Arnemo JM, Iles TL, Ditmer MA, Fröbert O, Garshelis DL, Iaizzo PA (2018) Development and utilization of implantable cardiac monitors in free-ranging American black and Eurasian brown bears: system evolution and lessons learned. Anim Biotelem 6: 13. [Google Scholar]
  20. Laske TG, Iaizzo PA, Garshelis DL (2017) Six years in the life of a mother bear—The longest continuous heart rate recordings from a free-ranging mammal. Sci Rep 7: 40732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Madliger CL, Love OP, Hultine KR, Cooke SJ, Franklin C (2018) The conservation physiology toolbox: status and opportunities. Conserv Physiol 6, 10.1093/conphys/coy029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. McGowan J, Beger M, Lewison RL, Harcourt R, Campbell H, Priest M, Dwyer RG, Lin H-Y, Lentini P, Dudgeon C, et al. (2017) Integrating research using animal-borne telemetry with the needs of conservation management. J Appl Ecol 54: 423–429. [Google Scholar]
  23. McMahon CR, Harcourt R, Bateson P, Hindell MA (2012) Animal welfare and decision making in wildlife research. Biol Conserv 153: 254–256. [Google Scholar]
  24. Mulero-Pázmány M, Barasona JÁ, Acevedo P, Vicente J, Negro JJ (2015) Unmanned aircraft systems complement biologging in spatial ecology studies. Ecol Evol 21: 4808–4818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Mulero-Pázmány M, Jenni-Eiermann S, Strebel N, Sattler T, Negro JJ, Tablado Z (2017) Unmanned aircraft systems as a new source of disturbance for wildlife: A systematic review. PLoS One 12: e0178448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Mulero-Pázmány M, Stolper R, van Essen LD, Negro JJ, Sassen T (2014) Remotely piloted aircraft systems as a rhinoceros anti-poaching tool in Africa. PLoS One 9: e83873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Olsoy Peter J, Shipley Lisa A, Rachlow Janet L, Forbey Jennifer S, Glenn Nancy F, Burgess Matthew A, Thornton Daniel H (2018) Unmanned aerial systems measure structural habitat features for wildlife across multiple scales. Methods Ecol Evol 9: 594–604. McMahon Sean. [Google Scholar]
  28. O’Donoghue P, Rutz C (2016) Real-time anti-poaching tags could help prevent imminent species extinctions. J Appl Ecol 53: 5–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, R Development Core Team (2017) Nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models.
  30. Pomeroy P, O’Connor L, Davies P (2015) Assessing use of and reaction to unmanned aerial systems in gray and harbor seals during breeding and molt in the UK. J Unmanned Veh Syst 3: 102–113. [Google Scholar]
  31. Putman RJ. (1995) Ethical considerations and animal welfare in ecological field studies. Biodivers Conserv 4: 903–915. [Google Scholar]
  32. R Core Team (2018) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.
  33. Ratcliffe N, Guihen D, Robst J, Crofts S, Stanworth A, Enderlein P (2015) A protocol for the aerial survey of penguin colonies using UAVs. J Unmanned Veh Syst 3: 95–101. [Google Scholar]
  34. Rümmler M-C, Mustafa O, Maercker J, Peter H-U, Esefeld J (2018) Sensitivity of Adélie and Gentoo penguins to various flight activities of a micro UAV. Polar Biol. 10.1007/s00300-018-2385-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Schofield G, Katselidis KA, Lilley MKS, Reina RD, Hays GC (2017) Detecting elusive aspects of wildlife ecology using drones: new insights on the mating dynamics and operational sex ratios of sea turtles. Funct Ecol 31: 2310–2319. [Google Scholar]
  36. Scobie CA, Hugenholtz CH (2016) Wildlife monitoring with unmanned aerial vehicles: Quantifying distance to auditory detection. Wildl Soc Bull 40: 781–785. [Google Scholar]
  37. Seymour AC, Dale J, Hammill M, Halpin PN, Johnston DW (2017) Automated detection and enumeration of marine wildlife using unmanned aircraft systems (UAS) and thermal imagery. Sci Rep 7: 45127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Vas E, Lescroël A, Duriez O, Boguszewski G, Grémillet D (2015) Approaching birds with drones: first experiments and ethical guidelines. Biol Lett 11: 20140754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Venter O, Sanderson EW, Magrach A, Allan JR, Beher J, Jones KR, Possingham HP, Laurance WF, Wood P, Fekete BM, et al. (2016) Sixteen years of change in the global terrestrial human footprint and implications for biodiversity conservation. Nat Commun 7: 12558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Vincent JB, Werden LK, Ditmer MA (2015) Barriers to adding UAVs to the ecologist’s toolbox. Front Ecol Environ 13: 74–75. [Google Scholar]
  41. Weissensteiner MH, Poelstra JW, Wolf JBW (2015) Low-budget ready-to-fly unmanned aerial vehicles: an effective tool for evaluating the nesting status of canopy-breeding bird species. J Avian Biol 46: 425–430. [Google Scholar]
  42. Werden LK, Vincent JB, Tanner JC, Ditmer MA (2015) Not quite free yet: clarifying UAV regulatory progress for ecologists. Front Ecol Environ 13: 534–535. [Google Scholar]
  43. Wheat RE, Wilmers CC (2016) Habituation reverses fear-based ecological effects in brown bears (Ursus arctos). Ecosphere 7: e01408. [Google Scholar]
  44. Wilmers CC, Nickel B, Bryce CM, Smith JA, Wheat RE, Yovovich V (2015) The golden age of bio-logging: how animal-borne sensors are advancing the frontiers of ecology. Ecology 96: 1741–1753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Wilson ADM, Wikelski M, Wilson RP, Cooke SJ (2015) Utility of biological sensor tags in animal conservation. Conserv Biol 29: 1065–1075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Witczuk J, Pagacz S, Zmarz A, Cypel M (2017) Exploring the feasibility of unmanned aerial vehicles and thermal imaging for ungulate surveys in forests—preliminary results. Int J Remote Sens 0: 1–18. [Google Scholar]
  47. Wolinsky H. (2017) Biology goes in the air. EMBO Rep 18: 1284–1289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wright AJ, Aguilar Soto N, Baldwin AL, Bateson M, Beale CM, Clark C, Deak T, Edwards EF, Fernández A, Godinho A, et al. (2007) Anthropogenic noise as a stressor in animals: a multidisciplinary perspective. Int J Comp Psychol 20: 250–273. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Data

Articles from Conservation Physiology are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES