Abstract
Human evolutionary success is often argued to be rooted in specialized social skills and motivations that result in more prosocial, rational and cooperative decisions. One manifestation of human ultra-sociality is the tendency to engage in social comparison. While social comparison studies typically focus on cooperative behaviour and emphasize concern for fairness and equality, here we investigate the competitive dimension of social comparison: a preference for getting more than others, expressed in a willingness to maximize relative payoff at the cost of absolute payoff. Chimpanzees and human children (5–6- and 9–10-year-olds) could decide between an option that maximized their absolute payoff (but put their partner at an advantage) and an option that maximized their relative payoff (but decreased their own and their partner's payoff). Results show that, in contrast to chimpanzees and young children, who consistently selected the rational and payoff-maximizing option, older children paid a cost to reduce their partner's payoff to a level below their own. This finding demonstrates that uniquely human social skills and motivations do not necessarily lead to more prosocial, rational and cooperative decision-making.
Keywords: social comparison, fairness, cooperation
1. Introduction
Compared with chimpanzees, our closest and highly social primate relatives, humans exhibit a suite of species-unique socio-cognitive skills and motivations [1]. The evolutionary emergence of these so-called ultra-social tendencies has been linked to the fact that humans live not only in highly complex social groups, like many other animals, but in cultural groups [2–6]. Cultural adaptations—from imitation learning and pedagogical sensitivity to flexible prosociality and fairness-based cooperation—develop early in human ontogeny [7–10]. Across a large variety of domains, the emergence of species-typical social skills and motivations has been shown to make humans more flexible communicators, more proficient social learners, more competent mind readers and more stable cooperators than chimpanzees [11–18].
According to several theorists, social comparison-based fairness represents one especially important psychological mechanism underlying uniquely human patterns of cooperation [19,20]. One key challenge in sustaining cooperation lies in distributing collaboratively acquired resources so that everyone is satisfied and motivated to collaborate in the future [21]. Human sensitivity to fairness is argued to support the resolution of such conflicts of interests by guiding agreed upon distributions [20]. Central principles of fairness, such as equality, make sense only in light of social comparison, which has been singled out as one of the main psychological building blocks supporting a sense of fairness [21,22].
This theoretical perspective is supported by empirical research revealing a potential species difference regarding social comparison-based fairness: while chimpanzees do not seem to compare their payoffs those of others, young children's fairness judgements seem to be mediated by social comparison concerns. In particular, McAuliffe & Blake [23] provided evidence that from about 4 years of age, young children reject unfair distributions in which a partner receives a better reward than themselves (see also [24,25]). Importantly, children's behaviour shows the signature of social comparison: if the partner is absent, rejection rates drop sharply and children are more likely to accept the lower-quality reward [26]. By contrast, although some have taken chimpanzees' refusal of low-quality food in the presence of a partner who received high-quality food as evidence for social comparison [27–30], a number of studies have failed to replicate these results [31–34], and Engelmann et al. [34] have recently provided data that point to an alternative explanation for chimpanzee behaviour: the social disappointment hypothesis. According to this hypothesis, chimpanzees’ negative emotional reactions to receiving the low-quality reward are grounded in dyadic interpersonal expectations rather than social comparison. It has thus been argued that social comparison-based fairness stabilizes cooperation in humans, but not in chimpanzees [35].
In previous work, social comparison has been framed mostly in a cooperative light, as a psychological mechanism that manifests itself in an aversion to unequal outcomes and a concurrent motivation to equalize outcomes [36]. This is reflected in the fact that all previous experimental paradigms have studied children's behaviour in situations where they can choose between an unequal option and an equal option (for a review, see [37]). However, as pointed out by Sheskin et al. [38], when the focus is on the other fundamental dimension of animal sociality—competition—the tendency to compare payoffs likely manifests itself in a preference for maximizing relative payoffs, that is, for getting more than others [37–41], and associated social emotions like envy and Schadenfreude [42,43]. Thus, while social comparison can facilitate fair outcomes when cooperative motives in humans are evoked, it can also motivate actions for achieving a relative advantage over others when competitive motives dominate.
This raises the possibility that the human propensity for social comparison might lead human children, but not chimpanzees, to non-rational (i.e. non-payoff-maximizing) courses of action in competitive contexts, though this has yet to be tested empirically. We here define a competitive context as one in which, in contrast to previous studies, participants choose between an option that advantages them and an option that disadvantages them relative to their partner (without an option that equalizes payoffs). In the current study, participants thus made a choice between ‘I get more’ or ‘You get more’. The crux was that the ‘You get more’ option simultaneously maximized the actor's payoffs, thereby representing the rational decision from a payoff perspective. In the crucial condition, chimpanzees and human children (two age groups: 5–6 years and 9–10 years) decided between two options: 2–1 (actor receives two items and partner receives one) and 3–6 (actor receives three items and partner receives six). We predicted that chimpanzees and young children would choose the payoff-maximizing option whereas older children would select the option that maximized relative payoff, given that previous research has shown that social comparison concerns intensify between kindergarten and fourth grade [44].
We compared subjects' behaviour in this condition with a control condition in which they decided between a 2–5 and 3–6 option. Here we predicted that chimpanzees and children of both age groups would preferentially choose the payoff-maximizing option (3–6) because both options put the partner at the same relative advantage.
2. Material and methods
(a). Child experiment
(i). Participants
We tested 96 Kikuyu children from three rural schools near Nanyuki in Laikipia county in central Kenya. The sample was made up of 48 five- to six-year-old children and 48 nine- to ten-year-old children. Twenty-four subjects of each age group participated in each condition (12 girls, 12 boys). Ten additional children had to be excluded because of experimenter mistake (incorrect reward distribution in the control condition) and one child because she shared her rewards with her partner.
(ii). Materials and design
Participants sat opposite each other at a small table (figure 1; electronic supplementary material). In a between-subjects design, children participated in four trials in one condition (test or control). The procedure consisted of two consecutive steps: a familiarization phase (see electronic supplementary material) and a test phase (see below). All sessions were videotaped for later coding.
(iii). Procedure
For the test phase, participants were paired with new partners (compared with the familiarization phase) to make sure that decisions during the familiarization phase would not influence decisions during the test phase. Once the new partner had sat down on the blue side, the first experimenter (E1) briefly repeated the rules of the game. Then the test phase started. The procedure of one trial was as follows: E1 presented the board with two trays to the subjects (2–1 versus 3–6 in the test condition and 2–5 versus 3–6 in the control condition; locations of allocations on board were counterbalanced), the subject indicated her choice of tray, and then the children placed their respective rewards on their collection sticks. While subjects picked either distribution, E1 lowered her head so as not to influence the subject's decision. Finally, at the end of each trial, E1 asked children how many rewards they had collected in total (if the answer was wrong, E1 asked again or counted out loud if the child could not give the correct answer after the second prompt). This represented the end of the trial. The procedure was repeated four times for a total of four trials. At the end of the four trials, E1 placed the rewards collected by children in envelopes and told them that they would receive their envelope after class.
(iv). Coding and analysis
A research assistant coded from tape whether or not children had selected the distribution that maximized their payoff (3–6). A second research assistant, who was unaware of the study design and hypothesis, independently coded 25% of all trials. Interrater agreement was excellent (Cohen's κ = 1).
(b). Chimpanzee experiment
(i). Participants
Fifteen chimpanzees (8 females) ranging in age from 8 to 37 years (M = 20 years) participated in this study. Six chimpanzees were tested at Sweetwaters Chimpanzee Sanctuary, Kenya, and nine chimpanzees were tested at Wolfgang Köhler Primate Research Center in Leipzig Zoo, Germany, using identical procedures at the two sites. Each subject was paired with a same-sex partner (see electronic supplementary material).
(ii). Materials and design
Testing took place in two opposing rooms (see electronic supplementary material). In a within-subjects design, chimpanzees participated in two sessions of six trials in each condition. Whether individuals started with the test condition or the control condition was counterbalanced across subjects. The procedure consisted of two consecutive steps: a familiarization phase and a test phase.
(iii). Procedure
During the familiarization phase, subjects were introduced to the apparatus and the contingencies of the task in a series of four steps (see electronic supplementary material). Once subjects had passed this phase, they moved to the test phase. At the beginning of each trial, the partner was located in the partner's room and the subject was located in a room adjacent to the subject's room. Once the first experimenter (E1) had baited the four platforms, a second experimenter opened the door for the subject to enter the subject's room. The subject then had 60 s to choose either option. Once the subject had selected an option by pulling the respective rope, the other rope was removed (either by the first or by a third experimenter). When subject and partner had consumed their rewards, the subject chimpanzee moved back to the first room. E1 then reset the apparatus and the next trial began.
