Abstract
Introduction
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and electroencephalography (EEG) are a promising means to an objectified assessment of cognitive impairment in Alzheimer's disease (AD). Individually, however, these modalities tend to lack precision in both AD diagnosis and AD staging. A joint MRI–EEG approach that combines structural with functional information has the potential to overcome these limitations.
Materials and Methods
This cross‐sectional study systematically investigated the link between MRI and EEG markers and the global cognitive status in early AD. We hypothesized that the joint modalities would identify cognitive deficits with higher accuracy than the individual modalities. In a cohort of 111 AD patients, we combined MRI measures of cortical thickness and regional brain volume with EEG measures of rhythmic activity, information processing and functional coupling in a generalized multiple regression model. Machine learning classification was used to evaluate the markers’ utility in accurately separating the subjects according to their cognitive score.
Results
We found that joint measures of temporal volume, cortical thickness, and EEG slowing were well associated with the cognitive status and explained 38.2% of ifs variation. The inclusion of the covariates age, sex, and education considerably improved the model. The joint markers separated the subjects with an accuracy of 84.7%, which was considerably higher than by using individual modalities.
Conclusions
These results suggest that including joint MRI–EEG markers may be beneficial in the diagnostic workup, thus allowing for adequate treatment. Further studies in larger populations, with a longitudinal design and validated against functional‐metabolic imaging are warranted to confirm the results.
Keywords: Alzheimer disease, cognition, electroencephalography, magnetic resonance imaging
1. INTRODUCTION
Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a fatal disorder that is associated with the accumulation of β‐amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tau tangles causing progressive neurodegeneration in certain cortical and subcortical regions (Hyman et al., 2012). AD invariably affects episodic memory and other complex cognitive processes, but the perceived onset and early course of the symptoms are highly subjective and depend on the individual cognitive reserve (Stern, 2012). An accurate clinical assessment of the cognitive deficits is crucial for disease staging and thus for optimal pharmacological treatment and therapy planning. Typically, cognitive impairment is assessed in a doctor–patient/–caregiver interview and neuropsychological screening tests such as the mini‐mental state examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). These tests, however, are susceptible to daily variations, and their outcome is affected by sociodemographic factors and the individual cognitive reserve (Crum, Anthony, Bassett, & Folstein, 1993). Consequently, there is a need for accurate alternatives to measure the progression of cognitive impairment in AD.
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and electroencephalography (EEG) are potential surrogate in vivo measures of AD progression that are noninvasive, inexpensive, and widely available. MRI measures of regional brain volumes and cortical thickness are promising markers of both AD neuropathology and cognitive decline (Babiloni et al., 2015; Dubois et al., 2014; Scheltens, Fox, Barkhof, & De Carli, 2002). Especially, the limbic system in the medial temporal lobe has been identified to be vulnerable to AD: Typically, the entorhinal cortex is among the regions affected in the earliest disease stages (Killiany et al., 2002). The hippocampal volume appears to be a good marker of both disease onset and its progression (Bateman et al., 2012; Den Heijer et al., 2010; Dickerson et al., 2001; Jack et al., 2010; Lo et al., 2011). Other affected brain regions include amygdala, posterior association cortex, and the cholinergic basal forebrain (Bottino et al., 2002; Braak & Braak, 1991; Dubois et al., 2014). The cortical atrophic topography is often in line with the clinical phenotype; subjects with verbal memory impairment, for instance, frequently exhibit early atrophy in the left temporal lobe (Johnson, Fox, Sperling, & Klunk, 2012).
Electroencephalography, on the other hand, is a promising tool to assess the AD‐related functional disintegration of large scale brain networks such as the default mode network during resting state (Dillen et al., 2017; Horn, Ostwald, Reisert, & Blankenburg, 2014). Visual resting state EEG analyses in AD patients have revealed a slow dominant posterior rhythm and an increase in widespread delta and theta activity combined with a reduction in alpha and beta (Berger, 1937; Brenner, Reynolds, & Ulrich, 1988; Gordon & Sim, 1967; Letemendia & Pampiglione, 1958; Liddell, 1958; Rae‐Grant et al., 1987; Soininen, Partanen, Helkala, & Riekkinen, 1982; Weiner & Schuster, 1956). These abnormal EEG patterns have shown correlations with the cognitive status as well (Brenner et al., 1988; Gordon & Sim, 1967; Johannsen, Jakobsen, Bruhn, & Gjedde, 1999; Kaszniak, Garron, Fox, Bergen, & Huckman, 1979; Liddell, 1958; Merskey et al., 1980; Mundy‐Castle, Hurst, Beerstecher, & Prinsloo, 1954; Obrist, Busse, Eisdorfer, & Kleemeier, 1962; Rae‐Grant et al., 1987; Roberts, McGeorge, & Caird, 1978; Weiner & Schuster, 1956). Computerized resting state EEG studies have confirmed these early studies; they used the spectral power in predefined frequency bands to quantify EEG rhythmicity, synchrony‐measures such as coherence to quantify EEG connectivity, and measures from information theory to quantify EEG complexity (see Jeong, 2004 and Dauwels, Vialatte, & Cichocki, 2010 for extensive reviews). Besides resting state analyses, growing evidence suggests that the EEG recorded during memory encoding tasks carries essential information about other AD‐affected large‐scale brain networks (Garn et al., 2014, 2015; Hidasi, Czigler, Salacz, Csibri, & Molnár, 2007; Hogan, Swanwick, Kaiser, Rowan, & Lawlor, 2003; Jiang, 2005; Jiang & Zheng, 2006; Klimesch, Sauseng, & Hanslmayr, 2007; Pijnenburg et al., 2004; Stam, 2000; Stam, van Cappellen van Walsum, & Micheloyannis, 2002; Van der Hiele et al., 2007; Waser et al., 2016). With this in mind, EEG measures such as upper alpha desynchronization and theta synchronization during memory encoding might be the potential markers of impaired memory performance (Klimesch, 1999).
Despite their evident potential as noninvasive assistive tools in AD diagnose, MRI is routinely used solely for the purpose of excluding other possible causes such as vascular lesions, strategic lunar infarcts, or cerebral haemorrhages (Dubois et al., 2014; Schmidt et al., 2010), and EEG is not included in the standard diagnostic workup at all (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Dubois et al., 2007, 2010, 2014; Hyman et al., 2012; McKhann et al., 1984, 2011; Schmidt et al., 2010). A reason is that, individually, these modalities tend to lack precision in both AD diagnosis and staging. However, combining the structural and functional information from MRI and EEG in a multimodal approach has the potential to overcome the limitations of the individual modalities. This cross‐sectional study systematically investigates the usefulness of different MRI and EEG measures as symptom‐independent markers of the cognitive deficits in a cohort of early AD patients. We hypothesize that (a) these markers are significantly related to global cognition as measured by MMSE in early AD and that (b) joint MRI–EEG markers allow for an accurate identification of significant cognitive deficits.
2. MATERIALS AND METHODS
2.1. Ethics statement
This research was approved by the ethics committees of the Medical Universities Graz, Innsbruck, Vienna and the Ethics Committee of the State of Upper Austria.
2.2. Study cohort
The study cohort consisted of 111 subjects diagnosed with probable (N = 77) or possible (N = 34) AD according to NINCDS‐ADRDA1 criteria (McKhann et al., 1984) who participated in the prospective dementia (PRODEM) study of the Austrian Alzheimer Society (Seiler et al., 2012). Enrollment criteria included the availability of a caregiver, written informed consent of participant and caregiver, no need for 24‐hr care, and the absence of other physical or neurological causes of dementia‐like symptoms. All patients underwent a routine laboratory assessment, measurement of serum vitamin B12 and folic acid levels, as well as serologic (HIV, Lues) and thyroid testing. AD‐inconsistent patterns of cerebral atrophy and lesion were excluded in T1‐weighted MRI (3D MPRAGE) sequences. The dementia severity was assessed by clinical dementia rating (CDR) on a scale of 0 (no dementia) to 3 (severe dementia) (Hughes, Berg, Danziger, Coben, & Martin, 1982; Morris, 1993). Our cohort consisted of subjects with CDR scores of 0.5 (very mild dementia) or 1 (mild dementia). The global cognition was assessed by MMSE scores on a scale of 0–30 where scores below 24 indicate significant cognitive deficits (Folstein et al., 1975; Mungas, 1991). The participants’ age, sex, and completed years of education were included as potential confounders (Crum et al., 1993; Kittner et al., 1986; O'Connor, Pollitt, Treasure, Brook, & Reiss, 1989).
2.3. MRI assessment
The brain structure was assessed by T1‐weighted whole‐brain MRI scanning (3D MPRAGE sequence) on one of the three different systems (Avanto 1.5 T, Symphony Tim 1.5 T and Trio Tim 3.0 T, all were manufactured by Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany). The selected sequence parameters were echo time 3.0 ms (1.9–4.2 ms), repetition time 2,020 ms (1,410–2,300 ms), inversion time 1,017 ms (800–1,100 ms), flip angle 9°, 10° and 15°, resolution 0.9 mm (0.83–1 mm) × 0.9 mm (0.83–1 mm) × 1 mm (1–1.2 mm), and bandwidth 130 Hz.
MRI scans were processed with the FreeSurfer neuroimaging software (Version 5.3, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston MA, USA) that included automated methods for volume segmentation and measurement of cortical thickness of various brain regions. Briefly, the processing flow consisted of skull stripping followed by segmentation of gray/white matter and mapping of different brain structures in Talairach space (Desikan et al., 2006; Fischl et al., 2002). A detailed technical description can be found, for example, in Fischl et al., 2002 and Reuter, Schmansky, Rosas, & Fischl, 2012. In previous studies, these morphometric FreeSurfer procedures were validated against manual mapping (Fischl et al., 2002; Kuperberg et al., 2003; Salat et al., 2004) and proved to be reliable across scanner types and field strengths (Han et al., 2006; Reuter et al., 2012).
The regional volume and mean cortical thickness of the frontal, parietal, left and right temporal, and occipital lobe of the cerebral cortex – the major control of higher cognitive function – were employed as potential markers of cognitive impairment. The left and right temporal lobes were hereby assessed separately since the left temporal lobe has been described to be more affected in early AD stages (Johnson et al., 2012). In addition, we included the volume of entorhinal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala due to their vulnerability to atrophy in early AD (Juottonen, Laakso, Partanen, & Soininen, 1999; Poulin, Dautoff, Morris, Barrett, & Dickerson, 2011). All MRI measures were normalized by the individual total intracranial volume to account for anatomical differences between the subjects (Fischl, 2012).
2.4. EEG assessment
The brain function was assessed by EEG recordings collected from 19 gold cup electrodes (Fp1, Fp2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, T7, C3, Cz, C4, T8, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, O1, and O2; ground between Fz and Cz; connected mastoids as reference) placed according to the international 10–20 system (Jasper, 1958). Vertical and horizontal EOG and wrist‐ECG were recorded in parallel. All clinics used identical EEG systems (AlphaEEG amplifier by Alpha Trace Medical Systems, Vienna, Austria with NeuroSpeed software, bandpass 0.3–70 Hz (3 dB), notch 50 Hz, sampling rate 256 Hz, and resolution 16 bits). Impedances were kept below 10 kOhm.
The recordings were conducted in quiet and separated rooms with soft light in accordance with a predefined paradigm. Participants sat upright in comfortable chairs with neck support and a monitor positioned in front of them. They were asked to reduce movements to a minimum. A recording session included a 30‐s resting phase with closed eyes (REC) and a 30‐s memory encoding test with open eyes (ENC) where on‐screen face–name combinations had to be memorized. This test was designed to capture AD‐specific visual‐verbal memory deficits. The details of the paradigm were described elsewhere (Garn et al., 2015).
We used Matlab software (Version R2016b, MathWorks, Natick MA, USA) to remove non‐neuronal artifacts from the EEG. More specifically, each recording was downsampled to 128 Hz to reduce computational cost and bandpass filtered in the range of 1–30 Hz (type 1 FIR filter, 60 dB). Eye artifacts were removed by constrained independent component analysis using the EOG (Lu & Rajapakse, 2006). Cardiac artifacts were corrected by a modified Pan‐Tompkins algorithm using the ECG (Waser & Garn, 2013). The resulting EEG samples were divided in 2‐s epochs with 1‐s overlap (Blanco, Garcia, Quiroga, Romanelli, & Rosso, 1995). Epochs with residual artifacts were identified by a thresholding algorithm (Waser et al., 2017), visually validated by an EEG expert and omitted from further analyses.
