Skip to main content
Translational Psychiatry logoLink to Translational Psychiatry
. 2019 Jan 17;9:18. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0361-x

Two novel genomic regions associated with fearfulness in dogs overlap human neuropsychiatric loci

R Sarviaho 1,2,3,#, O Hakosalo 1,2,3,#, K Tiira 1,2,3,4, S Sulkama 1,2,3, E Salmela 1,2,3,5, M K Hytönen 1,2,3, M J Sillanpää 6, H Lohi 1,2,3,
PMCID: PMC6336819  PMID: 30655508

Abstract

Anxiety disorders are among the leading health issues in human medicine. The complex phenotypic and allelic nature of these traits as well as the challenge of establishing reliable measures of the heritable component of behaviour from the associated environmental factors hampers progress in their molecular aetiology. Dogs exhibit large natural variation in fearful and anxious behaviour and could facilitate progress in the molecular aetiology due to their unique genetic architecture. We have performed a genome-wide association study with a canine high-density SNP array in a cohort of 330 German Shepherds for two phenotypes, fear of loud noises (noise sensitivity) and fear of strangers or in novel situations. Genome-widely significant loci were discovered for the traits on chromosomes 20 and 7, respectively. The regions overlap human neuropsychiatric loci, including 18p11.2, with physiologically relevant candidate genes that contribute to glutamatergic and dopaminergic neurotransmission in the brain. In addition, the noise-sensitivity locus includes hearing-related candidate genes. These results indicate a genetic contribution for canine fear and suggest a shared molecular aetiology of anxiety across species. Further characterisation of the identified loci will pave the way to molecular understanding of the conditions as a prerequisite for improved therapy.

Introduction

Anxiety disorders include a large spectrum of heterogeneous conditions and rank among the most common health concerns in human medicine with a lifetime prevalence of > 15%1. Better understanding of the biological background of anxiety is essential due to current lack of efficient treatment and therapy. Anxiety disorders are heritable but genetically complex, which emphasizes the need of various approaches for identifying genetic determinants, including comparative physiologically relevant animal models1,2.

Since the annotation of their genome, domestic dogs have emerged as powerful models for genetic studies due to their unique genetic architecture combined with a large body size, extensive breed variety and the presence of hundreds of naturally occurring morphological, behavioural and disease phenotypes3. Over 230 genes have been recently implicated in various canine Mendelian disorders and traits4,5 and successful examples include also discoveries for several complex phenotypes such as cancer6, autoimmune disease7 and size8. Although discoveries in canine behaviour are still rare and largely suggestive, candidate loci and genes have been reported for obsessive-compulsive disorder9,10, social behaviour11,12, fear and aggression13. The relative scarcity of the genetic discoveries in behaviour is likely due to the complex phenotypic and allelic nature of the traits, as well as the challenge of establishing reliable measures of the heritable component of behaviour from the associated environmental factors14.

Fear is an evolutionarily important and conserved emotional state crucial for fitness and survival in all animals. Increased fearfulness in dogs can, however, cause several behavioural problems. Many anxiety-related behavioural conditions, such as generalized anxiety disorders, phobias and separation anxiety are seen and diagnosed in dogs and comprise substantial welfare issues15. Fear often motivates aggressiveness16, and bite injuries resulting from dog or human-directed aggression can be considered a serious health concern. In dogs, fearfulness can be categorised based on the object and the situation in which the behaviour occurs into social and non-social fearfulness17. The social category includes fear of unfamiliar people and dogs, whereas the non-social category includes fear of different objects such as new situations, loud noises (known as noise phobia or noise sensitivity (NS)), heights, or shiny and slippery floors16.

Fear of loud noises, such as thunder, gunshot or fireworks, is often referred to as noise phobia because of the strong panic reactions that are seen in some dogs18. However, in many cases the actual behavioural reactions are milder, and fear of loud noises is thus considered (and referred to) as NS19. Although NS and general fearfulness are somewhat overlapping, there are many otherwise non-fearful dogs with extreme NS20, and NS is considered an independent veterinary behavioural diagnosis21,22. Despite the high prevalence of NS across breeds that varies from 25 to 49%15,16,23, the pathophysiology is not known. In humans, sound sensitivity includes various conditions such as hyperacusis, phonophobia and misophonia24,25, which are sensitivities to certain frequencies, intensities or types of sounds, associated with strong feelings of dislike, fear or even aggression. Although environmental factors such as trauma may be responsible for some noise-sensitivity cases, both human and canine noise sensitivities have high heritability estimates (0.40 and 0.56, respectively), suggesting a substantial genetic component to this trait26,27.

Fear of strangers and of new situations are highly correlated in dogs16. Both traits are considered to be signs of generalized fear21,22. Dogs vary in their response to novel situations and unfamiliar people, with reactions ranging from extreme fearfulness to high sociability and curiosity28. Environmental factors such as lack of socialization, poor maternal care and aversive learning are known risk factors for canine fear16,28,29. On the other hand, high heritability estimates have been reported for fearfulness (from 0.36 to 0.49), suggesting a substantial genetic component to this trait30,31.

As a part of our larger effort to understand the molecular aetiology of canine anxiety28,32,33, we have performed here a genome-wide association study of 330 German Shepherds and identified novel loci for NS and fearfulness on chromosomes 20 and 7, respectively. These two loci overlap regions that have been mapped for human neuropsychiatric traits and harbour functional candidate genes that affect glutamatergic and dopaminergic neurotransmission and the hearing system.

Materials and methods

Study cohorts

Privately owned Finnish German Shepherd dogs originating from both working and show breeding lines were recruited to the study using a previously-published and validated owner-completed behavioural survey focusing on fearfulness and NS16,20. The breed was chosen for its known large variation in reacting to loud noises, strangers and novel situations (shyness–boldness personality). Besides detailed demographics, the owners were asked about their dogs’ reactions in various specific situations such as when meeting unfamiliar people, in new situations, or when hearing loud noises (thunder, fireworks and gunshot). The owners were also asked to specify the observed behaviour and its frequency.

Altogether 330 German Shepherd dogs were phenotyped (Fig. 1) using our behavioural questionnaire. For the NS cohort, dogs were categorized to cases and controls based on the noise reactivity score (NRS)16. The NRS describes the frequency and intensity of a fearful reaction towards loud noise and was calculated as follows: (sum of fearful behavioural reactions to fireworks) × (frequency of fear reaction to fireworks) + (sum of fearful behavioural reactions to thunder) × (frequency of fear reaction to thunder) + (sum of fearful behavioural reactions to gunshot) × (frequency of fear reaction to gunshot) (Supplementary Table 1). The range of possible NRS values is from 0 to 156 with intervals of 1. Dogs showing the most severe, generalized and frequent fear reactions gain the highest scores. The NRS of the control dogs in the study cohort was 0 and the dogs were over 5 years old. The owners of the mildest cases (scores 1–3) were contacted by phone or email to confirm the NS status. The age of onset was available only for 32 (out of 91) NS cases. In these dogs the age of onset varied from three months to ten years with a median age of onset of 4 years. In a larger study cohort (N > 3000 dogs) only 15.1% of the dogs were reported with NS onset of over 5 years16. According to our behavioural survey data, the owners of eight controls were not aware how their dogs react to gunshot (these dogs were not afraid of thunder or fireworks). This is only 3.8% of all our controls. Additionally, there were altogether 13 cases without reported exposure to gunshots.

Fig. 1. Distribution of the 330 dogs in the genetic analysis of fear and NS.

Fig. 1

The 330 German Shepherd dogs included in the study were categorised into noise sensitivity, fear and the respective control groups. Several dogs were included in two different groups

For the fear cohort, the dogs were categorized according to a fear reaction score (FRS), which was based on the dogs’ reactions (intensity and frequency) towards unfamiliar people and new situations. Fear of strangers and new situations are highly correlated. Our previous study shows that the more often the dog shows fearful behaviour towards strangers, the more often it also shows fear in new situations16. Fear towards strange dogs was not included in the score for fearfulness, since it is heavily affected by dogs’ experiences of other dogs and their level of training. The FRS was calculated as an average score as follows: ((sum of fearful behavioural reactions when meeting unfamiliar people, where withdrawal was multiplied by 5) × (frequency of fear reactions when meeting unfamiliar people) + (sum of fearful behavioural reactions in novel situations) × (frequency of fear reactions in novel situations)) × 0.5 (Supplementary Table 2). Avoidance was emphasized in the scoring by a factor of 5 because it is a common, clear and strong fear reaction, easily recognized by owners34. Avoidance is the only behaviour among the possible fear reactions where the dog can be interpreted to flee the object of fear, and where the dog’s sole motivation for the observed behaviour can be interpreted to be fear for that object. For behaviours such as barking or growling, the motivation can also be protective. Additionally, avoidance or withdrawal is the most frequent option marked in our questionnaire according to our previous study16. The range of possible FRS values is from 0 to 42 with intervals of 0.5. The FRS value for control dogs in the fear study cohort was 0 and none showed fear towards loud noise. All case and control dogs were required to be older than one year, as the canine personality has been found to be stable from one year onwards35.

For the Extended Bayesian Lasso analysis, the NRS and FRS scores were additionally categorized into five groups. In this categorization all control dogs had a value of 0, while case dogs were divided into four categories with even intervals.

Samples

EDTA-blood samples were collected from 330 privately owned German Shepherd dogs for DNA and the samples were stored at −20 °C until genomic DNA was extracted using a semi-automated Chemagen extraction robot (PerkinElmer Chemagen Technologie). DNA concentration was determined either with a Qubit 3.0 Fluorometer (Thermo Fisher Scientific) or a NanoDrop ND-1000 UV/Vis Spectrophotometer. Sample collection was ethically approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of State Provincial Office of Southern Finland, Hämeenlinna, Finland (ESAVI/6054/04.10.03/ 2012).

Genome-wide association study

Altogether 330 German Shepherds were genotyped using Illumina’s Canine HD (173k) SNP arrays (Fig. 1). A total of 91 cases and 210 controls were included in the analysis of NS and 80 cases and 193 controls in fear (Fig. 1). The same set of control dogs was partially utilized in the fear study, excluding the dogs that had any fear reactions. In addition, thirteen control dogs that were too young to be included as controls in the NS study were used as controls in the fear study (Fig. 1). The genotype data were filtered with a SNP call rate of > 90%, array call rate of > 90%, minor allele frequency of > 1% and by using a Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium of P ≥ 1 × 10–8. No individual dogs were removed and 94 552 (NS) or 94 434 (fear) SNPs remained for analysis. Genotyping data was analysed using various statistical methods including single-locus association analysis (PLINK)36, single-locus mixed model approaches of Grammar-gamma and FASTA implemented in R package GenABEL37 and a multilocus Extended Bayesian Lasso (EBL) method3840. The PLINK and GenABEL analyses were performed using a quantitative trait approach for the given phenotype measurements. In EBL, an ordinal categorical phenotype of five classes was used.

Existence of potential confounding signals due to cryptic relatedness and population stratification was evaluated from multidimensional scaling (MDS) and quantile–quantile (q–q) plots in the study populations (Fig. 2). The data were adjusted for relatedness and population stratification either by including a polygenic effect term with marker-estimated relationship matrix to the statistical model (GenABEL)41 or adjusting by genomic control approach (PLINK)42. In PLINK and GenABEL, the statistical significance of loci was tested using Bonferroni-corrected threshold value accounting for multiple testing. The significance level after correcting for multiple testing was 5.4E-07 in the genome-wide association analysis (GWAS) of NS (94,552 tests) and 5.4E-07 in the GWAS of fear (94,434 tests).

Fig. 2. Multidimensional scaling and quantile–quantile plots for the NS and fear cohorts.

Fig. 2

a MDS plot for NS b q–q plot for NS (λ = 1.022). c MDS plot for fear. d q–q plot for fear (λ = 1.020) The study cohorts include show and working line German Shepherds, which are seen in separate genetic clusters. Working line dogs are grouped on the left side of the plot while show line dogs are seen on the right, with dogs with a mixed heritage in the middle

In the multilocus EBL model, the false signals due to cryptic relatedness do not need to be separately corrected39,4345. Following Kärkkäinen and Sillanpää 201239, to reduce number of explanatory variables before EBL analysis, we performed sure-independence-screening46, where 5000 best-ranking SNPs were selected out of the original SNPs. Estimation in EBL was performed using in-house C + + /R program implementing Generalized Expectation-Maximization algorithm to find Bayesian point-estimates for quantitative trait locus (QTL) effects39,40,47. In the Bayesian approach, priors need to be specified. Hyper-parameters of the prior distributions were tuned for this specific data set by using the values that seemed to provide reasonable-looking outputs while not being exceedingly sensitive to small changes of given parameter values.

Throughout the study, the CanFam 3.1 annotation was used.

Results

Altogether 330 German Shepherds were carefully phenotyped for two behavioural traits, NS and fear of strangers or novel situations (fear), using quantitative categorical “noise reactivity” and “fear reaction” scores (NRS and FRS). The scores were established in order to be able to evaluate the intensity of the phenotype. The NRS values of NS cases varied between 1 and 60 with a median value of 12 and an average of 15.5 while controls had a value of 0 (Fig. 3a). In dogs that were used as cases in the fear cohort, FRS varied between 0.5 and 13.5 with a median value of 2 and an average of 3.5, while all controls had a value of 0 (Fig. 3b). The study cohorts included both show and working line German Shepherds, which clustered separately in MDS plots (Fig. 2a, c). The genomic inflation factors (λ) were 1.022 and 1.020 for the NS and fear cohorts, respectively, suggesting low levels of population stratification (Fig. 2b, d).

Fig. 3. Distribution of the noise reactivity and fear reaction scores.

Fig. 3

The noise (a) and fear (b) reactivity scores in the noise sensitivity (n = 301) and fear (n = 273) cohorts were distributed from 0 to 60 or from 0 to 13.5, respectively. In both cohorts, the dogs with a score of 0 were used as controls

The phenotypic scores were not normally distributed (Fig. 3) and therefore, the data were analysed using three different approaches. First, a quantitative analysis with the NRS and FRS scores was performed using PLINK and GenABEL, erroneously assuming a normal distribution. To further assess which findings replicate, a multilocus Extended Bayesian Lasso (EBL) analysis, not expecting a normal distribution, was performed for an ordinal categorical phenotype in five subgroups (0–4). Replication in categorical data analysis gives information on whether the earlier PLINK & GenABEL findings are false positive due to non-normal phenotype distribution, or true positive resulting from real phenotype associations.

NS maps to chromosome 20

To map loci for NS, dogs were grouped to cases and controls based on the NRS (Fig. 3a). Altogether 301 German Shepherds were genotyped, including 91 cases and 210 control dogs. The quantitative PLINK and GenABEL GWA analyses revealed a statistically significant association on chromosome 20 after Bonferroni correction (Fig. 4a, b). The two best-associated SNPs with genome-wide significance span a region from 9,451,007 to 9,731,317 bp on CFA20 (Supplementary Table 3). The EBL analysis revealed the best signal in the same region (Fig. 4c), further supporting the fact that the association in chromosome 20 is true and not an artefact due to non-normal phenotype distribution. The locus includes several candidate genes for neuropsychiatric and hearing-related phenotypes, including the oxytocin receptor gene OXTR, a Rho GTPase gene SRGAP3, a metabotropic glutamate receptor gene GRM7 and a plasma membrane calcium ATPase gene ATP2B2.

Fig. 4. GWAS for NS.

Fig. 4

a A single-locus-analysed Manhattan plot (PLINK) indicates best p-values at chromosome 20 in a genome-wide analysis. b A Manhattan plot of a mixed model association analysis (GenABEL) indicates best p-values at chromosome 20 in a genome-wide analysis. c An Extended Bayesian Lasso method indicates the best signal (posterior QTL-effect) in chromosome 20. d The associated genomic region includes several known anxiety and hearing-related candidate genes

Fear towards strangers and new situations maps to chromosome 7

To map loci for fear towards strangers and new situations, dogs were categorised to cases and controls based on the FRS (Fig. 3b). Examples of a fearful and a non-fearful dog in a behavioural test are shown in Supplementary Video 1. Altogether 273 German Shepherds were genotyped, including 80 cases and 193 control dogs. Both PLINK and GenABEL GWA analyses revealed a significant association on chromosome 7 after Bonferroni correction (Fig. 5a, b). The two best-associated SNPs span from 75,935,406 to 77,639,464 bp on CFA7 (Supplementary Table 4). The EBL analysis revealed several signals in various chromosomes, however, the best signal was seen in chromosome 7 (Fig. 5c), suggesting that the association found by frequentist approaches is true. The associated region is largely syntenic to a human 18p11.2 locus that has been linked to neuropsychiatric disorders. In addition, an isolated single SNP showing association was seen on chromosome 3 (BICF2G630701448, p = 1.93E-08).

Fig. 5. GWAS for fear.

Fig. 5

a A single-locus-analysed Manhattan plot (PLINK) indicates best p-values at chromosome 7 in a genome-wide analysis. b A Manhattan plot of a mixed model association analysis (GenABEL) indicates best p-values at chromosome 7 in a genome-wide analysis. c An Extended Bayesian Lasso method indicates the best signal (posterior QTL-effect) in chromosome 7. d The associated genomic region is syntenic to the human 18p11.2 locus that has been associated with various psychiatric disorders

The patterns of the background noise in the PLINK and GenABEL analyses are rather similar in both NS and fear analyses (Figs. 4, 5). We expect that this phenomenon is a consequence of the non-normal phenotype distribution, which shows up as similar background noise patterns. Since the Bayesian analyses support the PLINK and GenABEL signals in both traits, we believe our findings do not occur because of non-normality of phenotype distributions.

Discussion

We have identified two novel loci for fearfulness in German Shepherd dogs. The significance of the findings is highlighted by the fact that these loci overlap genomic regions that include candidate genes that participate in the regulation of the neuronal glutamate and dopamine systems and have been linked to both neuropsychiatric illnesses and hearing defects. Although larger replication studies are necessary to confirm the findings, this study demonstrates the genetic susceptibility to fear in dogs and facilitates the search for underlying pathophysiology, which may be shared across species.

NS is a common trait in dogs with up to 50% of individuals across breeds suffering from it16. Noise-sensitive dogs react to sounds in several ways, most commonly by panting, pacing, by having their tails between their legs, trembling or escaping, but reactions may also include salivation, urination or destroying16. Dogs with NS have previously been suggested to be natural models for human panic disorder, exhibiting some physiological similarities and shared symptoms with human patients18. However, while panic disorder does not have a specific focus of fear, NS does, which composes a marked difference between the two disorders. A recent study suggested a slight change in the auditory response in NS-affected dogs48, and other animal models have supported a link between noise-induced or genetic hearing loss and loudness intolerance25,49. It has also been proposed that dogs with NS might feel pain as a result of loud noise50 and many symptoms of NS do resemble the way dogs react to pain16.

Canine and human noise sensitivities appear to share similar features, but both genetic and morphological backgrounds of these conditions remain poorly characterized. Human sound sensitivity disorders include hyperacusis, misophonia and phonophobia24,25,49. Hyperacusis refers to decreased sound tolerance, in which the subjects can feel for example annoyance, fear, panic, rage, physical discomfort or pain due to sounds that are perceived as (too) loud. Misophonia is characterized by abnormally strong psychological reactions of annoyance or even rage towards context-related, soft or loud sounds, while in phonophobia certain sounds cause strong fear reactions. Sound intolerance is also a feature in some human neuropsychiatric conditions, such as autism spectrum disorders, in which auditory hypersensitivity is one of the major complications51.

The NS locus on chromosome 20 includes candidate genes that have been linked to human neuropsychiatric conditions and the hearing system. The candidate genes closest to the best-associated markers include the oxytocin receptor gene OXTR, a Rho GTPase gene SRGAP3 and a metabotropic glutamate receptor gene GRM7.

OXTR encodes a receptor for oxytocin, which has been studied widely as a neural modulator of mammalian behaviour, including maternal, social and stress behaviours in both humans and dogs52,53. Studies also suggest that oxytocin and OXTR play a role in psychiatric disorders, such as anxiety disorders and autism5457. In dogs, OXTR polymorphisms have been widely linked to human-directed social behaviour, greeting behaviour, proximity seeking and friendliness5860 but so far not to NS or anxiety. SRGAP3 regulates the actin cytoskeleton, is involved in neuronal development and, based on mouse models, is crucial for the normal cognitive behaviour61. SRGAP3, also known as ‘Mental disorder-associated GAP protein’ MEGAP, has been linked to severe mental retardation62 and a schizophrenia-related phenotype63.

GRM7 encodes a G-protein-coupled metabotropic glutamate receptor subtype 7, known as mGlu7 or mGluR764. The glutamate system, including mGlu7, seems to have a prominent role in anxiety-related behaviour and the link is supported by several genetic and functional studies6468. In addition to the neuropsychiatric phenotypes, GRM7 has been linked to age-related hearing loss and impairment in several studies69. Further downstream of the best-associated SNPs resides another hearing-related candidate gene, a plasma membrane calcium ATPase gene ATP2B2 that plays a role in maintaining the Ca2+ balance vital for the proper functioning of the hearing sense70. PMCA2 dysfunctions have been linked to deafness and partial, noise-induced or progressive hearing loss in human and mice studies71, but also autism70,72, and it is suggested to have a role in nociception73. Since a role of possible hearing defects in canine NS has been suggested, the hearing-related genes lying in the NS associated locus could propose a novel target for future investigations.

The novel canine locus for fear against strangers and new situations on chromosome 7 is largely syntenic to human locus 18p11.2. This region has been repeatedly linked to psychiatric illnesses such as bipolar disease and schizophrenia74,75. Different anxiety disorders and fear are highly co-morbid with schizophrenia, but also with other psychiatric diseases, such as bipolar disorder76. The prevalence of anxiety disorders such as panic disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, generalized anxiety disorder and social anxiety disorder are increased among both bipolar and schizophrenia patients when compared to the general population77,78. Mapping canine fear to a locus syntenic to the human 18p11 that has been strongly linked to schizophrenia and bipolar disorder is an intriguing finding. It has been previously suggested that dogs with extremely fearful, shy and nervous behaviour towards unfamiliar situations or humans could share similarities with some symptoms or characteristics of schizophrenia18. Although the fearful dogs in this study exhibit normal variation of canine fearfulness rather than a pathological extremity, the finding suggests similar genetic background for canine fearfulness and some human psychiatric diagnoses with a strong component of anxiety and fear.

The CFA7 locus includes several interesting candidate genes. The genes include GNAL, a gene that encodes a stimulatory G-protein alpha subunit and has been linked to the dopaminergic neurotransmission pathway79. Interesting candidate genes also include IMPA2, a myoinositol monophosphatase gene that has been studied as a candidate for bipolar disorder80 and schizophrenia81. Three more genes in the region, metallophosphoesterase 1 (MPPE1), a SNAP-gamma gene NAPG and the vesicle-associated membrane protein-associated protein A (VAPA) have all been suggested to be candidate genes for bipolar disorder8284. NDUFV2, encoding a subunit of the inner mitochondrial enzyme complex I, has been linked to bipolar disorder, major depression, schizophrenia and Parkinson’s disease85,86. Other possible, neuronally relevant candidate genes in the fear-associated locus in CFA7 that lie outside the human 18p11 locus include a myocin gene MYO5B that has been linked to schizophrenia and bipolar disorder87,88.

A single-SNP association was seen on chromosome 3 (at 13,525,675 bp) in the GWAS of fear. Closest genes to this isolated SNP include a pro-protein convertase gene PCSK1 and an elongation factor gene ELL2, neither of which have been linked to anxiety in any species. This finding may represent a spurious association and requires a replication prior to further focus.

The genetics of canine behaviour have been studied using several differing approaches within or across breeds using clinic-, survey- or breed standard-based behavioural measures. Beside this study, successful examples with associations at genome-wide significance include canine compulsion9,10 and interspecies interaction11, while many other associations have remained at suggestive level. No significant associations have been reported to NS before our study, but a suggestive single variant association was reported by Ilska et al.14 Zapata et al.13 identified loci for fear and aggression in a cross-breed setup with averaged phenotypes, but the loci do not overlap with ours. The approach in Zapata et al. markedly differs from our within-breed analysis with individual phenotypic scores and the studies thus likely render two different views to the genetics of canine fear.

In summary, we have discovered two novel loci for canine anxiety. The loci harbour several relevant candidate genes that may contribute to predisposition to fear. Of particular interest are the genes that regulate glutamatergic and dopaminergic pathways and genes that affect the hearing system. Ongoing research aims to replicate the loci in larger cohorts and characterize the associated regions to find causative variants that would further facilitate the investigations into the molecular mechanisms. There is a current need for large animal models for the development of better anxiolytic drugs and fearful dogs may provide such natural models for human anxieties.

Supplementary information

41398_2018_361_MOESM1_ESM.docx (14KB, docx)

Legends for the supplementary tables and videos

Supplementary Table 1 (105.3KB, docx)
Supplementary Table 2 (120.8KB, docx)
Supplementary Table 3 (13.5KB, docx)
Supplementary Table 4 (13.6KB, docx)
Supplementary Video 1 (383.5MB, mp4)

Acknowledgements

We thank Keijo Kodis, Vesa-Pekka Hirvioja and Eeva Linnala for their support in the recruitment of dogs; Petra Jaakonsaari, Ranja Eklund, Minna Virta and Sini Karjalainen for technical assistance; and all the participating dog owners. The study was supported by the European Research Council, ERC (Grant no: 260997 DOGPSYCH) (https://erc.europa.eu/); Academy of Finland (268019); ERA-NET Neuron Mental Disorders; the Jane and Aatos Erkko Foundation: the Sigrid Juselius Foundation, the Finnish Cultural Foundation, Jenny and Antti Wihuri, Ella and Georg Ehrnrooth Foundation, ILS Doctoral Program of University of Helsinki, the Folkhälsan Institute of Genetics, DVM Doctoral Program of University of Helsinki and University of Oulu.

Author contributions

H.L., O.H. and K.T. designed research. R.S., O.H., K.T. and S.S. performed the experiments. R.S., O.H., M.J.S. and H.L. analysed the data. R.S., O.H., K.T., S.S., E.S., M.K.H., M.J.S. and H.L. contributed to materials and methods. R.S. and H.L. wrote the manuscript with the help from the other authors. All authors read the manuscript, contributed comments and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

These authors contributed equally: Sarviaho R, Hakosalo O

Supplementary information

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at (10.1038/s41398-018-0361-x).

References

  • 1.Erhardt A, Spoormaker VI. Translational approaches to anxiety: focus on genetics, fear extinction and brain imaging. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2013;15:417. doi: 10.1007/s11920-013-0417-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Domschke K, Deckert J. Genetics of anxiety disorders—status quo and quo vadis. Curr. Pharm. Des. 2012;18:5691–5698. doi: 10.2174/138161212803530781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lindblad-Toh K, et al. Genome sequence, comparative analysis and haplotype structure of the domestic dog. Nature. 2005;438:803–819. doi: 10.1038/nature04338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.OMIA - Online Mendelian Inheritance in Animals. (2018). Available at: http://omia.org/home/. Accessed 9 April 2018
  • 5.Hytönen MK, Lohi H. Canine models of human rare disorders. Rare Dis. 2016;4:e1241362. doi: 10.1080/21675511.2016.1241362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Melin M, et al. Genome-wide analysis identifies germ-line risk factors associated with canine mammary tumours. PLoS Genet. 2016;12:e1006029. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wilbe M, et al. DLA Class II alleles are associated with risk for canine symmetrical lupoid onychodystropy (SLO) PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e12332. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rimbault M, et al. Derived variants at six genes explain nearly half of size reduction in dog breeds. Genome Res. 2013;23:1985–1995. doi: 10.1101/gr.157339.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Tang R, et al. Candidate genes and functional noncoding variants identified in a canine model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Genome Biol. 2014;15:R25. doi: 10.1186/gb-2014-15-3-r25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dodman NH, et al. A canine chromosome 7 locus confers compulsive disorder susceptibility. Mol. Psychiatry. 2010;15:8–10. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Persson ME, Wright D, Roth LSV, Batakis P, Jensen P. Genomic regions associated with interspecies communication in dogs contain genes related to human social disorders. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:33439. doi: 10.1038/srep33439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.vonHoldt BM, et al. Structural variants in genes associated with human Williams-Beuren syndrome underlie stereotypical hypersociability in domestic dogs. Sci. Adv. 2017;3:e1700398. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1700398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Zapata I, Serpell JA, Alvarez CE. Genetic mapping of canine fear and aggression. BMC Genom. 2016;17:572. doi: 10.1186/s12864-016-2936-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ilska J, et al. Genetic characterization of dog personality traits. Genetics. 2017;206:1101–1111. doi: 10.1534/genetics.116.192674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Blackwell EJ, Bradshaw JWS, Casey RA. Fear responses to noises in domestic dogs: prevalence, risk factors and co-occurrence with other fear related behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013;145:15–25. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.12.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Tiira K, Sulkama S, Lohi H. Prevalence, comorbidity, and behavioral variation in canine anxiety. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2016;16:36–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.06.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Svartberg, K. Individual differences in behaviour—dog personality. in The behavioural biology of dogs 182–206 (CABI, Wallingford, Oxfordshire, United Kingdom, 2007). 10.1079/9781845931872.0182
  • 18.Overall KL. Natural animal models of human psychiatric conditions: assessment of mechanism and validity. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2000;24:727–776. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(00)00104-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Sherman BL, Mills DS. Canine anxieties and phobias: an update on separation anxiety and noise aversions. Vet. Clin. North Am. Small Anim. Pract. 2008;38:1081–1106. doi: 10.1016/j.cvsm.2008.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Tiira K, Lohi H. Reliability and validity of a questionnaire survey in canine anxiety research. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014;155:82–92. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.03.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Overall, K. L. Manual of clinical behavioral medicine for dogs and cats. (Elsevier Saunders, Philadelphia, 2013).
  • 22.Horwitz, D., Mills, D. S. & British Small Animal Veterinary Association. BSAVA manual of canine and feline behavioural medicine. (British Small Animal Veterinary Association, Gloucester, 2009).
  • 23.Voith, V. L. & Borchelt, P. L. Readings in companion animal behavior. Veterinary Learning Systems, Trenton. 140–152 (1996).
  • 24.Cavanna AE, Seri S. Misophonia: current perspectives. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2015;11:2117. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S81438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Jastreboff PJ, Jastreboff MM. Decreased sound tolerance. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2015;129:375–387. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-62630-1.00021-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.van der Waaij EH, Wilsson E, Strandberg E. Genetic analysis of results of a Swedish behavior test on German Shepherd Dogs and Labrador Retrievers. J. Anim. Sci. 2008;86:2853–2861. doi: 10.2527/jas.2007-0616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Heinonen-Guzejev M, et al. Genetic component of noise sensitivity. Twin. Res. Hum. Genet. 2005;8:245–249. doi: 10.1375/twin.8.3.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Tiira K, Lohi H. Early life experiences and exercise associate with canine anxieties. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0141907. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Scott, J. P. & Fuller, J. L. Genetics and the social behavior of the dog. (The University of Chicago Press Books, Chicago and London, 1965)
  • 30.Goddard ME, Beilharz RG. A multivariate analysis of the genetics of fearfulness in potential guide dogs. Behav. Genet. 1985;15:69–89. doi: 10.1007/BF01071933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Arvelius P, Eken Asp H, Fikse WF, Strandberg E, Nilsson K. Genetic analysis of a temperament test as a tool to select against everyday life fearfulness in Rough Collie. J. Anim. Sci. 2014;92:4843–4855. doi: 10.2527/jas.2014-8169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Tiira K, et al. Environmental effects on compulsive tail chasing in dogs. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e41684. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Puurunen J, et al. A non-targeted metabolite profiling pilot study suggests that tryptophan and lipid metabolisms are linked with ADHD-like behaviours in dogs. Behav. Brain. Funct. 2016;12:27. doi: 10.1186/s12993-016-0112-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Horwitz, D., Mills, D. S., Heath, S. & British Small Animal Veterinary Association. BSAVA manual of canine and feline behavioural medicine. (British Small Animal Veterinary Association, Gloucester, 2002).
