Skip to main content
Sleep Science logoLink to Sleep Science
. 2018 Jul-Aug;11(4):231–238. doi: 10.5935/1984-0063.20180037

Associations Between Sleep Quality, Sleep Architecture and Sleep Disordered Breathing and Memory After Continuous Positive Airway Pressure in Patients with Obstructive Sleep Apnea in the Apnea Positive Pressure Long-term Efficacy Study (APPLES)

Stuart F Quan 1,2,3,, Rohit Budhiraja 1, Clete A Kushida 4
PMCID: PMC6361302  PMID: 30746040

Abstract

Objective

The role of sleep architecture in consolidation of memory has not been extensively investigated. In this study, the association of continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) and sleep architecture and quality, and sleep disordered breathing on changes in memory are explored during the course of a 6 month clinical trial of CPAP or sham CPAP (APPLES).

Methods

848 participants had polysomnographic and memory assessments (Buschke Selective Reminding Test [Buschke] and Digit Symbol Substitution Test [DSST]) at baseline, CPAP/Sham CPAP titration, and the 2 and 6 month time points. Half were assigned to the CPAP and Sham CPAP groups respectively. Changes in performance on the Buschke and the DSST were analyzed over the course of the study between CPAP and Sham CPAP as well as in relationship to changes in sleep architecture, sleep quality and sleep disordered breathing (SDB).

Results

Sleep architecture, sleep quality and SDB improved in the CPAP group at 6 months; performance on the Buschke and DSST improved equally in both CPAP and Sham CPAP groups. There also were no significant correlations between changes in the amount or percentage of sleep stages between baseline and the 6 months, and corresponding changes in either the Buschke or the DSST. However, when stratified by the upper quartile and lower 3 quartiles, greater changes in the Buschke occurred over 6 months in the top quartile of total sleep time (5.7±7.3 vs. 4.0±6.8, p≤0.01) and amount of N3 sleep (55.9±7.7 vs. 53.6±8.9 min, p≤0.01). Those with more %N3 at 6 months scored better on the Buschke as well (55.9±7.8 vs. 53.6±8.9, p≤0.01). Borderline improvement in the DSST over 6 months was observed in the top quartiles of amount of N3 and %N3. Those in the top quartile of the amount of REM and %REM also showed greater improvement in the Buschke after 6 months. No differences were observed for the AHI, but those in the top quartile of oxygen desaturation had worse scores on the Buschke at 6 months. CPAP/Sham CPAP adherence did not impact 6 month Buschke or DSST performance.

Conclusions

CPAP improved long-term sleep duration, quality and architecture, but did not memory. However, large changes in REM and N3 sleep as well as moderate amounts of nocturnal hypoxemia are associated with changes in assessments of memory.

Keywords: Sleep, Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome, Memory, Continuous Positive Airway Pressure

INTRODUCTION

There is general consensus that retention of memories occurs through a two-step process in which sleep plays an essential role for optimum performance. Initially, new information is encoded into temporary storage. Subsequently, some of this information is consolidated into long-term storage where it can be recalled when needed1. Sleep has been identified as a critical component for this latter activity to occur2. However, the role that various sleep stages play in this process appear to differ. Substantial evidence points to the importance of neuronal activity in the hippocampus during slow wave sleep (SWS) in strengthening declarative memory3. In contrast, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep may be important in enhancing procedural learning as well as for contextual and spatial memory consolidation4.

Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) is often characterized by neurocognitive deficits including impairment in memory5. Some, but not all studies have demonstrated that memory improves after OSA treatment with continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP)6-10. Whether improvement in memory after treatment with CPAP is correlated with changes in sleep architecture is unclear and has not been extensively studied2.

The Apnea Positive Pressure Long-term Efficacy Study (APPLES) was a 6 month randomized sham-controlled study of the impact of CPAP on various neurocognitive domains in patients with OSA. Polysomnograms and neurocognitive assessments were performed before and after intervention with CPAP or sham CPAP11. Thus, the study provides a vehicle for studying whether changes in memory are associated with corresponding changes in sleep architecture. It is hypothesized that improvement in memory occurs with CPAP and that it is associated with increases in REM sleep and SWS.

METHODS

participants and study design

The study design, recruitment procedures, and inclusion and exclusion criteria for APPLES have been described extensively11. The institutional review board (IRB) at each site approved the study protocol and the study was registered at ClinTrials.gov (NCT00051363). Briefly, APPLES was a multisite study conducted at 5 clinical centers: Stanford University, Stanford, CA; University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ; Providence St. Mary Medical Center, Walla Walla, WA; St. Luke's Hospital, Chesterfield, MO; and Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston, MA. Participants were recruited into the study primarily from patients scheduled into a regular sleep clinic for evaluation of possible OSA, and from local advertising. Recruitment began in November 2003 and was completed in August 2008.

Initial enrollment required age > 18 years and clinical symptoms of OSA, as defined by American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) criteria12. At enrollment, participants underwent a screening diagnostic polysomnogram (PSG) and baseline neurocognitive testing including the standardized assessments described below. Only participants with an apnea hypopnea index (AHI) > 10 events per hour continued to the clinical trial and were randomized subsequently to sham or active CPAP for 6 months as previously reported13. Those participants had a CPAP or sham CPAP titration polysomnogram and subsequently had a repeat polysomnogram on their assigned treatment 2 and 6 months later.

Excluded were individuals who had 1) prior OSA treatment with CPAP or surgery, 2) household members with current/past CPAP use, 3) a sleepiness-related automobile accident within the year prior to potential enrollment, (4) oxygen saturations < 75% for > 10% of the diagnostic polysomnogram (PSG) total sleep time; or (5) conditions or use of medications that could potentially affect neurocognitive function and/or alertness. For the present analysis, only data from randomized participants who had polysomnography and neurocognitive testing at the baseline, 2 and 6 month time points were used. Some of the material related to sleepiness and neurocognitive testing reported herein represent reanalysis of data in a different format from what has been published in a previous paper9.

Polysomnography

Polysomnography was conducted as previously described using signals from a nasal pressure cannula, nasal/oral thermistor, thoracic and abdominal piezo bands, and a pulse oximeter to classify apnea and hypopnea events11. An apnea was identified by a > 90% amplitude decrease from baseline of the nasal pressure signal lasting > 10 sec. Hypopneas were scored if there was a > 50%, but < 90% decrease from baseline of the nasal pressure signal, or if there was a clear amplitude reduction of the nasal pressure signal that did not reach the above criterion but it was associated with either an oxygen desaturation ≥ 3% or an arousal, and the event duration was ≥ 10 seconds.

Obstructive apneas were identified by persistence of chest or abdominal respiratory effort during flow cessation. Central apneas were noted if no displacement occurred on either the thoracic or abdominal channels. The severity of OSA was expressed using the apnea hypopnea index (number of apneas + hypopneas/total sleep time, AHI). Magnitude of oxygen desaturation was indicated by the % of the total sleep time less than 85% (O2LT85%). All studies were scored at the central reading center located at Stanford University.

CPAP Adherence

Adherence to CPAP or sham CPAP was measured objectively using Encore Pro SmartCards (Phillips Respironics, Inc., Murrysville, PA) that were returned by the participant twice monthly. For this report, the mean hours of daily use were analyzed for the preceding 1-month period before the 2- and 6-month study visits.

Assessment of Sleepiness

Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS)

The ESS is a validated self-administered questionnaire that asks an individual to rate his or her probability of falling asleep on a scale of increasing probability from 0 to 3 in 8 different situations14. The scores for the 8 questions are summed to obtain a single score from 0 to 24 that is indicative of self-reported sleep propensity. The ESS prior to randomization was administered at the time of the clinical evaluation and on the night of the diagnostic PSG. For a baseline, the value at the time of the diagnostic PSG was used, but if not available, then the value at the time of the clinical evaluation was substituted.

Assessments of Learning and Memory

Buschke Selective Reminding Test (Buschke)

The Buschke is an assessment of verbal learning and memory15. The version used in APPLES consisted of a list of 12 unrelated verbally presented words which the participant was asked to recall on successive trials. Non-recalled words on 6 successive trials were selectively re-presented or until the participant recalled the entire list on 3 consecutive trials. A delayed recall trial with forewarning was administered 30 minutes later. The sum recall (total number of word recalled over 6 trials) was used in these primary analyses.

Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST)

The CogScreen analogue of the DSST was used in APPLES16. The DSST evaluates sustained attention, visual scanning, information processing speed, immediate and delayed visual paired-associate memory and working memory. The participant is asked to match numbers (1-9) to their corresponding hieroglyphic-like symbol, and type them on a keyboard as quickly as possible within a 90-second time frame. The percent correct was used for these analyses.

Statistical analyses

Inasmuch as this was a subgroup analysis, outcome variables were defined post hoc based on our initial hypotheses pertaining to the impact of CPAP on memory and preliminary exploration of the data. Changes in outcome variables over time were analyzed using a mixed model repeated measures analysis of variance with participants stratified by their randomization group (CPAP or Sham CPAP). Groups differences were determined using analysis of variance or Student's t test as appropriate. Associations between variables were assessed by calculating Pearson correlation coefficients. Data are expressed as mean + standard deviation (SD) or percentages. P<0.05 was considered statistically significant. Analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 24 (Chicago, IL).

Results

As shown in Table 1, of the 1104 participants randomized at baseline, there were 848 who had polysomnographic, Buschke and DSST data at baseline, CPAP/Sham CPAP titration, and the 2 and 6 month time points. There were 442 assigned to CPAP and 406 assigned to Sham CPAP. Approximately 66% were men, and most were non Hispanic white. Their mean age was 52±12 years with a BMI of 32.2±7.1 kg/m2. Overall, participants had severe OSA with a mean AHI of 40.7±24.9 /hour. There were no differences in OSA severity between groups (results not shown).

Table 1.

Baseline Demographic Information.

  N %
Total Participants 848  
% Men 552 65.1
Ethnicity    
Non Hispanic White 647 76.3
Black 75 8.8
Hispanic 60 7.1
Asian 50 5.9
Native American 13 1.5
Other 3 0.4
Age 52±12 years
Body Mass Index (kg/m2) 32.2±7.1
Apnea Hypopnea Index (#/hour) 40.7±24.9
% Time Oxygen Saturations < 85% 2.2±6.2
Group Assignment    
Sham 406 47.9
CPAP 442 52.1
Compliance with CPAP/Sham CPAP at 6 months, Hours)    
Sham 354 3.5±2.3
CPAP 403 4.5±2.3

Changes in sleep and sleep architecture are displayed in Table 2. Total sleep time, sleep efficiency and arousal index improved immediately with use of CPAP in comparison to Sham CPAP. In addition, the absolute amount and % of N1 sleep decreased with a corresponding increase in the absolute amount and % of the remaining sleep stages. These changes in sleep duration, quality and architecture were generally maintained over the entire 6 month length of the study. In addition, improvements in sleep architecture correlated with better adherence to CPAP. No associations with adherence were observed in the Sham CPAP group (results not shown).

Table 2.

Changes in Sleep and Sleep Architecture after Sham or CPAP.

  Diagnostic CPAP 2 Months 6 Months
Total Sleep Time (TST)        
Sham 375.9±63.2 346.2±71.5 391.6±62.7 389.8±59.5
CPAP 376.1 ± 66.7 358.7±72.3 395.5±61.7 397.8±61.2
Sleep Efficiency (%TST)        
Sham 78.0±12.2 75.4±13.5 81.7±11.6 81.3±11.6
CPAP 78.4±13.3 77.6±13.5 82.0±11.7 82.8±12.3*
Arousal Index (#/h)        
Sham 30.4±21.9 32.3±19.7 30.5±20.1 30.0±20.5
CPAP 29.1±18.9 16.2±11.8§ 15.5±10.3§ 16.1±11.5§
N1 Sleep (Min)        
Sham 71.5±55.2 67.3±49.3 66.7±51.7 68.0±56.3
CPAP 66.9±49.8 43.3±30.1§ 49.3±33.4§ 46.5±31.0§
N1 Sleep (% TST)        
Sham 19.5±14.9 20.5±16.0 17.8±14.6 18.1±15.4
CPAP 18.5±14.3 12.4±8.4§ 12.8±9.1§ 12.1±8.5§
N2 Sleep (Min)        
Sham 227.0±67.7 214.8±70.7 237.5±70.2 236.5±66.1
CPAP 229.0±66.1 227.0±56.4 250.0±57.3 253.3±52.6§
N2 Sleep (%TST)        
Sham 60.2±14.4 61.4±15.1 60.2±14.0 60.5±13.8
CPAP 60.9±13.5 63.6±10.6 63.1±10.4§ 63.7±9.0§
N3 Sleep (Min)        
Sham 11.4±22.2 11.7±22.3 13.1±23.3 11.7±22.2
CPAP 11.1±20.3 16.6±26.1 15.2±26.7 14.8±26.1*
N3 Sleep (%TST)        
Sham 3.0±5.8 3.4±6.5 3.3±5.8 2.8±5.2
CPAP 2.9±5.2 4.6±7.1 3.8±6.7 3.6±6.3
REM Sleep (Min)        
Sham 65.4±29.2 51.9±27.5 73.3±30.9 73.2±31.4
CPAP 67.5±30.4 71.2±33.2§ 80.5±31.2 82.7±33.5§
REM Sleep (%)        
Sham 17.3±7.1 14.6±6.9 18.5±7.1 18.5±7.0
CPAP 17.5±6.9 19.3±7.7 20.2±6.8 20.4±7.3
*

p<0.1 vs. Sham

p<.05 vs. Sham

p<.01 vs. Sham

§

p<.001 vs. Sham

In Table 3 is presented the changes in the Buschke, DSST, ESS, AHI and O2LT85% over the course of the study. Scores on the Buschke and the DSST improved at the 2 and 6 month time points, but did so in the CPAP and Sham CPAP groups equally. In contrast, the ESS, AHI and O2LT85% were abnormally high at baseline, and had decreased at 2 and 6 months with CPAP. No changes over 6 months were observed in the Sham CPAP group.

Table 3.

Change in Buschke, Digit Symbol Sum Recall and Epworth Sleepiness Scale After Sham or CPAP.

        Change
  Diagnostic 2 Month 6 Month 6 Month-Diagnostic
Buschke Sum Recall        
Sham 49.8±8.9 52.2±8.8 54.3±8.7 4.6±7.1
CPAP 49.7±9.1 52.4±8.6 54.1±8.7 4.3±6.9
Digit Sum Recall        
Sham 46.6±26.5 53.7±28.4 55.5±28.1 9.2±27.8
CPAP 44.6±26.9 54.0±27.7 53.7±28.8 8.6±28.3
Epworth Sleepiness Scale*        
Sham 10.2±4.6 8.5±4.8 8.3±4.6 -1.7±3.4
CPAP 10.0±4.3 7.1±4.8 6.8±4.6 -2.8±4.1
Apnea Hypopnea Index        
Sham 41.2±25.2 31.5±25.5 30.5±25.2 10.9±21.3
CPAP 40.2±24.4 6.2±7.7 6.4±8.5 34.0±24.2
% Time Oxygen Saturations < 85%        
Sham 2.2±6.0 2.8±8.0 2.3±7.1  0.1±6.4
CPAP 2.3±6.4 0.2±1.0 0.3±2.3 -2.0±6.4
*

Adjusted for 6 month adherence

p<.001 vs. Sham

After combining both groups, there were no significant correlations between changes in the amount or percentage of sleep stages between baseline and the 6 month time point, and corresponding changes in either the Buschke or the DSST. In addition, there was no correlation between 6 month CPAP/Sham CPAP adherence at the 6 month time point, and changes in either the Buschke or the DSST (results for both analyses not shown). To explore whether there was a threshold for the amount of change in sleep, sleep architecture, sleep related breathing, and CPAP/Sham CPAP adherence variables necessary to be associated with changes in the Buschke and DSST, the distributions of these variables were divided into their top quartile and bottom 3 quartiles.

In Table 4 is shown the comparison between these 2 groups in their 6 month Buschke and DSST scores and their change over 6 months. Although there were few significant differences, greater changes in the Buschke occurred over 6 months in those participants in the top quartile of total sleep time (5.7±7.3 vs. 4.0±6.8, p≤0.01). Similarly, those in the top quartile of amount of N3 sleep (55.9±7.7 vs. 53.6±8.9 min, p≤0.01) and %N3 (55.9±7.8 vs. 53.6±8.9, p≤0.01) at 6 months scored better on the Buschke. In parallel, greater improvement in the DSST over 6 months was observed in the top quartiles of amount of N3 and %N3 distribution although they were only of borderline statistical significance. Those in the top quartile of the amount of REM and %REM also showed greater improvement in the Buschke after 6 months. No differences were observed for the AHI, but those in the top quartile of O2LT85% had worse scores on the Buschke at 6 months. There was no impact of overall CPAP or sham CPAP adherence on the Buschke or the DSST at 6 months or their change over 6 months. Stratification by treatment group also found no differences (results not shown).

Table 4.

Six Month and Change After 6 Month Scores on the Buschke Selective Reminding and Symbol Digit Coding Tasks.

    Buschke Selective Reminding Test Sum Recall   Symbol Digit Coding Delay Recall Task
  N Six Month Score N Change in Score: 6 Month - Baseline N Six Month Score N Change in Score: 6 Month - Baseline
Total Sleep Time (Min)                
Top Quartile 212 54.4±8.7 211 5.7±7.3 211 53.3±29.1 210 8.4±26.4
Bottom 3 Quartiles 636 54.1±8.7 636 4.0±6.8 625 54.8±28.2 624 9.0±28.6
Sleep Efficiency (%)                
Top Quartile 212 53.6±9.5 212 4.7±7.3 212 51.4±29.2* 212 9.2±28.3
Bottom 3 Quartiles 636 54.4±8.4 635 4.4±6.9 624 55.5±28.1 622 8.8±28.0
Arousal Index (#/h TST)                
Top Quartile 211 53.7±8.7 211 4.5±6.8 209 53.5±27.3 208 7.5±28.1
Bottom 3 Quartiles 638 54.3±8.7 637 4.4±7.0 628 54.7±28.8 627 9.4±28.0
N1 Sleep (Min)                
Top Quartile 211 54.0±8.9 211 4.6±7.2 210 55.4±28.0 208 10.3±25.6
Bottom 3 Quartiles 638 54.2±8.6 637 4.4±6.9 627 54.2±28.6 627 8.4±28.8
N1 Sleep (%)                
Top Quartile 213 54.3±8.6 213 4.9±7.0 212 55.8±28.0 210 9.0±29.1
Bottom 3 Quartiles 636 54.1±8.7 635 4.3±7.0 625 54.0±28.6 625 8.9±27.7
N2 Sleep (Min)                
Top Quartile 213 54.1±8.5 213 5.1±7.3* 211 54.8±29.8 210 8.8±26.8
Bottom 3 Quartiles 636 54.2±8.8 635 4.2±6.9 626 54.3±28.0 625 8.9±28.5
N2 Sleep (%)                
Top Quartile 212 53.4±9.1 212 4.3±7.0 209 52.3±29.8 209 9.2±30.0
Bottom 3 Quartiles 637 54.4±8.5 636 4.5±7.0 628 55.1±27.9 626 8.8±27.3
N3 Sleep (Min)                
Top Quartile 213 55.9±7.7 213 4.9±6.7 208 55.7±27.2 207 12.0±28.9*
Bottom 3 Quartiles 636 53.6±8.9 635 4.3±7.1 629 54.0±28.8 628 7.9±27.7
N3 Sleep (%)                
Top Quartile 212 55.9±7.8‡ 212 5.0±6.6 208 55.3±27.2 207 11.6±28.7*
Bottom 3 Quartiles 636 53.6±8.9 635 4.3±7.1 628 54.1±28.9 627 7.9±27.8
REM Sleep (Min)                
Top Quartile 213 54.9±8.8 213 5.5±7.3* 211 55.8±29.2 211 11.0±27.7
Bottom 3 Quartiles 636 53.9±8.6 635 4.2±6.9 626 54.0±28.1 624 8.2±28.2
REM Sleep (%)                
Top Quartile 214 54.7±8.8 214 5.3±7.3 212 55.7±28.5 212 10.9±26.8
Bottom 3 Quartiles 635 54.0±8.6 634 4.1±6.9 625 54.0±28.4 623 8.2±28.4
Apnea Hypopnea Index (#/h)                
Top Quartile 212 54.4±8.6 211 4.4±7.1 212 52.1±28.1 210 8.9±28.5
Bottom 3 Quartiles 637 53.6±8.8 637 4.3±6.7 625 55.2±28.4 625 8.9±27.9
% Time O2 Saturation < 85%                
Top Quartile 212 52.9±8.8 211 4.4±7.4 212 53.3±27.5 211 9.9±27.5
Bottom 3 Quartiles 639 54.6±8.6 637 4.4±6.9 625 54.8±28.7 624 8.6±28.3
Compliance with CPAP/Sham CPAP at 6 months, Hours)                
Top Quartile 180 53.4±8.8 180 4.0±7.0 178 52.9±28.5 178 9.0±26.6
Bottom 3 Quartiles 645 54.4±8.6 644 4.6±6.9 635 54.7±28.4 633 8.7±28.3
*

p<0.10

p<0.05

p<0.01

DISCUSSION

In this analysis, after intervention with CPAP or Sham CPAP, sleep architecture and sleep quality improved immediately with CPAP, but not Sham CPAP, and these changes were maintained over the 6 months of the trial. In contrast, memory assessed by the Buschke and DSST improved to the same degree with both CPAP and Sham CPAP. Furthermore, changes in sleep, sleep architecture or indices of sleep-related breathing disorder from baseline to 6 months did not correlate with corresponding changes in memory. However, those participants in the highest quartile of change in REM sleep and SWS had greater improvements in memory than those in the lower quartiles; those in the top quartile of O2LT85% had worse scores on the Buschke. These observations support the contention that both REM sleep and SWS are important in the retention of memories and suggest that oxygen desaturation may be detrimental.

In the CPAP group, sleep duration and sleep quality as well as indices of sleep-related breathing disorder improved after this intervention; these changes were maintained for 6 months. Although these observations were expected17-20, not all studies have found that CPAP improves sleep quality21, and similar results from previous investigations were of shorter duration. Improvement in sleep architecture was correlated with CPAP adherence further strengthening a causal relationship. Thus, our results extend previous findings by objectively confirming that improvements in sleep and sleep quality resulting from CPAP are maintained in the long-term.

Our finding that CPAP did not improve performance on the Buschke has been reported in previous publications documenting the results from APPLES9,22; we now extend this finding to the DSST. The absence of a difference between the 2 groups could be a function of the low level of compliance among the CPAP users and/or the very low level of positive pressure inherent with Sham CPAP. However, we did not find any impact of CPAP compliance on Buschke or DSST performance which argues against the former explanation. In both the CPAP and Sham CPAP groups, performance on both of these memory tasks improved to the same extent thus indicating a substantial practice/learning effect. Future studies using either of these assessments need to account for this observation.