The EEG measures listed in Table 1 were computed epoch‐ and channel‐wise. Global markers were derived separately for REC and ENC by taking the average over all channels and epochs (cf. Table 1). A technical marker description can be found in Supporting information Material section A. In brief, the dominant posterior EEG rhythm was measured by the individual alpha frequency (IAF) defined as the mean frequency‐position of the spectral center of gravity between 8 and 13 Hz. Using the IAF as anchor frequency, the spectral power was computed in individualized frequency bands delta from IAF‐7 to IAF‐5 Hz, theta from IAF‐5 to IAF‐2 Hz, alpha1 from IAF‐2 to IAF Hz, alpha2 from IAF to IAF + 2 Hz, beta1 from IAF + 2 to IAF + 8 Hz and beta2 from IAF + 8 to IAF + 16 Hz. The auto‐mutual information (aMI) quantified the similarity of an EEG signal at different points in time and was designed to capture EEG complexity and information processing mechanisms (Jeong, Gore, & Peterson, 2001; Shannon & Weaver, 1949). Inter and intrahemispheric EEG connectivity was measured by coherence in a linear and by mutual information (cMI) in a nonlinear way. Figure 1 illustrates the topographic logic of the marker assessment.
Table 1.
EEG markers | Channels | Assessment phase |
---|---|---|
Individual alpha frequency | P3, Pz, P4, O1, O2 | Rest, eyes closed |
Spectral delta‐power | F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, O2 | Visual‐verbal encoding |
Spectral theta power | F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, O2 | Visual‐verbal encoding |
Spectral alpha1 power | F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, O2 | Visual‐verbal encoding |
Spectral alpha2 power | F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, O2 | Visual‐verbal encoding |
Spectral beta1 power | F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, O2 | Visual‐verbal encoding |
Spectral beta2 power | F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, O2 | Visual‐verbal encoding |
Auto‐mutual information | F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, O2 | Visual‐verbal encoding |
Interhemispheric coherence | F3‐F4, C3‐C4, O1‐O2 | Visual‐verbal encoding |
Intrahemispheric coherence | F3‐C3, F3‐O1, C3‐O1, F4‐C4, F4‐O2, C4‐O2 | Visual‐verbal encoding |
Interhemispheric mutual info | F3‐F4, C3‐C4, O1‐O2 | Visual‐verbal encoding |
Intrahemispheric mutual info | F3‐C3, F3‐O1, C3‐O1, F4‐C4, F4‐O2, C4‐O2 | Visual‐verbal encoding |
2.5. Statistical analyses
Statistical analyses were performed with the R statistics software (Version 3.4.1, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria). We inspected the MMSE scores, covariates (age, sex, and years of education), as well as MRI and EEG markers in histograms, scatter‐ and boxplots. MMSE scores were log‐transformed to make them conform to normality. All data were rescaled to z‐scores with mean 0 and standard deviation 1. First, we tested the correlation of MMSE with each individual marker using F‐tests (α = 0.05). Second, a generalized multiple regression model was used to relate a combination of markers (regressors) to the cognitive scores (outcome) while accounting for age, sex, and education (covariates). More specifically, we used all‐subset selection to identify the marker subset that described the most MMSE variance in terms of R 2 value. To avoid overfitting, models with a large number of regressors were hereby penalized using the corrected Akaike information criterion (Akaike, 1973; Sugiura, 1978). The model assumptions were visually verified in diagnostic residual plots. The regression fit was tested by an overall F‐test and two‐tailed t tests of individual terms (α = 0.05). The central statistical concepts are described in detail in Supporting information Material Section B.
2.6. Diagnostic utility assessment
The same marker subset was then used to distinguish subjects with MMSE ≥ 24 from those with MMSE < 24 by a machine learning classification approach (see Supporting information Material Section B.3). We used a support vector machine (SVM) with radial basis function kernel to separate the two groups (Cortes & Vapnik, 1995). Parameters were hereby tuned using a grid search over defined parameter ranges. Classification was performed with leave‐one‐out cross‐validation and evaluated by its sensitivity (true positive rate), specificity (true negative rate), and accuracy. Group differences regarding age, sex, and years of education were tested by χ²‐ and Kruskal–Wallis tests.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Sample characteristics
Table 2 summarizes the demographic and clinical sample characteristics. The subjects’ age was negatively correlated with the cognitive scores (r = −0.22, F (1,109) = 5.29, p = 0.023). There was, however, no significant age difference between the subjects with MMSE ≥ 24 and those with scores below 24. Neither the subjects’ sex nor their completed years of education were significantly related to the cognitive status. The rather weak MMSE–CDR relation ( = 4.19, p = 0.041) in the earliest AD stages is in line with previous findings (Perneczky et al., 2006).
Table 2.
Total | Correlation with MMSE | MMSE ≥ 24 | MMSE < 24 | Difference | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pearson r | p value | p value | ||||
Subject count | 111 | 63 | 48 | |||
Age (years) | 74.6 ± 8.1 | −0.22 | 0.023* | 74.8 ± 7.2 | 74.3 ± 9.3 | 0.744 |
Sex (female) | 61 | −0.18 | 0.065 | 32 | 29 | 0.414 |
Education (years) | 10.9 ± 2.9 | 0.16 | 0.091 | 11.2 ± 3.2 | 10.6 ± 2.4 | 0.253 |
MMSE | 23.4 ± 3.1 | ‐ | ‐ | 25.5 ± 1.4 | 20.5 ± 2.3 | ‐ |
CDR (0.5 | 1) | 73 | 38 | −0.18 | 0.055 | 47 | 16 | 26 | 22 | 0.041* |
Note. The p values in bold font with an asterisk (*) indicate statistical significance at alpha level 0.05.
3.2. Neuroimaging markers across the spectrum of cognitive impairment
3.2.1. Individual markers
Table 3 lists the MRI and EEG markers and their respective (non‐normalized) mean values ± standard deviation. In relating individual markers with MMSE scores, we found significant results for the volumes of the parietal and the left temporal lobe, the spectral power in theta, alpha1, beta1, and beta2, as well as aMI. More specifically, reduced lobar volume, increased portions of spectral power in a low frequency range, and increased aMI were all associated with lower MMSE scores.
Table 3.
Potential markers | Values | Regression analysis | |
---|---|---|---|
Mean ± SD | Slope β | p value | |
MRI markers | |||
Cortical volumes [cm³] | |||
Frontal lobe | 122.91 ± 17.41 | 0.053 | 0.579 |
Parietal lobe | 77.31 ± 11.61 | 0.194 | 0.037* |
Temporal lobe left | 38.45 ± 6.22 | 0.188 | 0.045* |
Temporal lobe right | 38.48 ± 6.13 | 0.109 | 0.244 |
Occipital lobe | 35.97 ± 5.48 | 0.126 | 0.176 |
Cortical thickness [mm] | |||
Frontal lobe | 2.12 ± 0.22 | 0.137 | 0.144 |
Parietal lobe | 1.78 ± 0.19 | −0.009 | 0.921 |
Temporal lobe left | 2.27 ± 0.30 | −0.001 | 0.988 |
Temporal lobe right | 2.34 ± 0.33 | −0.034 | 0.721 |
Occipital lobe | 1.60 ± 0.12 | 0.112 | 0.234 |
Limbic volumes [cm³] | |||
Entorhinal cortex | 2.82 ± 0.67 | 0.080 | 0.393 |
Hippocampus | 6.28 ± 1.19 | −0.055 | 0.564 |
Amygdala | 2.27 ± 0.55 | 0.058 | 0.543 |
EEG markers | |||
Posterior dominant rhythm in rest | |||
Individual alpha frequency | 9.71 ± 0.44 | 0.093 | 0.328 |
Rhythmic activity | |||
Spectral delta power | 0.11 ± 0.04 | −0.176 | 0.057 |
Spectral theta power | 0.15 ± 0.06 | −0.380 | 0.001* |
Spectral alpha1 power | 0.09 ± 0.04 | −0.220 | 0.018* |
Spectral alpha2 power | 0.07 ± 0.02 | 0.087 | 0.359 |
Spectral beta1 power | 0.15 ± 0.04 | 0.284 | 0.002* |
Spectral beta2 power | 0.17 ± 0.07 | 0.225 | 0.014* |
Information processing | |||
Auto‐mutual information | 0.31 ± 0.01 | −0.213 | 0.021* |
Functional coupling | |||
Interhemispheric coherence | 0.57 ± 0.09 | 0.177 | 0.058 |
Intrahemispheric coherence | 0.41 ± 0.07 | 0.114 | 0.223 |
Interhemispheric mutual information | 0.19 ± 0.01 | −0.020 | 0.834 |
Intrahemispheric mutual information | 0.17 ± 0.01 | −0.037 | 0.691 |
The p values in bold font with an asterisk (*) indicate statistical significance at alpha level 0.05.
SD: standard deviation.
Figure 2 shows the between‐marker correlation (Pearson's r) color‐coded from blue (−1) to red (1) and tagged with a (*) in case of a high statistical significance (p < 0.01). We observed a widespread positive correlation between the MRI markers that were most pronounced in the cortical thickness markers. The relative spectral power in the delta and theta frequency bands was negatively correlated with the power in the higher beta1 and beta2 bands. The aMI showed high correlations with beta1 and beta2 power, indicating that information processing mechanisms were mainly reflected by a large portion of high‐frequency EEG oscillations. As for MRI–EEG correlations, reduced volume and cortical thickness of the parietal lobe were significantly associated with high delta‐power, the parietal volume was positively correlated with beta1 power as well as the left temporal volume with interhemispheric coherence.
3.2.2. Marker combinations
The following marker subset was selected to be included in the regression model: the left temporal volume, the cortical thickness of frontal, parietal and occipital lobe, and the spectral power in theta and alpha2. The regression was significant (p < 0.001) and the combined regressors explained 38.2% of the variation in MMSE scores. Figure 3 shows the relation of each regressor to the MMSE given that the remaining regressors were included in the same model. Among the individual effects, theta power (p < 0.001), left temporal volume (p = 0.007), frontal thickness (p = 0.001), and parietal thickness (p = 0.003) were significant with the latter two having the steepest slopes.
3.3. Diagnostic utility
Table 4 summarizes the results of discriminating the two groups (MMSE ≥ 24 and MMSE < 24) using the same marker subset from MRI and EEG individually, as well as in a modality‐combined manner. Using the latter, individuals were classified with an accuracy of 84.7% (sensitivity 92.1%, specificity 75.0%). Figure 4 visualizes the SVM results in greater detail. The confusion matrix in Figure 4a contains the true positive and negative (green) and false positive and negative (red) elements resulting from the combined MRI–EEG classification, which correctly identified 58 of 63 subjects with MMSE ≥ 24 and 36 of 48 subjects with MMSE < 24. Figure 4b illustrates that the combined MRI–EEG approach yielded better results as compared to the individual modalities. MRI markers separated the groups with an accuracy of only 66.7% (sensitivity 77.8%, specificity 52.1%). EEG markers apparently reflected the cognitive deficits better than MRI, and they separated the groups with an accuracy of 79.3% (sensitivity 87.3%, specificity 68.8%).
Table 4.
Sensitivity (%) | Specificity (%) | PPV (%) | NPV (%) | Accuracy (%) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
MRI | 77.78 | 52.08 | 68.06 | 64.10 | 66.67 |
EEG | 87.30 | 68.75 | 78.57 | 80.49 | 79.28 |
MRI + EEG | 92.06 | 75.00 | 82.86 | 87.80 | 84.68 |
NPV: negative predictive value; PPV: positive predictive value.
4. DISCUSSION
The first study question sought to determine whether there was a significant relation between the MRI–EEG markers and the severity of cognitive impairment in early AD. Prior studies have reported that individual MRI measures of regional volume and cortical thickness as well as abnormal EEG patterns are related with the cognitive status. Here, only few individual markers were significantly correlated with MMSE scores. However, the combined MRI–EEG markers were well associated with the subjects’ cognitive status. By measuring the left temporal volume, the cortical thickness of frontal, parietal and occipital lobe, and the spectral power in the theta and alpha2 frequency bands, 38.2% of MMSE variation was explained. In line with previous studies, the inclusion of age, sex, and education improved these results (Crum et al., 1993; Kittner et al., 1986). O'Connor et al. (1989) suggested that also the sociocultural background, though difficult to quantify, should be included. Other potentially influential factors might include medication and lifestyle.
