Abstract
The objective of the study was to examine the association between biopsychosocial factors and developmental trajectories of childhood urinary incontinence (UI). We used developmental trajectories (latent classes) of childhood UI from 4–9 years including bedwetting alone, daytime wetting alone, delayed (daytime and nighttime) bladder control, and persistent (day and night) wetting (n = 8751, 4507 boys, 4244 girls). We examined whether biopsychosocial factors (developmental level, gestational age, birth weight, parental UI, temperament, behaviour/emotional problems, stressful events, maternal depression, age at initiation of toilet training, constipation) are associated with the trajectories using multinomial logistic regression (reference category = normative development of bladder control). Maternal history of bedwetting was associated with almost a fourfold increase in odds of persistent wetting [odds ratio and 95% confidence interval: 3.60 (1.75–7.40)]. In general, difficult temperament and behaviour/emotional problems were most strongly associated with combined (day and night) wetting, e.g. children with behavioural difficulties had increased odds of delayed (daytime and nighttime) bladder control [1.80 (1.59–2.03)]. Maternal postnatal depression was associated with persistent (day and night) wetting [2.09 (1.48–2.95)] and daytime wetting alone [2.38 (1.46–3.88)]. Developmental delay, stressful events, and later initiation of toilet training were not associated with bedwetting alone, but were associated with the other UI trajectories. Constipation was only associated with delayed bladder control. We find evidence that different trajectories of childhood UI are differentially associated with biopsychosocial factors. Increased understanding of factors associated with different trajectories of childhood UI could help clinicians to identify children at risk of persistent incontinence.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (10.1007/s00787-018-1193-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Keywords: Child urinary incontinence, Bedwetting, Daytime wetting, Developmental trajectory, Prospective cohort, ALSPAC
Introduction
Two major groups of childhood urinary incontinence (UI) can be differentiated: nocturnal enuresis (bedwetting) and daytime urinary incontinence (daytime wetting), which can co-occur. Both groups are heterogeneous and comprise different conditions with specific signs and symptoms, as delineated by the International Children’s Continence Society [1]. The aetiology of UI is believed to be multifactorial, involving a complex interrelationship of biological, genetic and psychosocial factors. Factors associated with childhood UI include family history [2–5], developmental and obstetric factors [2–7], behaviour/emotional problems [8–10], stress [11, 12], maternal psychopathology [10], toilet training [13–16] and constipation [17–19]. Prospective cohort studies focus either on bedwetting [2, 20–22] or daytime wetting [23]. Bedwetting alone and combined (day and night) wetting are thought to have differing underlying pathophysiological mechanisms [24, 25], but the extent to which factors are differentially associated with different types of childhood UI is currently not well understood. Studies have found higher levels of developmental and obstetric factors [3–5] and psychological problems [26, 27] in children with combined wetting compared with bedwetting alone. A recent study found that children who experienced persistent combined wetting had a much higher chance of bedwetting in adolescence than those with bedwetting alone [28]. Identification of factors that distinguish between different types of UI could increase understanding of the aetiology of childhood incontinence and help to identify children at risk of persistent UI. In this study, we use data from a large cohort to prospectively examine whether biopsychosocial factors are differentially associated with bedwetting, daytime wetting and combined wetting in primary school-aged children.
Methods
Participants
The sample comprised participants from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Detailed information is available at (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac), including a fully searchable data-dictionary (http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary). Pregnant women resident in the former Avon Health Authority in SW England having an estimated date of delivery between 1/4/91 and 31/12/92 were invited to take part, resulting in a cohort of 14,541 pregnancies [29]. Of the 13,978 singletons/twins alive at 1 year, a small number of participants withdrew consent (n = 24) leaving a starting sample of 13,954. Ethical approval was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics committee and local research ethics committees.
Parallel latent classes of bedwetting and daytime wetting
We used parallel latent classes previously extracted from ALSPAC data on bedwetting and daytime wetting at 41/2, 51/2, 61/2, 71/2 and 91/2 (hereafter referred to as 4–9 years) [28]. The classes were described as ‘normative’ (low probability of bedwetting or daytime wetting at any time point: 63.0% of the sample), ‘daytime wetting alone’ (daytime wetting with very low probability of bedwetting: 5.8%), ‘bedwetting alone’ (bedwetting with very low probability of daytime wetting: 15.6%), ‘delayed’ (decreasing probability of bedwetting or daytime wetting after 6 years: 8.6%) and ‘persistent wetting’ (relatively high probability of bedwetting and daytime wetting at all time points: 7.0%). The latter two classes comprised combined day and night wetting. Boys were more likely to be members of the bedwetting alone (68.4%), persistent wetting (63%) and delayed (52.9%) classes than girls, whilst girls were more likely to be members of the daytime wetting alone class (66.5%) [28].
Biopsychosocial factors
We examined biological and developmental factors (developmental level at 18 months [30], birth weight, gestation, parental history of UI [31]) and psychosocial factors (child temperament at 2 years [32, 33], behaviour/emotional problems [34] and temper tantrums at 3½ years, exposure to stressful events between 2½ and 3 years 11 months [22], maternal antenatal depression at 18 and 32 weeks gestation and postnatal depression when the child was 21 months [35, 36]). We also examined age at initiation of toilet training [37] and the presence of constipation at age 4 (see table S1 for details of all assessments).
Confounders
We adjusted for child’s sex and measures of socioeconomic position and adversity derived from responses to questionnaires completed by mothers during the antenatal period (Table 1). We also adjusted for developmental level and maternal depression (full details of the adjustments for each model are provided in the footnote for Table 2).
Table 1.
Normative (%) | Bedwetting alone (%) | Daytime wetting alone | Delayed | Persistent wetting | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Maternal educational qualificationsa | ||||||
None | 25.0 | 20.4 | 22.7 | 21.6 | 20.8 | 0.002 |
Parental social classb | ||||||
Manual | 15.5 | 14.7 | 17.9 | 13.9 | 17.4 | 0.361 |
Family size | ||||||
≥ 3 children | 0.9 | 1.1 | 1.0 | 1.6 | 1.3 | 0.345 |
Early parenthood (< 19 years) | ||||||
Yes | 4.8 | 4.8 | 4.4 | 3.9 | 5.8 | 0.639 |
Housing adequacyc | ||||||
No | 4.9 | 3.7 | 4.4 | 5.3 | 6.4 | 0.165 |
Major financial difficulties | ||||||
Yes | 8.1 | 7.7 | 10.5 | 8.7 | 10.7 | 0.145 |
We obtained these distributions by assigning each participant to their most likely class (modal assignment), generating individual probabilities of class membership and deriving conditional distributions of the variables within classes
aMaternal education defined as none versus high school qualifications or greater
bSocial class based on the higher of the mother or partner’s occupational social class using the 1991 British Office of Population and Census Statistics (OPCS) classification and dichotomized into non-manual (professional, managerial or skilled professions) and manual (partly or unskilled occupations)
cHousing adequacy: ‘no’ comprises crowding, periods of homelessness, poor living conditions and/or major defects/infestation
Table 2.
N | Bedwetting alone | Daytime wetting alone | Delayed | Persistent wetting | p value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Biological and developmental factors | ||||||
Developmental level at 18 monthsa | 7301 | 1.04 [0.94–1.15] | 1.43 [1.20–1.70] | 1.52 [1.33–1.72] | 1.35 [1.18–1.55] | < 0.001 |
Gestational age < 37 weeks (ref = ≥ 37 weeks)b | 7771 | 0.87 [0.55–1.38] | 0.84 [0.32–2.21] | 1.48 [0.90–2.43] | 1.17 [0.70–1.93] | 0.511 |
Birth weight < 2.5 kg (ref = ≥ 2.5 kg)b | 7622 | 0.97 [0.59–1.59] | 2.35 [1.27–4.35] | 1.12 [0.57–2.17] | 1.14 [0.64–2.04] | 0.081 |
Maternal history of bedwetting after age 5 (ref = none)c | 8516 | 2.75 [2.08–3.64] | 1.31 [0.82–2.38] | 1.66 [1.07–2.56] | 3.77 [2.81–5.06] | 0.001 |
Maternal history of daytime wetting after age 5 (ref = none)c | 8516 | 0.31 [0.03–3.66] | 2.41 [0.59–9.87] | 2.35 [0.92–6.03] | 3.60 [1.75–7.40] | 0.004 |
Paternal history of bedwetting after age 5 (ref = none)c | 6296 | 1.67 [1.20–2.32] | 1.49 [0.84–2.66] | 1.57 [1.04–2.37] | 1.53 [1.00–2.33] | 0.002 |
Paternal history of daytime wetting after age 5 (ref = none)c | 6296 | 0.95 [0.06–15.13] | 2.91 [0.32–26.47] | 8.82 [3.01–25.8] | 2.34 [0.33–16.81] | 0.002 |
Psychosocial factors | ||||||
Difficult temperament at 2 yearsd | ||||||
Activity | 6613 | 1.13 [1.03–1.25] | 1.07 [0.91–1.26] | 1.27 [1.12–1.44] | 1.03 [0.91–1.17] | < 0.001 |
Adaptability | 6568 | 1.17 [1.06–1.29] | 1.21 [1.02–1.45] | 1.27 [1.12–1.44] | 1.25 [1.10–1.41] | < 0.001 |
Intensity | 6390 | 1.07 [0.97–1.18] | 1.04 [0.87–1.26] | 1.18 [1.01–1.37] | 1.24 [1.09–1.41] | < 0.001 |
Mood | 6590 | 1.12 [1.01–1.23] | 1.34 [1.13–1.58] | 1.33 [1.18–1.51] | 1.26 [1.11–1.44] | < 0.001 |
Persistence | 6508 | 1.09 [0.99–1.21] | 1.04 [0.87–1.24] | 1.15 [1.00–1.33] | 1.21 [1.06–1.38] | 0.003 |
Behavior/emotional problems at 3½ yearse | ||||||
Emotional difficulties | 6696 | 1.04 [0.95–1.14] | 1.11 [0.94–1.32] | 1.21 [1.06–1.38] | 1.20 [1.06–1.36] | < 0.001 |
Conduct difficulties | 6696 | 1.17 [1.06–1.29] | 1.29 [1.08–1.54] | 1.61 [1.42–1.82] | 1.28 [1.14–1.44] | < 0.001 |
Hyperactivity | 6696 | 1.05 [0.95–1.15] | 1.05 [0.88–1.25] | 1.37 [1.22–1.55] | 1.24 [1.11–1.39] | < 0.001 |
Low level of prosocial behaviour | 6696 | 1.02 [0.92–1.13] | 1.05 [0.87–1.28] | 1.39 [1.21–1.59] | 1.17 [1.03–1.32] | < 0.001 |
Total behaviour difficulties | 6696 | 1.20 [1.08–1.32] | 1.33 [1.12–1.59] | 1.80 [1.59–2.03] | 1.56 [1.39–1.74] | < 0.001 |
Temper tantrums at least once a day at 3½ years (ref = once a week or rarer)e | 6644 | 1.15 [0.88–1.52] | 1.26 [0.80–2.00] | 1.37 [0.97–1.96] | 1.55 [1.13–2.13] | 0.010 |
Stressful events between 2½ and 3 years 11 months | 6626 | 1.05 [0.94–1.17] | 1.25 [1.05–1.49] | 1.20 [1.04–1.39] | 1.26 [1.11–1.43] | < 0.001 |
Maternal depression | ||||||
Antenatal (18 weeks)f | 7345 | 1.18 [0.88–1.60] | 0.96 [0.54–1.69] | 1.03 [0.66–1.63] | 1.47 [1.05–2.07] | 0.166 |
Antenatal (32 weeks)f | 7711 | 1.29 [0.98–1.69] | 1.27 [0.79–2.02] | 1.31 [0.89–1.92] | 1.29 [0.93–1.78] | 0.085 |
Postnatal (21 months)g | 6880 | 1.30 [0.93–1.83] | 2.38 [1.46–3.88] | 1.19 [0.69–2.03] | 2.09 [1.48–2.95] | < 0.001 |
Toilet training and constipation | ||||||
Age at initiation of toilet training (ref = 15–24 months)h | ||||||
Before 6 months | 6615 | 0.78 [0.36–1.69] | 0.71 [0.15–3.26] | 0.31 [0.02–4.06] | 1.08 [0.45–2.60] | 0.001 |
6–15 months | 1.12 [0.84–1.49] | 1.31 [0.78–2.20] | 0.89 [0.54–1.48] | 1.10 [0.74–1.64] | ||
After 24 months | 1.01 [0.82–1.25] | 1.73 [1.18–2.55] | 1.58 [1.18–2.10] | 1.59 [1.19–2.21] | ||
Constipation at 4 years 9 months (ref = none)i | 6624 | 0.82 [0.60–1.11] | 1.29 [0.74–2.27] | 1.49 [1.06–2.09] | 1.14 [0.81–1.61] | 0.035 |
The exact sample size for each analysis differed depending on the availability of non-missing data on biopsychosocial factors and confounders. Odds ratios for these analyses were derived in relation to the normative class. For the continuous variables (developmental level, difficult temperament, psychological problems, stressful events), results show the increase in odds of membership to each latent class per one standard deviation (SD) increase in the score
aAdjusted for child’s sex (0 = female vs. 1 = male), socioeconomic variables (parental social class [0 = non-manual: professional, managerial or skilled professions vs. 1 = manual: partly or unskilled occupations], early parenthood [0 = child born when mother was >=19 years vs. 1 = child born when mother was < 19 years], maternal education [0 = high school qualifications or greater vs. 1 = vocational or none], housing adequacy [0 = yes vs. 1 = no: comprising crowding, periods of homelessness, living conditions, major defects/infestation], major financial difficulties [0 = no vs. 1 = yes], family size [0 = less than 3 children vs. 1 = 3 or more children], presence of a social network [0 = presence of network vs. 1 = no network: comprising lack of emotional, practical and/or financial support], maternal antenatal depression (32 weeks) and postnatal depression (21 months)
bAdjusted for child’s sex, socioeconomic variables, and maternal antenatal depression (32 weeks)
cAdjusted for child’s sex
dAdjusted for child’s sex, socioeconomic variables, developmental level of child at 18 months, and maternal postnatal depression (21 months)
eAdjusted for child’s sex, socioeconomic variables, developmental level of child at 18 months, and maternal postnatal depression (33 months)
fAdjusted for child’s sex and socioeconomic variables
gAdjusted for child’s sex, socioeconomic variables, and maternal antenatal depression (18 weeks)
hAdjusted for child’s sex, socioeconomic variables, developmental level of child at 18 months, and maternal postnatal depression (21 months)
iAdjusted for child’s sex, socioeconomic variables, developmental level of child at 18 months, and maternal postnatal depression (33 months)
Statistical modelling
We estimated the association between the biopsychosocial factors and the latent classes using a series of univariable multinomial logistic regression models and employing the normative latent class as the baseline category for the outcome. Parameter estimates were obtained using the “Modal ML” 3-step method [38] implemented in Mplus. This has been shown to produce less-biased estimates than traditional three-step methods such as probability weighting, whilst avoiding the problem of covariates impacting on the measurement model itself [39]. Bias-adjusted estimates were obtained using the Mplus “auxiliary (r3step)” command.
Results
We focused on the sample of children (n = 8751, 4507 boys, 4244 girls) with bedwetting and daytime wetting data available for at least three of the five time points. Table 1 shows the distribution of socio-demographic variables across the latent classes. Apart from maternal education, there was little evidence of an association between the socio-demographic factors and childhood UI. Table 2 presents the results of the adjusted analysis examining the associations between the biopsychosocial factors and membership of the latent classes (see table S2 for unadjusted results).
Biological and developmental factors
Developmental delay at 18 months was strongly associated with increased odds of daytime wetting alone, delayed and persistent wetting, but not bedwetting alone. Low birth weight was associated with daytime wetting alone, but gestational age was not associated with childhood UI. Maternal history of bedwetting after age 5 was strongly associated with bedwetting alone, delayed bladder control and persistent wetting (associations were stronger than those found for paternal history of bedwetting). Maternal history of daytime wetting after age 5 was associated with persistent wetting, whilst paternal history of daytime wetting was associated with delayed bladder control. Child’s male sex was an important confounder for bedwetting alone, delayed and persistent wetting classes, whilst female sex was an important confounder for daytime wetting alone.
Psychosocial factors
There was an association between difficult temperament (especially adaptability and mood) and childhood UI. There was evidence that behaviour/emotional problems were associated with UI; the strongest associations were found for the delayed class. Emotional difficulties, hyperactivity and prosocial behaviour were only associated with the delayed and persistent wetting classes, whilst there was no evidence of an association with either bedwetting or daytime wetting alone. Conduct difficulties and total behaviour problems were associated with all the UI classes. There was strong evidence that temper tantrums in early childhood were associated with persistent wetting. In these analyses, the most important confounders were male sex (bedwetting alone, persistent wetting), female sex (daytime wetting alone), developmental delay and maternal postnatal depression.
It is notable that stressful events were associated with all the UI classes except bedwetting alone. There was some evidence of confounding when persistent wetting was the outcome (male sex, developmental delay and maternal postnatal depression were the most important confounders).
Antenatal maternal depression at 18 weeks (but not at 32 weeks) was associated with persistent wetting (male sex, maternal education and housing inadequacy were confounders). Postnatal depression was strongly associated with daytime wetting alone and persistent wetting (female sex was a confounder in the analysis with daytime wetting).
Toilet training and constipation
Initiation of toilet training after 24 months was strongly associated with daytime wetting alone, delayed bladder control and persistent wetting at school age, but not bedwetting alone. Confounders were male sex (female sex in analysis with daytime wetting alone), developmental delay and maternal postnatal depression.
Constipation in early childhood was associated with delayed attainment of bladder control after adjusting for confounders (developmental delay was the most influential confounder). Constipation was associated with persistent wetting in the unadjusted, but not the adjusted model (confounders were male sex, housing inadequacy, developmental delay and maternal postnatal depression).
Discussion
Main findings
This is the first study to prospectively examine a wide range of biopsychosocial factors in relation to different trajectories of childhood UI. Parental history of bedwetting after age 5 was strongly associated with bedwetting alone, delayed and persistent trajectories of wetting in offspring (the association was strongest for maternal bedwetting). Maternal history of daytime wetting was associated with persistent wetting, whilst paternal daytime wetting was associated with delayed bladder control in offspring. In general, difficult temperament and behaviour/emotional problems were most strongly associated with the combined trajectories of childhood UI (delayed and persistent wetting). Maternal (antenatal and postnatal) depression was associated with persistent wetting and postnatal depression was associated with daytime wetting. Developmental delay, stressful events, and later initiation of toilet training were not associated with bedwetting alone, but were strongly associated with the other trajectories of childhood UI.
Strengths and limitations
Major strengths of this study include the prospective design and use of parallel latent classes of UI derived from repeated measures of bedwetting and daytime wetting in a large cohort. Another strength is the availability of a wide range of biopsychosocial factors and extensive data on confounders. A possible limitation is the use of maternal reports of difficult temperament and behaviour/emotional problems since parents of children with UI may be more negatively predisposed to their children and, hence, more likely to report these problems. However, bedwetting is not normally considered problematic in early childhood (at the time of assessing these problems), reducing the likelihood of biased maternal assessments. We are also limited by having only five time points when parents were asked about their child’s UI.
Comparison with earlier findings
Biological and developmental factors
We found evidence that developmental delay at 18 months was associated with daytime wetting alone, delayed bladder control and persistent wetting, but not bedwetting alone. Earlier studies reported that maturational deficits are linked to bedwetting [2–7], but they did not differentiate between bedwetting with or without daytime wetting. One small study, comparing rates of delayed development in children with bedwetting, daytime wetting and mixed day and night wetting to controls, found that children with bedwetting had higher rates of delayed development than controls [3]. We found no evidence that gestational age is associated with childhood UI, but low birth weight was linked to daytime wetting. Earlier studies found that low birth weight children were more likely to have UI [4, 5].
Maternal history of bedwetting was strongly associated with bedwetting alone, delayed attainment of bladder control and persistent wetting in offspring. Weaker associations were found for paternal history of bedwetting. We also found associations between parental history of daytime wetting and offspring UI. The association between parental and offspring UI was previously examined in the ALSPAC cohort, but childhood UI was examined at a single time point (age 7½) and the study did not exclude children with daytime wetting from the bedwetting group and vice versa, nor did they examine the group with combined wetting separately [31]. Previous studies have shown that bedwetting is highly familial [2–5] and one study found a familial influence for both bedwetting and daytime wetting.3 In comparison to our finding of a stronger association for maternal bedwetting and offspring UI, a previous study found that paternal bedwetting history had a stronger association with offspring bedwetting, but the confidence intervals overlapped [4].
Psychosocial factors
Child temperament and behaviour/emotional problems generally had the strongest associations with the combined trajectories of UI (delayed bladder control and persistent wetting) and the weakest associations with bedwetting alone. Previous studies have found higher levels of psychological problems in children with combined wetting, but these were cross-sectional [26, 27]. It is notable that the hyperactivity scale was only associated with the combined trajectories of UI. Previous studies have reported higher rates of ADHD in children with bedwetting and daytime wetting [40] but the majority are cross-sectional and relied on parental reports, few distinguished between different trajectories of UI and some did not have a comparison group of children without UI.
It is commonly believed that bedwetting is associated with stress, but we found that stressful events are associated with all the UI classes except bedwetting alone. Two earlier prospective studies examined this link but they did not distinguish between bedwetting, daytime wetting alone and combined wetting [11, 22]. We did not distinguish between life events that are acute (e.g. bereavement) versus chronic (e.g. financial problems) or between normative (e.g. birth of a sibling) and non-normative (unpredictable) stressors (e.g. accidents). It is difficult to isolate the effect of single types of stressful events because they are often interrelated.
There was strong evidence that exposure to postnatal depression is associated with daytime wetting alone and persistent wetting. There was also evidence for an association between antenatal depression (at 18 weeks) and persistent wetting. Earlier studies have reported a link between maternal psychopathology and bedwetting, but they did not distinguish between bedwetting with or without daytime wetting [10, 33]. One study examined only parental lifetime psychopathology and did not separately examine maternal psychopathology or the timing of the disorder (antenatal or postnatal) [10]. The other study only examined exposure to maternal psychopathology in the postnatal period [33].