We did not observe that changes in sleep variables from baseline to 6 months corresponded to changes in either the Buschke or DSST. However, we did find that when analyses were focused on differences between more extreme changes in sleep variables, in particular, REM sleep and SWS, there was an impact on memory. As reviewed by Diekelmann and Born, reactivation of encoded memories during SWS strengthen them in the hippocampus and facilitate their transfer into neocortical and striatal networks23.

Thus, our findings that participants who had the largest increases in the amount and percentage of N3 sleep had better performance on the Buschke and DSST support this construct. Similarly, there is substantial evidence that REM sleep plays an important role in consolidation of procedural memory as well as declarative memories that are spatially complex or emotional4. Although neither the Buschke nor DSST are tasks that specifically evaluate these latter components of memory, we did observe trends for better performance in those participants who had the greatest changes in the amount and percentage of REM.

With respect to sleep disordered breathing, analyses noted that greater time spent with oxygen saturations less than 85% were associated with worse Buschke but not DSST scores at 6 months. For the DSST task, the current results differ somewhat from findings derived from the Sleep Heart Health Study in which performance on the composite variable “Procspeed”, a combination of Digit Symbol Coding and Symbol Search, was negatively associated with oxygen desaturation24. Previous human studies specifically addressing the impact of hypoxemia on memory in persons with OSA are sparse and conflicting. In a study of 50 OSA patients, nocturnal hypoxemia was associated with lower scores on some memory tasks25.

However, in another study of 40 OSA patients, those with greater hypoxemia paradoxically performed better on assessments of memory26. Mechanistically, studies in animals indicate that hypoxemia can result in neuronal damage to the hippocampus and neocortex, areas of the brain important for memory consolidation27. Thus, our results from a large dataset in humans provide some support to suggest that nocturnal hypoxemia is deleterious for memory, but additional human studies to specifically address this issue are needed.

There are some limitations to this study. First, assessments of memory were restricted to only the Buschke and the DSST. A broader spectrum of tasks to evaluate memory performance may have resulted in different findings. Second, all of the participants in the study had OSA which may have influenced the results independent of changes in sleep. Finally, our results must be considered preliminary and hypothesis generating. The analytic outcomes chosen were exploratory and derived post hoc; our hypothesis that memory is associated with increases in REM sleep and SWS was not an initial goal of APPLES. In addition, no statistical corrections were made for multiple analytic outcomes and hence there is the possibility of increased Type 1 error. Nevertheless, strengths of this analysis include the large number of participants, long duration of the intervention and use of home-based polysomnography, with the latter avoiding the artificial environment of a sleep laboratory.

In conclusion, CPAP produced long-standing improvements in sleep duration, quality and architecture, but did not improve performance on a limited battery of memory tasks. However, large changes in REM and N3 sleep as well as moderate amounts of nocturnal hypoxemia do impact memory.

Acknowledgments

The Apnea Positive Pressure Long-term Efficacy Study (APPLES) study was funded by contract 5UO1-HL-068060 from the National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute. The APPLES pilot studies were supported by grants from the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and the Sleep Medicine Education and Research Foundation to Stanford University and by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (N44-NS-002394) to SAM Technology. In addition, APPLES investigators gratefully recognize the vital input and support of Dr. Sylvan Green, who died before the results of this trial were analyzed, but was instrumental in its design and conduct.

Footnotes

Administrative Core: Clete A. Kushida, MD, PhD; Deborah A. Nichols, MS; Eileen B. Leary, BA, RPSGT; Pamela R. Hyde, MA; Tyson H. Holmes, PhD; Daniel A. Bloch, PhD; William C. Dement, MD, PhD

Data Coordinating Center: Daniel A. Bloch, PhD; Tyson H. Holmes, PhD; Deborah A. Nichols, MS; Rik Jadrnicek, Microflow, Ric Miller, Microflow Usman Aijaz, MS; Aamir Farooq, PhD; Darryl Thomander, PhD; Chia-Yu Cardell, RPSGT; Emily Kees, Michael E. Sorel, MPH; Oscar Carrillo, RPSGT; Tami Crabtree, MS; Booil Jo, PhD; Ray Balise, PhD; Tracy Kuo, PhD

Clinical Coordinating Center: Clete A. Kushida, MD, PhD, William C. Dement, MD, PhD, Pamela R. Hyde, MA, Rhonda M. Wong, BA, Pete Silva, Max Hirshkowitz, PhD, Alan Gevins, DSc, Gary Kay, PhD, Linda K. McEvoy, PhD, Cynthia S. Chan, BS, Sylvan Green, MD

