Abstract
The effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on motor cortical excitability are highly variable between individuals. Inter-individual differences in the electric fields generated in the brain by tDCS might play a role in the variability. Here, we explored whether these fields are related to excitability changes following anodal tDCS of the primary motor cortex (M1). Motor evoked potentials (MEPs) were measured in 28 healthy subjects before and after 20 min sham or 1 mA anodal tDCS of right M1 in a double-blind crossover design. The electric fields were individually modelled based on magnetic resonance images. Statistical analysis indicated that the variability in the MEPs could be partly explained by the electric fields, subjects with the weakest and strongest fields tending to produce opposite changes in excitability. To explain the findings, we hypothesized that the likely locus of action was in the hand area of M1, and the effective electric field component was that in the direction normal to the cortical surface. Our results demonstrate that a large part of inter-individual variability in tDCS may be due to differences in the electric fields. If this is the case, electric field dosimetry could be useful for controlling the neuroplastic effects of tDCS.
Introduction
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a widely used non-invasive method capable of eliciting changes in cortical excitability1–3. These neuroplastic changes have potential as a treatment for various psychiatric and neurological diseases that involve pathological changes in plasticity4,5. Cortical excitability changes induced by tDCS can be most reliably measured in the primary motor cortex (M1) using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to measure the amplitude of the motor evoked potentials (MEP)6. In such studies, the responses to tDCS have been found to be highly variable between individuals7–11. The underlying reasons of the variability are still unknown.
The physical agent of tDCS is thought to be the electric field (EF) that is generated in the brain and other tissues when direct current (usually 1–2 mA) is applied through electrodes attached to the scalp. The EF in the brain is weak, typically less than 1 V/m in strength12–16. Animal in vitro studies have shown that such weak EFs can affect the activity of M117,18. Long-lasting excitability changes produced by weak EFs may depend on NMDA receptors18, which is also supported by electrophysiological studies in humans, where oral intake of NMDA antagonist suppressed the after-effects of tDCS19.
We have previously found that there are large differences in the EFs between individuals15. The differences are due to anatomical factors, such as gyral and sulcal anatomy as well as the volume of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and the thicknesses of the scalp and skull14,15,20. However, the role of EFs in inter-individual variability is still unclear. Are the effects of EF on neural tissue sufficiently similar in each individual so that EFs were useful for predicting the effects of tDCS? If they were, individual EF models could hypothetically be used to reduce variability and control the effects.
Here, we studied whether the EF was related to the after-effects of tDCS. We first performed an exploratory sham-controlled motor cortical tDCS study and individually calculated the EFs in all our subjects. In the experiments, we applied 1 mA anodal tDCS for 20 min on the right M1. Previously, a similar protocol has been shown to decrease the excitability of the right M121. A similar inhibitory effect of long-duration anodal tDCS has also been shown using 26 min of 1 mA on the left M122. Notably, halving the duration to 13 min enhanced the excitability22, consistently with the typical facilitatory effect of 9–13 min long anodal stimulation23.
To find which cortical sites are potentially affected by the EF, we decided to use partial least squares (PLS) regression24,25, which is an effective method for finding relationships between dependent variables (here: MEP amplitude) and a large number of collinear predictor variables (here: EF in the cortex). Compared to other commonly used approaches for feature extraction from imaging data, such as random field theory26, PLS regression was advantageous because we needed not define a region of interest a priori, which would have been arbitrary as we did not know in advance which site in M1 or other regions27 was affected by tDCS. At the potentially important cortical site, the data were further analysed using linear mixed effects models to investigate the direction and time-dependence of the effects.
Methods
Subjects
Twenty-eight subjects (7 females and 21 males; mean age ± SD = 27 ± 6 years; 26 right and 2 left handed) participated in the experiments. The subjects were the same who participated in our previous study28. The subjects were neurologically healthy and had no family history of epilepsy. The handedness was assessed using the Oldfield handedness questionnaire29. All subjects gave informed consent before participating in the experiments. The Human Ethics Committee at the National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Okazaki, Japan, approved the experiments. All methods were carried out in accordance with approved institutional guidelines and regulations.
MRI
All subjects participated in MRI scanning. T1- and T2-weighted structural MRI scans of subjects participating were acquired using a 3.0 T MRI scanner (Verio; Siemens, Ltd., Erlangen, Germany). T1-weighted MRI were acquired using a Magnetization Prepared Rapid Acquisition in Gradient Echo (MPRAGE) sequence (TR/TE/TI/FA/FOV/voxel size/number of slices = 1800 ms/1.98 ms/800 ms/9°/256 mm/1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm/176). T2-weighted MRI were acquired with the following parameters: TR/TE/FOV/voxel size/slice number = 4500 ms/368 ms/256 mm/1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm/224 slices.
