Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 Feb 12;8:e40541. doi: 10.7554/eLife.40541

Offline impact of transcranial focused ultrasound on cortical activation in primates

Lennart Verhagen 1,2,†,, Cécile Gallea 3,, Davide Folloni 1,2, Charlotte Constans 4, Daria EA Jensen 1,2, Harry Ahnine 3, Léa Roumazeilles 1,2, Mathieu Santin 3, Bashir Ahmed 5, Stéphane Lehericy 3, Miriam C Klein-Flügge 1,2, Kristine Krug 5, Rogier B Mars 2,6, Matthew FS Rushworth 1,2,, Pierre Pouget 7,, Jean-François Aubry 8,, Jerome Sallet 1,2,
Editors: Joshua I Gold9, Sven Bestmann10
PMCID: PMC6372282  PMID: 30747105

Abstract

To understand brain circuits it is necessary both to record and manipulate their activity. Transcranial ultrasound stimulation (TUS) is a promising non-invasive brain stimulation technique. To date, investigations report short-lived neuromodulatory effects, but to deliver on its full potential for research and therapy, ultrasound protocols are required that induce longer-lasting ‘offline’ changes. Here, we present a TUS protocol that modulates brain activation in macaques for more than one hour after 40 s of stimulation, while circumventing auditory confounds. Normally activity in brain areas reflects activity in interconnected regions but TUS caused stimulated areas to interact more selectively with the rest of the brain. In a within-subject design, we observe regionally specific TUS effects for two medial frontal brain regions – supplementary motor area and frontal polar cortex. Independently of these site-specific effects, TUS also induced signal changes in the meningeal compartment. TUS effects were temporary and not associated with microstructural changes.

Research organism: Rhesus macaque

eLife digest

Ultrasound is well known for making visible what is hidden, for example, when giving parents a glimpse of their child before birth. But researchers are now using these high-frequency sound waves – beyond the range of human hearing – for a wholly different purpose: to manipulate the activity of the brain. Conventional brain stimulation techniques use electric currents or magnetic fields to alter brain activity. These techniques, however, have limitations. They can only reach the surface of the brain and are not particularly precise. By contrast, beams of ultrasound can be focused at a millimetre scale, even deep within the brain. Ultrasound thus has the potential to provide new insights into how the brain works.

Most studies of ultrasound stimulation have looked at what happens to the brain during the stimulation itself. But could ultrasound also induce longer-lasting changes in brain activity? Changes that persist after the stimulation has ended would be valuable for research. They would also make it more likely that we could use ultrasound to treat brain disorders by changing brain activity.

Verhagen, Gallea et al. used a brain scanner to measure brain activity in macaque monkeys after ultrasound stimulation. The results showed that 40 seconds of repetitive ultrasound changed brain activity for up to two hours. Ultrasound caused the stimulated brain area to interact more selectively with the rest of the brain. Notably, only the stimulated area changed its activity in this way. This helps rule out the possibility that the changes reflect non-specific effects. If the monkeys had been able to hear the ultrasound, for example, it would have changed the activity of the parts of the brain related to hearing. Most important of all, the changes were reversible and did not harm the brain.

The results of Verhagen, Gallea et al. show that repetitive ultrasound can induce long-lasting alterations in brain activity. It can target areas deep within the brain, including those that are out of reach with other techniques. If this procedure also shows longer-lasting effects in people, it could yield valuable insights into the links between brain and behaviour. It could also help us develop new treatments for neurological and psychiatric disorders.

Introduction

In neuroscience, to understand brain circuits a two-pronged approach, entailing both recording and manipulating brain activity, is essential. In recent years, there has been extensive progress in this field, which was in part made possible by the availability of new technologies (Bestmann and Walsh, 2017; Dayan et al., 2013; Polanía et al., 2018). While techniques for transiently manipulating activity in rodents, such as microstimulation, optogenetics, and chemogenetics (Sternson and Roth, 2014; Vanduffel, 2016), are increasingly accessible and applied, techniques for manipulating activity in the primate brain are less widely available and remain accessible to comparatively few researchers in a limited number of research centres worldwide (Galvan et al., 2018; Krug et al., 2015; Vanduffel, 2016). A prominent limitation for many brain stimulation tools for research and therapeutic interventions is the duration of the induced neuromodulatory effects, often not outlasting the stimulation by more than a few seconds or minutes.

Here we report on a particular protocol of low intensity pulsed transcranial focused ultrasound stimulation (TUS) that we show induces a sustained period of neuromodulation in primates without inducing structural damage. The TUS approach in general is a relatively new one (Dallapiazza et al., 2018; Tufail et al., 2011; Tyler et al., 2018; Yoo et al., 2011; Younan et al., 2013) and like transcranial magnetic stimulation (Dayan et al., 2013) and transcranial electric stimulation (Polanía et al., 2018) it can be applied in the absence of a craniotomy. In vitro recordings have identified several mechanisms by which ultrasound stimulation could affect neurons. It has been proposed that the sound pressure wave exerts a mechanical effect on neuronal activity through ion channel gating and changes to the membrane capacitance (Blackmore et al., 2018; Kubanek et al., 2018; Kubanek et al., 2016; Prieto et al., 2013). While the precise mechanism is being determined, early applications, including the current results, suggest TUS may be suitable as a tool for focal manipulation of activity in many brain areas in primates (Fomenko et al., 2018; Lee et al., 2016a; Legon et al., 2018; Munoz et al., 2018; Naor et al., 2016; Tufail et al., 2011; Tyler et al., 2018; Yoo et al., 2011). In the macaque, its application over the frontal eye field (FEF) affects the same aspects of oculomotor behaviour that are compromised by FEF lesion, whilst leaving intact those aspects of oculomotor behaviour that are unaffected by FEF lesion (Deffieux et al., 2013). Pioneering work in humans has focused on modulating and eliciting evoked responses by focused ultrasound in both cortical and subcortical sensorimotor regions (Lee et al., 2016a; Lee et al., 2015; Legon et al., 2018; Legon et al., 2014). To date ultrasonic applications are primarily focused on direct ‘online’ effects. Nonetheless, some studies have observed neuromodulatory effects outlasting the sonication by several minutes before returning to baseline, with more pronounced effects for protocols delivered at higher intensities (Isppa >5 W/cm2) and higher duty cycles (>5%; Kim et al., 2015; Yoo et al., 2011). For example, following ultrasound protocols with a relatively high rate of acoustic energy deposition, characterized by pulses tens of milliseconds long, repeated at 10 Hz, the neuromodulatory effects outlast the sonication period: for up to 10 min following 43.7 ms long 1.14MHz pulses repeated at 10 Hz for 40 s (Dallapiazza et al., 2018), or for up to 20 min following 30 ms long 350 kHz pulses repeated at 10 Hz for 40 s (Ahnine et al., 2018). We have built on such protocols when designing the current experiment.

Recent work has highlighted the possibility that in rodents some TUS protocols can evoke a startle response when the stimulation is modulated at audible frequencies (Guo et al., 2018; Sato et al., 2018). Importantly, this auditory effect is limited to the stimulation period and dissipates within 75 ms to 4 s. This work has also emphasized that in more deeply anaesthetized animals, when the intrinsic neural activity of a system is suppressed, some TUS protocols might fail to evoke action potentials at the site of stimulation. This suggests TUS’ actions might be primarily neuromodulatory in nature and/or that they are most prominently observable when they interact with ongoing physiological activity. However, as has been noted (Airan and Butts Pauly, 2018) the impact that any stimulation protocol might have, will be a function of the animal model being used and the precise details of the ultrasound frequencies, pulse shape, protocol, and ongoing brain activity. With the offline protocol and anaesthesia regime we used we control for such potential artefacts and show that TUS has an effect that cannot be attributed to them.

Here we focused on the effects of TUS outlasting the stimulation period, investigating the impact of 40 s trains of TUS on measurements of neural activity in three macaque monkeys provided by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) up to 2 hr after stimulation (Figure 1, top panel). FMRI is one of the most widely used methods for estimating neural activity. Despite limitations in its spatial and temporal resolution, fMRI remains important because it is non-invasive and can often be used to provide information about activity throughout the whole brain. Rather than providing a direct measure of neural activity, however, it provides an estimate of how activity changes in tandem with sensory, cognitive, or motor events or with activity in another brain region. Typically, fMRI-measured activity in any given brain area is a function of activity in other brain areas, especially those with which it is closely interconnected (Neubert et al., 2015; O'Reilly et al., 2013). Although many brain areas may share any given individual connection (for example both areas A and B may project to C), the overall pattern of connections of each area is unique (Passingham et al., 2002); as such, the overall pattern of connections therefore constitutes a ‘connectional fingerprint’. As a result it is possible to use fMRI measurements of correlations in the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal across brain regions to estimate the connectivity fingerprints of a given brain area (Figure 1, bottom right panel; Caspari et al., 2018; Ghahremani et al., 2017; Margulies et al., 2016, Margulies et al., 2009; Mars et al., 2013; Sallet et al., 2013; Shen, 2015; Shen et al., 2015). This implies that activity in any given brain area is a function of the activity in the areas with which it is interconnected.

Figure 1. Overview of the experimental design, time-line, pre-processing pipeline, and analysis strategy.

Figure 1.

Top panel: The same three macaque monkeys (MK1, MK2, MK3) participated in experiments 1 and 2, and a control experiment conducted in the absence of TUS. Three other monkeys (MK4, MK5, MK6) participated in experiment 3. In all experiments anaesthesia was induced ~1.45 hr before the TUS intervention or no-TUS control. In experiment 1 the 40 s TUS protocol was delivered over the supplementary motor area (SMA), while in experiments 2 and 3, the protocol was targeted at frontal polar cortex (FPC). In all sessions, three consecutive runs of resting fMRI (26 min per run) were acquired starting ~40 min after the TUS or control protocol. Bottom left panel: Data from the resting fMRI runs were pre-processed following a standardized pipeline to address artefacts, improve image quality and signal-to-noise ratio, and prepare for connectivity analyses. By default, non-neuronal confounds were removed from the timeseries, but to allow an analysis of the effect of TUS on non-neuronal signal the data was also processed in parallel in a second pipeline, distinct from the default by omitting the non-neuronal confound regression procedure. Bottom right panel: The effect of TUS on the coupling patterns of the stimulated regions were quantified as ‘connectional fingerprints’ employing pre-defined targets (here illustrated for the SMA in the control state). These fingerprint analyses allowed us to test for the presence and duration of TUS effects in the stimulated areas (SMA after SMA TUS; FPC after FPC TUS), and in control areas, including non-stimulated areas (FPC after SMA TUS; SMA after FPC TUS) and the auditory cortex. Whole brain connectivity maps supported exploratory analyses and an assessment of the impact of TUS on non-neuronal signal (here illustrated for SMA in the control state).

We exploited this feature of activity to examine the impact of TUS application to two brain regions in the frontal cortex: supplementary motor area (SMA; experiment 1) and frontal polar cortex (FPC; experiments 2 and 3). Simulations showed we were able to selectively target these regions (Figure 2; see Acoustic and thermal modelling for more details). These regions have distinct anatomical and functional connections; SMA is most strongly coupled with the sensorimotor system, while the FPC interacts primarily with the prefrontal cortex and only interacts indirectly with the sensorimotor system via SMA (Petrides and Pandya, 2007; Sallet et al., 2013). This allows us to test the spatial and connectional specificity of TUS effects. In the control state, each area’s activity is normally a function of the activity in the areas that constitute its connectional fingerprint. If this pattern is altered by TUS in a manner that is dependent on the location of the stimulation, then this will constitute evidence that TUS exerts a spatially selective effect on neural activity. In a within-subject design, the same three animals participated in experiments 1 and 2, and a control experiment conducted in the absence of TUS (Figure 1). As such, fMRI was acquired in three conditions for all animals: following SMA TUS, following FPC TUS, and in a control state. In turn, each of the MRI sessions consisted of three consecutive runs. We also validated the results of experiment two in a new set of three different animals (experiment 3).

Figure 2. Stimulation targets and thermal modelling.

Figure 2.

(a) Stimulation target positions in SMA are shown for each of the three individual animals in three different colours on sagittal and coronal views. (b) Estimates of the focused ultrasound peak intensities and spatial distribution when targeting SMA are derived from numerical simulations using a high-resolution macaque whole-head CT scan, here displayed on a midline sagittal section. (c) FPC targets in three animals are shown on sagittal and coronal sections. (d) Estimated peak intensities and spatial distribution when targeting FPC shown on a sagittal section. (e) Whole-head simulations of the acoustic wave and thermal dynamics provided estimates of the maximum pressure amplitude (left panel) and the temperature after 40 s TUS (right panel). The data depict a cropped plane of the whole-head simulations with the sonic coupling cone at the top and the brain at the bottom; the skull is outlined in black. Pressure and temperature are maximal in the skull, which is more absorbing than soft tissue. (f) Temperature dynamics for the maximum temperature in the skull (blue), maximum temperature in the brain (red) and at the geometrical focal point in the cortex (yellow).

Ultrasound modulation of SMA and FPC led to focal, area-specific changes in each stimulated region’s connectivity profile. In each area, a region’s activity pattern after TUS application was more a function of its own activity and that of strongly connected regions, but less a function of activity in more remote and weakly interconnected areas. Independent of these specific grey matter signal changes, TUS also interacted with non-neuronal structures, as evidenced by more widespread signal changes observed in the meningeal compartment (including cerebral spinal fluid and vasculature). Finally, in experiment four we demonstrate that TUS application had no observable impact on cortical microstructure apparent on histological examination. In summary, TUS over dorsomedial frontal cortex causes spatially specific sharpening of the stimulated region’s connectivity profile with high efficacy and reproducibility, independent of non-neuronal signal changes.

Results

Experiment 1, TUS modulation of SMA connectivity

Transcranial focused ultrasound stimulation of SMA induced spatially specific changes to the connectivity profile of SMA. At rest, in the control state, SMA’s activity is coupled with activity throughout sensorimotor regions in frontal and parietal cortex, inferior parietal, prefrontal, and parts of cingulate cortex (Figure 3a). Many of these regions are anatomically connected with SMA (Dum and Strick, 2005; Geyer et al., 2000; Strick et al., 1998). If we change the responsiveness of SMA neurons to such activity in interconnected regions by artificially modulating SMA activity with TUS, then we should see a change in the coupling between SMA activity and activity in other regions. Following ultrasound stimulation, SMA changed its coupling with the sensorimotor system, anterior and posterior cingulate, anterior temporal, inferior parietal, and prefrontal cortex (Figure 3b). This can be seen on the whole brain functional connectivity maps for the SMA region (Figure 3, compare panels a and b, representative changes highlighted by dashed black circles) and on the whole brain differential connectivity maps in Figure 4 (panels b and c).

Figure 3. Coupling of activity between stimulated areas and the rest of the brain in experiments 1 (SMA) and 2 (FPC).

The left panels show activity coupling between SMA and the rest of the brain in the control state (a), after SMA TUS (b), and after FPC TUS (c). The right panels show activity coupling between FPC and the rest of the brain in the control state (d), after SMA TUS (e), and after FPC TUS (f). Functional connectivity from TUS-targeted regions is therefore summarized in panels (b) and (f) (i.e. SMA connectivity after SMA TUS and FPC connectivity after FPC TUS). Each type of TUS had a relatively selective effect on the stimulated area: SMA coupling was changed by SMA TUS (b) and FPC coupling was changed by FPC TUS (f). Positive correlations are represented in warm colours from red to yellow, negative correlations are represented in cool colours from blue to green. Key regions of change are highlighted by black dashed ovals. TUS target sites are indicated with arrows. Connectivity seed regions are indicated with black asterisks. Key anatomical features are labelled in panel (a): pos, parieto-occipital sulcus; cal, calcarine sulcus; cgs, cingulate sulcus; ps, principal sulcus; as, arcuate sulcus; cs, central sulcus; ips, intraparietal sulcus; sts, superior temporal sulcus; ls, lunate sulcus.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Specific patterns of change in the coupling of activity between stimulated areas and the rest of the brain were replicated in experiments 2 and 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Panels on the left show activity coupling between SMA and the rest of the brain in the control state (a), after FPC TUS in experiment 2 (b), and after FPC TUS in experiment 3 (in three new animals) (c). Panels on the right show activity coupling between FPC and the rest of the brain in the control state (d), after FPC TUS in experiment 2 (e), and after FPC TUS in experiment 3 (f). SMA’s connectional fingerprint was largely unaltered after FPC TUS in both experiments 2 and 3 (g) but FPC’s connectivity fingerprint was altered in a similar manner in both experiments 2 and 3 (h). Panels (a, b) and (d, e) are reproduced here from Figure 3 for comparison. All conventions as in Figure 3. Lighter coloured error bands indicate the standard-error of the mean across individual animals.