Toilet training and constipation
We found strong evidence that later initiation of toilet training is associated with daytime and combined wetting, but not bedwetting alone. This is consistent with an earlier study reporting that the development of nocturnal bladder control is a maturational process that is not affected by parental potty training practices [13]. Previous studies have found that delayed or inadequate toilet training is associated with bladder dysfunction (e.g. urgency, urge incontinence, emptying difficulties, bladder instability and/or dyscoordinated micturition) [14–16]. Another study found that early toilet training (before 18 months) was associated with a lower rate of daytime wetting and bedwetting [41].
We found that constipation in early childhood is only associated with delayed bladder control at school age. Constipation has frequently been found to be associated with childhood UI, but the majority of the studies are cross-sectional [17–19].
Possible mechanisms explaining the associations between the biopsychosocial factors and childhood UI
The current findings provide evidence for differential associations between biopsychosocial factors and bedwetting alone, daytime wetting alone and combined wetting. Developmental delay was related to daytime wetting alone and combined (day and night) wetting, but not with bedwetting alone. According to the ICCS classification, monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis (bedwetting alone) is attributed to nocturnal polyuria and a deficit in the basic inhibitory function of the brainstem [24] while non-monosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis (bedwetting accompanied by daytime urinary symptoms) is often attributed to overactive bladder [25]. Bedwetting has been described as a genetically determined maturational disorder of the CNS [7] and, consistent with earlier studies, we find evidence for a strong association between parent and offspring bedwetting. Compared with the combined trajectories of childhood UI, bedwetting alone generally had weaker associations with early behaviour/emotional problems and was not associated with exposure to early stressful life events or maternal depression. Additionally, age at initiation of toilet training was not associated with bedwetting alone, but was associated with daytime and combined trajectories of UI. This suggests that external stressors or toilet training practices do not affect processes relating to nocturnal bladder control and that bedwetting alone is more strongly related to inherited factors.
We found strong associations between maternal history of bedwetting and offspring bedwetting alone and combined (day and night) wetting in offspring. Maternal-offspring associations were stronger than those found for paternal history of wetting and the most likely reason for this is the Carter effect [42]. Bedwetting is mainly transmitted by polygenic inheritance and males are more affected than females [43]. The less commonly affected sex (females) will carry a higher load of genes; therefore, the strength of the mother–offspring association will be greater than the father–offspring association. The associations we found between parental history of daytime wetting and offspring UI were less consistent. Few studies have examined heritability of daytime wetting because daytime wetting is even more heterogeneous than bedwetting [43] and environmental factors are thought to play a greater role in some subtypes of daytime wetting [44].
Exposure to maternal depression in the antenatal (18 weeks) and postnatal periods was associated with persistent wetting and postnatal depression was related to daytime wetting alone. Antenatal depression is linked to higher cortisol levels in offspring [45] and HPA axis alterations are associated with adverse developmental outcomes in offspring [46]. Exposure to postnatal depression at 21 months coincides with a peak period of toilet training initiation in this cohort [37]. At around this age, there is thought to be a sensitive period for the attainment of bladder control and stress can interfere with this process [12]. The association with postnatal depression could be due to depressed mothers being less able to cope with the demands of toilet training and less responsive to their child’s toileting needs which, in turn, may increase the child’s stress levels. Further studies are needed with detailed data on toilet training practices to allow examination of the relationship between maternal depression and offspring behaviour/emotional problems in influencing toilet training outcomes.
The associations we report here could also be due to a shared genetic susceptibility for negative affect since there is strong evidence for a link between affective symptoms and lower urinary tract symptoms, particularly overactive bladder [47]. Further research is needed to determine whether there is a causal association between affective symptoms and urinary symptoms.
Constipation was only associated with delayed attainment of bladder control. Previous studies have found that constipation is more common in children with developmental delay [48], which could explain our finding.
Conclusion
We find evidence that different trajectories of childhood UI are differentially associated with biopsychosocial factors. This knowledge could inform medical history taking and potentially aid the identification of children at risk of persistent continence problems. Our finding of prospective associations between psychological problems and continence problems has important clinical implications. It is important to know about associations between psychological factors and UI, because these factors can affect treatment adherence and persistence of continence problems could be due to untreated psychological factors [49]. There is a need for an increased understanding of the role of psychological factors in relation to continence problems and further research is needed to determine whether these associations are likely to be causal. Prospective associations are not definitive evidence of causal effects because residual confounding and reverse causation are key problems in observational epidemiology. If causal effects exist between psychological factors and UI, treatments aimed at reducing psychological problems in children and mothers could lead to a reduction in childhood continence problems.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Acknowledgements
This study is based on the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council, the Wellcome Trust (Grant reference: 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. This research was specifically funded by a Grant from the Medical Research Council (Increasing understanding of risk factors and outcomes associated with continence problems in children and adolescents. MRC reference: MR/L007231/1).
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.
References
- 1.Austin PF, Bauer S, Bower W, et al. The standardization of terminology of bladder function in children and adolescents: update report from the Standardization Committee of the International Children’s Continence Society (ICCS) Neurourol Urodyn. 2016;35:471–481. doi: 10.1002/nau.22751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Shannon FT. Factors related to the age of attainment of nighttime bladder control: an 8-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics. 1986;78:884–890. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Järvelin MR, Moilanen I, Kangas P, et al. Aetiological and precipitating factors for childhood enuresis. Acta Paediatr Scand. 1991;80:361–369. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1991.tb11863.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Järvelin MR, Vikeväinen-Tervonen L, Moilanen I, et al. Enuresis in seven-year-old children. Acta Paediatr Scand. 1988;77:148–153. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1988.tb10614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Jarvelin MR. Developmental history and neurological findings in enuretic children. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1989;31:728–736. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1989.tb04068.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Touchette E, Petit D, Paquet J, et al. Bed-wetting and its association with developmental milestones in early childhood. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159:1129–1134. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.12.1129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.von Gontard A, Freitag CM, Seifen S, et al. Neuromotor development in nighttime enuresis. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:744–750. doi: 10.1017/S0012162206001599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Byrd RS, Weitzman M, Lanphear NE, et al. Bed-wetting in US children: epidemiology and related behaviour problems. Pediatrics. 1996;98:414–419. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.von Gontard A, Baeyens D, Van Hoecke E, et al. Psychological and psychiatric issues in urinary and fecal incontinence. J Urol. 2011;185:1432–1436. doi: 10.1016/j.juro.2010.11.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kessel EM, Allmann AE, Goldstein BL, et al. Predictors and outcomes of childhood primary enuresis. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;56:250–257. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Douglas JWB (1973) Early disturbing events and later enuresis. In: Kolvin I, MacKeith RC, Meadow SR (eds) Bladder control and enuresis. Heinemann, London, pp. 109–117
- 12.Järvelin MR, Moilanen I, Vikeväinen-Tervonen L, et al. Life changes and protective capacities in enuretic and non-enuretic children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1990;31:763–774. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb00816.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Largo RH, Molinari L, von Siebenthal K, et al. Does a profound change in toilet-training affect development of bowel and bladder control? Dev Med Child Neurol. 1996;38:1106–1116. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1996.tb15074.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bakker E, Wyndaele JJ. Changes in the toilet training of children during the last 60 years: the cause of an increase in lower urinary tract dysfunction? BJU Int. 2000;86:248–252. doi: 10.1046/j.1464-410x.2000.00737.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hellstrom AL. Influence of potty training habits on dysfunctional bladder in children. Lancet. 2000;356:1787. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)03228-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hodges SJ, Richards KA, Gorbachinsky I, et al. The association of age of toilet training and dysfunctional voiding. Res Rep Urol. 2014;6:127–130. doi: 10.2147/RRU.S66839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cayan S, Doruk E, Bozlu M, et al. The assessment of constipation in monosymptomatic primary nocturnal enuresis. Int Urol Nephrol. 2001;33:513–516. doi: 10.1023/A:1019546108685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Loening-Baucke V. Urinary incontinence and urinary tract infection and their resolution with treatment of chronic constipation of childhood. Pediatrics. 1997;100:228–232. doi: 10.1542/peds.100.2.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.O’Regan S, Yazbeck S, Hamberger B, et al. Constipation: a commonly unrecognized cause of enuresis. Am J Dis Child. 1986;140:260–261. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1986.02140170086039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sullivan S, Joinson C, Heron J. Factors predicting atypical development of nighttime bladder control. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2015;36:724–733. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Joinson C, Sullivan S, von Gontard A, et al. Early childhood psychological factors and risk for bedwetting at school age in a UK cohort. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;25:519–528. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0756-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Joinson C, Sullivan S, von Gontard A, et al. Stressful events in early childhood and developmental trajectories of bedwetting at school age. J Pediatr Psychol. 2016;41:1002–1010. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsw025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Joinson C, Heron J, von Gontard A, et al. Early childhood risk factors associated with daytime wetting and soiling in school-age children. J Pediatr Psychol. 2008;33:739–750. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Neveus T, Eggert P, Evans J, et al. Evaluation of and treatment for monosymptomatic enuresis: a standardization document from the International Children’s Continence Society. J Urol. 2010;183:441–447. doi: 10.1016/j.juro.2009.10.