(iv). Coding and analysis
Whether chimpanzees selected the distribution that maximized their payoff (3–6) was coded from tape by the first experimenter. A research assistant, who was unaware of the study design and hypothesis, independently coded 25% of all trials. Interrater agreement was excellent (Cohen's κ = 1).
3. Results
(a). Children
Figure 2 presents the average selection of the payoff-maximizing option in the four conditions of our 2 × 2 design. Five- to six-year-old children chose the payoff-maximizing option in 73% of trials in the test condition and in 79.2% in the control condition. Nine- to 10-year-old children selected the payoff-maximizing option in 50% of trials in the test condition and in 84.5% of trials in the control condition.
To test the effects of age and condition (and their potential interaction) on children's likelihood to select the payoff-maximizing option we ran an analysis of variance (ANOVA) with age and condition as between-subject factors. This revealed a significant interaction between age and condition on children's choice (F1,92 = 7.7, p = 0.007). To further investigate the interaction between age and condition we conducted post hoc pairwise comparisons. We found a significant difference between the test and control conditions for the 9- to 10-year-old children (p < 0.001), showing that older children were significantly less likely to pick the 3–6 option in the test condition—even though this option maximized their absolute payoffs. There was no significant difference between the test and control conditions for the 5- to 6-year-old children (p = 0.35). This overall pattern is also reflected when comparing children's behaviour with chance. Five- to six-year-old children were significantly more likely than expected by chance to select the payoff-maximizing choice in both conditions, test (p < 0.001) and control (p < 0.001). Nine- to 10-year-old children, on the other hand, selected the 3–6 option significantly more often than chance only in the control (p < 0.001), but not in the test condition (p = 1).
(b). Chimpanzees
Chimpanzees selected the payoff-maximizing option (3–6) in 88.3% of trials in the test condition and in 75% of trials in the control condition. In both conditions, chimpanzees pulled the payoff-maximizing option more often than expected by chance (test condition: p < 0.001, control condition: p < 0.001). We ran a Wilcoxon matched-pairs exact test to determine whether there was a statistical difference between the test and control condition in how often chimpanzees pulled the payoff-maximizing option. The test revealed a significant difference: chimpanzees were more likely to pull the payoff-maximizing choice in the test condition (Mdn = 10.6) than in the control condition (Mdn = 9, n = 15, z = −2.27, three ties, p = 0.025).
4. Discussion
In this study, we investigated the effects of social comparison on children's and chimpanzees’ allocation decisions in a competitive context. Participants could decide between an option that maximized their relative payoff compared with a peer—the ‘I get more’ option—and an option that maximized their absolute payoff but simultaneously put them at a disadvantage compared with a peer—the ‘You get more’ option. Young children and chimpanzees behaved in a rational manner: they were highly consistent in their choice of the option that maximized absolute payoff. Older children, however, acted in an irrational manner (from a payoff perspective): they paid a cost to be at a relative advantage compared with a peer and thus maximized relative rather than the absolute payoff. Our results bear out the view that the development of novel social motivations does not necessarily result in more rational decision-making [45], but on the contrary, can lead older children to make suboptimal decisions compared to both younger children and chimpanzees.
While a previous study with participants from an industrialized Western population has shown that children will sometimes prefer a relative advantage to absolute payoffs [38], the current results extend this finding to children from a traditional, small-scale, non-Western group, the Kikuyu. The Kikuyu children in the current sample came from a low socio-economic background and owned no (or very few) personal possessions [46,47]. One might argue that rather than showing a preference for relative advantage, the current results provide evidence that older children are concerned with minimizing inequality. Proportionally speaking, the two options in the test condition, 2–1 and 3–6, are equally fair (0.5), whereas one option in the control condition is more equal (3–6) than the other (2–5). This might explain why older children do not show a preference for either option in the test condition but preferred the 3–6 option in the control condition. To rule out this alternative explanation, we ran an additional control condition for the older children in which they decided between a 2–4 and 3–6 distribution.1 If children indeed aimed at proportionally equal payoffs rather than maximizing their own payoffs, they also should have been at chance when deciding between these two options. However, older children chose the 3–6 option in 74% of trials, and thus displayed a statistically significant preference (p < 0.001) for the option that maximized their payoffs (for details, see electronic supplementary material).
The finding that older children had a stronger preference for relative advantage than younger children is in line with previous research showing that social comparison concerns intensify as children move from early to middle childhood [44,48,49]. At the same time, recent research has shown that inequity aversion increases with age and that children are more likely to select equal rather than unequal distributions as they get older [23,24,37,42,50,51]. One caveat arises regarding the interpretation of our results. The current findings show that older children maximize relative payoff at the cost of absolute payoff when the choice is between a distribution that puts them at a relative advantage and a distribution that puts them at a relative disadvantage. The results do not speak to the relative strength of a preference to get more than others versus a preference for equality. It might well be the case that if we had added a third, equal option to the test condition, older children would have shown a strong preference for this option [24,50,52]. But the question of interest in the current study was not the development of preferences for equal payoff—as in previous studies—but the development of a costly preference to maximize relative payoffs. Future research should investigate how a growing tendency to care about relative payoffs and a concurrent increase in concern for equality can be reconciled. Steinbeis & Singer [42] provide the interesting suggestion that an increase in children's social comparison concerns might in some settings be masked by a simultaneous improvement in emotion regulation, particularly in public settings where exhibiting strong social comparison concerns might come at a reputational cost. This proposal is supported by the finding that reputational concerns increase over childhood [53] and by research showing that children replace overt forms of social comparison with more subtle forms as they grow older [54].
In the present study, chimpanzees, like the younger children, consistently selected the payoff-maximizing course of action. Chimpanzees' pattern of decision-making did not show the signature of social comparison concerns and a preference for a relative over absolute payoffs. This finding is consistent with previous work revealing an absence of social comparison-based fairness concerns in chimpanzees [34]. In fact, in the current study, chimpanzees exhibited a pattern of behaviour opposite to what would be expected by social comparison concerns, as they selected the payoff-maximizing option more often in the test compared with the control condition, a finding that we had not predicted. One potential explanation for this counterintuitive result is that it was more difficult for chimpanzees to decide between the two options in the control condition because their absolute amounts were more similar compared with the test condition. However, step 4 of the familiarization provides conclusive evidence that chimpanzees in the current set-up did not simply go for the option that featured the greatest total reward. In addition, and crucially, chimpanzees showed a highly robust tendency to select the option that maximized their payoffs in both test (88%) and control (75%) conditions.
Taken together, this work presents evidence for the view that chimpanzees are rational-maximizers in the context of resource distributions [33,55]. However, while this idea has been mostly linked to an absence of concerns for fairness and equality in chimpanzees, the current findings highlight that motivations such as social comparison can be a double-edged sword as they influence human behaviour not only in cooperative but also in competitive contexts. That is to say, while social comparison underlies fairness, a hallmark of human social life which gives rise to a concern for equality, at the same time it manifests itself in more negative emotions such as envy and Schadenfreude. Chimpanzees might not demonstrate concern for equality, but neither is their behaviour influenced by social comparison's more self-centred expressions. While even children as young as 9 and 10 years of age showed a willingness to reduce their own and their partner's payoff simply in order to come out on top, chimpanzees' decisions—in terms of their outcome—maximized both their own and their partner's payoffs, dispelling the notion that more sociality is always associated with more prosociality.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Richard Vigne, Stephen Ngulu, the board members and all the staff of Sweetwaters Chimpanzee Sanctuary in Kenya for their crucial support during all stages of this research. Special thanks go to David Mundia, Steven Mukundi and Charles Mussasia. We also thank Ol Pejeta Conservancy, Kenya Wildlife Service (KWS) and National Council for Science and Technology (NCST) for approving our research. In addition, we thank all the staff at Zoo Leipzig for their help in data collection. We also thank the following schools for child data collection: Matanya, Yard, DEB and Sweetwaters Primary School. We also thank Nacosti and the District Education Office of Laikipia Central. Thanks go to Roger Mundy for statistical advice; Nadin Bobovnikov for reliability coding; Cristina Zickert for the apparatus figures; Raik Pieszek for building the apparatus; and Anna-Claire Schneider, Yarrow Dunham, Sebastian Grüneisen and Katherine McAuliffe for very helpful comments on previous versions of this manuscript.
Endnote
We thank an anonymous reviewer for suggesting this additional control condition.
Ethics
Chimpanzee research was performed in accordance with the recommendations of the Weatherall report (see electronic supplementary material). Informed written consent was obtained from the school guardians of the children who participated in this study.
Data accessibility
Data associated with this manuscript can be accessed as part of the electronic supplementary material.
Authors' contributions
E.H., L.M.H., H.Z. and J.M.E. designed the study, analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. L.M.H. and H.Z. conducted the study.
Competing interests
The authors have no competing interests.
Funding
The authors have no funding to declare.
References
- 1.Tomasello M. 2014. The ultra-social animal. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 44, 187–194. ( 10.1002/ejsp.2015) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Henrich J. 2015. The secret of our success: how culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Tomasello M. 1999. The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Laland KN. 2017. Darwin's unfinished symphony: how culture made the human mind. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Richerson PJ, Boyd R. 2005. Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Whiten A. 2010. Ape behaviour and the origins of human culture. In Mind the gap: tracing the origins of human universals (eds Kappeler PM, Silk J), pp. 429–450. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Warneken F. 2018. How children solve the two challenges of cooperation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 69, 205–229. ( 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011813) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Csibra G, Gergely G. 2011. Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 1149–1157. ( 10.1098/rstb.2010.0319) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Butler LP, Markman EM. 2016. Navigating pedagogy: children's developing capacities for learning from pedagogical interactions. Cognit. Dev. 38, 27–35. ( 10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.01.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Haun DBM, van Leeuwen EJC, Edelson MG. 2013. Majority influence in children and other animals. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 3, 61–71. ( 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.09.003) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Herrmann E, Call J, Hernández-Lloreda MV, Hare B, Tomasello M. 2007. Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science 317, 1360–1366. ( 10.1126/science.1146282) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hamann K, Warneken F, Greenberg JR, Tomasello M. 2011. Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees. Nature 476, 328–331. ( 10.1038/nature10278) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Melis AP, Grocke P, Kalbitz J, Tomasello M. 2016. One for you, one for me: humans' unique turn-taking skills. Psychol. Sci. 27, 987–996. ( 10.1177/0956797616644070) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Liszkowski U, Schäfer M, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. 2009. Prelinguistic infants, but not chimpanzees, communicate about absent entities. Psychol. Sci. 20, 654–660. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02346.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dean LG, Kendal RL, Schapiro SJ, Thierry B, Laland KN. 2012. Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science 335, 1114–1118. ( 10.1126/science.1213969) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Whiten A, McGuigan N, Marshall-Pescini S, Hopper LM. 2009. Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364, 2417–2428. ( 10.1098/rstb.2009.0069) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.van Leeuwen EJ, Call J. 2017. Conservatism and ‘copy-if-better’ in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim. Cogn. 20, 575–579. ( 10.1007/s10071-016-1061-7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Santos LR, Rosati AG. 2015. The evolutionary roots of human decision making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 3, 321–347. ( 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015310) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fehr E, Fischbacher U. 2004. Social norms and human cooperation. Trends Cognit. Sci. 8, 187–190. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tomasello M. 2016. A natural history of human morality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Baumard N, Andre J-B, Sperber D. 2013. A mutualistic approach to morality: the evolution of fairness by partner choice. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 59–122. ( 10.1017/S0140525X11002202) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fiske AP. 1991. Structures of social life: the four elementary forms of human relations. New York, NY: Free Press. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Blake PR, McAuliffe K. 2011. ‘I had so much it didn't seem fair’: eight-year-olds reject two forms of inequity. Cognition 120, 215–224. ( 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.04.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Blake PR, et al. 2015. The ontogeny of fairness in seven societies. Nature 528, 258–262. ( 10.1038/nature15703) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Shaw A, Olson KR. 2012. Children discard a resource to avoid inequity. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 382–395. ( 10.1037/a0025907) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.McAuliffe K, Blake PR, Kim G, Wrangham RW, Warneken F. 2013. Social influences on inequity aversion in children. PLoS ONE 8, e80966 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0080966) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Brosnan SF, Schiff HC, de Waal FBM. 2005. Tolerance for inequity may increase with social closeness in chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 20042947 ( 10.1098/rspb.2004.2947) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hopper LM, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Brosnan SF. 2014. Social comparison mediates chimpanzees' responses to loss, not frustration. Anim. Cogn. 17, 1303–1311. ( 10.1007/s10071-014-0765-9) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Brosnan SF, Talbot C, Ahlgren M, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ. 2010. Mechanisms underlying responses to inequitable outcomes in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Anim. Behav. 79, 1229–1237. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.019) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brosnan SF, Hopper LM, Richey S, Freeman HD, Talbot CF, Gosling SD, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ. 2015. Personality influences responses to inequity and contrast in chimpanzees. Anim. Behav. 