Figure 4. Differential effect of TUS on coupling of activity between stimulated areas and the rest of the brain in experiments 1 (SMA) and 2 (FPC).

Figure 4.

The left panels show activity coupling between SMA and the rest of the brain in the control state (a), the differential effect of SMA TUS for areas positively coupled (z > 0.1) with SMA in the control state (b), and for areas negatively coupled (z < −0.1) with SMA in the control state (c). The right panels show activity coupling between FPC and the rest of the brain in the control state (d), the differential effect of FPC TUS for areas positively coupled (z > 0.1) with FPC in the control state (e), and for areas negatively coupled (z < −0.1) with FPC in the control state (f). Panels (a) and (b) are reproduced here from Figure 3 for reference and to illustrate the location and extent of the ROIs used in the fingerprint analyses (Figure 5). Hot colours in (b) and (e) indicate enhanced coupling following TUS compared to the control state, while cool colours indicate reduced coupling. In (c) and (f) hot colours indicate reduced negative coupling, while cool colours indicate further negative coupling. All other conventions as in Figure 3. The TUS induced changes to the coupling of the stimulated regions were not limited to the a-priori defined ROIs but extended across many regions according to the connectional topography of the stimulated region.

It is also apparent in the illustration of SMA’s connectional fingerprint (Figure 5a). The distance of each coloured line from the centre of the figure (and hence its proximity to the circumference of the figure) indicates the strength of activity coupling between SMA and each of the other brain areas indicated on the circumference. Compared to the control state (blue line), after TUS over SMA (red line), SMA’s positive coupling is enhanced with proximal areas in the sensorimotor system but reduced in many long-range connections (non-parametric permutation test, p=0.017). The primary motor cortex (M1), superior parietal lobe (SPL), and middle cingulate cortex (MCC) in the dorsomedial sensorimotor network have been reported to be closely connected with the SMA, whereas prefrontal regions on the dorsomedial (area 9m and FPC), dorsolateral (areas 9-46d and 8A), and ventromedial (area 11m) surface, and those in the temporal lobe (anterior superior temporal gyrus, aSTG; middle superior temporal sulcus, midSTS), and parietal cortex (caudal inferior parietal lobule, IPLc; posterior parietal cortex, PCC) have been reported to be less closely connected with the SMA (Dum and Strick, 2005; Geyer et al., 2000; Strick et al., 1998). TUS increased positive coupling between the stimulated area and proximal areas normally closely connected with it, while, at the same time, decreasing coupling between the stimulated area and many areas normally less closely connected with it. This pattern not only emerges from the fingerprint analyses, but constitutes a principle evident across the brain, as illustrated by whole-brain differential SMA-connectivity maps of the effect of SMA TUS (Figure 4).

Figure 5. Connectional fingerprints of the stimulated areas.

Fingerprint target regions were drawn from the literature and chosen based on their known and distinct connectivity, either strong or weak, with the seed region. Activity coupling in the control state is indicated in blue, following SMA TUS in red, and FPC TUS in yellow. (a) SMA connectional fingerprint. In the control state (blue) SMA is strongly coupled to areas in the dorsomedial sensorimotor system (M1, SPL, MCC). Coupling with each of these areas is enhanced after SMA TUS (red). In contrast, coupling with regions that SMA is weakly connected with in the control state are even further reduced after SMA TUS (red). However, SMA’s fingerprint is relatively unaffected after FPC TUS (yellow), but some effects are visible for regions that are strongly coupled with FPC (compare to panel b). (b) FPC connectional fingerprint is sharpened following FPC TUS, but not following SMA TUS. The latter only affected coupling with SMA itself and regions strongly connected to it (MCC). (c) TUS induced more homogenous activity within the stimulated area (SMA in red on the left, FPC in yellow on the right). (d–f) Temporal evolution of the effect of SMA TUS on the SMA connectional fingerprint. The left panel depicts effects observed in the first fMRI run, followed by the second run in the middle, and the third and last run on the right. For each run, the time after TUS refers to the duration between the end of TUS and the midpoint of the run, averaged across the three animals. (g–h) TUS-induced effects on the connectional fingerprints of SMA and FPC could not be explained by auditory activity. (i) There were no changes in the coupling between the stimulation sites and auditory cortex. In all plots, thick lines and histograms indicate mean activity coupling; lighter coloured error bands and error bars depict standard-error of the mean; the asterisk denotes the interaction between TUS and connectivity seed (p<0.001).

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. TUS did not affect the temporal variability of the BOLD signal.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

Whole-brain pattern of the standard deviation of the BOLD signal over time in the control state (top), after SMA TUS (middle), and after FPC TUS (bottom). The temporal standard deviation of the signal was not affected by TUS and was comparable across the stimulated regions SMA and FPC (ROIs depicted in black). The observed pattern of variability across the brain, with for example higher variability in ventral (pre-)motor areas, reflects basic properties of the data acquisition and signal (e.g. proximity to the RF receive coils).

These specific effects of TUS were sustained over the duration of our experiment, lasting up to 2 hr (Figure 5d–f). Disruptive effects of TUS on long-range coupling were especially prominent immediately following the end of TUS application (Figure 5d), and gradually reduced towards the end of our recording session (Figure 5f). The enhancing effects that TUS exerted on SMA’s coupling with adjacent and strongly connected areas had a relatively delayed appearance, arising well after the TUS had ended (more than 1 hr, Figure 5e), but again decreasing towards the end of the recording session. In summary, the most important finding was of a protracted period of connectivity change after TUS. While we are cautious about overinterpreting precise timing differences between long-range connectivity reductions and local connectivity increments, we note that the observed pattern could signify distinct time courses for TUS-induced long-term depression and long-term potentiation. However, the observed pattern is also consistent with the notion that early disruption of long-range input to a network (Figure 5d) leaves relatively more signal variance in this network to be explained by remaining local input, driving the observation of subsequent within-network coupling increments (Figure 5e). Both these mechanisms would lead to a sharpening of the stimulated region’s connectivity profile, as observed here.

Experiment 2, TUS modulation of FPC connectivity

Like SMA, FPC’s activity is coupled with that in interconnected brain regions even when animals are at rest in the control state (Figure 3d). FPC’s activity is positively correlated with activity in a number of adjacent dorsomedial and lateral prefrontal areas and in the central portion of the superior temporal sulcus (midSTS) and posterior cingulate cortex with which it is monosynaptically interconnected (Petrides and Pandya, 2007). By contrast there was, at a rest, a negative relationship with the activity in sensorimotor areas, with which FPC is indirectly connected via regions such as SMA.

In comparison to the control state, FPC stimulation induced an enhancement in the normal short-range connectivity between FPC and adjacent dorsomedial (area 9m) and lateral prefrontal cortex (area 9-46d) with which it is particularly strongly connected. In addition, a similar effect was seen in more distant regions with which it is also strongly connected – the midSTS, IPLc and PCC, together comprising temporal and parietal segments of the primate ‘default mode network’ (Petrides and Pandya, 2007). By contrast, there was reduced coupling with other areas in prefrontal cortex, including ventromedial (area 14m), subgenual cingulate (area 25), and lateral orbitofrontal cortex (area 47-12o). These are all areas that FPC is connected to but less strongly (Petrides and Pandya, 2007). Finally, TUS applied to FPC also led to a change in long-range connectivity between FPC and several motor association regions with which it is not directly connected, especially those in the ventrolateral parieto-frontal sensorimotor network (areas PF and F4 Petrides and Pandya, 2007). As noted, in the control state, the activity in FPC and these sensorimotor association areas is negatively or anti-correlated, but this anti-correlation was reduced by FPC TUS. These results are apparent in the whole brain functional connectivity maps for the FPC region (Figure 3, compare panels d and f, representative changes highlighted by dashed black circles) and on the whole brain differential connectivity maps in Figure 4 (panels d and f). It is also apparent in the illustration of FPC’s connectional fingerprint (Figure 5b). Here the blue line indicates the strength of activity coupling between FPC and each of the other brain areas indicated on the circumference in the control state. The yellow line shows that FPC’s coupling with each area is changed after FPC TUS (non-parametric permutation test, p=0.027).

Comparing experiments 1 (SMA) and 2 (FPC)

It is important to test the claim that TUS induces effects that are spatially specific to each sonicated area by directly comparing effects between stimulation sites. Although FPC TUS significantly altered FPC functional connectivity, it had comparatively little impact on SMA’s pattern of functional connectivity; there was no difference in SMA’s functional connectivity between the control state and after FPC TUS (non-parametric permutation test, p=0.231; whole-brain map in Figure 3c and yellow line in connectivity fingerprint in Figure 5a). Importantly, the effects of TUS over SMA on SMA’s connectivity were significantly dissociable from the effects of FPC TUS (non-parametric permutation test, p=0.041). Similarly, SMA TUS had some but comparatively little impact on FPC’s pattern of functional connectivity (non-parametric permutation tests, SMA versus control, p=0.047; SMA versus FPC, p=0.028; whole-brain map in Figure 3e and red line in connectivity fingerprint in Figure 5b). In fact, the most prominent changes in each area’s connectional fingerprint that were induced by stimulation of the other area were the disruption of functional connectivity between FPC and sensorimotor areas when SMA was stimulated (Figure 3e, encircled). This particular result may have occurred because, as already noted, FPC has no direct monosynaptic connections with these sensorimotor areas (Petrides and Pandya, 2007) and so its functional coupling with these areas is likely to be mediated by areas such as SMA and the areas that surround it such as the pre-supplementary motor area and the cingulate motor areas (Bates and Goldman-Rakic, 1993; Lu et al., 1994). In fact, these circumscribed exceptions may well confirm the rule that a region’s connectivity pattern is only affected by stimulation of the region itself. Namely, SMA TUS only affects FPC coupling with SMA itself and the regions which are coupled with FPC only through SMA. In conclusion, the effects of TUS in different frontal regions were clearly dissociable.

Experiment 3, replication of TUS effects on FPC connectivity

We investigated the reproducibility of TUS effects by examining the impact of TUS to FPC in three additional individuals in a biological replication experiment (experiment 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 1). TUS had the same effects as seen in experiment 2: a site-specific sharpening of the stimulated region’s connectional profile. While experiments 1 and 2 followed a within-subject design, for experiment three we conducted a between-subject analysis where the subjects in the two experiments differed in age. We are therefore careful not to draw too strong conclusions on any main effect of subject group but focus on the interaction of the TUS effect with the fingerprint shape. Notwithstanding, when reviewing the simple effects driving the well-matched interaction, we note that the observed sharpening was less prominently related to activity coupling decreases in experiment 3 than in experiment 2. Putatively, parameters of the general anaesthesia could impact on the effect of TUS. However, subjects used in experiment 2 and 3 did not differ regarding depth of anaesthesia or duration between sedation and fMRI data collection.

Effects of TUS on activation in the stimulated regions and on the auditory system

It has recently been suggested that certain TUS protocols might have a limited efficacy in evoking spiking activity at the stimulation site, but rather exert their influence on the brain through the auditory system, not unlike an auditory startle response (Guo et al., 2018; Sato et al., 2018). Although these online observations in rodents should perhaps not be extrapolated far outside the tested conditions (for example to our measurements taken tens of minutes after the stimulation ended), these observations do argue in favour of performing controlled experiments that address and exclude such confounds (Airan and Butts Pauly, 2018). Here we consider, first, why neural effects of TUS might be evident in the current study when they were not clear previously. Second, we consider whether neural effects may be due to an auditory artefact.

First, it is possible that the efficacy of TUS is a function of both the specifics of the stimulation protocol and of ongoing neural activity. A neuromodulatory technique may fail to elicit spiking activity in deeply anaesthetized rodents (Guo et al., 2018; Sato et al., 2018). However, in this study we specifically test whether it simply modulates ongoing activity, while adopting lighter anaesthesia levels. Importantly, it is known that whole-brain functional connectivity, as measured with the BOLD signal, is preserved at these levels (Mars et al., 2013; Neubert et al., 2015; Neubert et al., 2014; O'Reilly et al., 2013; Sallet et al., 2013; Vincent et al., 2007). Finally, the experiments were conducted in a primate model as opposed to a rodent model; the importance of species-specific effects in TUS models are currently unknown. Under these distinct conditions, we observed that TUS modulated the activity coupling of each stimulated area in a regionally specific manner.

Nevertheless, following these investigations of the effect of TUS on whole-brain connectivity patterns of the stimulated regions, we carried out a second line of investigation and examined the effect of TUS on the signal in the stimulated regions themselves (Figure 5c). While BOLD fMRI cannot provide an absolute measure of neural activity, we can characterize how homogeneous the activation signal is within the stimulated region, as quantified by the coupling strength of the signal at each point in the stimulated region of interest to all other points in that region. This analysis revealed that TUS induced more spatially homogenous activation within the stimulated area, but not in the non-stimulated region (interaction of TUS x connectivity seed: F(1,8)=1571.2, p=1.8044e-10, d = 11.4426, CI=[1.2733 1.1333]). This effect on spatial homogeneity of the signal was not accompanied by changes to the temporal variance of the BOLD signal fluctuations in the stimulated or other regions (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). In fact, the standard deviation of the BOLD signal fluctuations over time were strikingly similar between the two stimulation sites (SMA and FPC, highlighted in Figure 5—figure supplement 1) and across the different experimental conditions (control, SMA TUS, and FPC TUS). This suggests that TUS leaves intact basic haemodynamics and neurophysiology and instead has a circumscribed and specific impact on the coupling of the stimulated region with the rest of the brain.

Third, the presence of auditory and somatosensory confounds is likely to be a function of the specifics of the TUS protocol. In sonication protocols the ultrasound wave is often pulse modulated at ~1 kHz, well within the audible range of many rodents and primates. At these modulation frequencies auditory stimulation is perhaps not unexpected (Guo et al., 2018; Sato et al., 2018). This is something that we have avoided in our work with macaques: we pulse modulated the 250 kHz ultrasound wave at 10 Hz: as such we ensured that the frequency of both the ultrasound wave and its modulating envelope are well outside of the macaque hearing range. Moreover, here we adopted an offline experimental design where any potential audible stimulation associated with the TUS application was limited to the 40 s sonication period, while the neural activation measures were initiated tens of minutes later. Furthermore, the specificity of our results strengthens the suggestion that it might not be possible to explain away the current findings as the result of an auditory artefact having occurred up to two hours earlier.

Nevertheless, we also carried out a fourth line of inquiry and examined the activation in the primary auditory cortex (A1) and its relationship with activity in the rest of the brain (Figure 5g–i). The effects of TUS on SMA and FPC coupling patterns, as quantified in their connectional fingerprints, could not be explained by a potential impact of TUS on A1 activity coupling. In fact, TUS did not affect A1 activity coupling with SMA fingerprint targets (Figure 5g; non-parametric permutation tests, SMA TUS: p=0.8234, FPC TUS p=0.3452), nor its coupling with FPC fingerprint targets (Figure 5h; non-parametric permutation tests, SMA TUS: p=0.5411, FPC TUS p=0.2667). Moreover, neither SMA nor FPC changed its coupling with A1 as a function of TUS (Figure 5i; main effect of TUS: F(1,8)=0.015445, p=0.90416, d = 0.03583, CI=[−0.41483 0.46209]; interaction of TUS x connectivity seed: F(1,8)=0.18284, p=0.68022, d = 0.1234, CI=[−0.25058 0.36466]).

Non-neuronal signal changes

The ability of ultrasound to reversibly interact with biological tissue is not limited to grey matter. We were aware that our ultrasonic beam, placed over the central midline to target SMA or FPC in both hemispheres simultaneously, was also likely to reach the meningeal compartment in the interhemispheric fissure. In fMRI analyses this region is sometimes referred to as ‘cerebral spinal fluid’, although in reality it contains the cortical membranes (dura, arachnoid, and pia mater), some cerebral spinal fluid, and important vascular structures, such as the superior sagittal sinus. Ultrasound protocols designed to induce vasodilation or to temporally open the blood-brain-barrier are conventionally markedly distinct from those employed here, for example using higher intensities or supplemented with intravenously injected microbubbles. Nonetheless, we set out to test the influence of ultrasound on what we shall continue to refer to as the ‘meningeal’ signal and the grey matter signal. To do this it was obviously necessary to take a somewhat unconventional rs-fMRI analysis approach that did not remove the meningeal signal that is typically regarded as a confound.

A principal component analysis of the signal in the meningeal and grey matter compartments revealed the main components in either compartment explained significantly more variance following TUS compared to control (Figure 6c; main effect of TUS: F(1,9)=30.6, p=0.00036, d = 0.67031, CI=[2.1916 5.3343]; in grey matter: F(1,4)=10.743, p=0.0306, d = 1.3381, CI=[0.47735 5.7655]; in meningeal compartment: F(1,4)=16.263, p=0.0157, d = 1.6464, CI=[1.3379 7.2512]). The fact that this effect is present in both compartments could reflect the tight vascular coupling between grey matter and meningeal signal or be driven by partial-voluming effects (these are more pronounced when the size of the brain is relatively small, as for monkey fMRI). This observation suggests that after TUS the BOLD signal became more homogenous. In a seed-based connectivity analysis, as performed here, this would be reflected in a stronger contribution of global signal coupling. However, the impact of TUS presented above does not seem to exhibit this effect, as illustrated by the specificity of the TUS effects for SMA and FPC (Figures 3,5), and further underpinned by the absence of TUS effects in regions remote from the stimulation sites (for example, Figure 6a,d illustrates the case of the posterior parietal operculum, POp). Importantly, these coupling estimates are obtained when following the conventional rs-fMRI analysis approach to account for global signal confounds by removing WM and meningeal signal contributions before estimating grey matter coupling indices.

Figure 6. Non-neuronal signal changes driving widespread coupling.

In fully processed fMRI data, unconfounded by global nuisance sources, effects of TUS were not immediately apparent outside the stimulated areas. (a,d) For example, the whole-brain connectivity pattern of the parietal operculum area (POp, spatially removed from either stimulation site) was unaffected by TUS over SMA. (b) When nuisance contributions from the meningeal compartment are not removed (but those from WM are), the presence of globally shared signal is evident: POp activity is now coupled with that in many other areas. (e) POp’s global nuisance confounded functional connectivity pattern became stronger after SMA TUS. This was because SMA TUS induced broad changes in BOLD signal even in the meningeal compartment, captured in a principal component analysis (c). Error bars denote standard-error of the mean; asterisks denote effects of TUS and WM/meningeal compartment (p<0.05). (f) This led to many points in the cortex exhibiting stronger coupling with other brain areas; all areas shown in red are points that have stronger coupling with the rest of the brain after TUS. Note that this effect is present far beyond the stimulation site. (g) This widespread TUS-induced meningeal signal coupling persisted over time. All conventions as in Figures 3,5.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Widespread non-neuronal signal changes were replicated in experiments 2 and 3.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(a) Whole-brain connectivity pattern of area POp, a region removed from the stimulation sites, in the control state without TUS. (d) This region’s connectivity is unaffected by TUS over FPC, confirming the absence of remote effects following SMA TUS in experiment 1 (Figure 6). (g) The relative absence of remote effects (illustrated by POp) following FPC TUS in experiment two is replicated in experiment three in a new set of 3 animals. (b) When signal in the meningeal compartment is not considered in the noise estimation step POp’s connectivity is confounded by global nuisance signal. (e) Similar to the effects observed after SMA TUS, POp’s global nuisance confounded functional connectivity pattern became stronger after FPC TUS. (c) This was because FPC TUS induced broad changes in BOLD signal even in the meningeal compartment, captured in a principal component analysis, both in experiments 2 and 3. Error bars denote standard-error of the mean. (f) This led to many points in the cortex exhibiting stronger coupling with other brain areas; all areas shown in red are points that have stronger coupling with the rest of the brain after TUS. (h,i) This widespread meningeal signal coupling following FPC TUS and its effect on POp’s connectivity pattern was replicated in experiment 3. Panels (a) and (b) are reproduced here from Figure 6 for comparison. All conventions as in Figure 6.

We hypothesized that if TUS leads to more homogenous global signal, its contribution to the grey matter signal might have been accounted for when removing meningeal signal components in a linear regression framework (Verhagen, 2012). Accordingly, we have repeated the seed-based connectivity analyses after accounting for global signal confounds based on the white matter compartment alone, excluding the meningeal compartment. In these data, global signal contributions were indeed preserved as evidenced by anatomically implausible global connectivity patterns present in the control state (compare panels a and b in Figure 6, e.g. the prefrontal cortex, encircled). As such, this procedure allowed us to interrogate the global effects of TUS over dorsomedial frontal regions on BOLD signal (see Materials and Methods for full details). Following this procedure, we observed that SMA stimulation appeared to induce widespread increases in signal coupling compared to the control state. This effect was not limited to the stimulation site but also present in remote regions (e.g. when seeded in POp, compare panels b and e in Figure 6). This effect can be quantified by considering the strength of local connections for every point in the cortex. It is then apparent that the changes induced by stimulation are global in nature (Figure 6f). In general, when not fully accounting for global confounds, a region’s connectivity profile after TUS could be predicted by considering its profile in the control state and adding a spatially flat constant. This suggests an additive non-neuronal source, captured by signal components in the meningeal compartment, may explain the presence and enhancement of global signal observed following TUS over SMA (Figure 6e,f, e.g. primary sensorimotor cortex). These effects of TUS on widespread coupling mediated by meningeal signal persisted over time for more than 1 hr after stimulation had ended (Figure 6g).

Similar effects of TUS on meningeal signal were also observed after FPC TUS (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). We note that in unconfounded fMRI data FPC TUS did not have a strong impact outside the stimulated region (illustrated for POp in Figure 6—figure supplement 1). In contrast, in fMRI data confounded by meningeal-driven global nuisance, FPC stimulation led to widespread enhanced signal coupling compared to the control state. This effect of FPC TUS was replicated in a new set of animals in experiment 3 (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). Although the effect was perhaps not as strong as that seen after SMA TUS, this finding again suggests that TUS over the medial meningeal compartment may produce widespread changes that are non-neuronal in origin. Differences in morphology of the sagittal sinus along the rostro-caudal axis might explain this weakened global effect.

Experiment 4, meso- and micro-structural analyses

Some higher intensity ultrasound stimulation protocols, distinct from those used here, have been shown to induce thermal lesions or haemorrhage following cavitation (Elias et al., 2013). Despite the fact that 40 s trains of TUS induced sustained changes in the post-stimulation period in experiments 1 and 2, no structural changes remotely resembling those seen with higher intensity ultrasound protocols were observed. First of all, we did not observe any indication of TUS-induced oedema when comparing T1w MRI structural scans collected in baseline sessions with T1w scans collected after TUS (Figure 7). We also did not observe tissue alteration (e.g. tissue burn) at the post-mortem examination. Neither were any signs of neuronal alteration or haemorrhage observed in histological analyses of three macaques following pre-SMA TUS (Figure 8).

Figure 7. No effect of TUS was apparent on T1w structural MRI images.

Figure 7.

Examination of T1-weighted structural MRI images did not reveal any evidence for oedema after TUS. For two monkeys (top and bottom row, respectively), T1w images were acquired immediately following the resting-state fMRI runs in the control state (left column) and ~2 hr after TUS over FPC (right column). The sonication target regions are highlighted with dashed blue boxes.

Figure 8. No effect of TUS was apparent on histological examination.

Figure 8.

(a) Dorsal view of perfused macaque brain. A post-mortem examination of the brain did not reveal macroscopic damage to the brain. (b) Histological assessment of 50-micron thick sections obtained at the level of the stimulation site with Cresyl Violet Nissl staining and H and E staining did not reveal any evidence of thermal lesions or haemorrhage after the TUS protocol used here. Magnified images are centred at the focal point of the stimulation.

Acoustic and thermal modelling

To quantify the pressure amplitude, peak intensities, spatial distribution, and potential temperature changes in the monkey brain associated with the TUS protocol used in this study we simulated the acoustic wave propagation and its thermal effect in a whole head finite element model based on a high-resolution monkey CT scan. As estimated by these numerical simulations, the maximum spatial-peak pulse-averaged intensity (Isppa) at the acoustic focus point was 24.1 W/cm2 for the SMA target and 31.7 W/cm2 for the FPC target (spatial peak temporal average intensities, Ispta: 7.2 W/cm2 and 9.5 W/cm2 for SMA and FPC, respectively). Given that the skull is more acoustically absorbing than soft tissue, the highest thermal increase is located in the skull itself, estimated by the simulation to be 2.9°C. Given an approximate 0.5 mm thickness of the dura (Galashan et al., 2011) the maximum temperature below the dura was 38.0°C. The maximal thermal increase at the geometrical focus of the sonic transducer was less than 0.5°C (Figure 2).

Discussion

In this study we demonstrate a protocol for transcranial focused ultrasound stimulation (TUS) that can induce a sustained yet reversible change in neural activity. We focused our investigation on modulations of brain connectivity, following the notion that each brain area’s unique contribution to cognition and behaviour is shaped by how activity in each area is a function of a unique fingerprint of interconnected areas (Passingham et al., 2002). We found that each area’s connectional fingerprint was significantly changed by TUS, but only when it was applied to that area itself (Figures 3,5). The changes observed might be summarized as more uniform activation in the stimulated region combined with a sharpening of the normal coupling pattern that each area has even at rest. Activity coupling with strongly interconnected areas, which are often nearby, was increased but activity coupling with less strongly connected regions was reduced. Such changes in connectional fingerprints might constitute the mechanism by which TUS is able to induce regionally specific patterns of behavioural change when applied in awake behaving animals (Deffieux et al., 2013; Fouragnan et al., 2019). The pattern of inputs each area receives from other areas and the influence it wields over other areas are a major determinant of its function and here we have shown that this pattern is altered by TUS. TUS may therefore provide a relatively straightforward method for sustained but reversible manipulation of specific components of neural circuits in the primate brain (Wattiez et al., 2017). This may be important for investigating primate brain areas when homologues in non-primate species, such as rodents, are non-existent or disputed (Preuss, 1995; Wise, 2008). This work paves the way for the development and use of offline TUS protocols in primates, including humans, both as a research tool and as potential clinical intervention.

In experiments 1 and 2, adopting a within-subject design with three animals, we found that TUS application produced different effects when applied to different brain regions: the SMA, a part of motor association cortex, and FPC a part of granular prefrontal cortex (Figures 3,5). However, in each case the TUS effects were prominent within the connectional fingerprint of the area stimulated. The connectional fingerprints of SMA and FPC are distinct (Johansen-Berg et al., 2004; Neubert et al., 2014; Sallet et al., 2013). The effects of TUS are thus regionally specific.

Our results confirm that TUS can be used as a neuromodulatory technique that allows one to non-surgically target cortical and subcortical brain areas with superior spatial specificity and depth of stimulation (Folloni et al., 2019) compared to other transcranial stimulation approaches (e.g. TMS and TCS; Bestmann and Walsh, 2017; Dayan et al., 2013; Polanía et al., 2018). While successes have been achieved with some invasive techniques, such as electrical microstimulation (Krug et al., 2015; Vanduffel, 2016), it is not easy to use them to disrupt activity in all areas especially when they are not somatotopically mapped. Recently it has been reported that some online TUS protocols in rodents induce neural changes as an indirect consequence of the auditory stimulation they entail (Guo et al., 2018; Sato et al., 2018). The spatially specific effects that we observed after TUS cannot, however, be attributed to any common auditory impact that occurs at the time of stimulation. Moreover, in order to avoid both the confounding effect associated with the sound of TUS and interference of the ultrasonic wave field with fMRI measurement, we opted for an ‘offline’ stimulation protocol. Stimulation was a 40 s train that ended at least 20 min before the fMRI data acquisition period. The sustained nature of the train and other features of the stimulation pulses may make the protocol used here more effective for neuromodulation, while ensuring the thermal modulation of the cortex remains limited (<1°C, Figure 2). Such limited thermal changes are not associated with neuromodulatory effects observed more than 30 min after the stimulation: the thermal rise is short-lived (Dallapiazza et al., 2018), not accompanied by tissue damage and below the thermal effects observed with some protocols in rodents (for a review, see Constans et al., 2018).

In experiment 3 we found that TUS had reproducible effects. When TUS was applied to FPC in three other individuals, it induced spatially specific effects similar to those seen in experiment 2.

The effects of the non-invasive 40 s stimulation protocol used here are sustained and lasted over much of the two-hour period we investigated. These effects are more extended than those produced by other techniques commonly used for offline disruption of cortical activity such as TMS (Huang et al., 2005; O'Shea et al., 2007). Care should therefore be taken in using the technique in human cognitive neuroscience experiments; TUS effects may continue beyond the short periods that participants typically spend within the laboratory. It may therefore be important to carefully characterize the time course of TUS effects in animal models before their use with human participants. Some caution might also be warranted in relation to the potential of ultrasound to cause microstructural damage, especially when stimulating at higher intensities, longer durations, or for more repetitions. While our structural MRI and histological analyses did not reveal any evidence of damage and were comparable to previous studies (Dallapiazza et al., 2018; Lee et al., 2016b), it would be of interest in the future to include additional histological indices of apoptotic or inflammatory processes (Tufail et al., 2010) to further assess the safety of TUS. Similarly, the thermal modelling approach we adopted here was designed to estimate an informed upper-bound on potential thermal effects, but when further developing this protocol additional simulation validations – for example based on phantom measurements – might be informative.

In addition to the spatially specific effects of TUS we also observed changes in the BOLD signal originating from the meningeal compartment that were not specific to the area stimulated. Although similar effects were seen each time either the SMA or FPC was stimulated, the effect was generally more pronounced after SMA stimulation than after FPC stimulation. Our preliminary results from TUS of other brain areas suggest that these spatially non-specific effects may be even smaller when TUS is applied elsewhere (Folloni et al., 2019). While the precise origin of the non-specific effects was difficult to determine it is possible that they may result from a direct vascular effect of TUS; the sagittal sinus is directly above the midline frontal regions that we targeted and is included in the meningeal compartment during MRI analysis protocols. The presence of such non-specific effects again underlines the need for care in translating the technique to humans; especially when the targeted region is near venous sinuses or cerebral arteries. In addition, they underline the need for comparing the behavioural effects of TUS not just with a non-stimulation sham condition but with TUS application to another control brain region. The absence of marked histological changes in experiment 4, however, provides one important safety benchmark and confirms previous histological results in lagomorphs (Yoo et al., 2011).

Combining TUS and fMRI is a promising approach to overcome the restrictions of each of the individual techniques. Here we have shown that TUS has a detectable offline and sustained impact on the distinctive network of connectivity associated with the stimulated brain region – the connectional fingerprint. A brain region’s interactions with other regions – its unique connectional fingerprint or specific pattern of inputs and outputs – are an important determinant of its functional role. The current results are therefore consistent with TUS application exerting regionally specific effects on behaviour (Deffieux et al., 2013; Fouragnan et al., 2019). The fact that fMRI allows the effects of TUS to be studied with a high spatial resolution suggests the TUS-fMRI combination has the potential to become a powerful neuroscientific tool.

Materials and methods

Subject details

For this study, six healthy male macaques (Macaca mulatta, NCBITaxon:9544) were stimulated with transcranial focused ultrasound and scanned to acquire resting state functional magnetic resonance images (rs-fMRI) and anatomical MR images. Three animals participated in experiment 1 [SMA TUS] (all males, mean age and weight at time of scan: 5.6 years, 10.7 kg). The same three animals participated in experiment 2 [FPC TUS] (at time of scan: 6.1 years, 11.8 kg), and the control condition (at time of scan: 5.5 years, 10.2 kg). Three different animals participated in experiment 3 [FPC TUS validation] (all males, mean age and weight at time of scan: 10.3 years, 13 kg). In addition to this set of animals, six animals were included in the histology analysis (experiment 4): three control animals who did not receive TUS (two females; mean age and weight at time of perfusion: 9.3 years, 9.1 kg) and three pre-SMA TUS animals post stimulation (all males; mean age and weight at time of perfusion: 8.4 years, 13.1 kg).

All procedures were conducted under licenses from the United Kingdom (UK) Home Office in accordance with The Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986. In all cases they complied with the European Union guidelines (EU Directive 2010/63/EU).

Ultrasound stimulation

A single element ultrasound transducer (H115-MR, diameter 64 mm, Sonic Concept, Bothell, WA, USA) with 51.74 mm focal depth was used with a coupling cone filled with degassed water and sealed with a latex membrane (Durex). The resonance frequency of the ultrasonic wave was set at 250 kHz with 30 ms bursts of ultrasound generated every 100 ms, controlled through a digital function generator (Handyscope HS5, TiePie engineering, Sneek, The Netherlands). The stimulation lasted for 40 s. A 75-Watt amplifier (75A250A, Amplifier Research, Souderton, PA) was used to deliver the required power to the transducer. A TiePie probe (Handyscope HS5, TiePie engineering, Sneek, The Netherlands) connected to an oscilloscope was used to monitor the voltage delivered. The recorded peak-to-peak voltage was kept constant throughout the stimulation. Voltage values per session ranged from 130 to 142 V, corresponding to 1.17 to 1.35 MPa as measured in water with an in house heterodyne interferometer (Constans et al., 2017). Based on numerical simulations (see Acoustic and thermal modelling below for more details), the maximum peak pressure (Pmax) and Isppa at the acoustic focus point were estimated to be 0.88 MPa and 24.1 W/cm2 for the SMA target, and 1.01 MPa and 31.7 W/cm2 for the FPC target (Ispta: 7.2 W/cm2 and 9.5 W/cm2 for SMA and FPC, respectively). Each of the areas targeted in experiments 1–4 lie close to the midline. Therefore, we applied a single train over the midline stimulating the target region in both hemispheres simultaneously.

In order to direct TUS to the target region, we guided the stimulation using a frameless stereotaxic neuronavigation system (Rogue Research, Montreal, CA; RRID:SCR_009539) set up for each animal individually by registering a T1-weighted MR image to the animal’s head. Positions of both the ultrasound transducer and the head of the animal were tracked continuously with infrared reflectors to inform online and accurate positioning of the transducer over the targeted brain region: SMA in experiment 1, (Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) X, Y, and Z coordinates in mm [0.1 2 19]); FPC in experiment 2 [0.6 24 10]; FPC in experiment 3 [-0.7 24 11]; pre-SMA in experiment 4 [0.2 11 17]. The ultrasound transducer/coupling cone montage was placed directly onto previously shaved skin prepared with conductive gel (SignaGel Electrode; Parker Laboratories Inc.) to ensure ultrasonic coupling between the transducer and the animal's scalp. In the non-stimulation condition (control), all procedures (anaesthesia, pre-scan preparation, fMRI scan acquisition and timing), with the exception of actual TUS, matched the TUS sessions.

Acoustic and thermal modelling

The acoustic wave propagation of our focused ultrasound protocol (at 130 V peak-to-peak voltage) was simulated using a k-space pseudospectral method-based solver, k-Wave (Cox et al., 2007) to obtain estimates for the pressure amplitude, peak intensity, spatial distribution, and thermal impact at steady state. 3D maps of the skull were extracted from a monkey CT scan (Kyoto University online database, ID 1478, 0.26 mm isotropic resolution). Soft tissues were assumed to be homogeneous, with acoustic values of water (ρtissue =1000 kg/m3 and ctissue =1500 m/s). In the bone, a linear relationship between the Hounsfield Units (HU) from the CT scan and the sound speed, as well as the density, was used. The power law model for attenuation is αatt=α1ϕβ where the porosity ϕ is defined by ϕ=ρmaxρρmaxρwater in the skull (Aubry et al., 2003). The attenuation coefficient for the acoustic propagation α1 depends on the frequency: α1=α0fb. We set the parameters to ρmax=2200 kg/m3, cmax=3100 m/s, β=0.5, α0=8 dB/cm/MHzb,b=1.1 (Constans et al., 2018). The attenuation coefficient in bone accounts for both absorption and scattering.

The propagation simulation was performed at 250 kHz with a 150µs-long pulse signal (enough to reach a steady state). The transducer was modelled as a spherical section (63 mm radius of curvature and 64 mm active diameter). The simulated pulses were spatially apodized (r = 0.35) on the spherical section. Ultrasound propagates first through water before entering the skull cavity with the geometrical focal point located below the surface, inside the brain. Simulations were performed in free water, and the maximum amplitude obtained was used to rescale the results in skull (the transducer calibration indicates that the maximum amplitude in water at 130V is 1.2 MPa). The thermal modelling is based on the bio-heat equation (Pennes, 1948):

ρCTt=κ2T+q+wρbCb(TTa)

where T, ρ, C, κ and q are the temperature, density, specific heat, thermal conductivity and rate of heat production respectively. Heat production is defined as q=αabsP²2ρC, αabs being the absorption coefficient and P the peak negative pressure. κ is set to 0.528 W.m−1.K−1 in soft tissue and 0.4 W.m−1.K−1 in the skull; C is set to 3600 J.kg−1.K−1 in soft tissue and 1300 J.kg−1.K−1 in the skull (Duck, 2013). In the tissue, the absorption coefficient was set to αabs tissue=0.21 dB/cm/MHzb (Goss et al., 1979). In the skull the longitudinal absorption coefficient is proportional to the density with αabs max=a0/3=2.7dB/cm/MHzb(Pinton et al., 2012). The last term corresponds to the perfusion process: wρbCb, and Ta correspond to the blood perfusion rate, blood density, blood specific heat and blood ambient temperature respectively. These parameters are assumed homogeneous over the brain, although a more detailed description of the brain cooling processes can be found in the literature (Wang et al., 2015). The perfusion parameters are based on previous reports (Pulkkinen et al., 2011): w=0.008s−1; ρb= 1030 kg.m−3; Cb = 3620 J.kg−1.K−1 and Ta = 37°C.

The bioheat equation is solved by using a 3D finite-difference scheme in MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, USA) with Dirichlet boundary conditions. Initial temperature conditions were 37°C in the brain, skull and tissue, and 24°C in the water coupling cone. Simulations were run over 1 min pre-sonication, followed by 40 s of sonication, and 5 min post-sonication, closely following the experimental procedure.

Macaque MRI acquisition

We acquired one MRI session per monkey per condition: in total we performed three sessions per animal across experiments 1 and 2, and one session per animal in experiment 3. The ultrasound sonication and subsequent MRI scans were performed under inhalational isoflurane gas anaesthesia using a protocol which has previously proven successful in preserving whole-brain functional connectivity as measured with BOLD signal (Mars et al., 2013; Mars et al., 2011; Neubert et al., 2015; Neubert et al., 2014; O'Reilly et al., 2013; Sallet et al., 2013; Vincent et al., 2007). In the case of the TUS conditions, fMRI data collection began only after completion of the TUS train. Anaesthesia was induced using intramuscular injection of ketamine (10 mg/kg), xylazine (0.125–0.25 mg/kg), and midazolam (0.1 mg/kg). Macaques also received injections of atropine (0.05 mg/kg, intramuscularly), meloxicam (0.2 mg/kg, intravenously), and ranitidine (0.05 mg/kg, intravenously). The anaesthetized animals were placed in an MRI-compatible frame (Crist Instruments) in a sphinx position and placed in a horizontal 3 T MRI scanner with a full-size bore. Scanning commenced ~2 hr after induction, when the clinical peak of ketamine had passed. Anaesthesia was maintained, in accordance with veterinary recommendation, using the lowest possible concentration of isoflurane to ensure that macaques were anaesthetized. The depth of anaesthesia was assessed and monitored using physiological parameters (heart rate and blood pressure, as well as clinical checks before the scan for muscle relaxation). During the acquisition of the functional data the expired isoflurane concentration was in the range 0.6–0.8%. Isoflurane was selected for the scans as it was previously demonstrated to preserve rs-fMRI networks (Mars et al., 2013; Mars et al., 2011; Neubert et al., 2015; Neubert et al., 2014; O'Reilly et al., 2013; Sallet et al., 2013; Vincent et al., 2007). Macaques were maintained with intermittent positive pressure ventilation to ensure a constant respiration rate during the functional scan, and respiration rate, inspired and expired CO2, and inspired and expired isoflurane concentration were monitored and recorded using VitalMonitor software (Vetronic Services Ltd.). Core temperature and SpO2 were also constantly monitored throughout the scan. A four-channel phased-array coil was used for data acquisition (Dr. H. Kolster, Windmiller Kolster Scientific, Fresno, CA, USA).

FMRI data were collected in experiments 1–3. In each session whole-brain BOLD fMRI data were collectedfor 3 runs of approximately 26 min each, using the following parameters: 36 axial slices; in-plane resolution, 2 × 2 mm; slice thickness, 2 mm; no slice gap; TR, 2000 ms; TE, 19 ms; 800 volumes per run. A minimum period of 10 days elapsed between sessions.

T1-weighted structural MRI scans were collected in experiments 1–3. A structural scan (average over up to three T1w images acquired in the same session) was acquired for each macaque in the same session, using a T1 weighted magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient echo sequence (0.5 × 0.5×0.5 mm voxel resolution).