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Franco I, von Gontard A, De Gennaro M. Evaluation and treatment of nonmonosymptomatic nocturnal enuresis: a standardization document from the International Children’s Continence Society. J Pediatr Urol. 2013;9:234–243. doi: 10.1016/j.jpurol.2012.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Van Hoecke E, De Fruyt F, De Clercq B, et al. Internalizing and externalizing problem behavior in children with nocturnal and diurnal enuresis: a five-factor model perspective. J Pediatr Psychol. 2006;31(5):460–468. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Theunis M, Van Hoecke E, Paesbrugge S, et al. Self-image and performance in children with nocturnal enuresis. Eur Urol. 2002;41(6):660–667. doi: 10.1016/S0302-2838(02)00127-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Heron J, Grzeda MT, von Gontard A, et al. Trajectories of urinary incontinence in childhood and bladder and bowel symptoms in adolescence: prospective cohort study. BMJ Open. 2017;7:e014238. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod JAA, et al. Cohort Profile: the ‘Children of the 90s’—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:111–127. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Frankenburg WK, Dodds J, Archer P, et al. The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver Developmental Screening Test. Pediatrics. 1992;89:91–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.von Gontard A, Heron J, Joinson C. Family history of nocturnal enuresis and urinary incontinence—results from a large epidemiological study. J Urol. 2011;185:2303–2306. doi: 10.1016/j.juro.2011.02.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Fullard W, McDevitt SC, Carey WB. Assessing temperament in one-to-three year-old children. J Pediatr Psychol. 1984;9:205–216. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/9.2.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Joinson C, Heron J, Butler R, et al. Development of nighttime bladder control from 4–9 years: association with dimensions of parent rated child maturational level, child temperament and maternal psychopathology. Longitud Life Course Stud. 2009;1:73–94. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Elander J, Rutter M. Use and development of the Rutter parents’ and teachers’ scales. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 1996;6:63–78. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1234-988X(199607)6:2<63::AID-MPR151>3.3.CO;2-M. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150:782–786. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Evans J, Heron J, Francomb H, et al. Cohort study of depressed mood during pregnancy and after childbirth. BMJ. 2001;323:257–260. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7307.257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Joinson C, Heron J, Von Gontard A, et al. A prospective study of age at initiation of toilet training and subsequent daytime bladder control in school-age children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2009;30:385–393. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181ba0e77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Vermunt JK. Latent class modeling with covariates: two improved three-step approaches. Political Anal. 2010;18:450–469. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpq025. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Asparouhov T, Muthen BO (2015) Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: 3-step approaches using Mplus. Mplus Web Notes: No 15. http://www.statmodel.com/examples/webnotes/webnote15.pdf. Accessed 19 May 2017
- 40.von Gontard A, Equit M. Comorbidity of ADHD and incontinence in children. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015;24:127–140. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0577-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Yang S, Zhao L, Chang S. Early intiation of toilet training for urine was associated with early urinary continence and does not appear to be associated with bladder dysfunction. Neurourol Urodyn. 2011;30:1253–1257. doi: 10.1002/nau.20982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Carter CO. The inheritance of congenital pyloric stenosis. Br Med Bull. 1961;17:251–254. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a069918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.von Gontard A, Schaumburg H, Hollmann E, et al. The genetics of enuresis: a review. J Urol. 2001;166:2438–2443. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5347(05)65611-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.von Gontard A, de Jong TPVM, Badawi JK, et al. Psychological and physical environmental factors in the development of incontinence in adults and children. J Wound Ostomy Continence Nurs. 2017;44:181–187. doi: 10.1097/WON.0000000000000308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Brennan PA, Pargas R, Walker EF, et al. Maternal depression and infant cortisol: influences of timing, comorbidity and treatment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49:1099–1107. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01914.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Glover V, O’Connor TG, O’Donnell K. Prenatal stress and the programming of the HPA axis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;35:17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Vrijens D, Drossaerts J, van Koeveringe G, et al. Affective symptoms and the overactive bladder—a systematic review. J Psychosom Res. 2015;78:95–108. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Chaidez V, Hansen RL, Hertz-Picciotto I. Gastrointestinal problems in children with autism, developmental delays or typical development. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:1117–1127. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1973-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Van Herzeele C, De Bruyne P, De Bruyne E, Walle JV. Challenging factors for enuresis treatment: psychological problems and non-adherence. J Pediatr Urol. 2015;11:308–313. doi: 10.1016/j.jpurol.2015.04.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.