101, 75–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bräuer J, Call J, Tomasello M. 2006. Are apes really inequity averse? Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 3123–3128. ( 10.1098/rspb.2006.3693) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bräuer J, Call J, Tomasello M. 2009. Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange paradigm. Am. J. Primatol. 71, 175–181. ( 10.1002/ajp.20639) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hopper LM, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Brosnan SF. 2013. When given the opportunity, chimpanzees maximize personal gain rather than ‘level the playing field’. PeerJ 1, e165 ( 10.7717/peerj.165) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Engelmann JM, Clift J, Herrmann E, Tomasello M. 2017. Social disappointment explains chimpanzees' behavior in the inequity aversion task. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20171502 ( 10.1098/rspb.2017.1502) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Engelmann JM, Tomasello M. 2018. Prosociality and morality in chimpanzees and children. In New perspectives on moral development (ed. Helwig CC.), pp. 15–32. London, UK: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Brosnan SF, de Waal FBM. 2014. Evolution of responses to (un)fairness. Science 346, 12517761 ( 10.1126/science.1251776) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Festinger L. 1954. A theory of social comparison processes. Hum. Relat. 7, 117–140. ( 10.1177/001872675400700202) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sheskin M, Bloom P, Wynn K. 2014. Anti-equality: social comparison in young children. Cognition 130, 152–156. ( 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.10.008) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Fiske ST. 2010. Envy up, scorn down: how comparison divides us. Am. Psychol. 65, 698–706. ( 10.1037/0003-066X.65.8.698) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Dohmen T, Falk A, Fliessbach K, Sunde U, Weber B. 2011. Relative versus absolute income, joy, of winning and gender: brain imaging evidence. J. Publ. Econ. 95, 279–285. ( 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2010.11.025) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Cox CA. 2013. Inequity aversion and advantage seeking with asymmetric competition. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 86, 121–136. ( 10.1016/j.jebo.2012.12.020) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Steinbeis N, Singer T. 2013. The effects of social comparison on social emotions and behavior during childhood: the ontogeny of envy and Schadenfreude predicts developmental changes in equity-related decisions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 115, 198–209. ( 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.11.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Mendes N, Steinbeis N, Bueno-Guerra N, Call J, Singer T. 2017. Preschool children and chimpanzees incur costs to watch punishment of antisocial others. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 45–51. ( 10.1038/s41562-017-0264-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ruble DN, Boggiano AK, Feldman NS, Loebl J. 1980. Developmental analysis of the role of social comparison in self-evaluation. Dev. Psychol. 16, 105–115. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.16.2.105) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gopnik A, O'Grady S, Lucas CG, Griffiths TL, Wente A, Bridgers S et al. . 2017. Changes in cognitive flexibility and hypothesis search across human life history from childhood to adolescence to adulthood. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 7892–7899. ( 10.1073/pnas.1700811114) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kanngiesser P, Rossano F, Tomasello M. 2015. Late emergence of the first possession heuristic: evidence from a small-scale culture. Child Dev. 86, 1282–1289. ( 10.1111/cdev.12365) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Zeidler H, Herrmann E, Haun DBM, Tomasello M. 2016. Taking turns or not? Children's approach to limited resource problems in three different cultures. Child Dev. 87, 677–688. ( 10.1111/cdev.12505) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Butler R. 1989. Mastery versus ability appraisal: a developmental study of children's observations of peers' work. Child Dev. 60, 1350–1361. ( 10.2307/1130926) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Rhodes M, Brickman D. 2008. Preschoolers’ responses to social comparisons involving relative failure. Psychol. Sci. 19, 968–972. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02184.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fehr E, Bernhard H, Rockenbach B. 2008. Egalitarianism in young children. Nature 454, 1079–1083. ( 10.1038/nature07155) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.House BR, Tomasello M. 2018. Modeling social norms increasingly influences costly sharing in middle childhood. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 171, 84–98. ( 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.12.014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.McAuliffe K, Blake PR, Steinbeis N, Warneken F. 2017. The developmental foundations of human fairness. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 42 ( 10.1038/s41562-016-0042) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Engelmann JM, Rapp DJ. 2018. The influence of reputational concerns on children's prosociality. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, 92–95. ( 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.024) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Pomerantz EM, Ruble DN, Frey KS, Greulich F. 1995. Meeting goals and confronting conflict: children's changing perceptions of social comparison. Child Dev. 66, 723–738. ( 10.2307/1131946) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Jensen K, Call J, Tomasello M. 2007. Chimpanzees are rational maximizers in an ultimatum game. Science 318, 107–109. ( 10.1126/science.1145850) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data associated with this manuscript can be accessed as part of the electronic supplementary material.