On the question of the diagnostic utility of MRI and EEG markers, a joint MRI–EEG approach demonstrated higher diagnostic accuracy (84.7%) as compared to the individual modalities. Our results are in agreement with previous research, showing AD classification accuracies between 74%–100% for temporal lobe atrophy (O'Brien et al., 2004), 76.9%–81.7% for volumetric measures of medial temporal structures (Bottino et al., 2002), and 87%–99% for measures of cortical volume and thickness (Du et al., 2007). However, considering the small samples of these studies, the results need to be interpreted with caution. Similar results have been found in EEG studies, with EEG markers yielding accuracies from 76% to 85% (Jelic & Kowalski, 2009; Triggiani et al., 2016). Thereby, it seems that the MRI and EEG modalities might be complementary due to an incomplete overlap in subjects regarding MRI and EEG abnormalities (Strijers et al., 1997). This view got further support by a recent study showing superior AD‐control classification accuracy (90%) for combined MRI and EEG markers as compared to the individual modalities as well. These data suggest that MRI–EEG markers have potential as accurate cognitive staging tools.
One interesting finding is that four of the six selected markers were measures of cortical lobe atrophy. Especially, atrophy patterns in the left temporal, frontal, and parietal lobe are in accord with previous studies (Du et al., 2007; Hwang et al., 2016). Hartikainen et al. (2012) also found occipito–parietal cortical thinning in AD. Caution is advised; however, for the cortical thickness, measures are highly inter‐correlated and any conclusion on topographic patterns needs further validation. In contrast to earlier findings, the medial temporal lobe structures hippocampus, amygdala, and entorhinal cortex were not included in the final model. A possible explanation might be the strong innervation of these structures and the cerebral cortex and, consequently, that limbic atrophy was implicitly included through the coarser cortical measures. Another reason might be that the included EEG markers reflected the changes in the limbic structures. It also seems possible that these relatively small brain structures were subject to measurement bias in contrast to the greater cortical lobes. The most plausible explanation, however, is that these brain structures have already been affected long before AD was diagnosed and that, during early AD, they do not vary as much.
Among the abnormal EEG patterns, increased theta and decreased alpha rhythms, commonly referred to as EEG slowing, were the most significant markers. These findings are consistent with previous studies and the role of theta and alpha rhythms in cognitive performance: Klimesch (1999) observed that “upper alpha desynchronization correlates with semantic memory performance whereas theta synchronization correlates with working memory or episodic memory performance in particular.” The same author stated: “Because alpha frequency varies to a large extent as a function of age, neurological diseases, memory performance, brain volume, and task demands, the use of fixed frequency bands does not seem justified”. The current approach of recording the EEG during a memory task and computing markers using individualized frequency bands corroborate these ideas. Surprisingly, neither coherence nor cMI – both measures of functional coupling – were selected into the final set of markers. Especially, changes in EEG coherence have been related to AD progression (Jeong, 2004). There are several possible explanations for this result. On the one hand, coherence and cMI are clearly correlated with other EEG markers and by including those parts of the functional coupling information is implicitly included in our model. On the other hand, our approach of global markers might not be well‐suited to capture the topographic characteristic of functional coupling markers. Finally, the changes in functional coupling during early AD might be too subtle to be used as accurate marker of cognitive decline.
A limitation of this study is the sole use of MMSE scores as measure of the global cognitive status. AD typically impairs cognitive complex domains, the sequence and severity of impairment varies from patient to patient, even more so in the earliest disease stages. Another well‐described issue of the MMSE is its susceptibility to demographic factors such as age and education. In the current study, we tried to overcome this limitation by including demographic information as covariates. However, a single MMSE cutoff to distinguish stages of cognitive impairment is thus problematic, and it is important to bear in mind the possible resulting bias. By using more elaborate cognitive tests that are less sensitive to demographic factors, further research should be undertaken to investigate which markers reflect deficits in which cognitive domain.
5. CONCLUSION
The purpose of this study was a systematic assessment of the usefulness of different MRI and EEG measures as symptom‐independent markers of the severity of cognitive deficits in early AD. Our study has demonstrated the potential of a combined MRI–EEG approach by separating subjects with MMSE ≥ 24 from those with MMSE < 24 with an accuracy of 84.7%. These results suggest that the current diagnostic workup might benefit from an inclusion of joint MRI–EEG markers. These markers may represent noninvasive and cost‐effective means to gain information on early AD progression that could be essential in timely treatment and facilitate additional clinical research. Further studies in larger populations are warranted to validate the current results. Longitudinal study designs are particularly relevant to confirm the evident potential of these markers and to estimate their true diagnostic accuracy. Finally, cross‐modality validation against functional–metabolic imaging might further increase the understanding of disease progression.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
None declared.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Parts of this research were funded by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF, project number J3766) and the Austrian Research Promotion Agency (FFG, project number 827462). In addition, the authors would like to thank Dr. Dieter Grossegger (Alpha Trace Medical Systems, Vienna, Austria) for providing a temporary license of the NeuroSpeed software.
Waser M, Benke T, Dal‐Bianco P, et al. Neuroimaging markers of global cognition in early Alzheimer's disease: A magnetic resonance imaging–electroencephalography study. Brain Behav. 2019;9:e01197 10.1002/brb3.1197
Note
National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association.