Experimental parameters
The experiment employed a double blind, sham-controlled, crossover design to study the effects of anodal tDCS over the right M1 on the MEPs. The experimental parameters are summarized in Fig. 1.
TDCS (1 mA) was applied using a DC STIMULATOR PLUS (neuroConn GmbH, Ilmenau, Germany) with two conditions. Conditions were separated by a washout period of at least three days. The MRI of each subject were acquired prior to the experiments.
The stimulation (anode) electrode (surface area 5 × 5 cm2) was placed over the hand M1 in the right hemisphere. The cathode (surface area 5 × 5 cm2) was placed over the contralateral orbit. Stimulation lasted for 20 min. In the sham condition, current (1 mA) was applied only for the first 15 s. The fade-in/fade-out time was 10 s in each condition. The subjects were asked to sit on a chair during the experiments, and the experimenter observed that the subject was maintaining rest.
The location of the anode was identified using an individual T1-weighted MRI and a frameless stereotaxic navigation system (Brainsight 2; Rogue Research Inc., Montreal, Canada). The experimenter first identified the “hand knob” structure of the precentral gyrus, the centre of which was projected to the scalp, which is illustrated in Fig. 1B. The centre of the anode was placed at the projected point on the scalp. TMS was also applied at the same point, in the direction perpendicular to the central sulcus, as identified using the navigation system.
As a measure of cortical excitability, MEPs were elicited using a Magstim 2002 magnetic stimulator (The Magstim Company Ltd, Whitland, UK). At the beginning of each condition, we determined the resting motor threshold (RMT). RMT was defined as the lowest stimulation intensity required for eliciting MEPs of 50 μV peak-to-peak amplitude in five of ten trials in the fully relaxed left abductor pollicis brevis (APB) muscle30. The TMS coil was held in place manually, and its position was kept constant using the navigation system.
MEPs of the fully relaxed left APB muscle were recorded before and 0–30 min (with 10 min intervals) after tDCS. During test stimulation, TMS with the intensity of 130% of the RMT was applied 30 times for each time point. The TMS pulse interval was randomly assigned at 6, 7, 8, 9, or 10 s. Recorded electromyograms were amplified (1000x), band-pass filtered (10–1000 Hz), and sampled at 5 kHz. Finally, the mean peak-to-peak MEP amplitude was calculated.
Anatomic models and inter-subject registration
T1- and T2-weighted MRI were segmented into distinct tissue compartments. Brain tissues were segmented using the FreeSurfer image analysis software31–34, and the remaining tissues were segmented using custom methods implemented in MATLAB (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, US). The segmentation process and the tissue conductivities were identical to our previous studies16,28. The conductivities were (unit: S/m): grey matter 0.2, white matter 0.14, blood 0.7, compact bone 0.008, spongy bone 0.027, CSF 1.8, dura and muscle 0.16, skin and fat 0.08, and eye 1.5.
FreeSurfer with the default parameter values was used to generate a mapping from the surface of each individual subject’s brain to that of the standard brain; details of the procedure have been described earlier16. The standard brain was based on Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) ICBM 2009a nonlinear asymmetric template35,36.
Electric field modelling
The EFs calculated in each subject were identical to those reported in our previous study28. Briefly, the EFs were modelled using the following methods. The electrodes were modelled using a two-compartment model consisting of a 1 mm thick rubber pad (0.1 S/m) inserted in a 6 mm thick sponge saturated with physiological saline (1.6 S/m)16. The electrical sources were a current source (1 mA) and sink (−1 mA) placed inside the rubber pad of the anode and cathode, respectively.
The FEM with cubical 0.5 mm × 0.5 mm × 0.5 mm first-order elements was used to determine the electric scalar potential from the Laplace-type equation . The equation was numerically solved using the geometric multigrid method37 to the relative residual of 10−6.
In each subject, the EF was calculated from at the depth of 1 mm below the grey matter surface. We then calculated the absolute value (Eabs) and the normal component of the EF (, where is the inner normal vector of the cortical surface).
In order to compare the EFs from different subjects, Eabs and En were mapped to the MNI brain16. The final EFs were represented on a triangular surface mesh of the MNI brain, which consisted of 149319 vertices in the right hemisphere and 148076 vertices in the left hemisphere.
Data analysis
MATLAB (version 2017b, The MathWorks, Inc.) was used for all statistical tests. The significance level was P < 0.05. Outliers were detected using Grubbs’ test.
