Abstract
Birdsong is a complex vocal communication signal, and like humans, birds need to discriminate between similar sequences of sound with different meanings. The caudal mesopallium (CM) is a cortical-level auditory area implicated in song discrimination. CM neurons respond sparsely to conspecific song and are tolerant of production variability. Intracellular recordings in CM have identified a diversity of intrinsic membrane dynamics, which could contribute to the emergence of these higher-order functional properties. We investigated this hypothesis using a novel linear-dynamical cascade model that incorporated detailed biophysical dynamics to simulate auditory responses to birdsong. Neuron models that included a low-threshold potassium current present in a subset of CM neurons showed increased selectivity and coding efficiency relative to models without this current. These results demonstrate the impact of intrinsic dynamics on sensory coding and the importance of including the biophysical characteristics of neural populations in simulation studies.
Author summary
Maintaining a stable mental representation of an object is an important task for sensory systems, requiring both recognizing the features required for identification and ignoring incidental changes in its presentation. The prevailing explanation for these processes emphasizes precise sets of connections between neurons that capture only the essential features of an object. However, the intrinsic dynamics of the neurons themselves, which determine how these inputs are transformed into spiking outputs, may also contribute to the neural computations underlying object recognition. To understand how intrinsic dynamics contribute to sensory coding, we constructed a computational model capable of simulating a neural response to an auditory stimulus using a detailed description of different intrinsic dynamics in a higher-order avian auditory area. The results of our simulation showed that intrinsic dynamics can have a profound effect on processes underlying object recognition. These findings challenge the view that patterns of connectivity alone account for the emergence of stable object representations and encourage greater consideration of the functional implications of the diversity of neurons in the brain.
Introduction
Vocal communication requires an auditory system that can reliably classify signals. For example, in human speech, phonemes produced by different speakers and in different contexts vary broadly in acoustic structure. Despite this, they are perceived as discrete, invariant categories [1], even when boundaries between phonemes are exceptionally sharp, as with the approximately 10 ms difference in voice-onset time that separates the English phonemes /d/ and /t/ [2]. Categorization of vocal signals is not limited to human speech perception; many other species that communicate vocally show similar abilities [3–6].
As in other sensory systems, categorical responses to auditory objects emerge in higher-order areas of the cortex [7–12]. Neurons in these areas are characterized by a high degree of selectivity, or sparseness, in their responses to exemplars from different categories [13, 14], as well as tolerance, or invariance, in their responses to exemplars of the same category [15]. Similar properties are observed in the responses of secondary auditory areas in humans to phonemes [16–18]. However, in spite of a substantial body of theoretical work [19–21], the circuit and cellular mechanisms underlying the emergence of categorical responses remains poorly understood.
Songbirds communicate with acoustically complex vocalizations, which requires them to perform many of the same kinds of auditory discrimination tasks as humans [22]. In the avian auditory system, the caudal mesopallium (CM) is a cortical-level area that contains a population of neurons highly selective for particular song elements, yet tolerant of low-level acoustic differences between renditions [8, 10]. In contrast, the neurons in Field L, immediately upstream of CM, show low selectivity and tolerance [23]. In the classical model of selectivity, complex feature representations emerge through feedforward synaptic connections that pool sparsely from upstream sources [24]. This model does not match experimental evidence from CM, however, which shows that selective neurons receive a more distributed pool of inputs than the sparse model would predict [25]. A distributed scheme of selectivity could arise from nonlinear dynamics within the neurons themselves, but the mechanisms of this have not been explored.
In many of the auditory areas in the hindbrain and midbrain, nonlinear neural dynamics profoundly affect how a variety of low-level acoustic features are encoded [26–28]. For example, neurons that express a low-threshold potassium current () produce highly phasic responses, responding to rapid increases in excitation but not to slow or steady-state depolarizations [29, 30]. These dynamics are critical to temporal precision in sound localization circuits [28, 31] and can enhance signal detection in noisy conditions [32]. Could intrinsic dynamics also contribute to sensory processing at cortical levels? In CM, the putatively excitatory neurons exhibit diverse intrinsic firing patterns [33]. About 30% exclusively produce phasic responses that depend on a low-threshold potassium current, whereas the remainder produce mostly tonic responses. The functional significance of this diversity remains unclear.
The goal of the present study is to understand how and the phasic dynamics it produces could contribute to the emergence of selectivity and tolerance in CM. To investigate how intrinsic dynamics could interact with sensory-driven inputs, we developed an auditory response model that combines a spectrotemporal receptive field (RF) with a biophysical spike generation mechanism. In this linear-dynamical cascade model, the RF component approximates the integration over multiple spectral channels and time lags performed by the circuits upstream of the neuron and by the neuron’s dendritic tree. This part of the model is identical to the linear filter component of the classic linear-nonlinear Poisson (LNP) model [34, 35]. However, instead of using the output of the filter as the conditional intensity of a probabilistic spiking process, we feed it into a single-compartment, biophysical model that produces spikes through Hodgkin-Huxley dynamics. Using this approach, which allows us to integrate our knowledge about intracellular properties in CM into a model for high-level coding properties, we found that enhances selectivity and robustness to noise.