Clinical Centers

Stanford University: Christian Guilleminault, MD; Eileen B. Leary, BA, RPSGT; David Claman, MD; Stephen Brooks, MD; Julianne Blythe, PA-C, RPSGT; Jennifer Blair, BA; Pam Simi, Ronelle Broussard, BA; Emily Greenberg, MPH; Bethany Franklin, MS; Amirah Khouzam, MA; Sanjana Behari Black, BS, RPSGT; Viola Arias, RPSGT; Romelyn Delos Santos, BS; Tara Tanaka, PhD

University of Arizona: Stuart F. Quan, MD; James L. Goodwin, PhD; Wei Shen, MD; Phillip Eichling, MD; Rohit Budhiraja, MD; Charles Wynstra, MBA; Cathy Ward, Colleen Dunn, BS; Terry Smith, BS; Dane Holderman, Michael Robinson, BS; Osmara Molina, BS; Aaron Ostrovsky, Jesus Wences, Sean Priefert, Julia Rogers, BS; Megan Ruiter, BS; Leslie Crosby, BS, RN

St. Mary Medical Center: Richard D. Simon Jr., MD; Kevin Hurlburt, RPSGT; Michael Bernstein, MD; Timothy Davidson, MD; Jeannine Orock-Takele, RPSGT; Shelly Rubin, MA; Phillip Smith, RPSGT; Erica Roth, RPSGT; Julie Flaa, RPSGT; Jennifer Blair, BA; Jennifer Schwartz, BA; Anna Simon, BA; Amber Randall, BA

St. Luke's Hospital: James K. Walsh, PhD, Paula K. Schweitzer, PhD, Anup Katyal, MD, Rhody Eisenstein, MD, Stephen Feren, MD, Nancy Cline, Dena Robertson, RN, Sheri Compton, RN, Susan Greene, Kara Griffin, MS, Janine Hall, PhD

Brigham and Women's Hospital: Daniel J. Gottlieb, MD, MPH, David P. White, MD, Denise Clarke, BSc, RPSGT, Kevin Moore, BA, Grace Brown, BA, Paige Hardy, MS, Kerry Eudy, PhD, Lawrence Epstein, MD, Sanjay Patel, MD

Sleep HealthCenters for the use of their clinical facilities to conduct this research

Consultant Teams

Methodology Team: Daniel A. Bloch, PhD, Sylvan Green, MD, Tyson H. Holmes, PhD, Maurice M. Ohayon, MD, DSc, David White, MD, Terry Young, PhD

Sleep-Disordered Breathing Protocol Team: Christian Guilleminault, MD, Stuart Quan, MD, David White, MD

EEG/Neurocognitive Function Team: Jed Black, MD, Alan Gevins, DSc, Max Hirshkowitz, PhD, Gary Kay, PhD, Tracy Kuo, PhD

Mood and Sleepiness Assessment Team: Ruth Benca, MD, PhD, William C. Dement, MD, PhD, Karl Doghramji, MD, Tracy Kuo, PhD, James K. Walsh, PhD

Quality of Life Assessment Team: W. Ward Flemons, MD, Robert M. Kaplan, PhD

APPLES Secondary Analysis-Neurocognitive (ASA-NC) Team: Dean Beebe, PhD, Robert Heaton, PhD, Joel Kramer, PsyD, Ronald Lazar, PhD, David Loewenstein, PhD, Frederick Schmitt, PhD

National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI)

Michael J. Twery, PhD, Gail G. Weinmann, MD, Colin O. Wu, PhD

Data and Safety Monitoring Board (DSMB)

Seven-year term: Richard J. Martin, MD (Chair), David F. Dinges, PhD, Charles F. Emery, PhD, Susan M. Harding MD, John M. Lachin, ScD, Phyllis C. Zee, MD, PhD

Other term: Xihong Lin, PhD (2 y), Thomas H. Murray, PhD (1 y).

REFERENCES

  • 1.Dudai Y. The restless engram: consolidations never end. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012;35:227–247. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Stickgold R. Parsing the role of sleep in memory processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(5):847–853. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Inostroza M, Born J. Sleep for preserving and transforming episodic memory. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2013;36:79–102. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Boyce R, Williams S, Adamantidis A. REM sleep and memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2017;44:167–177. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bucks RS, Olaithe M, Rosenzweig I, Morrell MJ. Reviewing the relationship between OSA and cognition: Where do we go from here? Respirology. 2017;22(7):1253–1261. doi: 10.1111/resp.13140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Davies CR, Harrington JJ. Impact of Obstructive Sleep Apnea on Neurocognitive Function and Impact of Continuous Positive Air Pressure. Sleep Med Clin. 2016;11(3):287–298. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2016.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jackson ML, McEvoy RD, Banks S, Barnes M. Neurobehavioral Impairment and CPAP Treatment Response in Mild-Moderate Obstructive Sleep Apneas. J Clin Sleep Med. 2018;14(1):47–56. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.6878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Joyeux-Faure M, Naegelé B, Pépin JL, Tamisier R, Lévy P, Launois SH. Continuous positive airway pressure treatment impact on memory processes in obstructive sleep apnea patients: a randomized sham-controlled trial. Sleep Med. 2016;24:44–50. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.06.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kushida CA, Nichols DA, Holmes TH, Quan SF, Walsh JK, Gottlieb DJ, et al. Effects of continuous positive airway pressure on neurocognitive function in obstructive sleep apnea patients: The Apnea Positive Pressure Long-term Efficacy Study (APPLES) Sleep. 2012;35(12):1593–1602. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lee IS, Bardwell WA, Kamat R, Tomfohr L, Heaton RK, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. A Model for Studying Neuropsychological Effects of Sleep Intervention: The Effect of 3-week Continuous Positive Airway Pressure Treatment. Drug Discov Today Dis Models. 2011;8(4):147–154. doi: 10.1016/j.ddmod.2011.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kushida CA, Nichols DA, Quan SF, Goodwin JL, White DP, Gottlieb DJ, et al. The Apnea Positive Pressure Long-term Efficacy Study (APPLES): rationale, design, methods, and procedures. J Clin Sleep Med. 2006;2(3):288–300. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sleep-related breathing disorders in adults: recommendations for syndrome definition and measurement techniques in clinical research. The Report of an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Task Force. Sleep. 1999;22(5):667–689. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Quan SF, Chan CS, Dement WC, Gevins A, Goodwin JL, Gottlieb DJ, et al. The association between obstructive sleep apnea and neurocognitive performance--the Apnea Positive Pressure Long-term Efficacy Study (APPLES) Sleep. 2011;34(3):303–14B. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.3.303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. 1991;14(6):540–545. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Buschke H. Selective reminding for analysis of memory and learning. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav. 1973;12(5):543–550. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kay GG. Cogscreen. [2010 Apr 4]. Available from. http://www.cogscreen.com/overview.shtml.
  • 17.Somiah M, Taxin Z, Keating J, Mooney AM, Norman RG, Rapoport DM, et al. Sleep quality, short-term and long-term CPAP adherence. J Clin Sleep Med. 2012;8(5):489–500. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.2138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.McArdle N, Douglas NJ. Effect of continuous positive airway pressure on sleep architecture in the sleep apnea-hypopnea syndrome: a randomized controlled trial. Pt 1Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2001;164(8):1459–1463. doi: 10.1164/ajrccm.164.8.2008146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Singhal P, Gupta R, Sharma R, Mishra P. Association of naso-oro-pharyngeal structures with the sleep architecture in suspected obstructive sleep apnea. Indian J Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2014;66(Suppl 1):81–87. doi: 10.1007/s12070-011-0326-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Issa FG, Sullivan CE. The immediate effects of nasal continuous positive airway pressure treatment on sleep pattern in patients with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1986;63(1):10–17. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(86)90056-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Loredo JS, Ancoli-Israel S, Dimsdale JE. Effect of continuous positive airway pressure vs placebo continuous positive airway pressure on sleep quality in obstructive sleep apnea. Chest. 1999;116(6):1545–1549. doi: 10.1378/chest.116.6.1545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Holmes TH, Kushida CA. Adherence to continuous positive airway pressure improves attention/psychomotor function and sleepiness: a bias-reduction method with further assessment of APPLES. Sleep Med. 2017;37:130–134. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(2):114–126. doi: 10.1038/nrn2762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Quan SF, Wright R, Baldwin CM, Kaemingk KL, Goodwin JL, Kuo TF, et al. Obstructive sleep apnea-hypopnea and neurocognitive functioning in the sleep heart health study. Sleep Med. 2006;7(6):498–507. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2006.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Peng Y, Ouyang R, Cao Y, Chen P, Chen Y. Memory and executive dysfunction in patients with severe obstructive sleep apnea and hypopnea syndrome. Zhonghua Jie He He Hu Xi Za Zhi. 2015;38(10):756–760. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hoth KF, Zimmerman ME, Meschede KA, Arnedt JT, Aloia MS. Obstructive sleep apnea: impact of hypoxemia on memory. Sleep Breath. 2013;17(2):811–817. doi: 10.1007/s11325-012-0769-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Row BW. Intermittent hypoxia and cognitive function: implications from chronic animal models. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2007;618:51–67. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-75434-5_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Sleep Science are provided here courtesy of Brazilian Association of Sleep and Latin American Federation of Sleep

RESOURCES