  • 35.Fratkin JL, Sinn DL, Patall EA, Gosling SD. Personality consistency in dogs: a meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e54907. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Purcell S, et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2007;81:559–575. doi: 10.1086/519795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Aulchenko YS, Ripke S, Isaacs A, van Duijn CM. GenABEL: an R library for genome-wide association analysis. Bioinformatics. 2007;23:1294–1296. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btm108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Mutshinda CM, Sillanpää MJ. Extended Bayesian LASSO for multiple quantitative trait loci mapping and unobserved phenotype prediction. Genetics. 2010;186:1067–1075. doi: 10.1534/genetics.110.119586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kärkkāinen HP, Sillanpää MJ. Robustness of Bayesian multilocus association models to cryptic relatedness. Ann. Hum. Genet. 2012;76:510–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-1809.2012.00729.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kärkkäinen HP, Sillanpää MJ. Fast genomic predictions via Bayesian G-BLUP and multilocus models of threshold traits including censored Gaussian data. G3 (Bethesda). 2013;3:1511–1523. doi: 10.1534/g3.113.007096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Amin N, van Duijn CM, Aulchenko YS. A genomic background based method for association analysis in related individuals. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e1274. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Devlin B, Roeder K. Genomic control for association studies. Biometrics. 1999;55:997–1004. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.1999.00997.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Iwata H, Uga Y, Yoshioka Y, Ebana K, Hayashi T. Bayesian association mapping of multiple quantitative trait loci and its application to the analysis of genetic variation among Oryza sativa L. germplasms. Theor. Appl. Genet. 2007;114:1437–1449. doi: 10.1007/s00122-007-0529-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Iwata H, Ebana K, Fukuoka S, Jannink JL, Hayashi T. Bayesian multilocus association mapping on ordinal and censored traits and its application to the analysis of genetic variation among Oryza sativa L. germplasms. Theor. Appl. Genet. 2009;118:865–880. doi: 10.1007/s00122-008-0945-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Toosi A, Fernando RL, Dekkers JCM. Genome-wide mapping of quantitative trait loci in admixed populations using mixed linear model and Bayesian multiple regression analysis. Genet. Sel. Evol. 2018;50:32. doi: 10.1186/s12711-018-0402-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Fan J, Lv J. Sure independence screening for ultrahigh dimensional feature space. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Stat. Methodol. 2008;70:849–911. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9868.2008.00674.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kärkkäinen HP, Sillanpää MJ. Back to basics for Bayesian model building in genomic selection. Genetics. 2012;191:969–987. doi: 10.1534/genetics.112.139014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Scheifele PM, Sonstrom KE, Dunham AE, Overall KL. Is noise reactivity reflected in auditory response variables, including those that measure cognition, in dogs? Initial Findings. 2016;16:65–75. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.09.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Manohar S, Spoth J, Radziwon K, Auerbach BD, Salvi R. Noise-induced hearing loss induces loudness intolerance in a rat Active Sound Avoidance Paradigm (ASAP) Hear. Res. 2017;353:197–203. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2017.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Hydbring-Sandberg E, et al. Physiological reactions to fear provocation in dogs. J. Endocrinol. 2004;180:439–448. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1800439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lawson RP, Aylward J, White S, Rees G. A striking reduction of simple loudness adaptation in autism. Sci. Rep. 2015;5:16157. doi: 10.1038/srep16157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Caldwell HK, Aulino EA, Freeman AR, Miller TV, Witchey SK. Oxytocin and behavior: lessons from knockout mice. Dev. Neurobiol. 2017;77:190–201. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Buttner AP. Neurobiological underpinnings of dogs’ human-like social competence: How interactions between stress response systems and oxytocin mediate dogs’ social skills. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016;71:198–214. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kirsch P. Oxytocin in the socioemotional brain: implications for psychiatric disorders. Dialog. Clin. Neurosci. 2015;17:463–476. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2015.17.4/pkirsch. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Costa B, et al. Oxytocin receptor and G-protein polymorphisms in patients with depression and separation anxiety. J. Affect Disord. 2017;218:365–373. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.03.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Moons WG, Way BM, Taylor SE. Oxytocin and vasopressin receptor polymorphisms interact with circulating neuropeptides to predict human emotional reactions to stress. Emotion. 2014;14:562–572. doi: 10.1037/a0035503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Thompson RJ, Parker KJ, Hallmayer JF, Waugh CE, Gotlib IH. Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (rs2254298) interacts with familial risk for psychopathology to predict symptoms of depression and anxiety in adolescent girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:144–147. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Kubinyi E, et al. Oxytocin and opioid receptor gene polymorphisms associated with greeting behavior in dogs. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:1520. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Persson ME, Trottier AJ, Bélteky J, Roth LSV, Jensen P. Intranasal oxytocin and a polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene are associated with human-directed social behavior in golden retriever dogs. Horm. Behav. 2017;95:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Kis A, et al. Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms are associated with human directed social behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris) PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e83993. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Bacon C, Endris V, Rappold GA. The cellular function of srGAP3 and its role in neuronal morphogenesis. Mech. Dev. 2013;130:391–395. doi: 10.1016/j.mod.2012.