REFERENCES
- Akaike, H. (1973). Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. In Petrov, B. N. & Csaki, F. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2nd International Symposium on Information Theory (pp. 267–281). Budapest: Akademiai Kiado. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association . (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Babiloni, C. , Del Percio, C. , Boccardi, M. , Lizio, R. , Lopez, S. , Carducci, F. , … Frisoni, G. B. (2015). Occipital sources of resting state alpha rhythms are related to local gray matter density in subjects with amnesic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 36, 556–570. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.09.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bateman, R. J. , Xiong, C. , Benzinger, T. L. , Fagan, A. M. , Goate, A. , Fox, N. C. , … Dominantly Inherited Alzheimer Network (2012). Clinical and biomarker changes in dominantly inherited Alzheimer's disease. New England Journal of Medicine, 367, 795–804. 10.1056/NEJMoa1202753 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berger, H. (1937). Über das Elektrenkephalogramm des Menschen. Zwölfte Mitteilung. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 106, 165–187. 10.1007/BF01987704 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Blanco, S. , Garcia, H. , Quiroga, R. Q. , Romanelli, L. , & Rosso, O. A. (1995). Stationarity of the EEG series. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Magazine, 14, 395–399. 10.1109/51.395321 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bottino, C. M. , Castro, C. C. , Gomes, R. L. , Buchpiguel, C. A. , Marchetti, R. L. , & Neto, M. R. (2002). Volumetric MRI measurements can differentiate Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment, and normal aging. International Psychogeriatrics, 14, 59–72. 10.1017/S1041610202008281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braak, H. , & Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological staging of Alzheimer‐related changes. Acta Neuropathologica, 82, 239–259. 10.1007/BF00308809 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brenner, R. P. , Reynolds, C. F. , & Ulrich, R. F. (1988). Diagnostic efficacy of computerized spectral versus visual EEG analysis in elderly normal, demented and depressed subjects. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 69, 110–117. 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90206-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cortes, C. , & Vapnik, V. (1995). Support‐vector networks. Machine Learning, 20(3), 273–297. [Google Scholar]
- Crum, R. M. , Anthony, J. C. , Bassett, S. S. , & Folstein, M. F. (1993). Population‐based norms for the mini‐mental state examination by age and educational level. JAMA, 269, 2386–2391. 10.1001/jama.1993.03500180078038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dauwels, J. , Vialatte, F. , & Cichocki, A. (2010). Diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease from EEG signals: Where are we standing? Current Alzheimer Research, 7, 487–505. 10.2174/156720510792231720 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Den Heijer, T. , Van der Lijn, F. , Koudstaal, P. J. , Hofman, A. , Van der Lugt, A. , Krestin, G. P. , & Breteler, M. M. (2010). A 10‐year follow‐up of hippocampal volume on magnetic resonance imaging in early dementia and cognitive decline. Brain, 133, 1163–1172. 10.1093/brain/awq048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desikan, R. , Segonne, F. , Fischl, B. , Quinn, B. , Dickerson, B. , Blacker, D. , … Killiany, R. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage, 31(3), 968–980. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson, B. C. , Goncharova, I. , Sullivan, M. P. , Forchetti, C. , Wilson, R. S. , & Bennett, D. A. (2001). MRI‐derived entorhinal and hippocampal atrophy in incipient and very mild Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 22, 747–754. 10.1016/S0197-4580(01)00271-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dillen, K. N. H. , Jacobs, H. I. L. , Kukolja, J. , Richter, N. , von Reutern, B. , Onur, Ö. A. , … Fink, G. R. (2017). Functional disintegration of the default mode network in prodromal Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimers Disease, 59(1), 169–187. 10.3233/JAD-161120 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Du, A. T. , Schuff, N. , Kramer, J. H. , Rosen, H. J. , Gorno‐Tempini, M. L. , Rankin, K. , … Weiner, M. W. (2007). Different regional patterns of cortical thinning in Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia. Brain, 130(4), 1159–1166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubois, B. , Feldman, H. H. , Jacova, C. , Cummings, J. L. , Dekosky, S. T. , Barberger‐Gateau, P. , … Scheltens, P. (2010). Revising the definition of Alzheimer's disease: A new lexicon. The Lancet Neurology, 9, 1118–1127. 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70223-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubois, B. , Feldman, H. H. , Jacova, C. , Dekosky, S. T. , Barberger‐Gateau, P. , Cummings, J. L. , … Scheltens, P. (2007). Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Revising the NINCDS–ADRDA criteria. The Lancet Neurology, 6, 734–746. 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70178-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubois, B. , Feldman, H. H. , Jacova, C. , Hampel, H. , Molinuevo, J. L. , Blennow, K. , & Cummings, J. L. (2014). Advancing research diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer's disease: The IWG‐2 criteria. The Lancet Neurology, 13, 614–629. 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70090-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl, B. (2012). FreeSurfer. NeuroImage, 62, 774–781. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl, B. , Salat, D. H. , Busa, E. , Albert, M. , Dieterich, M. , & Haselgrove, C. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33, 341–355. 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein, M. F. , Folstein, S. E. , & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini‐mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garn, H. , Waser, M. , Deistler, M. , Benke, T. , Dal‐Bianco, P. , Ransmayr, G. , … Schmidt, R. (2015). Quantitative EEG markers relate to Alzheimer's disease severity in the Prospective Dementia Registry Austria (PRODEM). Clinical Neurophysiology, 126, 505–513. 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garn, H. , Waser, M. , Deistler, M. , Schmidt, R. , Dal‐Bianco, P. , Ransmayr, G. , … Benke, T. (2014). Quantitative EEG in Alzheimer's disease: Cognitive state, resting state and association with disease severity. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 93, 390–397. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon, E. , & Sim, M. (1967). The EEG in presenile dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 30, 285–291. 10.1136/jnnp.30.3.285 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han, X. , Jovicich, J. , Salat, D. , van der Kouwe, A. , Quinn, B. , Czanner, S. , ... Fischl, B. (2006). Reliability of MRI-derived measurements of human cerebral cortical thickness: The effects of field strength, scanner upgrade and manufacturer. Neuroimage, 32, 180–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartikainen, P. , Räsänen, J. , Julkunen, V. , Niskanen, E. , Hallikainen, M. , Kivipelto, M. , … Soininen, H. (2012). Cortical thickness in frontotemporal dementia, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 30(4), 857–874. 10.3233/JAD-2012-112060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hidasi, Z. , Czigler, B. , Salacz, P. , Csibri, P. , & Molnár, M. (2007). Changes of EEG spectra and coherence following performance in a cognitive task in Alzheimer's disease. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 65, 252–260. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogan, M. J. , Swanwick, G. R. J. , Kaiser, J. , Rowan, M. , & Lawlor, B. (2003). Memory‐related EEG power and coherence reductions in mild Alzheimer's disease. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 49, 147–163. 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00118-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horn, A. , Ostwald, D. , Reisert, M. , & Blankenburg, F. (2014). The structural‐functional connectome and the default mode network of the human brain. NeuroImage, 102, 142–151. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes, C. P. , Berg, L. , Danziger, W. L. , Coben, L. A. , & Martin, R. L. (1982). A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 566–572. 10.1192/bjp.140.6.566 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hwang, J. , Kim, C. M. , Jeon, S. , Lee, J. M. , Hong, Y. J. , Roh, J. H. , … Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2016). Prediction of Alzheimer's disease pathophysiology based on cortical thickness patterns. Alzheimer's and Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment and Disease Monitoring, 2, 58–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman, B. T. , Phelps, C. H. , Beach, T. G. , Bigio, E. H. , Cairns, N. J. , Carrillo, M. C. , … Montine, T. J. (2012). National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer's Association guidelines for the neuropathologic assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer's Association, 8, 1–13. 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.10.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack, C. R. Jr , Knopman, D. S. , Jagust, W. J. , Shaw, L. M. , Aisen, P. S. , Weiner, M. W. , … Trojanowski, J. Q. (2010). Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer's pathological cascade. The Lancet Neurology, 9, 119–128. 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70299-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jasper, H. H. (1958). The ten‐twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371–375. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jelic, V. , & Kowalski, J. E. (2009). Evidence‐based evaluation of diagnostic accuracy of resting EEG in dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 40, 129–142. 10.1177/155005940904000211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeong, J. (2004). EEG dynamics in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 1490–1505. 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeong, J. , Gore, J. , & Peterson, B. (2001). Mutual information analysis of the EEG in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 827–835. 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00513-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiang, Z. (2005). Study on EEG power and coherence in patients with mild cognitive impairment during working memory task. Journal of Zhejiang University Science B, 6, 1213–1219. 10.1631/jzus.2005.B1213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiang, Z. , & Zheng, L. (2006). Inter‐ and intra‐hemispheric EEG coherence in patients with mild cognitive impairment at rest and during working memory task. Journal of Zhejiang University Science B, 7, 357–364. 10.1631/jzus.2006.B0357 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johannsen, P. , Jakobsen, J. , Bruhn, P. , & Gjedde, A. (1999). Cortical responses to sustained and divided attention in Alzheimer's disease. NeuroImage, 10, 269–281. 10.1006/nimg.1999.0475 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, K. A. , Fox, N. C. , Sperling, R. A. , & Klunk, W. E. (2012). Brain Imaging in Alzheimer Disease. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Medicine, 2, a006213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juottonen, K. , Laakso, M. P. , Partanen, K. , & Soininen, H. (1999). Comparative MR analysis of the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus in diagnosing Alzheimer disease. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 20, 139–144. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaszniak, A. W. , Garron, D. C. , Fox, J. H. , Bergen, D. , & Huckman, M. (1979). Cerebral atrophy, EEG slowing, age, education, and cognitive functioning in suspected dementia. Neurology, 29(1), 1273–1279. 10.1212/WNL.29.9_Part_1.1273 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killiany, R. J. , Hyman, B. T. , Gomez‐Isla, T. , Moss, M. B. , Kikinis, R. , Jolesz, F. , … Albert, M. S. (2002). MRI measures of entorhinal cortex vs hippocampus in preclinical AD. Neurology, 58, 1188–1196. 10.1212/WNL.58.8.1188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kittner, S. J. , White, L. R. , Farmer, M. E. , Wolz, M. , Kaplan, E. , Moes, E. , … Feinleib, M. (1986). Methodological issues in screening for dementia: The problem of education adjustment. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 39(3), 163–170. 10.1016/0021-9681(86)90019-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews, 29, 169–195. 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch, W. , Sauseng, P. , & Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews, 53, 63–88. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuperberg, G. R. , Broome, M. R. , McGuire, P. K. , David, A. S. , Eddy, M. , Ozawa, F. , … Fischl, B. (2003). Regionally localized thinning of the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(9), 878–888. 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.878 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Letemendia, F. , & Pampiglione, G. (1958). Clinical and electroencephalographic observations in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 21, 167–172. 10.1136/jnnp.21.3.167 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liddell, D. W. (1958). Investigations of E.E.G. findings in presenile dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 21(3), 173–176. 10.1136/jnnp.21.3.173 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lo, R. Y. , Hubbard, A. E. , Shaw, L. M. , Trojanowski, J. Q. , Petersen, R. C. , Aisen, P. S. , … Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative . (2011). Longitudinal change of biomarkers in cognitive decline. Archives of Neurology, 68, 1257–1266. 10.1001/archneurol.2011.123 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu, W. , & Rajapakse, J. C. (2006). ICA with reference. Neurocomputing, 69, 2244–2257. 10.1016/j.neucom.2005.06.021 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McKhann, G. , Drachman, D. , Folstein, M. , Katzman, R. , Price, D. , & Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Report of the NINCDS‐ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology, 34, 939–944. 10.1212/WNL.34.7.939 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKhann, G. M. , Knopman, D. S. , Chertkow, H. , Hyman, B. T. , Jack, C. R. , Kawas, C. H. , … Phelps, C. H. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging‐Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia: the Journal of the Alzheimer's Association, 7, 263–269. 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merskey, H. , Ball, M. J. , Blume, W. T. , Fox, A. J. , Fox, H. , Hersch, E. L. , … Palmer, R. B. (1980). Relationships between psychological measurements and cerebral organic changes in Alzheimer's disease. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 7(1), 45–49. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris, J. C. (1993). The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): Current version and scoring rules. Neurology, 43(11), 2412–2414. 10.1212/WNL.43.11.2412-a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mundy‐Castle, A. C. , Hurst, L. A. , Beerstecher, D. M. , & Prinsloo, T. (1954). The electroencephalogram in the senile psychoses. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 6(2), 245–252. 