Skip to main content
Northern Clinics of Istanbul logoLink to Northern Clinics of Istanbul
. 2018 May 29;5(4):295–301. doi: 10.14744/nci.2017.42103

Effect of increase in cortisol level due to stress in healthy young individuals on dynamic and static balance scores

Mahmut Cay 1, Cihat Ucar 2, Deniz Senol 3,, Furkan Cevirgen 3, Davut Ozbag 3, Zuhal Altay 4, Sedat Yildiz 5
PMCID: PMC6371989  PMID: 30859159

Abstract

OBJECTIVE:

Stress is a condition caused by various factors and characterized by imbalance in body functioning, impair in nervous system, and tension. The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of cortisol level, which increases in healthy young individuals due to stress, on dynamic and static balance scores as well as to present the results caused by high levels of stress.

METHODS:

In this study, 107 healthy medicine faculty students in their second year (who will take the same committee exam) aged between 19 and 23 years were included. The first balance measurements and saliva samples were taken 40 days before the committee exam, and this period was acknowledged as the relaxed period. The same students were considered for balance measurements again on the day of committee exam; saliva samples were collected, and cortisol concentration was determined. This period was acknowledged as the stressful period. The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) was given to the participants in their relaxed and stressful periods. Dynamic balance scores were measured with Star Excursion Balance Test (SEBT). Static balance scores were measured with One Leg Standing Balance Test (OLSBT).

RESULTS:

The mean cortisol level was found to increase approximately 9 times in stressful periods compared with that in relaxed periods. STAI, which shows state anxiety, showed an increase supporting this increase. In stressful periods, dynamic balance scores showed obvious decrease in all directions. In addition, in stressful periods, an obvious decrease was observed in static balance scores compared with those in relaxed periods.

CONCLUSION:

This study showed that stress negatively affected dynamic and static balance, even for short periods of time. We believe that our study will form a positive source and basis when correlated with long terms stress and balance measurements.

Keywords: Cortisol, dynamic balance, static balance, stress


Stress is a situation caused by various factors and characterized by an imbalance in body functioning, impair in the nervous system and tension [1]. An individual who is stressed because of an unpleasant event responds to this situation with physiological and emotional changes as well as changes in perception and behavior. Even though people may think that they are not very much affected by events, they may develop reactions without realizing [2]. Stress most frequently occurs in uncontrollable, unwanted situations and when a person has more workload than he can handle [3]. A chemical or physical imbalance that occurs in cells or tissue fluid as a result of a change in the body or in the external environment is called physiological stress. There are 3 components of physiological stress. These are exogenous or endogenous stress factors and chemical or physical imbalance caused by stress factors and the adaptation response of the body to these conditions [4]. The results of the researchers examined the association between stress and immune system and monoaminergic system show that 2 stress sensitive endocrine response systems are hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and sympathetic adrenal medullary system [5]. When a person is faced with a stressor that he cannot control with existing cope mechanisms, HPA axis is activated through the association cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus, which causes the cortisol blood level to rise and brain functions to be affected through the neurons in the brain and glucocorticoid receptors in glial cells [6]. Cortisol is a steroid structured hormone released from the outer part of the cortex of suprarenal gland and exhibits glucocorticoid effect [7, 8]. Due to its small, fat-soluble structure (MW~362 Da), it passes from capillary vessels to cells primarily through passive diffusion [8, 9] and is distributed to body parts such as saliva, cerebrospinal fluid, sweat, hair, and urine [10]. Cortisol has circadian oscillation rhythm. While cortisol level in blood is high in the early hours of the morning, it drops to its lowest level at midnight [810]. While a major amount of the cortisol in blood is bound to corticosteroid-binding protein (CBG or transcortin) with high affinity, a small amount is carried as bound to albumin. Free cortisol is found in blood in a low rate (3–10% of the total cortisol) [8, 9]. Cortisol concentration in saliva has been reported to reflect the free cortisol in blood [811]. Salivary cortisol is used in psychoneuroendocrinological monitoring as an important parameter with reasons such as measuring the free cortisol level noninvasively, not causing pain, and allowing a person to get sample alone easily whenever wanted [12]. Maintaining balance and a stabilized posture is an indispensable part of a great number of movement practices [13]. Balance control is a complex motor skill that includes planning and applying flexible movement figures as well as the integration of sensory input [14]. The integration of the information coming from sensory system gives information to a person about his/her orientation to maintain posture control in space that allows for regulatory reflexive movements [15]. However, sensory inputs are not responsible alone for the maintenance of postural control. Postural stability is bound to complete neural pathways for the integrity of muscle mass and efficiency of the muscles in the central nervous system for motor control [16]. Environmental components on balance include somatosensorial, visual, and vestibular systems. The central nervous system combines the environmental inputs coming from these systems and chooses the responses of many suitable muscles in order to control body position and posture on support basis [17, 18]. The purpose of this study is to examine the effects of cortisol level, which increases in healthy young individuals as a result of stress, on dynamic and static balance scores and to present the factors that caused by high levels of stress.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

This study was conducted with the 2016/45 numbered permission of the Clinical Researches Ethical Board. In this study, 107 medicine faculty students in their second year aged between 19 and 23 years, who did not drink or smoke, did not have any orthopedic diseases or surgical intervention, did not participate in any sportive exercises, and did not used any psychiatric drugs during the period measurements were made were included in the study voluntarily. Consent forms were taken from all the students. The first balance measurements and saliva samples were taken 40 days before the committee exam and this period was acknowledged as the relaxed period. The same students were taken for balance measurements again on the day of committee exam and saliva samples were collected and cortisol concentration was determined. This period was acknowledged as the stressful period. STAI was given to students in their relaxed or stressful periods. STAI is a scale that is frequently used to assess STAI anxiety [19].

Analysis of cortisol in saliva

The passive droll method as described by Granger et al. [20] (2007) was used to collect saliva samples. The samples taken were kept at −20°C in a laboratory freezer. First, they were stored at thawing, then they were centrifuged for 10 min at 4000×g, and supernatants were analyzed with ELİSA. Assay buffer was used to dilute each sample 1:5 and all samples were assayed in triplicate. ELISA procedure; carbonate buffer, pH 9.6 was used to dilute cortisol-BSA stock solution (1 mg/mL) and at 200 µL/well it was added to a 96-well microtiter plate. The plate was incubated overnight at 4°C and using an 8-channel pipette, it was washed 5 times with wash buffer. The blocking buffer (200 µL/well) blocked some binding sites that were not occupied by the coating antigen 2 h at 37°C. After washing steps (5 times), standard solutions or samples (40 µL/well) and diluted 1st Ab (antiserum) (40 µL/well) were added in duplicate and incubated for 45 min at 37°C. Biotinylated anti-rabbit antibody was added (100 µL/well) after washing 5 times, and the plate was incubated for 30 min at 37°C. After the plate was washed 5 times, streptavidin peroxidase solution (100 µL/well) was added and the plate was incubated at 4°C for 15 min. Following this, the plate was washed again for 5 times, and then the substrate solution (150 µL/well) was added and incubated for 10 min in dark. Stop solution (50 µL/well) was added after incubation, and then using a microplate reader, the absorbance was measured at 450 nm. Intra-assay variation (CV) was found to be 5.6%, while inter-assay variation was found to be 7.8%.

Balance measurements

Dynamic balance scores were measured with SEBT. SEBT is a frequently used simple and reliable measurement method used in the assessment of dynamic postural control [2123]. SEBT has been reported to have enough sensitivity to find out balance deficits associated with musculoskeletal injuries such as ankle instability by previous studies [2426]. SEBT was used to assess dynamic balance. With the subjects standing in the middle of a grid constructed by 8 measure tapes extending out at 45° from each other, SEBT was performed. Along each of the 8 measure tapes, the subject had to reach as far as possible, touch the tape lightly, and then while maintaining a single-leg stance with the other leg in the center of the grid, he had to return the reaching leg back to the center. In order to complete the task, the subjects had to reach behind the stance leg while reaching in lateral and posterolateral directions. The subjects began with the anterior direction and continued clockwise around the grid. All subjects started in the center of the grid with a right stance leg. Following the completion of the 3 trials in the 8 directions, a 5-min rest was given, after this rest, the test continued with a left stance leg. With a mark on the tape, each of the reach distances was recorded as the distance from the center of the grid to point of maximum excursion by the reach leg [2730]. OLSBT was used to measure static balance. It was measured on stable platform with both eyes open for 60 s and closed for 30 s (EOSB; eyes open static balance, ECSB; eyes closed static balance), and the subjects were told to maintain their balance for maximum duration. When the stance foot shifted in any way or the non-stance foot touched the ground, the measurement was stopped [3032].