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Endris V, et al. The novel Rho-GTPase activating gene MEGAP/ srGAP3 has a putative role in severe mental retardation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2002;99:11754–11759. doi: 10.1073/pnas.162241099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Waltereit R, et al. Srgap3−/− mice present a neurodevelopmental disorder with schizophrenia-related intermediate phenotypes. FASEB J. 2012;26:4418–4428. doi: 10.1096/fj.11-202317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Peterlik D, Flor PJ, Uschold-Schmidt N. The emerging role of metabotropic glutamate receptors in the pathophysiology of chronic stress-related disorders. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 2016;14:514–539. doi: 10.2174/1570159X13666150515234920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Peterlik D, et al. Relief from detrimental consequences of chronic psychosocial stress in mice deficient for the metabotropic glutamate receptor subtype 7. Neuropharmacology. 2017;115:139–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.04.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Gee CE, et al. Blocking metabotropic glutamate receptor subtype 7 (mGlu7) via the Venus flytrap domain (VFTD) inhibits amygdala plasticity, stress, and anxiety-related behavior. J. Biol. Chem. 2014;289:10975–10987. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M113.542654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Hazard D, et al. Identification of QTLs for behavioral reactivity to social separation and humans in sheep using the OvineSNP50 BeadChip. BMC Genom. 2014;15:778. doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-15-778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Alliey-Rodriguez N, et al. Genome-wide association study of personality traits in bipolar patients. Psychiatr. Genet. 2011;21:190–194. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e3283457a31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Luo H, et al. Association of GRM7 variants with different phenotype patterns of age-related hearing impairment in an elderly male Han Chinese population. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e77153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Yang W, et al. The evidence for association of ATP2B2 polymorphisms with autism in Chinese Han population. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e61021. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Carpinelli MR, Manning MG, Kile BT, Burt RA, Rachel AB. Two ENU-induced alleles of Atp2b2 cause deafness in mice. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e67479. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Takata A, et al. Integrative analyses of de novo mutations provide deeper biological insights into autism spectrum disorder. Cell Rep. 2018;22:734–747. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.12.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Khariv V, Elkabes S. Contribution of plasma membrane calcium ATPases to neuronal maladaptive responses: focus on spinal nociceptive mechanisms and neurodegeneration. Neurosci. Lett. 2018;663:60–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Meerabux JMA, et al. Analysis of a t(18;21)(p11.1; p11.1) translocation in a family with schizophrenia. J. Hum. Genet. 2009;54:386–391. doi: 10.1038/jhg.2009.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Berrettini WH, et al. Chromosome 18 DNA markers and manic-depressive illness: evidence for a susceptibility gene. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1994;91:5918–5921. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.13.5918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Cosoff SJ, Hafner RJ. The prevalence of comorbid anxiety in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder and bipolar disorder. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry. 1998;32:67–72. doi: 10.3109/00048679809062708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Nabavi B, Mitchell AJ, Nutt D. A lifetime prevalence of comorbidity between bipolar affective disorder and anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis of 52 interview-based studies of psychiatric population. EBioMedicine. 2015;2:1405–1419. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2015.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Buckley PF, Miller BJ, Lehrer DS, Castle DJ. Psychiatric comorbidities and schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2009;35:383–402. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Laurin N, et al. Investigation of the G protein subunit Gαolf gene (GNAL) in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2008;42:117–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Sjøholt G, et al. Examination of IMPA1 and IMPA2 genes in manic-depressive patients: association between IMPA2 promoter polymorphisms and bipolar disorder. Mol. Psychiatry. 2004;9:621–629. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Yoshikawa T, et al. Evidence for association of the myo-inositol monophosphatase 2 (IMPA2) gene with schizophrenia in Japanese samples. Mol. Psychiatry. 2001;6:202–210. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Li X, et al. Association study on the NAPG gene and bipolar disorder in the Chinese Han population. Neurosci. Lett. 2009;457:159–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.03.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Lohoff FW, et al. Association between polymorphisms in the vesicle-associated membrane protein-associated protein A (VAPA) gene on chromosome 18p and bipolar disorder. J. Neural Transm. 2008;115:1339–1345. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0093-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Lohoff FW, Ferraro TN, Brodkin ES, Weller AE, Bloch PJ. Association between polymorphisms in the metallophosphoesterase (MPPE1) gene and bipolar disorder. Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2010;153B:830–836. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Zhang Z, et al. A haplotype in the 5′-upstream region of the NDUFV2 gene is associated with major depressive disorder in Han Chinese. J. Affect Disord. 2016;190:329–332. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.10.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Chen T, Wu Q, Zhang Y, Zhang D. NDUFV2 regulates neuronal migration in the developing cerebral cortex through modulation of the multipolar-bipolar transition. Brain Res. 2015;1625:102–110. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Chen Y, et al. Myosin Vb gene is associated with schizophrenia in Chinese Han population. Psychiatry Res. 2013;207:13–18. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.02.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Sklar P, et al. Whole-genome association study of bipolar disorder. Mol. Psychiatry. 2008;13:558–569. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

41398_2018_361_MOESM1_ESM.docx (14KB, docx)

Legends for the supplementary tables and videos

Supplementary Table 1 (105.3KB, docx)
Supplementary Table 2 (120.8KB, docx)
Supplementary Table 3 (13.5KB, docx)
Supplementary Table 4 (13.6KB, docx)
Supplementary Video 1 (383.5MB, mp4)

Articles from Translational Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES