Abstract
Locomotion occurs sporadically and needs to be started, maintained, and stopped. The neural substrate underlying the activation of locomotion is partly known, but little is known about mechanisms involved in termination of locomotion. Recently, reticulospinal neurons (stop cells) were found to play a crucial role in stopping locomotion in the lamprey: their activation halts ongoing locomotion and their inactivation slows down the termination process. Intracellular recordings of these cells revealed a distinct activity pattern, with a burst of action potentials at the beginning of a locomotor bout and one at the end (termination burst). The termination burst was shown to be time linked to the end of locomotion, but the mechanisms by which it is triggered have remained unknown. We studied this in larval sea lampreys (Petromyzon marinus; the sex of the animals was not taken into account). We found that the mesencephalic locomotor region (MLR), which is known to initiate and control locomotion, stops ongoing locomotion by providing synaptic inputs that trigger the termination burst in stop cells. When locomotion is elicited by MLR stimulation, a second MLR stimulation stops the locomotor bout if it is of lower intensity than the initial stimulation. This occurs for MLR-induced, sensory-evoked, and spontaneous locomotion. Furthermore, we show that glutamatergic and, most likely, monosynaptic projections from the MLR activate stop cells during locomotion. Therefore, activation of the MLR not only initiates locomotion, but can also control the end of a locomotor bout. These results provide new insights onto the neural mechanisms responsible for stopping locomotion.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The mesencephalic locomotor region (MLR) is a brainstem region well known to initiate and control locomotion. Since its discovery in cats in the 1960s, the MLR has been identified in all vertebrate species tested from lampreys to humans. We now demonstrate that stimulation of the MLR not only activates locomotion, but can also stop it. This is achieved through a descending glutamatergic signal, most likely monosynaptic, from the MLR to the reticular formation that activates reticulospinal stop cells. Together, our findings have uncovered a neural mechanism for stopping locomotion and bring new insights into the function of the MLR.
Keywords: brainstem, lamprey, locomotion, locomotor termination, mesencephalic locomotor region, reticulospinal neurons
Introduction
Locomotion occurs in bouts of activity that must be efficiently started, maintained, and stopped. In vertebrates, the spinal cord contains neural networks that generate the muscle synergies essential for body propulsion (for review, see Grillner et al., 2008). These spinal networks are in turn activated by brainstem reticulospinal (RS) neurons, which are controlled by locomotor centers such as the mesencephalic locomotor region (MLR) (Shik et al., 1966; for review, see Jordan, 1998; Ryczko and Dubuc, 2013). The MLR has been shown to initiate and control locomotion in all vertebrate species tested (e.g., cat: Shik et al., 1966; rat: Skinner and Garcia-Rill, 1984; mouse: Lee et al., 2014; salamander: Cabelguen et al., 2003; birds: Sholomenko et al., 1991; and lamprey: Sirota et al., 2000). Located at the border between the midbrain and hindbrain, it initiates locomotion when stimulated electrically, pharmacologically, or optogenetically (Shik et al., 1966; Garcia-Rill et al., 1985; Lee et al., 2014; Roseberry et al., 2016; Caggiano et al., 2018; Josset et al., 2018). There is still a controversy relative to the different motor behaviors that can be elicited by MLR stimulation. In mammals, the MLR occupies a large area and stimulation of its subregions elicits different locomotor behaviors that are associated with food seeking, defense, or exploration (Sinnamon, 1993).
The MLR projects to RS cells (Orlovskiı̆, 1970; Steeves and Jordan, 1984; Le Ray et al., 2003; Ryczko et al., 2016), the activity of which is strongly correlated with motor behavior (Drew et al., 1986; Deliagina et al., 2000; Bretzner and Brownstone, 2013; Kimura et al., 2013; Thiele et al., 2014). We have recently examined discharge patterns of RS cells during MLR-induced locomotion in the lamprey, a basal vertebrate (Juvin et al., 2016). Three activity patterns were identified and related to the locomotor output: one group of RS cells discharged transiently at the beginning of a locomotor episode, a second group fired action potentials throughout a whole locomotor bout, and a third group responded with a burst of action potentials at the beginning and another burst at the end of a locomotor episode (termination burst). The activity pattern of the third cell group was particularly interesting because it had not been described before in vertebrates. We demonstrated that pharmacological activation of these cells halted ongoing swimming activity, whereas inactivation slowed down the termination process. Therefore, we named them stop cells. Recently, there has been growing research interest on the neural mechanisms involved in stopping locomotion. A group of glutamatergic RS cells that play a crucial role in halting locomotion has been identified in mice (Bouvier et al., 2015). Optogenetic activation of these neurons (V2a “stop neurons”) terminates ongoing locomotion, whereas blocking their synaptic output increases mobility. In another study, activation of inhibitory glycinergic brainstem neurons has also been shown to stop locomotion in mice (Capelli et al., 2017). Although these mammalian brainstem neurons clearly stop locomotion, their pattern of discharge has not been recorded as done in lampreys.
In lampreys, one key question remaining concerns the mechanism that triggers the termination burst in stop cells. It was hypothesized that synaptic inputs rather than intrinsic properties were involved (Juvin et al., 2016). In the present study, we unexpectedly discovered that the MLR provides such a synaptic input and we show that MLR stimulation not only initiates locomotion, but also stops it. Experiments were performed in semi-intact preparations in which intracellular recordings of RS cells can be correlated to active swimming movements of the intact body. We found that, during MLR-induced swimming, a second MLR stimulation delivered at an intensity lower than that used to start locomotion stopped ongoing locomotion. Moreover, this low-intensity MLR stimulation elicited a termination burst in stop cells. We found direct projections from the MLR to the stop cell region and evidence of glutamatergic and, most likely, monosynaptic connectivity. Our findings reveal a new function of the MLR in terminating locomotion via activation of stop cells.
Materials and Methods
Ethics statement.
All procedures conformed to the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care and were approved by the animal care and use committees of the Université de Montréal and Université du Québec à Montréal (Québec, Canada). Care was taken to minimize the number of animals used and their suffering. All experiments were performed in larval sea lampreys (Petromyzon marinus) collected in a river near Notre-Dame-de-Stanbridge (Rivière aux Brochets, Québec, Canada). The animals were kept in aerated water at 5°C and received every other week ∼2 mg of yeast per animal.
Semi-intact and isolated brain preparations.
Semi-intact preparations (n = 58) were used to simultaneously record RS cell activity and locomotor movements (Antri et al., 2009; Ryczko et al., 2013). For this purpose, the brain and rostral spinal cord segments were dissected free and the caudal part of the body was kept intact. Animals were deeply anesthetized with tricaine methanesulphonate (MS 222, 100 mg/L; Sigma-Aldrich) and transferred into cold, oxygenated Ringer's solution containing the following (in mm): NaCl 130.0, KCl 2.1, CaCl2 2.6, MgCl2 1.8, HEPES 4.0, dextrose 4.0, and NaHCO3 1.0, adjusted to a pH of 7.4. A transverse incision was made on the ventral side at the level of the last pair of gills. Skin and muscle tissue was removed from the rostral part of the body and around the head. The brain and the rostral spinal cord segments were exposed dorsally by removing the surrounding tissue, skin, muscles, and cranial cartilage. The choroid plexus over the mesencephalic and fourth ventricles was removed to gain access to RS cells and the MLR. Decerebration was achieved by a complete transverse section of the neuraxis rostral to the mesencephalon. A dorsal midsagittal transection was performed at the isthmus to provide easier access to the MLR. The animals were transferred into a recording chamber continuously perfused with cold, oxygenated Ringer's solution. One part of the chamber was shallow and designed to pin down the rostral part of the preparation onto the Sylgard (Dow-Corning) lining at the bottom to record the activity of the brainstem neurons. The other part of the chamber was deeper and allowed the intact body to swim freely (Fig. 1C). Animals were allowed to recover for at least 1 h before recording. For anatomical experiments, isolated brain preparations of larval lampreys were used (n = 11). The dissection procedure was the same as that described above except a complete transverse cut was made at the level of the last gills to remove the body.
Electrophysiological recordings and stimulation.
Intracellular recordings of RS cells were made using sharp microelectrodes (80–120 MΩ), filled with 4 m potassium acetate. The signals were amplified, sampled at a rate of 10 kHz (Axoclamp 2A; Molecular Devices), and acquired through a Digidata 1200 series interface coupled to Clampex 8.1 software (Molecular Devices). Intracellular signals were analyzed using Clampfit 10.4 (Molecular Devices) or Spike2 5.19 software (Cambridge Electronic Design; RRID:SCR_000903). The MLR was electrically stimulated on one side to elicit swimming movements of the intact body. Trains of 2 ms pulses (frequency of 5 Hz for 10 s) were delivered through custom made glass-coated tungsten microelectrodes (4–5 MΩ with 10 μm tip exposure) using a Grass S88 stimulator (Astro Med). Stimulation intensities ranged from 0.5–15 μA, theoretically corresponding to a maximum current spread of 130–281 μm around the stimulation electrode (Ranck, 1975). Stimulation trains were delivered to the MLR with at least a 3 min waiting period in between. The location of the stimulation site was based on previous anatomical and physiological studies in the lamprey MLR, where the giant RS cell I1 (Rovainen, 1967) served as an MLR landmark (Ryczko et al., 2013; Juvin et al., 2016).
In a series of experiments, synaptic connectivity was tested using a high-divalent cation Ringer's solution (10.8 mm Ca2+/7.2 mm Mg2+; El Manira et al., 1997; Brocard and Dubuc, 2003). In these experiments, the recording chamber was split between the head and body using petroleum jelly (Vaseline) and the Ringer's solution in the head chamber was replaced by the high-divalent cation solution. After 30 min of exposure to the high-divalent cation solution, the MLR was stimulated with 2 electrical shocks (2 ms) applied at 25 Hz.
Drug application.
In a series of experiments, we performed local applications of drugs (all dissolved in Ringer's solution): d-glutamate (5 mm, Sigma-Aldrich); acetylcholine (1 mm, Sigma-Aldrich); a mixture of the glutamate antagonists 6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2.3-dione disodium (CNQX, 1.25 mm; Tocris Bioscience) and 2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (AP5, 5 mm; Sigma-Aldrich). Microinjections were performed as described in previous studies (Paggett et al., 2004; Jackson et al., 2007). A glass micropipette (diameter of opening 10–20 μm) was inserted in the MLR or the caudal middle rhombemcephalic reticular nucleus (MRRN) and the solutions were pressure ejected (2–6 pulses of 20–30 ms at 3–4 psi) using a Picospritzer (General Valve). The solutions were colored with the inactive dye fast green for visual guidance of the ejected droplets (Ryczko et al., 2017). Control injections consisted of Ringer's solution alone.
Kinematic analysis.
A video camera (HDR-XR200; Sony) was placed 1 m above the recording chamber to record swimming movements of the intact body (sampling rate: 30 frame/s). Video recordings were analyzed using a custom-made script in MATLABR2009A (The MathWorks, RRID:SCR_001622; Brocard et al., 2010; Ryczko et al., 2013). Swimming movements were analyzed by digitally adding equally spaced markers offline along the midline of the body. The lateral displacement of the body curvature was then monitored for each frame. For this, the angle between the longitudinal axis of the nonmoving body parts (line along the body midline) and a straight line drawn between two successive markers located in the middle of the body was measured for the entire locomotor bout. The values are expressed in radians (rad).
Anatomical tracing.
Anatomical experiments were performed to investigate the distribution of MLR cells projecting to different regions of the reticular formation. In these experiments, two injections were made on the same side of the reticular formation; fluorescein dextran amines were always used for the caudal injection and Texas red dextran amines for the rostral one. The first injection, the caudal one, consisted of a unilateral transverse section of the medial tegmentum using a microsurgical knife. The lesion was quickly filled with crystals of fluorescein dextran amines (3000 MW; Invitrogen) and left there to dissolve for 10 min, allowing tracer uptake by the cut axons. After thorough rinsing of the injected area, the preparation was placed in cold oxygenated Ringer's solution to allow the tracer to retrogradely travel past the location of the more rostral, future second injection. After 4 h, a second ipsilateral transverse section of the medial tegmentum was made and quickly filled with crystals of Texas red dextran amines (3000 MW; Invitrogen) and left there to dissolve for 10 min. Care was taken so that tracer from the second injection did not spread to the first injection area. After thoroughly rinsing the second injection site, the preparation was again placed in cold, oxygenated Ringer's solution overnight. The next morning, it was transferred into a fixative solution (4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 m phosphate buffer with 0.9% NaCl, pH 7.4, and PBS) for 24 h, followed by an immersion in a sucrose solution (20% in phosphate buffer) for at least 24 h. The brain was frozen and cross-sectioned (25 μm) on a cryostat. The sections were placed on ColorFrost Plus microscope slides (Fisher Scientific) and rinsed with PBS and coverslipped using Vectashield mounting medium (with DAPI; Vector Laboratories). Labeled cell bodies in the MLR were observed under an E600 epifluorescent microscope equipped with a digital camera (DXM 1200; Nikon). The sections were photographed and levels were adjusted in Photoshop CS5 (Adobe Systems; RRID:SCR_014199) so that all fluorophores were clearly visible. The size of labeled MLR neurons was measured using a micrometric scale incorporated in the ocular of the fluorescence microscope. As described in previous studies, the diameter of the somata was measured along the longest axis as seen on the cross-sections (Le Ray et al., 2003; Gariépy et al., 2012).
Experimental design and statistical analysis.
For the present study, sample size was not predetermined using a statistical method and was similar to the sample size used generally in the field. The sex of the individual larval animals was not taken into account. No blinding procedure or randomization was used in this study. Statistical analysis was performed with Sigma Plot 11.0 (Systat Software; RRID:SCR_010455) and R (R Core Team; http://www.r-project.org/; RRID:SCR_001905). Data in the text are represented as the mean ± SEM. Comparisons between two groups were made using a paired t test. In the cases in which normality and equal variance assumptions were not met, a Wilcoxon signed-rank test was applied to compare the two groups. When comparing more than two groups, a one-way ANOVA for repeated measures was used as parametric and a Friedman ANOVA on ranks for repeated measures as nonparametric analyses. These analyses were followed by a Student–Newman–Keuls post hoc test as a pairwise multiple-comparisons procedure. To calculate correlations between variables, the Pearson product–moment correlation test was used. For all statistical analyses performed in this study, differences were considered statistically significant when p ≤ 0.05 (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001). Illustrations were made using Illustrator CS5 (Adobe Systems; RRID:SCR_010279).
Data availability.
All relevant data are available from the authors upon request.
Results
MLR stimulation stops ongoing locomotion
In a previous study (Juvin et al., 2016), three types of discharge patterns were identified in RS cells in response to MLR stimulation: start, maintain, and stop patterns of discharge (Fig. 1A). In the present study, we focused on the RS cells that display a stop discharge pattern (stop cells) consisting of a burst at the beginning and one at the end of a locomotor bout (termination burst).
We then characterized the changes that occur in this termination burst as we increased the intensity of MLR stimulation. Stop cells were recorded intracellularly in semi-intact preparations, which allowed us to correlate the cellular discharge to the frequency of the swimming movements (Fig. 1C). Stimulation intensities below swimming threshold did not trigger the characteristic activation pattern of stop cells, including the termination burst (Fig. 1B). Only when the intensity of MLR stimulation was strong enough to elicit swimming did the stop cells produce the termination burst at the end of the locomotor bout. Interestingly, the higher the stimulation intensity was, the larger was the number of spikes in the termination bursts (r = 8.96 × 10−1, p = 2.61 × 10−3, Pearson product–moment correlation; n = 8 samples in one animal; Fig. 1D). The same was true for pooled data recorded in several neurons (r = 8.06 × 10−1, p = 5.71 × 10−13, Pearson product–moment correlation; n = 52 samples in 6 animals; Fig. 1E). There was also a positive correlation between the number of spikes in the termination burst and the swimming frequency of the whole locomotor bout (r = 7.57 × 10−1, p = 8.22 × 10−11, Pearson product–moment correlation; n = 52 samples in 6 animals).
The close relationship between the number of spikes in the termination burst and the intensity of MLR stimulation suggests that MLR inputs could trigger the termination burst. Therefore, the MLR would provide a signal that is responsible for stopping locomotion. To test this, we performed experiments in semi-intact preparations. Swimming activity was made to outlast the end of the MLR stimulation by using an intensity larger than needed to elicit swimming (Fig. 2A1). A second MLR stimulation was then applied during the swimming activity exceeding the duration of the stimulation. Applying a second stimulation at a low intensity (50% of control) but in the same MLR site stopped the swimming episode earlier than in the absence of a second stimulation (Fig. 2A2). It is noteworthy that such a low-intensity stimulation did not elicit locomotion at rest (Fig. 2A4). Interestingly, the locomotor bout was prolonged when the second MLR stimulation was made at the same intensity than the first one; that is, was sufficient to trigger locomotion at rest (Fig. 2A3). We then quantified the effects of a low-intensity MLR stimulation in five animals (Fig. 2B). On average, the intensity of the second MLR stimulation needed to significantly shorten the locomotor bout was 46.60% of control (ranging from 40% to 50% of the first stimulation). Overall, the duration of the locomotor activity outlasting the end of the MLR stimulation under control condition (i.e., without a second stimulation) was on average 24.29 ± 2.28 s (n = 25 trials in 5 animals; Fig. 2B, white boxes), with a range of 11.21–49.20 s. However, in the presence of a second low-intensity stimulation, the average duration of the locomotor activity outlasting the end of the first MLR stimulation was significantly decreased to 11.78 ± 0.49 s, ranging from 7.12–17.83 s (z = −4.37, p = 5.96 × 10−8, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Fig. 2B, green boxes). The animals stopped within 7.05 ± 0.48 s after the beginning of the second stimulation. In the same animals, we compared the effects of a second MLR stimulation of low versus high intensity (n = 25 trials for each condition; Fig. 2C). Here, the average duration of a whole swimming bout was significantly altered (χ2(2) = 4.47 × 101, p < 0.001, Friedman ANOVA on ranks for repeated measures) to 62.95 ± 3.96% of control when the MLR was stimulated at a low intensity (p < 0.5, Student–Newman–Keuls test; Fig. 2C, green bar) and to 133.05 ± 7.73% of control when the second MLR stimulation was delivered at a high intensity (p < 0.5, Student–Newman–Keuls test; Fig. 2C, gray bar). Interestingly, the swimming frequency was not significantly altered by the second stimulation (χ2(2) = 3.44, p = 1.79 × 10−1, Friedman ANOVA on ranks for repeated measures; Fig. 2D).
As shown in Figure 2B, the stimulation intensities of the second MLR simulation that significantly reduced the swimming duration varied from 40% to 50% of control. Another set of experiments was performed to define more precisely the range of intensities needed to shorten or prolong the swimming bouts (Fig. 2E). We first established the threshold intensity that was needed to elicit locomotion (1T) and then the control intensity was set to 2T (100%). The intensity of the second stimulation was then varied from 0% to 150% of control (with 12.5% steps), which altered the duration of the locomotor bouts (χ2(12) = 8.46 × 101, p = 5.34 × 10−13, Friedman ANOVA on ranks for repeated measures; n = 9 trials in 3 animals for each stimulation intensity). Under control conditions, when the MLR was stimulated only once at 100% intensity, the average duration of the locomotor bouts was 25.00 ± 1.18 s. When a second MLR stimulation was delivered during ongoing swimming, intensities <37.5% of control had no significant effect on the swimming duration (p > 0.5; Student–Newman–Keuls test). Intensities between 37.5% and 50% produced a significant decrease (19.22 ± 0.92 s and 19.67 ± 0.94 s, respectively; p < 0.5; Student–Newman–Keuls test), intensities between 50% and 75% produced no significant change in duration (p > 0.5; Student–Newman–Keuls test), and intensities of 75% or higher increased the swimming duration significantly (p < 0.5; Student–Newman–Keuls test).
To avoid activating fibers of passage in the MLR, electrical stimulation was replaced by pharmacological activation (n = 20 trials in 4 animals; Fig. 2F). The MLR was first electrically stimulated to elicit locomotion and then d-glutamate (5 mm) was locally injected (2–3 pulses of 20 ms for each injection; volume ejected: 0.36–0.55 pmol) in the MLR as a second stimulation. In the same animals, the d-glutamate solution was then exchanged for Ringer's solution. Swimming duration was altered (χ2(2) = 2.59 × 101, p = 2.38 × 10−6, Friedman ANOVA on ranks for repeated measures) and injection of d-glutamate shortened the locomotor bouts significantly (60.13 ± 3.47% of control; p < 0.5, Student–Newman–Keuls test; Fig. 2F, violet bar). Conversely, injecting Ringer's solution had no significant effect on swimming duration (87.01 ± 3.88% of control, p > 0.5; Student–Newman–Keuls test; Fig. 2F, gray bar).
To test whether there was a refractory period during which a second low-intensity MLR stimulation could not stop locomotion, the time interval between the end of the first stimulation and the beginning of the second stimulation was reduced in steps from 10 s, 5 s, to 0 s. In all cases, the second low-intensity stimulation shortened the locomotor bout compared with control condition (Fig. 3A,B). It was further observed that locomotion ended 6.27 ± 0.48 s, 6.56 ± 0.46 s, and 7.15 ± 0.52 s after the onset of the second stimulation for intervals of 10 s, 5 s, and 0 s, respectively (F(2,48) = 9.83 × 10−1; p = 3.81 × 10−1, one-way ANOVA for repeated measures, n = 25 trials in 5 animals; Fig. 3C).
In semi-intact preparations, swimming could be elicited by sensory stimulation or it could occur spontaneously (Di Prisco et al., 1997, 2000). Both sensory-evoked and spontaneous locomotor episodes could be stopped by low-intensity MLR stimulation (Fig. 4A,B). After pinching the dorsal fin (Stim; Fig. 4A1), long-lasting swimming movements were elicited in resting animals (n = 30 trials in 6 animals). Low-intensity MLR stimulation applied during the sensory-evoked swimming activity stopped the locomotor bout significantly earlier compared with the control condition (64.31 ± 3.57% of control; z = −3.96, p = 1.60 × 10−5, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Fig. 4A2,A3). Due to their rarity, spontaneous swimming bouts were not recorded kinematically, but were monitored through intracellular recordings of RS cells (n = 25 trials in 5 animals; Fig. 4B1). The spontaneous locomotor episodes were also stopped earlier by a MLR stimulation of low intensity (46.02 ± 5.03% of control; t(24) = 9.00; p = 7.92 × 10−9, paired t test; Fig. 4B2,B3). In both experiments, MLR stimulation intensities below swimming threshold were chosen as low-intensity MLR stimulation.
Termination burst in stop cells is time linked to the second MLR stimulation
Stop cells display a termination burst associated with the end of swimming regardless of the way it is initiated (MLR stimulation, cutaneous stimulation, spontaneous; Juvin et al., 2016). In the case of MLR-induced swimming, we investigated whether the burst occurs time linked with a second MLR stimulation at a low intensity (Fig. 5A1). The cellular activity of several stop cells was transformed into a raster display and the trials were temporally aligned on the onset of the second MLR stimulation (n = 15 trials in 3 animals; Fig. 5A2). This second MLR stimulation of low intensity stopped locomotion significantly earlier compared with the control condition (69.90 ± 7.26% from control; t(14) = 3.41; p = 4.23 × 10−3, paired t test). Both the raster plot and the peristimulus histogram showed an increase in spiking activity right after the onset of the second MLR stimulation (Fig. 5A2). This indicates that the termination burst is systematically time linked to the onset of the second MLR stimulation. Maintain cells (n = 16 trials in 4 animals; Fig. 5B) were also recorded while locomotion was stopped by MLR stimulation (decrease of swimming duration: 77.35 ± 4.60% of control; t(15) = 3.28; p = 5.00 × 10−3, paired t test). In contrast to stop cells, the maintain cells did not display a termination burs regardless of whether a second MLR stimulation was applied (Fig. 5B1, top and bottom). When a second MLR stimulation of low intensity was applied, it produced a sustained spiking activity until the cell repolarized at the end of the swimming bout, as illustrated in the raster display and the peristimulus histogram (Fig. 5B2).
Connectivity between the MLR and stop cells
We then examined the connectivity between the MLR and stop cells. Stop cells were intracellularly recorded to monitor their response to electrical shocks applied to the MLR (Fig. 6). The MLR stimulation intensity was set at 50% of the intensity needed to trigger a locomotor bout. Under these conditions, double shocks delivered at 20, 40, 60, and 80 Hz elicited short-latency EPSPs (2.8 to 3.2 ms; n = 4; Fig. 6A). As the time interval between shocks was shortened, the latency of the EPSPs remained unchanged. Next, a high concentration of divalent cations was added to the Ringer's solution to reduce the likelihood of polysynaptic transmission (El Manira et al., 1997; Brocard and Dubuc, 2003). Double shocks were delivered to the MLR at 25 Hz and in the recorded stop cells (n = 3) the EPSPs were not changed, suggesting that at least part of the connection between the MLR and stop cells is monosynaptic (Fig. 6B).
Anatomical experiments (n = 11) were then performed to examine MLR projections to different regions of the reticular formation (Fig. 7). In each animal, two different injections (two tracers) were made on the same side of the reticular formation. In all experiments, the most caudal injection was made using a green tracer (fluorescein dextran amines) and the second one using the red tracer (Texas red dextran amines). The rostral injection was made 4 h after the caudal one to allow the tracer used for the caudal injection to travel past the rostral injection site. The caudal injection was made larger than the rostral one. Because of this, MLR neurons with axons projecting to the rostral site were labeled only in red, whereas the MLR neurons that project to the caudal site were labeled by the two tracers (double-labeled) or only in green if they bifurcate from the midline. Using this double-labeling approach, we examined populations of MLR cells that projected to three different regions of the reticular formation (the stop cell area, the maintain cell area, and the rostral posterior rhombencephalic reticular nucleus). The experiments were performed in two groups of animals. In the first group, the rostral injection was made in the stop cell area (caudal MRRN), whereas the caudal one was made in the rostral pole of the posterior rhombencephalic reticular nucleus (rostral PRRN) (Fig. 7A1–A3; n = 4 animals). We found that the retrogradely labeled cells projecting to the stop cell area (Fig. 7B, red dots) were widely distributed on both sides of the MLR and were intermingled with the cells projecting more caudally to the rostral pole of the PRRN. In a second group of animals (n = 4), the rostral injection was made in the maintain cell area (rostral MRRN) and the caudal one in the stop cell area (caudal MRRN) (Fig. 7C1–C3). As described for the first group of animals, retrogradely labeled cells with projections to the maintain cell area (Fig. 7D, red dots) were widely distributed on both sides of the MLR and they were intermingled with the cells projecting more caudally in the stop cell area. When comparing retrogradely labeled MLR cells from the caudal MRRN (Fig. 7B) and the rostral MRRN (Fig. 7D), we found no difference in the diameter of the cell bodies. MLR cells projecting to the caudal MRRN (Fig. 7B) had an average diameter of 8.83 ± 0.23 μm and those projecting to the rostral MRRN (Fig. 7D) had an average diameter of 8.93 ± 0.18 μm (U = 2.75 × 103, n1 = 76, n2 = 77; p = 0.52; Mann–Whitney rank-sum test). Moreover, we found no apparent clustering of these two groups of cells. Because these comparisons were not made in the same animals, another series of experiments was performed with the injections made in the same animal (n = 3; Fig. 8A1–A3). Both injections were also made smaller with one made in the maintain cell area (rostral MRRN) and the other in the stop cell area (caudal MRRN) (Fig. 8A2). Results from these experiments were very similar to those obtained previously. MLR cells projecting to the two regions were intermingled (cf. green and yellow dots with red dots in Fig. 8B). Together, these observations suggest that there is no clear anatomical clustering of MLR cells projecting to different areas of the reticular formation.
Neurotransmitters involved in the stop signal from the MLR
Pharmacological experiments were performed to determine the neurotransmitters responsible for activating RS stop cells by the MLR (Fig. 9). In the lamprey, it has been shown that MLR inputs to RS cells are glutamatergic and cholinergic (Le Ray et al., 2003; Brocard et al., 2010). In the present experiments, locomotion was elicited using electrical MLR stimulation and, during the ongoing locomotor bout, d-glutamate (Fig. 9B) or acetylcholine (Fig. 9C) was bilaterally injected into the stop cell region (Fig. 9A). As described previously (Juvin et al., 2016), injections of d-glutamate (5 mm; 3–6 pulses of 30 ms for each injection) significantly shortened the duration of the locomotor bout (52.32 ± 4.12% of control condition; F(2,58) = 6.66 × 101; p = 9.57 × 10−16, one-way ANOVA for repeated measures; p < 0.001, Student–Newman–Keuls test; n = 30 trials in 6 animals). Conversely, bilateral injections of acetylcholine into the stop cell region (1 mm, 3–6 pulses of 30 ms for each injection) had no effects on the duration of the swimming bout (100.42 ± 3.19% of control; F(2,58) = 9.13 × 10−2; p = 9.13 × 10−1, one-way ANOVA for repeated measures, n = 30 trials in 6 animals).
The previous results strongly suggest that glutamatergic inputs from the MLR are responsible for the stop signal and not the cholinergic inputs. To test this further, another set of experiments was performed in which glutamatergic receptors were blocked in the stop cell region (Fig. 10A). First, locomotion was induced by MLR stimulation (Fig. 10B1) and then a second MLR stimulation of low intensity was applied to reduce the duration of the locomotor bout (Fig. 10B2). A mixture of the glutamate receptor antagonists CNQX (1.25 mm) and AP5 (5 mm) was then injected bilaterally over the stop cell region after locomotion was induced by electrical stimulation of the MLR (Fig. 10B3). When a second MLR stimulation of low intensity was delivered under CNQX and AP5, the duration of the swimming bout was no longer significantly different from the control condition (one-way ANOVA for repeated measures, n = 25 trials in 5 animals F(2,48) = 4.98 × 101; p = 1.99 × 10−12). The initial reduction of 60.74 ± 3.47% of control (p > 0.001, Student–Newman–Keuls test; Fig. 10B2 and C, green bar) was reversed to 109.92 ± 4.60% of control (p = 6.30 × 10−2; Student–Newman–Keuls test; Fig. 10B3 and C, orange bar). These results indicate that glutamatergic projections are responsible for transmitting the stop signal to the stop cells.
Discussion
In the present study, we uncovered a neural substrate that controls the termination of locomotion. It was previously shown in different vertebrate species that the MLR activates RS cells to start and maintain locomotion (Orlovskiı̆, 1970; Steeves and Jordan, 1984; Sirota et al., 2000; Brocard and Dubuc, 2003; for review see Ryczko and Dubuc, 2013). In the lamprey, three different RS cell populations were identified: start cells, maintain cells, and stop cells (Juvin et al., 2016). When the MLR is stimulated in resting animals, a descending start signal from the MLR activates all RS cell populations in the MRRN and initiates locomotion. The locomotor episode is maintained through the activity of a subgroup of RS cells, the maintain cells. We now show that MLR stimulation can also produce an opposite behavioral effect consisting in the termination of locomotion by providing a stop signal to RS cells that are crucial for stopping locomotion, the stop cells.
Synaptic inputs to stop cells
The study of RS cells that are localized in the brainstem and that could be involved in halting locomotion was performed only in a few vertebrate species (Xenopus tadpole: Perrins et al., 2002; cat: Takakusaki et al., 2003; mouse: Bouvier et al., 2015; Capelli et al., 2017; and lamprey: Juvin et al., 2016). Electrical, pharmacological, or optogenetic stimulation of these RS cell populations was shown to lead to the termination of ongoing locomotion. As yet, the detailed mechanisms responsible for activating these RS cells that stop locomotion have not been identified.
The MLR is known to project extensively to RS cells (Orlovskiı̆, 1970; Steeves and Jordan, 1984; Garcia-Rill and Skinner, 1987; Brocard and Dubuc, 2003; Brocard et al., 2010; Smetana et al., 2010; Bretzner and Brownstone, 2013; Ryczko et al., 2016). In the lamprey, these projections have been well characterized. Inputs from the MLR to RS cells were shown to differ in strength of connectivity depending on the localization of the RS cells in the hindbrain (Brocard and Dubuc, 2003). For example, rostral RS cells located in the MRRN receive stronger MLR inputs than those located more caudally in the PRRN. The connections from the MLR to RS cells were shown to be both monosynaptic and disynaptic (Brocard et al., 2010; Smetana et al., 2010) and glutamatergic and cholinergic projection neurons were identified to be involved in locomotor initiation and speed control (Brocard and Dubuc, 2003; Le Ray et al., 2003). Our anatomical data indicate that numerous MLR cells project to the area of the reticular formation that is rich in stop cells and electrophysiological data suggest that at least a part of the projections from the MLR to the stop cells is monosynaptic. Moreover, our results indicate that glutamatergic projections are involved in the MLR-induced termination of locomotion. For instance, injections of d-glutamate over the stop cell region significantly reduced the duration of an ongoing locomotor bout. Similar observations were previously made by Juvin et al. (2016). To further confirm the role of glutamate in the MLR–stop cell transmission, we have now shown that blocking glutamate receptors in the stop cell region prevented the reducing effect of a second low-intensity MLR stimulation on the duration of a locomotor bout. In addition, the activation of cholinergic receptors had no effect on the duration of the locomotor bout. This result is not surprising because it was shown previously that there is only a small component of MLR inputs to RS cells that is cholinergic (Le Ray et al., 2003).
Because locomotion could still end after blocking the glutamatergic excitation of stop cells, it is possible that there are also other neural mechanisms involved in the termination of locomotion. In mice, two different brainstem mechanisms have been described for halting locomotion. Glutamatergic V2a “stop cells” were shown to efficiently halt locomotion when activated optogenetically (Bouvier et al., 2015). Interestingly, these cells are located in the caudal pons/rostral medulla, a region that is very similar to that of the stop cells in the lamprey. In addition, that study showed that the mice V2a “stop cells” provide a descending excitatory projection to the spinal cord via glutamatergic inputs. It was also shown in mice that optogenetic activation of inhibitory RS cells halts locomotion (Capelli et al., 2017). The investigators proposed that inhibitory RS cells in different regions of the brainstem of mice could evoke different forms of behavioral arrest when activated. In the present study, it is not unlikely that a progressive decrease in descending excitation could be involved after the glutamatergic excitation to stop cells has been blocked. Therefore, it appears that there could be several means of halting locomotion.
How the MLR controls the termination of locomotion
Classically, the MLR has been described to initiate and control locomotion (for review, see Ryczko and Dubuc, 2013). The present findings indicating that activation of the MLR can also terminate locomotion were therefore unexpected. However, the MLR is a complex and large region in more recently evolved vertebrates, where it consists of several nuclei that seem to contribute in different ways to the locomotor repertoire. Sinnamon (1993) proposed that different MLR subregions control different behaviors such as appetitive, explorative, and defensive behavior. In addition, experiments in cats revealed that electrical stimulation of noncholinergic neurons of the cuneiform nucleus (CnF) and pendunclopontine nucleus (PPN), which are considered to be parts of the MLR, triggers movement. Conversely, stimulation of cholinergic PPN neurons stops ongoing spontaneous walking and induces muscle atonia (Takakusaki et al., 2003, 2004; for review, see Takakusaki, 2008). With the development of optogenetic techniques, it has recently become possible to use a more controlled approach to examine the multiple behaviors induced by the MLR (Roseberry et al., 2016; Caggiano et al., 2018; Josset et al., 2018). Roseberry et al. (2016) demonstrated that glutamatergic MLR cells drive locomotion and cholinergic neurons modulate its speed. Conversely, local GABAergic neurons were shown to inhibit glutamatergic MLR cells and thus stop locomotion when activated (Roseberry et al., 2016). The contribution of glutamatergic neurons in the PPN and CnF to the locomotor output has also been examined in more detail (Caggiano et al., 2018; Josset et al., 2018). It was shown that glutamatergic neurons in both nuclei contribute to slow movements, but only glutamatergic CnF neurons can control high-speed locomotion. The PPN was therefore associated with slow exploratory movements and the CnF with fast escape behavior (Caggiano et al., 2018). These results were confirmed by another study in which glutamatergic CnF neurons were shown to initiate and accelerate locomotion and activation of glutamatergic PPN neurons produced slow walking movements. Additionally, cholinergic PPN neurons were shown to modulate locomotor speed (Josset et al., 2018). Together, the recent studies indicate that the mammalian MLR is divided into different regions that contribute to different locomotor functions. In contrast to this, the lamprey MLR is much smaller and, in the present work, we did not find a segregation of MLR cells projecting to the stop cell versus maintain cell regions. Therefore, the MLR of lampreys would be less clustered.
A salient finding in the present study is that stimulation of the same MLR site can produce opposite behaviors (initiation vs termination of locomotion) when changing the intensity of the MLR stimulation. It is possible that changing the stimulation intensity activates different subpopulations of neurons. For example, MLR cells projecting to the stop cells could have intrinsic properties (e.g., membrane resistance or threshold) that would differ from those of other MLR cells. The excitability of these MLR cells could also change depending on the behavioral state of the animal. For example, the excitability could increase during locomotion, allowing the MLR cells to be activated by low-intensity stimulation. Conversely, MLR cells projecting to start and maintain RS cells would be highly excitable at rest and their excitability would decrease during the active locomotor state. This could explain the observations made in the present study, in which the second stimulation produces a termination burst in stop cells, but no increased activity in maintain cells. The descending inputs from the MLR to stop cells would be more efficient when occurring during locomotion. Additional experiments are needed in the future to test this hypothesis.
Conclusion
Results from the present study provide a better understanding of the neural mechanisms responsible for stopping locomotion. We show that electrical stimulation of the same MLR site can elicit opposing effects (initiation and termination of locomotion) depending on the stimulation intensity. These results could be important for the clinical research field because deep-brain stimulation of the MLR is presently performed to reduce symptoms in Parkinson's disease patients (Stefani et al., 2007; Arnulf et al., 2010; Wilcox et al., 2011; for review, see Ryczko et al., 2013). Our results close a gap in knowledge relative to the neural mechanism responsible for terminating locomotion.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (Grant 5129 to R.D.); the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (Grant 217435 to R.D.); and the Great Lakes Fishery Commission (Grants 54021 and 54035 to R.D.). S.G. received scholarships from the University of Cologne and the German Academic Exchange Service. We thank Danielle Veilleux for technical assistance, Frédéric Bernard for help with the graphics, and Dimitri Ryczko for valuable discussions.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Antri M, Fénelon K, Dubuc R (2009) The contribution of synaptic inputs to sustained depolarizations in reticulospinal neurons. J Neurosci 29:1140–1151. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3073-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnulf I, Ferraye M, Fraix V, Benabid AL, Chabardès S, Goetz L, Pollak P, Debû B (2010) Sleep induced by stimulation in the human pedunculopontine nucleus area. Ann Neurol 67:546–549. 10.1002/ana.21912 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouvier J, Caggiano V, Leiras R, Caldeira V, Bellardita C, Balueva K, Fuchs A, Kiehn O (2015) Descending command neurons in the brainstem that halt locomotion. Cell 163:1191–1203. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.10.074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bretzner F, Brownstone RM (2013) Lhx3-Chx10 reticulospinal neurons in locomotor circuits. J Neurosci 33:14681–14692. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5231-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brocard F, Dubuc R (2003) Differential contribution of reticulospinal cells to the control of locomotion induced by the mesencephalic locomotor region. J Neurophysiol 90:1714–1727. 10.1152/jn.00202.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brocard F, Ryczko D, Fénelon K, Hatem R, Gonzales D, Auclair F, Dubuc R (2010) The transformation of a unilateral locomotor command into a symmetrical bilateral activation in the brainstem. J Neurosci 30:523–533. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3433-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabelguen JM, Bourcier-Lucas C, Dubuc R (2003) Bimodal locomotion elicited by electrical stimulation of the midbrain in the salamander Notophthalmus viridescens. J Neurosci 23:2434–2439. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-06-02434.2003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caggiano V, Leiras R, Goñi-Erro H, Masini D, Bellardita C, Bouvier J, Caldeira V, Fisone G, Kiehn O (2018) Midbrain circuits that set locomotor speed and gait selection. Nature 553:455–460. 10.1038/nature25448 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capelli P, Pivetta C, Soledad Esposito M, Arber S (2017) Locomotor speed control circuits in the caudal brainstem. Nature 551:373–377. 10.1038/nature24064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deliagina TG, Zelenin PV, Fagerstedt P, Grillner S, Orlovsky GN (2000) Activity of reticulospinal neurons during locomotion in the freely behaving lamprey. J Neurophysiol 83:853–863. 10.1152/jn.2000.83.2.853 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Prisco GV, Pearlstein E, Robitaille R, Dubuc R (1997) Role of sensory-evoked NMDA plateau potentials in the initiation of locomotion. Science 278:1122–1125. 10.1126/science.278.5340.1122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Prisco GV, Pearlstein E, Le Ray D, Robitaille R, Dubuc R (2000) A cellular mechanism for the transformation of a sensory input into a motor command. J Neurosci 20:8169–8176. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-21-08169.2000 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drew T, Dubuc R, Rossignol S (1986) Discharge patterns of reticulospinal and other reticular neurons in chronic, unrestrained cats walking on a treadmill. J Neurophysiol 55:375–401. 10.1152/jn.1986.55.2.375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El Manira A, Pombal MA, Grillner S (1997) Diencephalic projection to reticulospinal neurons involved in the initiation of locomotion in adult lampreys Lampetra fluviatilis. J Comp Neurol 389:603–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia-Rill E, Skinner RD (1987) The mesencephalic locomotor region. II. projections to reticulospinal neurons. Brain Res 411:13–20. 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90676-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia-Rill E, Skinner RD, Fitzgerald JA (1985) Chemical activation of the mesencephalic locomotor region. Brain Res 330:43–54. 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90006-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gariépy JF, Missaghi K, Chartré S, Robert M, Auclair F, Dubuc R (2012) Bilateral connectivity in the brainstem respiratory networks of lampreys. J Comp Neurol 520:1442–1456. 10.1002/cne.22804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grillner S, Wallén P, Saitoh K, Kozlov A, Robertson B (2008) Neural bases of goal-directed locomotion in vertebrates–an overview. Brain Res Rev 57:2–12. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.06.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson AW, Pino FA, Wiebe ED, McClellan AD (2007) Movements and muscle activity initiated by brain locomotor areas in semi-intact preparations from larval lamprey. J Neurophysiol 97:3229–3241. 10.1152/jn.00967.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jordan LM. (1998) Initiation of locomotion in mammals. Ann N Y Acad Sci 860:83–93. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09040.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Josset N, Roussel M, Lemieux M, Lafrance-Zoubga D, Rastqar A, Bretzner F (2018) Distinct contributions of mesencephalic locomotor region nuclei to locomotor control in the freely behaving mouse. Curr Biol 28:884–901.e3. 10.1016/j.cub.2018.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juvin L, Grätsch S, Trillaud-Doppia E, Gariépy JF, Büschges A, Dubuc R (2016) A specific population of reticulospinal neurons controls the termination of locomotion. Cell Rep 15:2377–2386. 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.05.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimura Y, Satou C, Fujioka S, Shoji W, Umeda K, Ishizuka T, Yawo H, Higashijima S (2013) Hindbrain V2a neurons in the excitation of spinal locomotor circuits during zebrafish swimming. Curr Biol 23:843–849. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee AM, Hoy JL, Bonci A, Wilbrecht L, Stryker MP, Niell CM (2014) Identification of a brainstem circuit regulating visual cortical state in parallel with locomotion. Neuron 83:455–466. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Ray D, Brocard F, Bourcier-Lucas C, Auclair F, Lafaille P, Dubuc R (2003) Nicotinic activation of reticulospinal cells involved in the control of swimming in lampreys. Eur J Neurosci 17:137–148. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02417.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orlovskiı̆ GN. (1970) Relations between reticulo-spinal neurons and locomotor regions of the brain stem [Article in Russian]. Biofizika 15:171–178. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paggett KC, Jackson AW, McClellan AD (2004) Organization of higher-order brain areas that initiate locomotor activity in larval lamprey. Neuroscience 125:25–33. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.01.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perrins R, Walford A, Roberts A (2002) Sensory activation and role of inhibitory reticulospinal neurons that stop swimming in hatchling frog tadpoles. J Neurosci 22:4229–4240. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-10-04229.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranck JB., Jr (1975) Which elements are excited in electrical stimulation of mammalian central nervous system: a review. Brain Res 98:417–440. 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90364-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roseberry TK, Lee AM, Lalive AL, Wilbrecht L, Bonci A, Kreitzer AC (2016) Cell-type-specific control of brainstem locomotor circuits by basal ganglia. Cell 164:526–537. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.12.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rovainen CM. (1967) Physiological and anatomical studies on large neurons of central nervous system of the sea lamprey (Petromyzon marinus). I. Müller and Mauthner cells. J Neurophysiol 30:1000–1023. 10.1152/jn.1967.30.5.1000 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryczko D, Dubuc R (2013) The multifunctional mesencephalic locomotor region. Curr Pharm Des 19:4448–4470. 10.2174/1381612811319240011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryczko D, Grätsch S, Auclair F, Dubé C, Bergeron S, Alpert MH, Cone JJ, Roitman MF, Alford S, Dubuc R (2013) Forebrain dopamine neurons project down to a brainstem region controlling locomotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:E3235–E3242. 10.1073/pnas.1301125110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryczko D, Auclair F, Cabelguen JM, Dubuc R (2016) The mesencephalic locomotor region sends a bilateral glutamatergic drive to hindbrain reticulospinal neurons in a tetrapod. J Comp Neurol 524:1361–1383. 10.1002/cne.23911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryczko D, Grätsch S, Schläger L, Keuyalian A, Boukhatem Z, Garcia C, Auclair F, Büschges A, Dubuc R (2017) Nigral glutamatergic neurons control the speed of locomotion. J Neurosci 37:9759–9770. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1810-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shik ML, Severin FV, Orlovskiı̆ GN (1966) Control of walking and running by means of electric stimulation of the midbrain [Article in Russian]. Biofizika 11:659–666. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sholomenko GN, Funk GD, Steeves JD (1991) Avian locomotion activated by brainstem infusion of neurotransmitter agonists and antagonists. I. Acetylcholine excitatory amino acids and substance P. Exp Brain Res 85:659–673. 10.1007/BF00231752 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinnamon HM. (1993) Preoptic and hypothalamic neurons and the initiation of locomotion in the anesthetized rat. Prog Neurobiol 41:323–344. 10.1016/0301-0082(93)90003-B [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sirota MG, Di Prisco GV, Dubuc R (2000) Stimulation of the mesencephalic locomotor region elicits controlled swimming in semi-intact lampreys. Eur J Neurosci 12:4081–4092. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00301.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner RD, Garcia-Rill E (1984) The mesencephalic locomotor region (MLR) in the rat. Brain Res 323:385–389. 10.1016/0006-8993(84)90319-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smetana R, Juvin L, Dubuc R, Alford S (2010) A parallel cholinergic brainstem pathway for enhancing locomotor drive. Nat Neurosci 13:731–738. 10.1038/nn.2548 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steeves JD, Jordan LM (1984) Autoradiographic demonstration of the projections from the mesencephalic locomotor region. Brain Res 307:263–276. 10.1016/0006-8993(84)90480-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stefani A, Lozano AM, Peppe A, Stanzione P, Galati S, Tropepi D, Pierantozzi M, Brusa L, Scarnati E, Mazzone P (2007) Bilateral deep brain stimulation of the pedunculopontine and subthalamic nuclei in severe Parkinson's disease. Brain 130:1596–1607. 10.1093/brain/awl346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takakusaki K. (2008) Forebrain control of locomotor behaviors. Brain Res Rev 57:192–198. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.06.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takakusaki K, Kohyama J, Matsuyama K (2003) Medullary reticulospinal tract mediating a generalized motor inhibition in cats: III. Functional organization of spinal interneurons in the lower lumbar segments. Neuroscience 121:731–746. 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00542-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takakusaki K, Habaguchi T, Saitoh K, Kohyama J (2004) Changes in the excitability of hindlimb motoneurons during muscular atonia induced by stimulating the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus in cats. Neuroscience 124:467–480. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.12.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thiele TR, Donovan JC, Baier H (2014) Descending control of swim posture by a midbrain nucleus in zebrafish. Neuron 83:679–691. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilcox RA, Cole MH, Wong D, Coyne T, Silburn P, Kerr G (2011) Pedunculopontine nucleus deep brain stimulation produces sustained improvement in primary progressive freezing of gait. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 82:1256–1259. 10.1136/jnnp.2010.213462 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are available from the authors upon request.