Statistical analysis

Shapiro–Wilk test was used to find out whether the data were normally distributed. They were found to be abnormally distributed. Wilcoxon paired 2 sample test was used for the analyzes of data. Correlations were calculated with Spearmen Rho coefficient. A level of p<0.05 was considered as statistically significant. IBM SPSS Statistics 22.0 for Windows package program was used for statistical analysis.

RESULTS

The average age of the 107 people who participated in the study was calculated as 20.5±1.36. Cortisol analyses in the saliva samples taken in both relaxed and stressful days and averages and±standard deviation of STAI were given in Table 1. Average cortisol level was found to increase approximately 9 times in stressful periods compared with that in relaxed periods. STAI, which shows state anxiety, showed an increase supporting this increase. In order to assess whether the difference between relaxed and stressful period cortisol and STAI scale increase was significant, Wilcoxon Paired 2 sample test was conducted on the data. According to analysis results, a statistically significant (p<0.05) increase was found in cortisol and STAI scores during relaxed and stressful periods (Table 1).

TABLE 1.

Relaxed and stressed period cortisol levels, STAI average±SD and results of Wilcoxon paired 2 sample test

Parameter Relaxed Stressed p
Cortisol 18.05±18.98 159.83±460.36 <0.001
STAI 38.79±3.31 42.71±4.23 <0.001

STAI: The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; SD: Standard deviation.

Table 2 gives the average±standard deviation of measurements conducted in relaxed and stressed periods in terms of right and left foot SEBT anterior (A), anteromedial (AM), medial (M), posteromedial (PM), posterior (P), posterolateral (PL), lateral (L), anterolateral (AL) directions.

TABLE 2.

Averages and±SD of relaxed and stressed period SEBT and Wilcoxon paired 2 sample test results

SEBT Right leg Left leg


Relaxed Stressed p Relaxed Stressed P
A 57.41±12.07 46.49±10.93 <0.001 57.85±13.94 45.29±11.17 <0.001
AM 57.74±13.67 45.00±11.56 <0.001 56.01±13.28 43.67±11.78 <0.001
M 46.55±15.43 33.75±12.38 <0.001 49.39±14.50 35.17±11.92 <0.001
PM 55.18±14.57 43.39±12.22 <0.001 55.00±15.47 43.60±12.39 <0.001
P 55.97±13.43 44.65±11.95 <0.001 55.72±15.19 44.54±11.83 <0.001
PL 55.50±13.50 46.40±11.53 <0.001 55.21±14.48 44.84±11.07 <0.001
L 56.31±14.22 47.11±11.87 <0.001 56.01±15.81 46.78±10.87 <0.001
AL 57.81±13.77 46.22±11.98 <0.001 57.62±14.06 45.28±11.62 <0.001

SD: Standard deviation; SEBT: Star Excursion Balance Test; A: Anterior; AM: Anteromedial; M: Medial; PM: Posteromedial; P: Posterior; PL: Posterolateral; L: Lateral; AL: Anterolateral.

When SEBT was examined, it was found that for the right foot, dynamic balance scores showed 19% anterior, 22% anteromedial, 28% medial, 21% posteromedial, 20% posterior, 16% posterolateral, 12% lateral, and 20% anterolateral decreases in stressful periods compared with those in relaxed periods. For the left foot, dynamic balance scores showed 22% anterior, 22% anteromedial, 29% medial, 21% posteromedial, 20% posterior, 19% posterolateral, 17% lateral, and 21% anterolateral decreases in stressed periods compared with those in relaxed periods. According to Wilcoxon paired 2 sample test results, dynamic balance scores measured by SEBT were found to be statistically significant for the right and left feet between relaxed and stressed periods (p<0.05). When Table 2 was examined, it was found that stress had a negative effect on individuals’ dynamic balance scores. In stressed periods, dynamic balance scores showed significant decreases in all directions.

Table 3 gives the results of OLSBT and Wilcoxon paired 2 sample test results conducted showed that opening and closing of the eyes in relaxed and stressed periods for right and left foot. Eyes open stressed period right foot static balance scores showed 21% decrease when compared with relaxed period, while left foot static balance scores showed 26% decrease. Eyes closed stressed period right foot static balance scores showed 50% decrease when compared with relaxed period, while left foot static balance scores showed 47% decrease. According to Wilcoxon paired 2 sample test results, static balance scores measured by OLSBT were found to be statistically significant for the right and left feet between relaxed and stressed periods (p<0.05). When Table 3 was examined, it was found that exam stress had a negative effect on students’ static balance scores. When static balance scores in stressed periods were compared with those in relaxed periods, an obvious decrease was found.

TABLE 3.

Average and±SD of static balance scores measured by OLSBT

OLSBT Right leg Left leg


Relaxed Stressed p Relaxed Stressed p
EOSB 54.14±10.89 42.67±14.66 <0.001 53.94±9.80 39.75±14.12 <0.001
ECSB 18.37±7.69 9.20±5.42 <0.001 17.72±8.21 9.43±6.11 <0.001

SD: Standard deviation; OLSBT: One Leg Standing Balance Test; EOSB: Eyes open static balance; ECSB: Eyes closed static balance.

Table 4 gives the dynamic balance scores measured by SEBT and Spearman Rho correlation analysis results connected on static balance scores measured by OLSBT. According to the results of the analysis, cortisol increase, which increased with stress, was found to correlate negatively with balance tests. It was found that cortisol increase due to stress affected balance negatively. Correlation analysis conducted with STAI state anxiety questionnaire was found to support this result.

TABLE 4.

Spearman Rho correlation analysis results (dynamic balance scores - static balance scores)

Test Right leg Left leg


Cortisol STAI Cortisol STAI
A r -0.366 0.222 -0.304 0.245
p <0.001 0.001 <0.001 <0.001
AM r -0.373 213 -0.308 213
p <0.001 0.234 <0.001 0.253
M r -0.299 0.001 -0.295 0.000
p <0.001 213 <0.001 213
PM r -0.355 0.251 -0.373 0.251
Dynamic balance p <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001
P r -0.400 213 -0.304 213
p <0.001 0.255 <0.001 0.286
PL r -0.364 0.000 -0.308 0.000
p <0.001 213 <0.001 213
L r -0.252 0.256 -0.269 0.225
p <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 0.001
AL r -0.286 213 -0.352 213
p <0.001 0.213 <0.001 0.252
EO r -0.186 0.328 -0.322 0.365
Static balance p 0.006 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001
EC r -0.107 0.336 -0.173 0.343
p 0.117 <0.001 0.011 <0.001

STAI: The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; A: Anterior; AM: Anteromedial; M: Medial; PM: Posteromedial; P: Posterior; PL: Posterolateral; L: Lateral; AL: Anterolateral.

DISCUSSION

Human body tries to adjust to stressful events with various responses. Individuals who face the effect of a stress stimulus, respond to this by taking a physiological or psychological defense [33]. Stimulating messages from internal and external environments change a person’s balance and the organism tries to rebalance and maintain adjustment. If a person’s efforts are insufficient, his/her adjustment will be disrupted. The factors that disrupt adjustment put the organism in a difficult situation [34]. As long as a person adjusts to changes, he/she can keep balance, order, happiness, and health and tries consciously or unconsciously to maintain this balance and order. Sometimes he/she will be successful in these efforts, keeps his/her old balance and order and maintain adjustment [35]. Maintaining balance and a stable posture is an indispensable part of many movement practices [13]. Balance control is a motor skill that includes planning and practicing flexible movements as well as integrating sensory inputs [14]. The integration of the information coming from perceptive systems gives information to a person about orientation to maintain posture control in space that allows regulatory reflexive movements [15]. However, sensory inputs alone are not responsible for maintaining postural control. Postural stability depends on the integrity of muscle mass, the efficiency of the systems in the central nervous system, and the complete neural pathways for motor control [16]. As a response to stress, the endocrine system begins to release cortisol, and a sudden increase in cortisol levels is an adaptive function [36]. In our study, we compared the cortisol concentration of individuals in their relaxed and stressed periods. In their study conducted with 35 medicine faculty students, Singh et al. [37] (2012) compared cortisol levels in relaxed and stressed periods and made measurements on exam stress. They found cortisol levels as 2.48±1.5 ng/ml in male students and 2.92±1.9 ng/ml in female students during relaxed periods, while the levels were 5.02±3.1 ng/ml in male students and 5.19±3.1 ng/ml in female students during stressed periods. In a previous study, Schoofs et al. [38] (2008) found that academic exams caused very high increases in salivary cortisol levels. In our study, cortisol levels in stressed period were found to increase about 9 times compared with relaxed periods. This result was expected and our values were in line with the literature.

Some of the physical responses to stress are: shaking, muscle spasm, myotonia, cramps, numbness in fingers and toes [3941]. Frequent physical symptoms and complaints during stress are pain in neck, nape, waist and back, spasm and arthralgia, imbalance and swaying while standing, sitting and walking [42]. In recent study with cyclists, Filho et al. [43] (2015) stated that stress factors differed in short-term period and these changes affected performance. Bali [44] (2015) stated that sportive performance was not just a biomechanical and physiological event, psychological factors also affected performance significantly and in addition more than optimum stress affected performance negatively. In previous study on young (22.3±3.6 years) and old (82.3±9.6 years) individuals, Sarabon and Rosker (2015) found dynamic balance scores they measured with SEBT as 76.2±6.78 cm in young individuals and 37.3±7.26 cm in old individuals in anterior direction; as 65.1±8.63 cm in young individuals, 30.3±7.36 cm in old individuals in lateral direction, as 77.9±11.0 cm in young individuals, and as 36.1±8.05 cm in old individuals in posterior direction [45]. The results of this study are in line with that by Sarabon and Rosker (2015) on young individuals. In literature, we could not find any studies similar to our study that is on static and dynamic balance test in stressed and relaxed periods. The results of our study showed that static and dynamic balances of individuals were worse in stressed period when compared with relaxed period. The results of static and dynamic balance test showed unstableness and swaying in postural balances in stressed periods of individuals and also it was seen that they could not stand for long periods of time. When SEBT scores were analyzed, 12% and 28% decrease was found in all directions in dynamic balance scores of stressed period for right foot when compared with relaxed period. For left foot, a decrease between 17% and 29% was found in SEBT scores of stressed period when compared with relaxed period. In addition, significant differences were found in OLSBT results we conducted eyes open and closed in relaxed and stressed periods. Static balance scores measured with OLSBT were found to have lower values in stressed period when compared with relaxed period. As a conclusion; dynamic and static balance scores between stressed period and relaxed period in healthy individuals seem to be a response for stress. It is not known whether cortisol increase in stressed period has an effect on balance center. Cortisol increase and balance disruption were not in the same rates in this period. Thus, it should be researched whether cortisol increase has a positive effect on balance centers. Does the disruption of balance trigger cortisol increase or does cortisol increase contribute to the disruption of balance? In addition, it has been reported in literature that extreme cortisol released in healthy individuals due to long-term stress can cause negative effects such as increase in blood pressure, atherosclerosis, diabetes, immune suppression, osteolysis, and myolysis [36]. As a result of the levels short-term stress affected static and dynamic balance in our study, we predict that long-term stress may cause permanent disruption of postural balance in young individuals. Further studies will present more obviously how long-term stress can affect postural balance in healthy individuals. We believe that our study will form a positive source and basis to new studies and contribute to literature.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Financial Disclosure: The authors declared that this study has received no financial support.

Authorship contributions: Concept – M.C., D.S.; Design – D.O., Z.A., S.Y.; Supervision – M.C.; Materials – C.U., F.C.; Data collection &/or processing – M.C., D.S.; Analysis and/or interpretation – D.S., S.Y.; Writing – M.C., D.S., C.U.; Critical review – D.O., Z.A., S.Y.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Kocatürk U. AçıklamalıTıp Terimleri Sözlüğü. 6th ed. Ankara: Ankara Üniversitesi Basımevi; 1994. p. 730. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ostell A. Coping, problem solving and stress: a framework for intervention strategies. Br J Med Psychol. 1991;64:11–24. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1991.tb01638.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Irwin M, Daniels M, Smith TL, Bloom E, Weiner H. Impaired natural killer cell activity during bereavement. Brain Behav Immun. 1987;1:98–104. doi: 10.1016/0889-1591(87)90011-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Selye H. The general adaptation syndrome and the diseases of adaptation. J Clin Med. 1946;6:117–230. doi: 10.1210/jcem-6-2-117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Miller GE. Psychological stress and disease. JAMA. 2007;298:1685–7. doi: 10.1001/jama.298.14.1685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Huether G, Doering S, Rüger U, Rüther E, Schüssler G. The stress-reaction process and the adaptive modification and reorganization of neuronal networks. Psychiatry Res. 1999;87:83–95. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(99)00044-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Salivary Cortisol. Encyclopedia of Stress. 2000;3:379–83. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gatti R, Antonelli G, Prearo M, Spinella P, Cappellin E, De Palo EF. Cortisol assays and diagnostic laboratory procedures in human biological fluids. Clinical Biochem. 2009;42:1205–17. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiochem.2009.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Levine A, Zagoory-Sharon O, Feldman R, Lewis JG, Weller A. Measuring cortisol in human psychobiological studies. Physiol Behav. 2007;90:43–53. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.08.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kaushik A, Vasudev A, Arya SK, Pasha SK, Bhansali S. Recent advances in cortisol sensing technologies for point-of-care application. Biosens Bioelectron. 2014;53:499–512. doi: 10.1016/j.bios.2013.09.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kudielka BM, Hellhammer DH, Wüst S. Why do we respond so differently? Reviewing determinants of human salivary cortisol responses to challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:2–18. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hellhammer DH, Wüst S, Kudielka BM. Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:163–71. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Carr J, Shepherd R. Neurological Rehabilitation: Optimising Motor Performance. Oxford: Butterworth Heinemann; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ferdjallah M, Harris GF, Smith P, Wertsch JJ. Analysis of Postural Control Synergies During Quiet Standing in Healthy Children and Children with Cerebral Palsy. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon) 2002;17:203–10. doi: 10.1016/s0268-0033(01)00121-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cobb SV. Measurement of postural stability before and after immersion in a virtual environment. Appl Ergon. 1999;30:47–57. doi: 10.1016/s0003-6870(98)00038-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Horak FB, Shupert CL, Mirka A. Components of Postural Dyscontrol in The Elderly: A Review. Neurobiology of Aging. 1989;10:727–38. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(89)90010-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Nashner LM, Black FO, Wall C., 3rd Adaptation to Altered Support and Visual Conditions During Stance: Patients with Vestibular Deficits. J Neurosci. 1982;2:536–44. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-05-00536.1982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Shumway-Cook A, Horak FB. Assessing the influence of sensory interaction of balance. Suggestion from the field. Phys Ther. 1986;66:1548–50. doi: 10.1093/ptj/66.10.1548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Fernández-Blázquez MA, Ávila-Villanueva M, López-Pina JA, Zea-Sevilla MA, Frades-Payo B. Psychometric properties of a new short version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) for the assessment of anxiety in the elderly. Neurologia. 2015;30:352–8. doi: 10.1016/j.nrl.2013.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Granger DA, Kivlighan KT, el-Sheikh M, Gordis EB, Stroud LR. Salivary alpha-amylase in biobehavioral research: recent developments and applications. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1098:122–44. doi: 10.1196/annals.1384.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Munro AG, Herrington LC. Between-session reliability of the Star Excursion Balance Test. Phys Ther Sport. 2010;11:128–32. doi: 10.1016/j.ptsp.2010.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Clark RC, Saxion CE, Cameron KL, Gerber JP. Associations between three clinical assessment tools for postural stability. N Am J Sports Phys Ther. 2010;5:122–130. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kinzey SJ, Armstrong CW. The reliability of the star-excursion test in assessing dynamic balance. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 1998;27:356–60. doi: 10.2519/jospt.1998.27.5.356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Olmsted LC, Carcia CR, Hertel J, Shultz SJ. Efficacy of the Star Excursion Balance Tests in detecting reach deficits in subjects with chronic ankle instability. J Athl Train. 2002;37:501–6. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gribble PA, Hertel J, Denegar CR. Chronic ankle instability and fatigue create proximal joint alterations during performance of the Star Excursion Balance Test. Int J Sports Med. 2007;28:236–42. doi: 10.1055/s-2006-924289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gribble PA, Hertel J, Denegar CR, Buckley WE. The effects of fatigue and chronic ankle instability on dynamic postural control. J Athl Train. 2004;39:321–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Dabholkar A, Shah A, Yardi S. Comparison of dynamic balance between flat feet and normal individuals using star excursion balance test. Indian Journal of Physiotherapy and Occupational Therapy. 2012;6:33–7. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gribble PA, Hertel J. Considerations for Normalizing Measures of the Star Excursion Balance Test. Meas Phys Educ Exerc Sci. 2003;7:89–100. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hadadi M, Mousavi ME, Fardipour S, Vameghi R, Mazaheri M. Effect of soft and semirigid ankle orthoses on Star Excursion Balance Test performance in patients with functional ankle instability. J Sci Med Sport. 2014;17:430–3. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2013.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Muammer R, Calisgan S, Senol D. The effect of yoga training on balance and proprioception of ankle. Intl J Sport Std. 2015;5:1079–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gulbandilar E, Cimbiz A, Sari M, Ozden H. Relationship between skin resistance level and static balance in type II diabetic subjects. Diabetes Res Clin Pract. 2008;82:335–9. doi: 10.1016/j.diabres.2008.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Cımbız A, Gulbandılar E, Bayazıt V, Ozay Y, Dayıoglu H. Relationship between skin resistance level and one leg standing balance in healthy subjects. J Med Sci. 2006;6:286–91. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Sanlı T. In: Hemşirelikte Kişilerarasıİlişkiler. Geylan R, editor. Eskişehir: Açıkögretim Fakültesi Yayınları; 1993. pp. 48–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Morgan G. In: İşte ve Yasamda Stres'i Yenmenin Yolları. Çağla Ş, editor. Istanbul: Ruh Bilim Yayınları; 1996. p. 13. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Morgan G. In: İşte ve Yasamda Stres'i Yenmenin Yolları. Çağla Ş, editor. Istanbul: Ruh Bilim Yayınları; 1996. p. 16. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hansen-Grant SM, Pariante CM, Kalin NH, Miller AH. Neuroendocrine and immune system pathology in psychiatric disease. In: Schatzberg AF, Nemeroff CB, editors. Textbook of Psychopharmacology. 2nd ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1998. pp. 171–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Singh R, Goyal M, Tiwari S, Ghildiyal A, Nattu SM, Das S. Effect of Examination Stress on Mood, Performance And Cortisol Levels in Medical Students. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 2012;56:48–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Schoofs D, Hartmann R, Wolf OT. Neuroendocrine stress responses to an oral academic examination: no strong influence of sex, repeated participation and personality traits. Stress. 2008;11:52–61. doi: 10.1080/10253890701453943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Güçlü N. Stress Management [Article in Turkish] G.Ü. Gazi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi. 2001;21:91–109. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Aytaç S, Bayram N. Marmara Depremi SonrasıBireylerdeki Stres Tepkilerinin Analizi. DEÜSBE Dergisi. 2000;2:42–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Arsenault A, Dolan S. The role of personality, occupation and organization in understanding therelationship between job stress, performance and absenteeism. Journal of Occupational Psychology. 1983;56:227–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Morgan G. In: İşte ve Yasamda Stres'i Yenmenin Yolları. Çağla Ş, editor. İstanbul: Ruh Bilim Yayınları; 1996. p. 103. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Filho E, di Fronso S, Forzini F, Murgia M, Agostini T, Bortoli L, et al. Athletic performance and recovery-stress factors in cycling: An ever changing balance. Eur J Sport Sci. 2015;15:671–80. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2015.1048746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bali A. Psychological Factors Affecting Sports Performance. IJPESH. 2015;1:92–5. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sarabon N, Rosker J. Ability of different balance tests to discriminate between young and elderly subjects. Measurement. 2015;68:42–8. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Northern Clinics of Istanbul are provided here courtesy of Istanbul Northern Anatolian Association of Public Hospitals

RESOURCES