Effects of Time, Session and EF on the MEP
We first analysed the experimental results without considering the EF, as would conventionally be done in tDCS studies. A linear mixed effects model was used to study the effects on the MEP normalized to the baseline. As fixed effects, we entered the effects of Time (t = 0, 10, 20, and 30 min after stimulation, denoted t0–t30), Session (real tDCS and sham) and their interaction effect. To account for the effects of the baseline MEP (MEPbase) on the normalized MEPs, the fixed effects of MEPbase, MEPbase × Time, MEPbase × Session, and MEPbase × Time × Session were also included in the model. By-subject intercepts were treated as random effects. Maximum likelihood estimation was used for determining the linear mixed effects model parameters.
To study the effect of the EF on the normalized MEPs, the absolute value and normal component of the EF were calculated at an observation point, (to be defined later). We then added additional fixed effect terms for EF, Time × EF, Session × EF, and Time × Session × EF to the linear mixed effects model. The likelihood ratio test was used to test whether adding the EF terms improved the model significantly.
Using the linear mixed effects models, we studied the following questions: (1) was the mean value of the normalized MEP different from the baseline at any time point, (2) did the mean value depend on Session, (3) was the slope for MEPbase nonzero at any time point and (4) did it depend on Session, (5) was the slope for EF nonzero at any time point and (6) did it depend on Session, and (7) did any of the mean values or slopes depend on the time point? The coefTest function of MATLAB, which uses F-tests, was used for calculating the P-values. The Satterthwaite approximation was used for estimating the degrees of freedom. For visualization of the linear mixed effect models, we used a similar approach to calculate the P-values for each mean value, slope, and the difference in slopes between sham and real tDCS.
To test the effects of Session and EF on MEPbase, we used paired two-tailed t-tests and/or Pearson correlation coefficients.
As a measure of the overall change in the cortical excitability, we calculated the mean MEP amplitude normalized to the baseline over post-stimulation time points (t0–t30)
1 |
Before this calculation, we verified that there were no significant differences between the post-stimulation time points.
We have previously found in the same subjects that the RMT correlated with the EF strengths in hand M1, and therefore had potential as a simple estimate of the EF strength28. To investigate whether the RMT was useful for estimating the normalized MEPs, we repeated the analysis replacing the EF with the RMT.
Estimation of important brain regions using PLS regression
We used PLS regression in MATLAB to study whether the measured MEPs could be explained using the calculated EFs, and, if yes, which brain regions were important for the prediction.
The input data to the PLS regression model were the following. The predictor variables, matrix X (28 × n), were the highest EF values (Eabs or En) on the right hemisphere. The n vertices with the highest EF values were selected by calculating the (100 − rE)th percentile of the mean value of Eabs or |En| over all 149319 vertices. To test the robustness of the approach, rE was varied from 1 to 10%. The dependent variable, vector Y (28 × 1), was the mean normalized MEP. The columns of X were scaled by dividing them by their sample standard deviations and centred by subtracting their sample mean.
In the initial analysis, the number of PLS components (not to be confused with EF components) was varied from one to five, and the goodness of fit was measured in terms of R2 (multiple correlation coefficient) and Q2 (cross-validated R2). R2 and Q2 are the upper and lower bounds, respectively, of how well the model explains the data and predicts new observations25. To calculate Q2, we used 10-fold cross validation with 1000 Monte–Carlo repetitions. PLS component i was defined to be predictively significant if 38. The analysis was also repeated for the sham MEP data. In this case, the EF should be unrelated to the MEP, and thus, no PLS components should have predictive significance.
After finding the number of significant components, we calculated the variable importance for the projection (VIP) to identify which brain regions were important for predicting MEPs from the EFs. Based on the important variables of PLS regression, we selected a single observation point, , in an anatomically relevant location to interpret the effect of the EF on the MEP amplitude using linear mixed effects models.
Results
None of the participants reported side effects.
Overall effect of tDCS on the MEPs
The effects of tDCS on the MEPs were initially analysed without the EFs. Analysis of MEPbase showed that the baselines of real tDCS (mean ± SD: 0.66 ± 0.26 mV, range: 0.21–1.15 mV) and sham (mean ± SD: 0.65 ± 0.32 mV, range: 0.27–1.39 mV) were not significantly different [paired t-test, t(26) = −0.197, P = 0.8]. Grubb’s test and visual inspection revealed one subject with exceptionally high MEPbase (real: 1.74 mV and sham: 1.53 mV). The subject was excluded from this and all further analyses.
A linear mixed effect model with fixed effects of Time (t0–t30), Session (sham and real), MEPbase, and their interactions was fitted to the normalized MEP data. The model is visualized in Fig. 2A. Grubb’s test for the model residuals indicated no outliers.
F-tests showed that the mean value of the normalized MEP differed significantly from the baseline [, ]. The difference was not significantly different between real tDCS and sham [, ]. Inspection of individual time points (Fig. 2A) showed that both sham and real tDCS increased the MEP amplitude compared to the baseline. MEPbase had a significant effect on the normalized MEPs [, ], subjects with a smaller baseline tending to show a larger increase in the MEP (Fig. 2A). The effect of MEPbase was not significantly different between real tDCS and sham [, ]. None of the mean values or slopes were significantly different between the time points [, ].
As the post-stimulation time points did not differ significantly, we calculated the mean MEP amplitude normalized to the baseline over all four post-stimulation time points to study individual differences in the responses to tDCS. The group-level as well as individual data are presented in Fig. 2B. The mean normalized MEPs did not have significantly different group-mean values [paired t-test, , ] nor significant correlation (Spearman , ) between sham and real tDCS. Finally, we tested whether stimulation increased the difference between the MEPs of real tDCS and sham compared to the baseline, regardless of the direction of the difference. Wilcoxon signed-rank test showed that the absolute value of the difference between the MEPs of real tDCS and sham was significantly larger (, ) after stimulation (median of mean over post-stimulation time points: 0.27 mV) than at the baseline (median: 0.18 mV).
Despite no significant differences between sham and real tDCS at the group level, these results indicated that stimulation had some effect, as the normalized MEPs of sham and real tDCS did not correlate within individuals, and the difference between the MEPs of sham and real tDCS after stimulation was larger than that expected from the baseline measurements. This could mean that the size and/or direction of the response differed between individuals. Were these differences due to chance or due to some systematic factor, such as the EF?
Calculated EFs and PLS regression
The absolute value and normal component of the EF were calculated individually in 27 subjects and registered to the standard brain. Figure 3 shows the average EFs in the right hemisphere.
PLS regression analysis with either Eabs or En as the predictor gave up to one predictively significant PLS component, depending on the percentage of the highest EF values used for the analysis (Table 1). Increasing the number of PLS components past one or using sham MEPs as dependent variables did not result in any predictively significant PLS components (Table 1, components 4 and 5 are not shown for brevity).
Table 1.
Predictor | r E | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Real tDCS | |||||||
Eabs | 1% | 0.39 | 0.44 | 0.12 | 0.17* | −0.53 | −4.24 |
Eabs | 2% | 0.39 | 0.48 | 0.10 | 0.12* | −0.79 | −7.36 |
Eabs | 3% | 0.39 | 0.50 | 0.08 | 0.09 | −0.85 | −9.38 |
Eabs | 10% | 0.43 | 0.51 | 0.05 | 0.10* | −0.64 | −15.93 |
En | 1% | 0.54 | 0.32 | 0.11 | 0.12* | −0.99 | −5.19 |
En | 2% | 0.62 | 0.32 | 0.05 | 0.12* | −1.40 | −12.19 |
En | 3% | 0.67 | 0.28 | 0.04 | 0.09 | −1.94 | −19.39 |
En | 10% | 0.87 | 0.12 | 0.01 | −0.00 | −7.04 | −88.15 |
Sham | |||||||
Eabs | 1% | 0.23 | 0.50 | 0.17 | −0.23 | −1.33 | −5.61 |
Eabs | 2% | 0.30 | 0.51 | 0.15 | −0.28 | −1.46 | −8.27 |
Eabs | 3% | 0.33 | 0.51 | 0.14 | −0.32 | −1.58 | −9.85 |
Eabs | 10% | 0.39 | 0.54 | 0.06 | −0.38 | −1.67 | −20.76 |
En | 1% | 0.58 | 0.25 | 0.12 | −0.32 | −2.60 | −8.17 |
En | 2% | 0.66 | 0.23 | 0.09 | −0.25 | −3.21 | −12.70 |
En | 3% | 0.71 | 0.22 | 0.06 | −0.23 | −3.88 | −18.91 |
En | 10% | 0.89 | 0.10 | 0.01 | −0.13 | −9.03 | −122.36 |
The predictor variables are the EF absolute value or the normal component and the dependent variables are the mean normalized MEP of either real tDCS or sham stimulation. Ratio rE indicates the percentage of the highest EF values used for the PLS regression. *Q2 > 0.0975.
The models with one PLS component were used for the subsequent analysis. The loadings of the PLS regression model with rE = 2% are visualized in Fig. 4A. PLS score plots indicated no violations of homogeneity or curvature of the data. Normal probability plots were used to verify the normality of residuals, and no outliers were detected in residual plots either by visual inspection or by Grubbs’ test.
Next, we investigated which brain regions were important for predicting the mean normalized MEPs using the VIP as the measure. The regions with high VIP in Fig. 4B are candidates for the site of action where the EF has an effect on the MEPs. For the subsequent analysis, we selected the point with the maximum VIP as an observation point, , as shown in Fig. 4B. The same point was the global maximum for rE ≤ 5% and a local maximum for larger rE. In MNI coordinates, . Based on the probabilistic cytoarchitectonic atlas of FreeSurfer, is located at the border between Brodmann areas 6 and 4a. The choice of from among several candidates is motivated by the fact that is close to the TMS hotspot of the ABP muscle projected to the cortex, (−41 ± 4, −16 ± 4, 60 ± 4), measured in the left hemisphere39.
Effects of the EF
Next, we investigated how the EFs at affect the normalized MEPs and their time course. The summary statistics of were mean ± SD: 0.46 ± 0.14 V/m and range: 0.24–0.79 V/m. For , the summary statistics were mean ± SD: 0.39 ± 0.12 V/m and range: 0.20–0.60 V/m. The EFs correlated strongly (Pearson ), indicating that the EF was approximately normal to the cortical surface at . For this reason, we chose to focus on in the following analysis.
First, we tested the baseline effects of EF. The Pearson correlation coefficients between and MEPbase were () and () for sham and real tDCS, respectively. The regression slopes were not significantly different between sham and real tDCS [, ].
and its interactions with Time and Session were input into a linear mixed effects model as fixed effects. Adding the terms with into the model improved the model significantly compared to the model without the effects of the EF (likelihood ratio test, , ). Figure 5A visualizes the fitted linear mixed effects model. Grubb’s test for the model residuals detected one outlier data point (exceptionally low normalized MEP at t0 in the subject with the lowest EF). Inclusion or exclusion of the outlier did not change the conclusions, and therefore we have included it in the analysis.
F-tests showed that, similarly to the model without the EF, the mean values of normalized MEPs changed significantly from the baseline [, ], and the change depended on MEPbase [, ]. Neither effect differed significantly between sessions [, , and , , respectively]. had a significant effect on the normalized MEP [, ], and the effect of was significantly different for real tDCS and sham [, ]. Investigation of the slopes for showed that real tDCS tended to have more negative slopes than that at baseline or those of sham (Fig. 5A). The slopes for sham were not significantly different from the baseline at any time point.
Time did not have significant effects on any of the intercepts or slopes [, ]. Therefore, we used simple linear regression to characterize the effect of on the mean normalized MEP. The fitted linear model (Pearson , ) for real tDCS was
2 |
where V/m is the sample mean of . The 95% confidence intervals of the intercept and slope were and , respectively. The regression slope for sham stimulation was not significant (, ). The partial correlation coefficients without the confounding effect of MEPbase were () and () for real tDCS and sham, respectively. Figure 5B visualizes the linear regression model as well as the individual data. It can be seen that the individuals with the largest EFs showed decreased MEPs compared to sham, whereas the subjects with the lowest EFs showed either absence of effect or increased MEP compared to sham.
Effects of the RMT
Similarly to our previous study in the same subjects28, both (Pearson , ) and (, ) strongly correlated with the RMT (average of two measurements).
To study whether the RMT could be used instead of the EF to explain the changes in the normalized MEPs, we replaced with the RMT in the linear mixed effects model. Analysis of the model coefficients showed no significant effects of the RMT at any time point [, ] nor significant differences in the effects of the RMT between real tDCS and sham [, ]. The RMT tended to correlate with MEPbase (real tDCS: , , and sham: , ).
Simple linear regression showed that the RMT weakly correlated with the mean normalized MEPs of real tDCS (, ; partial correlation without the confounding effect of MEPbase: , ). After removing the effect of , the partial correlation between the RMT and the mean normalized MEP disappeared (, ), indicating that their correlation was due to the EF. For sham, the correlation coefficient between the RMT and the mean normalized MEP was (), and the partial correlations without MEPbase and the EF were () and (), respectively.
Discussion
We studied the effect of 20 min 1 mA anodal tDCS on the excitability of the right M1, and modelled the EF in each individual subject. The initial findings suggested no significant differences between sham and real tDCS in the group-level MEPs. However, the responses to sham and real tDCS differed at the individual level. Using regression analysis, we showed that individual differences in the EF could partly explain the variability in responses.
If the EF has an effect on the individual MEPs, from which brain regions does the effect originate from? We explored this question using PLS regression to find relationships between the absolute value or normal component of the EF and the MEPs. The results showed that it was possible to predict a part of the effect using linear regression involving the EF normal component. Furthermore, the EF values in the hand area were important for the prediction. While these results are not a direct proof of the exact origin of the effects, the hypothesis that the possible effect originated from the hand M1 is attractive due to several reasons. Firstly, the sites with high importance were located in the lateral part of the hand knob at the anterior bank of the central sulcus [MNI coordinate (±42, −13,66)], which is at the border between the primary motor cortex (BA4a) and premotor areas (BA6). This is in the immediate vicinity of the TMS hotspot of the studied APB muscle39, which could explain why the EFs at this site are important for predicting the changes in MEPs. Furthermore, the direction of the EF was approximately normal to the cortical surface. In previous in vitro studies, EFs applied in the normal direction have been shown to produce long lasting after-effects17,18. Therefore, a plausible hypothesis for explaining our findings is that the after-effects of tDCS are mediated by the normal component of the EF at or near the TMS hotspot. However, this needs to be confirmed in additional studies.
Linear mixed effects models were used to analyse how En in hand M1 affected the MEPs. The results showed that tDCS, but not sham, changed the slope between the EF and MEP amplitude, indicating that subjects with low and high EFs responded differently to stimulation. TDCS changed the slope to a more negative direction, i.e., subjects with a stronger En exhibited a larger decrease (or smaller increase) in the MEP compared to sham or baseline than subjects with a weaker En. A possible explanation for the negative effect of the EF is that, as shown in previous studies, long duration (≥20 min) 1 mA anodal tDCS may decrease the motor cortical excitability21,22. The negative slope is consistent with these findings; the inhibitory effect is stronger in individuals with a stronger EF and absent in subjects experiencing weak fields in M1. However, the effects of tDCS are known to be non-linearly dependent on the current intensity40–42 and stimulation duration22. Therefore, our findings should not be extrapolated to other experimental conditions.
As far as we know, our study is the first to combine individual EF modelling with tDCS experiments. However, a few recent studies have used EF models to design and analyse the results of tDCS experiments. The effects of different EF components were studied previously by Rawji et al.43, who investigated the excitability changes using two electrode montages that produced EFs dominantly in the posterior-anterior (PA) or medial-lateral (ML) directions (left M1, 1 mA, 10 min, FDI muscle, N = 15). They found that a larger EF in the PA direction decreased the excitability, whereas the EF in the ML direction did not43. Our experiments are not directly comparable due to different target muscle, duration, and electrode montage. However, common to both studies was that the effective EF component might have been the normal component, En, which depends on the representation of the target muscle in the curved surface of the hand area. In another study, Fischer et al.27 showed that a multifocal tDCS montage that produced a weaker EF in the left M1 resulted in a greater increase in excitability than conventional tDCS (2 mA, 10 min, FDI muscle, N = 15). Consequently, they argued that the effects of tDCS on the motor cortical excitability likely originated from regions outside M127. Although obtained using different stimulation parameters, our results and those of Rawji et al.43 show that the EF strength–response relationship can also be negative, and thus, the findings of Fischer et al.27 could also be explained by a local effect of EF in M1.
Existing tDCS protocols that have been used since the early 2000s typically apply the same input current to all subjects6,23,44. This approach may be problematic, as our results suggested that the subjects with the lowest and highest EF strengths may respond oppositely to the same input current. Therefore, the findings at the group level may become weak or not significant. Indeed, in our experiment, no significant group-level differences compared to sham or baseline would have been found without considering the inter-individual differences in the EF. For comparison, several previous studies have also reported small group-level responses but high inter-individual variability7–9. If our results generalize to other stimulation parameters, EF models could be used to select the stimulation current individually, which could reduce variability.
Separately from the effect of the EF, we found that MEPbase had a significant effect on the normalized MEPs. The effect was similar for both sham and real tDCS: subjects with a higher MEPbase tended to decrease the MEP, and subjects with a lower baseline tended to increase the MEP. Wiethoff et al.7 have also reported a similar negative effect. We believe that the effect directly follows from normalization and is unrelated to tDCS: If the MEP were a random variable, the conditional expectation of the normalized MEP would be a decreasing function of MEPbase.
Despite the correlation between the RMT and the EF28, we found that the RMT had relatively weak effects on the after-effects of tDCS. The correlation coefficient between the RMT and mean normalized MEP was , which was only marginally better than that obtained for sham. The correlation disappeared when the confounding effect of the EF was excluded, indicating that the RMT and and the post-stimulation MEPs were linked solely through the EF. Interestingly, a correlation of similar strength but with the opposite sign () has been reported previously for 15 min 1 mA anodal tDCS42.
Due to the exploratory nature, our study has several limitations that should be considered in future studies. Firstly, we only considered a single stimulation current (directly proportional to the EF) in each subject. Using additional EFs in each subject could have been used to confirm the regression slopes and take into account inter-individual differences in the sensitivity to EF in linear mixed effects models. If multiple currents are used in future studies, non-linearities40–42 should also be considered when selecting the current values. Secondly, repeating the measurements for each condition would have allowed incorporating intra-individual variability in the statistical models. Notably, previous studies have shown relatively low intra-individual variability for anodal TDCS11,45. Thirdly, only one electrode configuration was used in each subject. More than one electrode configuration would have increased the amount of input data to the PLS regression model and would have possibly improved the prediction of the affected regions. Fourthly, we unexpectedly found a significant facilitatory effect of sham stimulation. We are unsure of the reasons, as other recent sham-controlled studies have not shown any significant effects of sham11,42,43. The effect of sham persisted at least 30 min after stimulation, which highlights the importance of sham control in future studies. Future studies can also select the observation point or region of interest a priori. The observation point found in this study is useful for the APB muscle of the left hand. For other targets, the TMS hotspot projected on the cortex may be a reasonable choice. Our results can also be used to estimate the required sample size for future studies. Based on simple linear regression between the EF and normalized MEP, a reasonable estimate for the coefficient of determination is approximately 0.35, which would require at least 20 data points (subjects) to ensure a statistical power of 80%.
In conclusion, this exploratory study showed that individually calculated EFs were related to inter-individual differences in the responses to tDCS. A potential hypothesis for explaining our findings is that the individual effects of tDCS are mediated by the normal component of the EF in the hand area of M1, at or close to the TMS hotspot. If the effect is confirmed, EF modelling could be the key for reducing inter-individual variability in tDCS.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported in part by KAKENHI, No. 17H00869 (A.H. and S.T.) and No. 16H03201 (S.T.).
Author Contributions
I.L. designed the study, analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. M.M. analysed the data and assisted in writing the manuscript. S.K. collected and analysed the data. A.H. designed the study and assisted in writing the manuscript. S.T. designed the study, assisted in writing the manuscript and supervised the experiments. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
Data Availability
The datasets generated during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Priori A, Berardelli A, Rona S, Accornero N, Manfredi M. Polarization of the human motor cortex through the scalp. Neuroreport. 1998;9:2257–2260. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199807130-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol (Lond) 2000;527(Pt 3):633–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology. 2001;57:1899–1901. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kuo M-F, Paulus W, Nitsche MA. Therapeutic effects of non-invasive brain stimulation with direct currents (tDCS) in neuropsychiatric diseases. Neuroimage. 2014;85(Pt 3):948–960. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Flöel A. tDCS-enhanced motor and cognitive function in neurological diseases. Neuroimage. 2014;85(Pt 3):934–947. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Horvath JC, Forte JD, Carter O. Evidence that transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) generates little-to-no reliable neurophysiologic effect beyond MEP amplitude modulation in healthy human subjects: A systematic review. Neuropsychologia. 2015;66:213–236. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wiethoff S, Hamada M, Rothwell JC. Variability in response to transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor cortex. Brain Stimul. 2014;7:468–475. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.López-Alonso V, Cheeran B, Rio-Rodriguez D, Fernández-Del-Olmo M. Inter-individual variability in response to non-invasive brain stimulation paradigms. Brain Stimul. 2014;7:372–380. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Chew T, Ho K-A, Loo CK. Inter- and intra-individual variability in response to transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) at varying current intensities. Brain Stimul. 2015;8:1130–7. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Strube, W. et al. Bidirectional variability in motor cortex excitability modulation following 1 mA transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy participants. Physiological reports4 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 11.Ammann C, Lindquist MA, Celnik PA. Response variability of different anodal transcranial direct current stimulation intensities across multiple sessions. Brain Stimul. 2017;10:757–763. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Datta A, et al. Gyri-precise head model of transcranial direct current stimulation: Improved spatial focality using a ring electrode versus conventional rectangular pad. Brain Stimul. 2009;2:201–7. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2009.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Truong DQ, Magerowski G, Blackburn GL, Bikson M, Alonso-Alonso M. Computational modeling of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in obesity: Impact of head fat and dose guidelines. Neuroimage Clin. 2013;2:759–766. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.05.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Opitz A, Paulus W, Will S, Antunes A, Thielscher A. Determinants of the electric field during transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroimage. 2015;109:140–150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Laakso I, Tanaka S, Koyama S, De Santis V, Hirata A. Inter-subject variability in electric fields of motor cortical tDCS. Brain Stimul. 2015;8:906–913. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Laakso I, et al. Electric fields of motor and frontal tDCS in a standard brain space: A computer simulation study. Neuroimage. 2016;137:140–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bikson M, et al. Effects of uniform extracellular dc electric fields on excitability in rat hippocampal slices in vitro. J Physiol (Lond) 2004;557:175–190. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.055772. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Fritsch B, et al. Direct current stimulation promotes BDNF-dependent synaptic plasticity: potential implications for motor learning. Neuron. 2010;66:198–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Nitsche MA, et al. Pharmacological modulation of cortical excitability shifts induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in humans. J Physiol (Lond) 2003;553:293–301. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.049916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Datta A, Truong D, Minhas P, Parra LC, Bikson M. Inter-individual variation during transcranial direct current stimulation and normalization of dose using MRI-derived computational models. Front Psychiatry. 2012;3:91. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Simis M, et al. Motor cortex-induced plasticity by noninvasive brain stimulation: a comparison between transcranial direct current stimulation and transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroreport. 2013;24:973–975. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Monte-Silva K, et al. Induction of late LTP-like plasticity in the human motor cortex by repeated non-invasive brain stimulation. Brain Stimul. 2013;6:424–432. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Nitsche MA, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 2008;1:206–23. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Geladi P, Kowalski BR. Partial least-squares regression: a tutorial. Anal Chim Acta. 1986;185:1–17. doi: 10.1016/0003-2670(86)80028-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wold S, Sjöström M, Eriksson L. PLS-regression: a basic tool of chemometrics. Chemom Intell Lab Syst. 2001;58:109–130. doi: 10.1016/S0169-7439(01)00155-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Worsley KJ, et al. A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Hum Brain Mapp. 1996;4:58–73. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1<58::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-O. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fischer DB, et al. Multifocal tDCS targeting the resting state motor network increases cortical excitability beyond traditional tDCS targeting unilateral motor cortex. Neuroimage. 2017;157:34–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Mikkonen M, et al. TMS motor thresholds correlate with TDCS electric field strengths in hand motor area. Front Neurosci. 2018;12:426. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rossini PM, et al. Applications of magnetic cortical stimulation. the international federation of clinical neurophysiology. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl. 1999;52:171–85. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9:179–94. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Dale AM. High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999;8:272–84. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4<272::AID-HBM10>3.0.CO;2-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:11050–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Desikan RS, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Fonov V, Evans A, McKinstry R, Almli C, Collins D. Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthood. Neuroimage. 2009;47(Supplement 1):S102. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(09)70884-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fonov V, et al. Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. Neuroimage. 2011;54:313–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Laakso I, Hirata A. Fast multigrid-based computation of the induced electric field for transcranial magnetic stimulation. Phys Med Biol. 2012;57:7753–65. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/57/23/7753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Abdi H. Partial least squares regression and projection on latent structure regression (PLS regression) Wiley Interdiscip Rev Comput Stat. 2010;2:97–106. doi: 10.1002/wics.51. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Diekhoff S, et al. Functional localization in the human brain: Gradient-echo, spin-echo, and arterial spin-labeling fMRI compared with neuronavigated TMS. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32:341–57. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Batsikadze G, Moliadze V, Paulus W, Kuo M-F, Nitsche MA. Partially non-linear stimulation intensity-dependent effects of direct current stimulation on motor cortex excitability in humans. J Physiol (Lond) 2013;591:1987–2000. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.249730. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kidgell DJ, et al. Different current intensities of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation do not differentially modulate motor cortex plasticity. Neural Plast. 2013;2013:603502. doi: 10.1155/2013/603502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Jamil A, et al. Systematic evaluation of the impact of stimulation intensity on neuroplastic after-effects induced by transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol (Lond) 2017;595:1273–1288. doi: 10.1113/JP272738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Rawji V, et al. tDCS changes in motor excitability are specific to orientation of current flow. Brain Stimul. 2018;11:289–298. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Woods AJ, et al. A technical guide to tDCS, and related non-invasive brain stimulation tools. Clin Neurophysiol. 2016;127:1031–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.López-Alonso V, Fernández-Del-Olmo M, Costantini A, Gonzalez-Henriquez JJ, Cheeran B. Intra-individual variability in the response to anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 2015;126:2342–2347. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.