Results
Fig 1A represents the operation of the linear-dynamical cascade model. The spectrogram of a zebra finch song stimulus was convolved with the RF to produce a driving current Istim(t), which was injected into a single-compartment dynamical neuron model containing several voltage-dependent sodium and potassium currents. The mathematical descriptions of these currents were derived from an intracellular study of the excitatory neurons in the caudal mesopallium (CM) of zebra finches [33], and the RFs are drawn from parameterized features of zebra finch field L neurons [36]. A key feature of this model is that it can produce phasic or tonic responses depending on the conductance of a low-threshold potassium current (), which is experimentally known to be present in some CM neurons. Variability in the responses of the model was generated with the addition of the Inoise(t) current. This current adds random noise with a 1/f2 power spectrum (red noise), which approximates the statistics of spontaneous synaptic noise in vivo [37], at a signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of four.
As seen in Fig 1B, the model’s intrinsic dynamics affected its response to zebra finch song. When was high (phasic model), responses were more precise and more reliable than when was absent (tonic model). This effect was consistent across RFs with different spectral and temporal parameters and across multiple stimuli (Fig 2). It is important to note that although is an outward current with an overall hyperpolarizing influence, increasing it did not simply reduce excitability. In some cases, the phasic model responded at a much lower rate and to only a handful of notes across the stimulus set (Fig 2B). In other cases, the firing rates of the phasic and tonic models were nearly the same (Fig 2A and 2C), and the effects of manipulating were primarily on trial-to-trial variability. Overall, the average difference in firing rate between tonic and phasic models was significant but small and highly variable (0.36 ± 0.36 Hz; t59 = 7.74; p < 0.001).
To investigate how phasic excitability affects functional response properties, in this study we focused on rate-based theories of sensory coding. The fundamental idea of rate coding is that neurons convey information about sensory inputs by modulating their average firing rate over relatively long intervals. Following previous studies [10, 38], we defined these intervals by dividing the responses into segments corresponding to song syllables (as in Fig 1), which are well-defined units of zebra finch song that convey information about individual identity [39]. We then calculated metrics based on how the average rates within those intervals were distributed across syllables and trials (Fig 3A).
We first examined whether phasic dynamics made neurons more selective. Selectivity, also known as lifetime sparseness, is a well-established rate-coding metric defined as the tendency of a neuron to respond strongly to only a small subset of stimuli [13]. A selective neuron has a skewed, heavy-tailed response distribution in which a few stimuli account for the strongest responses, and the remainder produce only weak excitation. In contrast, a less selective neuron has a small-tailed, Gaussian response distribution, with most of the responses concentrated around the mean. For the examples shown in Fig 3A, the models with phasic dynamics tended to be significantly more selective than the corresponding models with tonic dynamics. This effect was not uniform, but appeared to be stronger in the models with higher temporal-modulation frequencies in their RFs.
The effects highlighted in these examples were also seen in a larger set of simulations with 60 different RFs, which were matched to the distribution of spectral and temporal parameters seen in the zebra finch auditory cortex [36]. Each RF was combined with phasic and tonic dynamics for a total of 120 models. Across this population, phasic models were consistently more selective than tonic models with the same RFs (Fig 3B and 3C), and the effects of intrinsic dynamics were larger for models with higher temporal modulation (Fig 3D).
We also observed that phasic models were more reliable across trials, indicating that they were less affected by the noise current Inoise. To quantify this effect in the context of rate coding, we calculated the mutual information (MI) between the response rate and syllable identity. MI is defined as the difference between the response (total) entropy, which represents how much information the neuron can carry based on its range of firing rates, and noise (or conditional) entropy, which represents how much information is lost due to the variability of a neuron’s response to the same stimulus. A neuron that responds more similarly across trials will have a low noise entropy, bringing its MI closer to its total entropy.
Fig 4 illustrates how intrinsic dynamics affected the distribution of response rates across trials for the same nine example RFs shown previously. The tonic models tended to have a higher total entropy across these examples, although in several cases, the phasic models are slightly higher. However, for all of these examples, the response rates of the phasic models cluster more closely around the mean response, resulting in lower noise entropy compared to the tonic models.
Across all 60 RF pairs, the total entropy was slightly higher for tonic neurons (Fig 5A and 5B). That advantage was more than canceled out by a much larger increase in noise entropy (Fig 5C and 5D). Every phasic simulation had lower noise entropy than its tonic pair. The net effect was that phasic models had higher MI than the corresponding tonic models (Fig 5E and 5F).
In the data described above, the noise current had a 1/f2 spectral distribution (red noise), which is thought to approximate how spontaneous synaptic currents are dominated by low-frequency fluctuations [37]. The total amplitude of the noise was set so that the inter-trial variations in firing patterns resembled what has been reported in extracellular recordings [23, 36, 40]. To test whether the results were robust to these assumptions, we varied the amplitude and spectral distribution of the noise current. As would be expected, selectivity and mutual information decreased overall as signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) decreased (Fig 6A–6D). However, at each SNR value tested, the phasic models consistently had lower noise entropy and higher MI and selectivity. We also found that the same pattern of results was seen when the noise current had a 1/f distribution, which is less dominated by low-frequency fluctuations but is also considered to be biologically valid [41]. In contrast, when the noise had a flat frequency distribution (white noise), the effects of intrinsic dynamics were not significant (Fig 6E–6H).
As a further test of parameter sensitivity, we compared results across a wide range of values for , which determines the magnitude of the low-threshold potassium current that causes phasic firing. As shown previously [33, 42], increasing over a narrow range leads to a dramatic change in firing patterns: once the low-threshold current is strong enough to counteract voltage-gated sodium currents, it becomes nearly impossible for the neuron to fire more than one action potential in response to a step current. This may explain the bimodal distribution of firing properties in CM [33]. The phasic and tonic dynamical models examined here represent these two modes, but it is likely that the distribution of varies more broadly. Interestingly, we did not see any evidence of a switch-like change in encoding properties as we varied over the range encompassed by the two exemplar models (Fig 6I–6L). Increasing instead produced nearly linear increases in selectivity and mutual information, and linear decreases in total and noise entropy.
How are selectivity and MI related? In these simulations, total entropy and selectivity were negatively correlated (Fig 7A). This effect is unsurprising given that selective neurons respond similarly to a large proportion of stimuli. They encode more information about a few stimuli at the expense of encoding less information about the entire stimulus set. However, when we consider coding efficiency, which quantifies how much of this potential bandwidth is actually used instead of being lost to noise (Fig 7C), two trends emerge. First, tonic models have lower coding efficiency than phasic models, consistent with the observation that they have higher noise entropy and lower MI. Second, only phasic neurons are able to achieve both high selectivity and high coding efficiency.
As an independent test of the validity of this model, we applied the same selectivity and MI analyses to a public corpus of recordings from zebra finch CM [43]. The relationships between selectivity and MI predicted by the model were largely borne out in the experimental data. We observed a similar tradeoff between selectivity and total entropy (Fig 7B), and an even stronger positive correlation between selectivity and coding efficiency (Fig 7D). The average coding efficiency was lower in the experimental data, which likely reflects additional sources of variability in vivo. Selectivity was somewhat higher in vivo, perhaps because of additional nonlinearities in the actual CM receptive fields. Interestingly, the cluster of models with high coding efficiency and low selectivity seen in the simulated data is not present in the experimental data.
Why are models with phasic dynamics consistently more reliable and selective than models with tonic dynamics? Low-threshold potassium currents counteract the regenerative sodium current produced during spike initiation. As a result, cells expressing these currents only spike in response to a rapid increase in excitation. These dynamics enable phasic neurons in the auditory hindbrain and midbrain to detect coherent excitation with high temporal precision, even in noisy conditions [32, 44, 45]. Consistent with this idea, we observed in our sensory model that moments of high concordance between the RF and the stimulus created peaks in the driving current Istim(t). The phasic models spiked almost exclusively at these moments (Fig 8A). In contrast, the tonic models were more sensitive to the absolute level of V(t) and increased their firing rate as the integral of Istim(t) became more positive (Fig 8B).
Based on this observation and the positive correlation between selectivity and temporal modulation frequency (Fig 3D), we hypothesized that the effect of phasic excitability on selectivity would be stronger when the RF produced a driving current with sparse peaks of excitation. In the spectral domain, this corresponds to convolutions that have more power at higher frequencies and RFs that have higher temporal modulation frequencies. To test this effect specifically, we simulated another set of data with eight RFs, holding all RF parameters constant except temporal modulation (Ωt), which we varied between 10 and 80 Hz.
At low values of Ωt, convolutions of the RF and a stimulus produced mostly slow modulations, and the response of phasic and tonic models were similar, as the example in Fig 9A shows. As the modulations in the convolution became faster and large deflections became sparser (Fig 9A, 80 Hz), the response of the phasic model became more selective, while the tonic model, responding to the shape of the slow modulation still present in the signal, showed no inclination toward selectivity. Fig 9B presents the full set of simulations, showing a strong interaction between the temporal modulation of the RF and selectivity. Phasic models showed a strong increase in selectivity as Ωt of the RF increased, but tonic models were unaffected.
Discussion
This study investigated how intrinsic dynamics could contribute to the emergence of selectivity and tolerance in CM, a cortical-level auditory area. We used a novel linear-dynamical cascade model that combines a spectrotemporal receptive field with a biophysical description of intrinsic membrane dynamics. We found that a low-threshold potassium current () strongly affected how the model neurons encoded information about song stimuli in firing rates. Phasic models (with high ) were more selective and more tolerant of noise than models with identical RFs and tonic dynamics. Furthermore, a population of phasic and tonic models reproduced the distribution of selectivity and coding efficiency seen in vivo (Fig 7). These results suggest that a diversity of intrinsic intracellular properties contributes to the higher-order functional properties seen in CM.
The model we developed for this analysis is a special case of the linear-nonlinear (LN) cascade model used in many studies of sensory coding [46–48]. In the standard LN model, the nonlinearity is a history-independent function that transforms the output of the linear stage into an instantaneous spiking probability. More recent LN models incorporate history dependence through a linear kernel convolved with past spike times, as in the generalized linear model [35, 49], or through non-biological, dynamical state variables, as in the spike response and generalized integrate-and-fire models [50–52]. The present study required a more biologically realistic representation of the dynamics so that we could manipulate a specific current of interest, . We therefore used a conductance-based biophysical model to generate spikes. This model lacks many of the morphological and physiological properties of CM neurons and omits any circuit-level organization, but it is capable of reproducing the tonic and phasic firing patterns observed in CM slices [33].
For the linear stage of the model, we used simplified STRFs from a previous study of Field L [36], which is the primary source of ascending auditory input to CM [53]. The reason we did not use STRFs from CM is that linear RF estimates from this area have poor predictive power [40] and are therefore unlikely to be representative of the true synaptic tuning. As seen in another secondary auditory area, the caudomedial nidopallium [54], RFs in CM may involve nonlinear combinations of multiple feature vectors. These nonlinearities may reflect local excitatory and inhibitory circuitry, and as methods for estimating parameters in these more complex models improve [55], it will be important to also consider the contributions of intrinsic dynamics. Here, we chose to use a simplified, parametric RF model so that we could systematically investigate how different RF features interacted with nonlinear dynamics. The clear effects seen here with simplified RFs argue that intrinsic dynamics have the potential to facilitate categorical responses even in the absence of complex circuitry.
Our main finding is that increased caused responses to conspecific song to become sparser, more precise, and more reliable (Fig 2). We focused here on quantifying these effects using several well-established rate-based encoding metrics [10, 38], though the effects on temporal precision are worthy of future consideration. Selectivity, which is also called activity fraction or lifetime sparseness [13, 14], was strongly enhanced by (Fig 3). Syllables that produced strong responses remained strong, but weak responses became weaker, causing response rate distributions to become more heavy-tailed. This led to a reduction in the total entropy of the distributions (Fig 5), which reflects the fact that a more selective neuron responds similarly to all the weak stimuli and therefore encodes little information about the majority of the stimulus set. At the same time, the phasic models lost much less of their bandwidth to noise, which led to a net increase in mutual information and coding efficiency. Thus, can increase both the sparseness of the neural code and its reliability.
The effects of were not simply a product of reduced excitability. Rather, they arose from complex interactions between the RF, the stimulus, and the nonlinear dynamics of the membrane. Phasic dynamics resulted in larger increases in selectivity when RFs were tuned to rapid temporal modulations. Because zebra finch song has strong, broadband rhythmic structure, these RFs produced convolutions with brief peaks of excitation (Figs 8 and 9) [36]. Phasic dynamics also had stronger effects for realistic noise currents that were dominated by low-frequency fluctuations (Fig 6E–6H) [37, 41]. Together, these results indicate that enables neurons to reliably pick out brief moments of strong excitation against a background of low-frequency noise. This is consistent with extensive theoretical and experimental studies of in the auditory hindbrain and midbrain [29, 56–59].
Although these results provide strong support for the possibility that intrinsic dynamics are relevant to neural encoding in zebra finch CM, they do not rule out the contribution of synaptic mechanisms. Indeed, strong feedforward inhibition sharpens selectivity and increases temporal precision in cortical-level rodent and avian auditory areas [60–63] in a manner similar to the effects produced by in this study. Further work is needed to determine if the cell-intrinsic effects of are replicating the effects of feedforward inhibition, albeit in a more compact way, or if there are more consequential differences between these mechanisms.
This study shows that phasic dynamics can enhance selectivity and tolerance for conspecific song, which raises the question of why CM contains both tonic and phasic neurons. The functional significance of diverse intrinsic dynamics has been examined in a variety of brain areas. In the olfactory bulb, diverse dynamics in mitral cells help to decorrelate responses across neurons [64]; in the visual thalamus, bursting in neurons that project to the cortex enhances signal-to-noise ratios [65]; and in the electrosensory lateral line lobe, bursting dynamics provide a parallel information stream for low-frequency events [66]. In CM, some neurons are strongly selective and others are not [8, 10, 67], and a similar pattern is also seen in mammalian secondary auditory areas [15, 68]. Although we can only speculate at this point as to the behavioral significance, our data indicate that diversity in the intrinsic dynamics of CM neurons may contribute substantially to the functional diversity. The models in this study sampled from a distribution of RFs with a broad range of spectral and temporal statistics, but only when the population included both phasic and tonic dynamics did we observe the broad, correlated distribution of selectivity and coding efficiency seen in zebra finch CM (Fig 7). Interestingly, the model response properties were more diverse than seen in vivo, suggesting that may be selectively expressed in a subset of neurons, such as the ones tuned to rapid temporal modulations.
Animals from a broad range of species are able to perform invariant auditory object recognition in challenging conditions [69–72]. In many sensory pathways, there is a hierarchical increase in selectivity and tolerance for natural objects that is thought to underlie this remarkable ability. However, the circuit-level implementation of this computation remains largely theoretical [73]. Although many models have followed Hubel and Wiesel [24] in focusing on how excitatory and inhibitory connectivity enables neurons to aggregate inputs tuned to simpler features, the nonlinear mechanisms of spike generation, which determine how these inputs are summed, are also thought to be important [74]. The present study supports this idea by showing how a single voltage-gated current, , has the potential to dramatically shift how information about stimulus identity is encoded. As we become increasingly aware of the diversity of cell types in the brain [75–77] and the activity-dependent mechanisms that can modulate intrinsic electrophysiological properties [78–80], it is important to account for intrinsic dynamics in models of sensory processing.
Materials and methods
Linear-dynamical cascade model
Biophysical model
The model used in this study is a conductance-based, single-compartment model for CM neurons [33], which was adapted from the ventral cochlear nucleus model of Rothman and Manis [29]. The currents in the model include four voltage-gated potassium and sodium currents, a leak current, and a hyperpolarization-activated cation current. A key component of the model is the low-threshold potassium current (), which determines whether the neuron produces tonic or phasic responses to step depolarization. When (the maximal conductance of ) is low, the model neuron produces sustained responses to weak and moderate depolarizations; when is high, the model only fires phasically, at the onset of the current step. The model parameter values follow Rothman and Manis [29], with a few adjustments to match the resting potential and spike threshold for CM neurons. The calculations presented here used the consensus model parameters from Chen and Meliza [33] for tonic and phasic cells (Table 1). S1 Appendix. contains the full model specification. The model simulation code was generated using spyks (https://github.com/melizalab/spyks; version 0.6.6), and the dynamics were integrated using a 5th-order Runge-Kutta algorithm with an adaptive error tolerance of 1 × 10−5 and an interpolated step size of 0.025 ms.
Table 1. Parameter values for the tonic and phasic models.
Parameter | Tonic | Phasic |
---|---|---|
C | 60 pF | 40 pF |
glk | 1.3 nS | 1.3 nS |
Elk | −75 mV | −75 mV |
gNa | 750 nS | 750 nS |
ENa | 55 mV | 55 mV |
30 nS | 30 nS | |
95 nS | 95 nS | |
0 nS | 50 nS | |
EK | −82 mV | −82 mV |
gh | 0.5 nS | 0.5 nS |
Eh | −43 mV | −43 mV |
Bolded values differ between the tonic and phasic models.
Auditory response simulation
To simulate an auditory response, the model was driven by a current, Istim(t), which was calculated from the convolution of a spectrotemporal receptive field (RF) with the spectrogram of an auditory stimulus. In this formulation, the RF approximated the synaptic drive to the neuron as a weighted sum of linear, time-invariant filters over different frequency channels. To simulate stimulus-independent variability in the response, a second driving current, Inoise(t), was added. The noise current was randomly generated red noise (1/f2 power spectrum) scaled relative to the signal to achieve a set signal-to-noise ratio (SNR). Red noise was chosen for its power at low frequencies which efficiently drives response variability in the neuron models and for the biological validity of the power spectrum of the noise. 1/fα spectra are found in many complex, natural systems including the brain [41], and the statistics of synaptic noise are well approximated by a 1/f2 spectrum [37]. For all of the analyses presented in this paper with the exception of the analysis of the effect of the signal-to-noise ratio, SNR was set at 4.
RFs were constructed with a Gabor filter based on Woolley et al. [36]:
where H is the temporal dimension of the RF, G is the spectral dimension of the RF, t0 is the latency, f0 is the peak frequency, σt and σf are the temporal and spectral bandwidths, Ωt and Ωf are the temporal and spectral modulation frequencies, and Pt is the temporal phase. Parameter values were randomly drawn from distributions set to match the modulation transfer function (MTF) of the RF ensemble to the MTF of zebra finch song [36, 81]. The integral of each RF was normalized to one.
The models’ responses were simulated using 30 zebra finch songs recorded from our colony. Each recording was comprised of a single song motif repeated twice. Recordings were normalized to the same RMS amplitude and edited to be 2.025 s long, with 50 ms of microphone noise at the beginning to pad the convolution, and scaled to a consistent RMS amplitude. Start and end times of syllables were identified by visual inspection. Repeated syllables were grouped in the decoding analyses. Spectrograms of the stimuli were calculated using the short-time Fourier transform algorithm with a Hanning window of 256 points and then resampled to give a frequency resolution of 50 channels between 0 and 8 kHz. Successive windows were spaced 1.0 ms apart.
In the context of this simulation, a model neuron was defined as the combination of one RF and one biophysical parameter set (phasic or tonic). 60 RFs were generated to produce paired phasic and tonic simulations (n = 120 neurons or 60 pairs). For the analysis of the relationship between selectivity and temporal modulation, 8 RFs were generated and for each RF Ωt was set to 10, 20, …, 80 (n = 128 neurons or 8 sets of pairs). The 30 zebra finch songs were presented 10 times each to each neuron with different values of Inoise(t) producing trial-to-trial variability. Noise currents in each trial were identical between paired phasic and tonic neurons. The total amplitude of the convolution was normalized by the bandwidth of the RF on the frequency axis (σf) to account for the differences in amplitudes between narrowband and broadband RFs. The output of the model was a simulated voltage trace.
The stimuli and simulated responses have been deposited with Data Dryad (doi:10.5061/dryad.js11601).
Data analysis
Spike times were extracted from the simulated responses using a simple window discriminator (https://github.com/melizalab/quickspikes; version 1.3.3). We calculated spike rate, ri,j, as the number of spikes evoked by syllable i in trial j, divided by the duration of the syllable. Selectivity was quantified using activity fraction [10, 13], a nonparametric index defined as:
where ri is the rate for syllable i averaged across trials, and N is the total number of syllables.
Mutual information (MI), total entropy, and noise entropy were calculated following Jeanne et al. [38]. Response rates were discretized into 15 bins between the minimum and maximum rate of the model. Total entropy was calculated as H(R) = −∑p(r)log2 p(r), noise entropy as H(R|S) = −∑p(s)∑p(r|s)log2 p(r|s), and mutual information as I(R;S) = H(R) − H(R|S), where r is the rate and s is the syllable. Because of the large number of stimuli and trials, and because we were interested in differences between models presented with exactly the same stimuli, we did not correct entropy or MI for sample size bias. Coding efficiency was calculated as 1 − (H(R|S)/H(R)).
Stimuli
Thirty male zebra finches provided song recordings that were used as stimuli in the simulation experiments. All animal use was performed in accordance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Virginia. Adult zebra finches were obtained from the University of Virginia breeding colony. During recording, zebra finches were housed in a soundproof auditory isolation box (Eckel Industries, Cambridge, MA) with ad libitum food and water and were kept on a 16:8h light:dark schedule. A mirror was added to the box to stimulate singing. Recordings were made with an Audio-Technica Pro 70 microphone, digitized with a Focusrite Scarlett 2i2 at 44.1 kHz, and stored to disk using custom C++ software (https://github.com/melizalab/jill; version 2.1.4). A typical recording session lasted 2–3 days. A single representative song was selected from each bird’s recorded corpus and was high-pass filtered at 500Hz with a 4th-order Butterworth filter.
Extracellular data
Analyses based on extracellular data were performed on the publicly available dataset from Theunissen et al. [43] available at http://crcns.org/data-sets/aa/aa-2. Neural recordings were collected from adult male zebra finches as described in Gill et al. [82]. Only responses from CM neurons presented with conspecific song were used for these analyses (n = 37). Selectivity and MI analyses were performed as described above, except that 10 response bins were used for MI analysis instead of 15, due to the smaller stimulus set.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
We thank Tyler Robbins for assistance in model development and JC Cang and Alev Erisir for critical comments and discussions.
Data Availability
All stimuli and data files have been deposited in Data Dryad (doi:10.5061/dryad.js11601) and are publically available.
Funding Statement
This work received funding from the Thomas F. and Kate Miller Jeffress Memorial Trust, Bank of America, N.A., Trustee (CDM; https://hria.org/tmf/jeffress/) and the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Program (MCB; https://www.nsfgrfp.org/). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Iverson P, Kuhl PK. Perceptual magnet and phoneme boundary effects in speech perception: Do they arise from a common mechanism? Percept Psychophys. 2000;62(4):874–886. 10.3758/BF03206929 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Liberman AM, Harris KS, Kinney JA, Lane H. The discrimination of relative onset-time of the components of certain speech and nonspeech patterns. J Exp Psychol. 1961;61(5):379 10.1037/h0049038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. May B, Moody DB, Stebbins WC. Categorical perception of conspecific communication sounds by Japanese macaques, M acacafuscata. JASA. 1989;85(2):837–847. 10.1121/1.397555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Ehret G. Categorical perception of mouse-pup ultrasounds in the temporal domain. Animal Behav. 1992;43(3):409–416. 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80101-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Hauser MD, Marler P. Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): I. Socioecological factors. Behav Ecol. 1993;4(3):194–205. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Elie JE, Theunissen FE. Zebra finches identify individuals using vocal signatures unique to each call type. Nat Commun. 2018;9(1):4026 10.1038/s41467-018-06394-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Rauschecker JP, Tian B, Hauser M. Processing of complex sounds in the macaque nonprimary auditory cortex. Science. 1995;268(5207):111–114. 10.1126/science.7701330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Gentner TQ, Margoliash D. Neuronal populations and single cells representing learned auditory objects. Nature. 2003;424(6949):669–674. 10.1038/nature01731 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Tsunada J, Lee JH, Cohen YE. Differential representation of auditory categories between cell classes in primate auditory cortex. J Physiol (Lond). 2012;590(Pt 13):3129–3139. 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.232892 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Meliza CD, Margoliash D. Emergence of selectivity and tolerance in the avian auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2012;32(43):15158–15168. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0845-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Elie JE, Theunissen FE. Meaning in the avian auditory cortex: neural representation of communication calls. Eur J Neurosci. 2015;41(5):546–567. 10.1111/ejn.12812 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Mouterde SC, Elie JE, Mathevon N, Theunissen FE. Single neurons in the avian auditory cortex encode individual identity and propagation distance in naturally degraded communication calls. J Neurosci. 2017; p. 2220–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Rolls E, Tovee MJ. Sparseness of the neuronal representation of stimuli in the primate temporal visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1995;73(2):713–726. 10.1152/jn.1995.73.2.713 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Vinje WE, Gallant JL. Sparse coding and decorrelation in primary visual cortex during natural vision. Science. 2000;287(5456):1273–1276. 10.1126/science.287.5456.1273 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Zoccolan D, Kouh M, Poggio T, DiCarlo JJ. Trade-off between object selectivity and tolerance in monkey inferotemporal cortex. J Neurosci. 2007;27(45):12292–12307. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1897-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Chang EF, Rieger JW, Johnson K, Berger MS, Barbaro NM, Knight RT. Categorical speech representation in human superior temporal gyrus. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(11):1428–1432. 10.1038/nn.2641 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Mesgarani N, Cheung C, Johnson K, Chang EF. Phonetic feature encoding in human superior temporal gyrus. Science. 2014;343(6174):1006–1010. 10.1126/science.1245994 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. de Heer WA, Huth AG, Griffiths TL, Gallant JL, Theunissen FE. The hierarchical cortical organization of human speech processing. J Neurosci. 2017;37(27):6539–6557. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3267-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Riesenhuber M, Poggio T. Models of object recognition. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3 Suppl(Supp):1199–1204. 10.1038/81479 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Larson E, Billimoria CP, Sen K. A biologically plausible computational model for auditory object recognition. J Neurophysiol. 2009;101(1):323–331. 10.1152/jn.90664.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Bastos AM, Usrey WM, Adams RA, Mangun GR, Fries P, Friston KJ. Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron. 2012;76(4):695–711. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Gentner T. Neural systems for individual song recognition in adult birds. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1016(1):282–302. 10.1196/annals.1298.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Calabrese A, Woolley SMN. Coding principles of the canonical cortical microcircuit in the avian brain. PNAS. 2015;112(11):3517–3522. 10.1073/pnas.1408545112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Hubel D, Wiesel TN. Receptive fields and functional architecture in two nonstriate visual areas (18 and 19) of the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1965;28(2):229–289. 10.1152/jn.1965.28.2.229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Perks KE, Gentner TQ. Subthreshold membrane responses underlying sparse spiking to natural vocal signals in auditory cortex. Eur J Neurosci. 2015;41(5):725–733. 10.1111/ejn.12831 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Carr CE, Soares D. Evolutionary convergence and shared computational principles in the auditory system. Brain Behav Evol. 2002;59(5-6):294–311. 10.1159/000063565 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Khurana S, Remme MWH, Rinzel J, Golding NL. Dynamic interaction of Ih and IK-LVA during trains of synaptic potentials in principal neurons of the medial superior olive. J Neurosci. 2011;31(24):8936–8947. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1079-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Gai Y, Kotak VC, Sanes DH, Rinzel J. On the localization of complex sounds: temporal encoding based on input-slope coincidence detection of envelopes. J Neurophysiol. 2014;112(4):802–813. 10.1152/jn.00044.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Rothman JS, Manis PB. Differential expression of three distinct potassium currents in the ventral cochlear nucleus. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89(6):3070–3082. 10.1152/jn.00125.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Gai Y, Doiron B, Kotak V, Rinzel J. Noise-gated encoding of slow inputs by auditory brain stem neurons with a low-threshold K+ current. J Neurophysiol. 2009;102(6):3447–3460. 10.1152/jn.00538.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Carr CE, MacLeod KM. Microseconds matter. PLoS biology. 2010;8(6):e1000405 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000405 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Svirskis G, Kotak V, Sanes DH, Rinzel J. Enhancement of signal-to-noise ratio and phase locking for small inputs by a low-threshold outward current in auditory neurons. J Neurosci. 2002;22(24):11019–11025. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-11019.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Chen AN, Meliza CD. Phasic and Tonic Cell Types in the Zebra Finch Auditory Caudal Mesopallium. J Neurophysiol. 2018;119(3):1127–1139. 10.1152/jn.00694.2017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Theunissen FE, Sen K, Doupe AJ. Spectral-temporal receptive fields of nonlinear auditory neurons obtained using natural sounds. J Neurosci. 2000;20(6):2315–2331. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-02315.2000 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Pillow JW, Paninski L, Uzzell VJ, Simoncelli EP, Chichilnisky EJ. Prediction and decoding of retinal ganglion cell responses with a probabilistic spiking model. J Neurosci. 2005;25(47):11003–11013. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3305-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Woolley SMN, Gill PR, Fremouw T, Theunissen FE. Functional groups in the avian auditory system. J Neurosci. 2009;29(9):2780–2793. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2042-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Destexhe A, Rudolph M, Fellous JM, Sejnowski TJ. Fluctuating synaptic conductances recreate in vivo-like activity in neocortical neurons. Neuroscience. 2001;107(1):13–24. 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00344-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Jeanne JM, Thompson JV, Sharpee TO, Gentner TQ. Emergence of learned categorical representations within an auditory forebrain circuit. J Neurosci. 2011;31(7):2595–2606. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3930-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Cynx J. Experimental determination of a unit of song production in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). J Comp Psychol. 1990;104(1):3 10.1037/0735-7036.104.1.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Sen K, Theunissen FE, Doupe AJ. Feature analysis of natural sounds in the songbird auditory forebrain. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86(3):1445–1458. 10.1152/jn.2001.86.3.1445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Bedard C, Kroeger H, Destexhe A. Does the 1/f frequency scaling of brain signals reflect self-organized critical states? Phys Rev Lett. 2006;97(11):118102 10.1103/PhysRevLett.97.118102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Rothman JS, Manis PB. The roles potassium currents play in regulating the electrical activity of ventral cochlear nucleus neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;89(6):3097–3113. 10.1152/jn.00127.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Theunissen FE, Gill P, Noopur A, Zhang J, Woolley SMN, Fremouw T. Single-unit recordings from multiple auditory areas in male zebra finches. CRCNS.org. 2011.
- 44. Rathouz M, Trussell L. Characterization of outward currents in neurons of the avian nucleus magnocellularis. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80(6):2824–2835. 10.1152/jn.1998.80.6.2824 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Khurana S, Liu Z, Lewis AS, Rosa K, Chetkovich D, Golding NL. An essential role for modulation of hyperpolarization-activated current in the development of binaural temporal precision. J Neurosci. 2012;32(8):2814–2823. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3882-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Eggermont JJ, Aertsen A, Johannesma PIM. Quantitative characterisation procedure for auditory neurons based on the spectro-temporal receptive field. Hear Res. 1983;10(2):167–190 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Keat J, Reinagel P, Reid RC, Meister M. Predicting every spike: a model for the responses of visual neurons. Neuron. 2001;30(3):803–817. 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00322-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Schwartz O, Pillow JW, Rust NC, Simoncelli EP. Spike-triggered neural characterization. J Vis. 2006;6(4):484–507. 10.1167/6.4.13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Paninski L. Maximum likelihood estimation of cascade point-process neural encoding models. Network. 2004;15(4):243–262. 10.1088/0954-898X_15_4_002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Jolivet R, Lewis TJ, Gerstner W. Generalized integrate-and-fire models of neuronal activity approximate spike trains of a detailed model to a high degree of accuracy. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92(2):959–976. 10.1152/jn.00190.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Kobayashi R, Tsubo Y, Shinomoto S. Made-to-order spiking neuron model equipped with a multi-timescale adaptive threshold. Front Comput Neurosci. 2009;3:9 10.3389/neuro.10.009.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Lynch EP, Houghton CJ. Parameter estimation of neuron models using in-vitro and in-vivo electrophysiological data. Front Neuroinform. 2015;9:10 10.3389/fninf.2015.00010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Vates GE, Broome BM, Mello CV, Nottebohm F. Auditory pathways of caudal telencephalon and their relation to the song system of adult male zebra finches. J Comp Neurol. 1996;366(4):613–642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Kozlov AS, Gentner TQ. Central auditory neurons have composite receptive fields. PNAS. 2016;113(5):1441–1446. 10.1073/pnas.1506903113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Kaardal JT, Theunissen FE, Sharpee TO. A low-rank method for characterizing high-level neural computations. Front Comput Neurosci. 2017;11:68 10.3389/fncom.2017.00068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Golding NL, Robertson D, Oertel D. Recordings from slices indicate that octopus cells of the cochlear nucleus detect coincident firing of auditory nerve fibers with temporal precision. J Neurosci. 1995;15(4):3138–3153. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-04-03138.1995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Locke RE, Nerbonne JM. Role of Voltage-Gated K+ Currents in Mediating the Regular-Spiking Phenotype of Callosal-Projecting Rat Visual Cortical Neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1997;78(5):2321–2335. 10.1152/jn.1997.78.5.2321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Sivaramakrishnan S, Oliver DL. Distinct K currents result in physiologically distinct cell types in the inferior colliculus of the rat. J Neurosci. 2001;21(8):2861–2877. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-08-02861.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Meng X, Huguet G, Rinzel J. Type III Excitability, Slope Sensitivity and Coincidence Detection. Discrete Contin Dyn Syst Ser A. 2012;32(8):2729–2757. 10.3934/dcds.2012.32.2729 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Wehr M, Zador AM. Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory cortex. Nature. 2003;426(6965):442–446. 10.1038/nature02116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Froemke RC, Merzenich MM, Schreiner CE. A synaptic memory trace for cortical receptive field plasticity. Nature. 2007;450(7168):425–429. 10.1038/nature06289 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Thompson JV, Jeanne JM, Gentner TQ. Local inhibition modulates learning-dependent song encoding in the songbird auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2013;109(3):721–733. 10.1152/jn.00262.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Schneider DM, Woolley SMN. Sparse and background-invariant coding of vocalizations in auditory scenes. Neuron. 2013;79(1):141–152. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.04.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Padmanabhan K, Urban NN. Intrinsic biophysical diversity decorrelates neuronal firing while increasing information content. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(10):1276–1282. 10.1038/nn.2630 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Krahe R, Gabbiani F. Burst firing in sensory systems. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(1):13 10.1038/nrn1296 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Oswald AMM, Chacron MJ, Doiron B, Bastian J, Maler L. Parallel processing of sensory input by bursts and isolated spikes. J Neurosci. 2004;24(18):4351–4362. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0459-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Meliza CD, Chi Z, Margoliash D. Representations of conspecific song by starling secondary forebrain auditory neurons: toward a hierarchical framework. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103(3):1195–1208. 10.1152/jn.00464.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Cohen YE. Selectivity for the Spatial and Nonspatial Attributes of Auditory Stimuli in the Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex. J Neurosci. 2004;24(50):11307–11316. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3935-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Griffiths TD, Warren JD. What is an auditory object? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(11):887–892. 10.1038/nrn1538 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Rauschecker JP, Scott SK. Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(6):718 10.1038/nn.2331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. DeWitt I, Rauschecker JP. Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream. PNAS. 2012;109(8):E505–E514. 10.1073/pnas.1113427109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Heald SL, Van Hedger SC, Nusbaum HC. Perceptual plasticity for auditory object recognition. Front Psychol. 2017;8:781 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00781 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. DiCarlo JJ, Zoccolan D, Rust NC. How does the brain solve visual object recognition? Neuron. 2012;73(3):415–434. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.01.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Riesenhuber M, Poggio T. Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(11):1019–1025. 10.1038/14819 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Wang Y, Markram H, Goodman PH, Berger TK, Ma J, Goldman-Rakic PS. Heterogeneity in the pyramidal network of the medial prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(4):534–542. 10.1038/nn1670 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. van Aerde KI, Feldmeyer D. Morphological and physiological characterization of pyramidal neuron subtypes in rat medial prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25(3):788–805. 10.1093/cercor/bht278 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Tasic B, Menon V, Nguyen TN, Kim TK, Jarsky T, Yao Z, et al. Adult mouse cortical cell taxonomy revealed by single cell transcriptomics. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19(2):335–346. 10.1038/nn.4216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Nataraj K, Turrigiano G. Regional and temporal specificity of intrinsic plasticity mechanisms in rodent primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2011;31(49):17932–17940. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4455-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Mahon S, Charpier S. Bidirectional plasticity of intrinsic excitability controls sensory inputs efficiency in layer 5 barrel cortex neurons in vivo. J Neurosci. 2012;32(33):11377–11389. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0415-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Dehorter N, Ciceri G, Bartolini G, Lim L, del Pino I, Marin O. Tuning of fast-spiking interneuron properties by an activity-dependent transcriptional switch. Science. 2015;349(6253):1216–1220. 10.1126/science.aab3415 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Singh NC, Theunissen FE. Modulation spectra of natural sounds and ethological theories of auditory processing. JASA. 2003;114(6 Pt 1):3394–3411. 10.1121/1.1624067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Gill P, Zhang J, Woolley SMN, Fremouw T, Theunissen FE. Sound representation methods for spectro-temporal receptive field estimation. J Comput Neurosci. 2006;21(1):5–20. 10.1007/s10827-006-7059-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All stimuli and data files have been deposited in Data Dryad (doi:10.5061/dryad.js11601) and are publically available.