10.1016/0013-4694(54)90026-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mungas, D. (1991). In‐office mental status testing: A practical guide. Geriatrics, 46(7), 54–58. 63, 66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Brien, J. T. , Colloby, S. , Fenwick, J. , Williams, E. D. , Firbank, M. , Burn, D. , … McKeith, I. G. (2004). Dopamine transporter loss visualized with FP‐CIT SPECT in the differential diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies. Archives of Neurology, 61(6), 919–925. 10.1001/archneur.61.6.919 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Obrist, W. D. , Busse, E. W. , Eisdorfer, C. , & Kleemeier, R. W. (1962). Relation of the electroencephalogram to intellectual function in senescence. Journal of Gerontology, 17, 197–206. 10.1093/geronj/17.2.197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Connor, D. W. , Pollitt, P. A. , Treasure, F. P. , Brook, C. P. , & Reiss, B. B. (1989). The influence of education, social class and sex on Mini‐Mental State scores. Psychological Medicine, 19, 771–776. 10.1017/S0033291700024375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perneczky, R. , Wagenpfeil, S. , Komossa, K. , Grimmer, T. , Diehl, J. , & Kurz, A. (2006). Mapping scores onto stages: Mini‐mental state examination and clinical dementia rating. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14(2), 139–144. 10.1097/01.JGP.0000192478.82189.a8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pijnenburg, Y. A. L. , Van der Made, Y. , Van Cappellen van Walsum, A. M. , Knol, D. L. , Scheltens, P. H. , & Stam, C. J. (2004). EEG synchronization likelihood in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease during a working memory task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 1332–1339. 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.12.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poulin, S. P. , Dautoff, R. , Morris, J. C. , Barrett, L. F. , Dickerson, B. C. , Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2011). Amygdala atrophy is prominent in early Alzheimer's disease and relates to symptom severity. Psychiatry Research, 194(1), 7–13. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.06.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rae‐Grant, A. , Blume, W. , Lau, C. , Hachinski, V. C. , Fisman, M. , & Merskey, H. (1987). The electroencephalogram in Alzheimer‐type dementia. A sequential study correlating the electroencephalogram with psychometric and quantitative pathologic data. Archives of Neurology, 44, 50–54. 10.1001/archneur.1987.00520130042015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reuter, M. , Schmansky, N. J. , Rosas, H. D. , & Fischl, B. (2012). Within‐subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. NeuroImage, 61, 1402–1418. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, M. A. , McGeorge, A. P. , & Caird, F. I. (1978). EEG and computerized tomography in vascular and non‐vascular dementia in old age. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 41, 903–906. 10.1136/jnnp.41.10.903 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salat, D. H. , Buckner, R. L. , Snyder, A. Z. , Greve, D. N. , Desikan, R. S. , Busa, E. , … Fischl, B. (2004). Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 721–730. 10.1093/cercor/bhh032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheltens, P. , Fox, N. , Barkhof, F. , & De Carli, C. (2002). Structural magnetic resonance imaging in the practical assessment of dementia: Beyond exclusion. The Lancet Neurology, 1, 13–21. 10.1016/S1474-4422(02)00002-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt, R. , Marksteiner, J. , Dal‐Bianco, P. , Ransmayr, G. , Bancher, C. , Benke, T. , … Walter, A. (2010). Consensus statement ‘Dementia 2010’ of the Austrian Alzheimer Society. Neuropsychiatry, 24, 67–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seiler, S. , Schmidt, H. , Lechner, A. , Benke, T. , Sanin, G. , Ransmayr, G. , … PRODEM Study Group . (2012). Driving cessation and dementia: Results of the prospective registry on dementia in Austria (PRODEM). PLoS ONE, 7, e52710 10.1371/journal.pone.0052710 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shannon, C. E. , & Weaver, W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press. [Google Scholar]
- Soininen, H. , Partanen, V. J. , Helkala, E.‐L. , & Riekkinen, P. J. (1982). EEG findings in senile dementia and normal aging. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 65, 59–70. 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1982.tb03062.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stam, C. J. (2000). Brain dynamics in theta and alpha frequency bands and working memory performance in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 286, 115–118. 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01109-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stam, C. J. , van Cappellen van Walsum, A. M. , & Micheloyannis, S. (2002). Variability of EEG synchronization during a working memory task in healthy subjects. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 46, 53–66. 10.1016/S0167-8760(02)00041-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. The Lancet Neurology, 11, 1006–1012. 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strijers, R. L. , Scheltens, P. , Jonkman, E. J. , de Rijke, W. , Hooijer, C. , & Jonker, C. (1997). Diagnosing Alzheimer's disease in community‐dwelling elderly: A comparison of EEG and MRI. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 8, 198–202. 10.1159/000106631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugiura, N. (1978). Further analysis of the data by Akaike's information criterion and the finite corrections. Communications in Statistics, A7, 13–26. 10.1080/03610927808827599 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Triggiani, A. I. , Bevilacqua, V. , Brunetti, A. , Lizio, R. , Tattoli, G. , Cassano, F. , … Babiloni, C. (2016). Classification of healthy subjects and Alzheimer's disease patients with Dementia from cortical sources of resting state EEG rhythms: A study using artificial neural networks. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10, 604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Hiele, K. , Vein, A. A. , Reijntjes, R. H. A. M. , Westendorp, R. G. J. , Bollen, E. L. E. M. , Van Buchem, M. A. , … Middelkoop, H. A. (2007). EEG correlates in the spectrum of cognitive decline. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 1931–1939. 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.05.070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waser, M. , & Garn, H. (2013). Removing cardiac interference from the electroencephalogram using a modified Pan‐Tompkins algorithm and linear regression. Conference Proceedings IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, 35, 2028–2031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waser, M. , Garn, H. , Benke, T. , Dal‐Bianco, P. , Ransmayr, G. , Schmidt, R. , … Sorensen, H. B. D. (2017). A flexible method for the automated offline‐detection of artifacts in multi‐channel electroencephalogram recordings. Conference Proceedings IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, 39, 3793–3796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waser, M. , Garn, H. , Schmidt, R. , Benke, T. , Dal‐Bianco, P. , Ransmayr, G. , … Deistler, M. (2016). Quantifying synchrony patterns in the EEG of Alzheimer's patients with linear and non‐linear connectivity markers. Journal of Neural Transmission, 123, 297–316. 10.1007/s00702-015-1461-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiner, H. , & Schuster, D. B. (1956). The electroencephalogram in dementia: Some preliminary observations and correlations. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 8, 479–488. 10.1016/0013-4694(56)90014-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.