Macaque anatomical MRI pre-processing

The pre-processing and analysis of the MRI data was designed to follow the HCP Minimal Processing Pipeline (Glasser et al., 2013), using tools of FSL (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki; RRID:SCR_002823), HCP Workbench (https://www.humanconnectome.org/software/connectome-workbench; RRID:SCR_008750), and the Magnetic Resonance Comparative Anatomy Toolbox (MrCat; https://github.com/neuroecology/MrCat; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/MrCatVerhagen, 2019). The T1w images were processed in an iterative fashion cycling through brain-extraction, RF bias-field correction, and linear and non-linear template registration to the Macaca mulatta F99 atlas (Van Essen and Dierker, 2007). The initial skull stripping was performed using a multi-seeded implementation of BET (Smith, 2002) optimized for macaque brains, while subsequent brain extraction was based on a high-fidelity template registered to the F99 macaque space. The RF bias-field was estimated and corrected using a robust implementation of FAST (Zhang et al., 2001). Linear and non-linear registration to F99 space was achieved using FLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002; Jenkinson and Smith, 2001) and FNIRT (Andersson et al., 2007; Jenkinson et al., 2012) with configurations adjusted to reflect macaque rather than human brain characteristics. The application of a robust and macaque-optimised version of FAST also resulted inestimated compartments for grey matter, white matter, and meninges with cerebral spinal fluid. For each compartment a posterior-probability map was created by integrating a set of prior probability maps based on 112 Macaca mullata individuals (McLaren et al., 2009) with the dataset-specific evidence provided by FAST. Segmentation of subcortical structures was achieved by registration to the D99 atlas (Reveley et al., 2017).

Macaque rs-fMRI pre-processing

The first 5 volumes of the functional EPI datasets were discarded to ensure a steady RF excitation state. EPI timeseries were motion corrected using MCFLIRT. Given that the animals were anaesthetized and their heads were held in a steady position, any apparent image motion, if present at all, is caused by changes to the B0 field and temperature, rather than by head motion. Accordingly, the parameter estimates from MCFLIRT can be considered to be ‘B0-confound parameters’ instead. Each timeseries was checked rigorously for spikes and other artefacts, both visually and using automated algorithms; where applicable slices with spikes were linearly interpolated based on temporally neighbouring slices. Brain extraction, bias-correction, and registration was achieved for the functional EPI datasets in an iterative manner, similar to the pre-processing of the structural images with the only difference that the mean of each functional dataset was registered to its corresponding T1w image using rigid-body boundary-based registration (FLIRT). EPI signal noise was reduced both in the frequency and temporal domain. The functional timeseries were high-pass filtered with a frequency cut-off at 2000 s. Temporally cyclical noise, for example originating from the respiration apparatus, was removed using band-stop filters set dynamically to noise peaks in the frequency domain of the first three principal components of the timeseries.

To account for remaining global signal confounds we considered the signal timeseries in white matter (WM) and meningeal compartments. Specifically, the WM + meningeal confound timeseries was described by the mean time course and the first five subsequent principal components of the combined WM and meningeal compartment (considering only voxels with a high posterior probability of belonging to the WM or meningeal compartment, obtained in the T1w image using FAST). The principal components of the WM + meningeal signal were estimated using a singular value decomposition approach. The B0 confound parameter estimates obtained from MCFLIRT were expanded as a second degree Volterra series to capture both linear and non-linear B0 effects. Together the WM + meningeal and expanded B0 confound parameters were regressed out of the BOLD signal for each voxel.

In a separate analysis (Figure 6 and Figure 6—figure supplement 1), to assess the contribution of the meningeal compartment signal we repeated the identical procedure as above, with the only difference that the mean and principal components were extracted from signal in the WM compartment alone, excluding the meningeal compartment.

Following this confound cleaning step, the timeseries were low-pass filtered with a cut-off at 10 s. The cleaned and filtered signal was projected from the conventional volumetric representation (2 mm voxels) to the F99 cortical surface (~1.4 mm spaced vertices), while maintaining the subcortical volumetric structures. The data were spatially smoothed using a 3 mm FWHM gaussian kernel, while considering the folding of the cortex and the anatomical boundaries of the subcortical structures. Lastly, the data timeseries were demeaned to prepare for functional connectivity analyses.

Macaque rs-fMRI analyses

To represent subject effects, the timeseries from the three runs were concatenated to create a single timeseries of 3×800-5=2385 volumes per animal per intervention (control, SMA TUS, FPC TUS). To represent group effects the run-concatenated timeseries of all animals were combined using a group-PCA approach resulting in a series of 200 volumes representing the principal components (Smith et al., 2014).

We report on the whole-brain functional coupling of the stimulation sites in the control state and after TUS, adopting a seed-based correlation analysis approach. Effects of TUS are quantified based on a limited set of regions, a ‘fingerprint’, whose strength of interconnection with macaque SMA or FPC is known from anatomical tracing studies (Bates and Goldman-Rakic, 1993; Dum and Strick, 2005; Geyer et al., 2000; Lu et al., 1994; Petrides and Pandya, 2007; Strick et al., 1998) and from studies of fMRI activity coupling under anaesthesia (Neubert et al., 2015; Neubert et al., 2014; Sallet et al., 2013). For example, SMA is strongly interconnected with core nodes in the sensorimotor network, including M1, SPL, and MCC. The unique profile of interconnections characterizing a functional area is defined by a pattern of both strong and limited interconnections (Passingham et al., 2002). Accordingly, we selected a set of regions based on either distinctively strong or distinctively limited interconnection and fMRI coupling with SMA or FPC. In fact, as a result of the complementary patterns of connections defining SMA and FPC (Petrides and Pandya, 2007), areas that were included based on their known strong connection and coupling with FPC (9m, 9-46d, PCC, IPLc, midSTS) were also included because of their known limited connections and coupling with SMA. Furthermore, to characterize the effect of TUS targeted at FPC on adjacent prefrontal cortex, we also included additional ventromedial and orbital prefrontal regions with which FPC shared less strong connections. To ensure a balanced distribution of fingerprint targets across the brain and to ensure that both SMA and FPC fingerprints were comprised of the same number of regions we consider areas 11m and aSTG in the SMA fingerprint, both regions with which the SMA is weakly interconnected. Lastly, to test for specificity of the TUS effects, we included SMA as a target for the FPC fingerprint and FPC as a target for the SMA fingerprint.

To construct regions-of-interest (ROI) for SMA and FPC, circles of 4 mm radius were drawn on the cortical surface around the point closest to the average stimulation coordinate (Figure 2), in both the left and the right hemisphere. The same procedure was used to define other bilateral cortical regions of interest, based on literature coordinates (Mars et al., 2011; Neubert et al., 2015; Neubert et al., 2014; Sallet et al., 2013), to serve as seeds for connectivity analyses (A1, Figure 5g–i; POp, Figure 6, Figure 6—figure supplement 1) or targets for the fingerprint analyses (Figure 5). Apart from the stimulation sites and primary auditory cortex, all ROIs were selected based on their known anatomical connectivity, of relevance because they are known to be distinctively strongly or distinctively weakly connected with the stimulation sites.

Coupling between the activity of each region of interest and the rest of the brain was estimated by calculating the correlation coefficient between each point in the ROI and all other data points (Fisher’s z: inverse hyperbolic tangent of Pearson’s r, bounded at [−2 2]). The resulting ‘connectivity-maps’ were averaged across all vertices/voxels in the ROI, and subsequently averaged across hemispheres. Accordingly, the final maps represent the average coupling of a bilateral ROI with the rest of the brain. The fingerprints are obtained by extracting the average coupling with each target ROI and averaging across the two hemispheres.

The strength of self-connections within SMA and FPC ROIs was estimated in the same way as the coupling of either SMA or FPC with any remote ROI was determined. Namely, for each point in the ROI the Fisher’s z-transformed correlation between this point’s timeseries and that of each of the other points in the ROI was calculated. Subsequently, for each point the resultant z-values were averaged, describing local coupling at that point, and averaged across the whole ROI to obtain a single estimate of self-coupling per ROI (Figure 5c).

To assess the contribution of the meningeal compartment signal, we quantified the effect of TUS for each point on the cortical surface in data that was not corrected for the meningeal signal (see Macaque rs-fMRI pre-processing). For each point, we calculated it’s coupling with the rest of the brain, sorted all Fisher’s z-values in ascending order, and extracted the z-value at the 98th-percentile. Z-values at this level describe relatively strong coupling with the seed point and are often observed in the vicinity of the seed region or other strongly connected regions (depicted in bright yellow in Figure 6), ensuring that irrelevant connections are ignored. As such, this simple statistic is well-suited to capture any main effects of TUS on coupling strength, including those observed when comparing panels (b) and (e) in Figure 6. This statistic allows us to quantify the effect of TUS in a single value for each point on the cortical surface, and to directly contrast the values obtained in the control state with those obtained after TUS (Figure 6f, Figure 6—figure supplement 1f,i).

We described the signal variance in the GM and meningeal compartments, or more specifically, the variance explained by the first five principal components (Figure 6c, Figure 6—figure supplement 1 panel c), using the same approach that was used to decompose signal in WM or WM + meningeal compartments in the EPI timeseries cleaning step, as described above. We defined the explained variance as the sum of the first five eigenvalues of the covariance matrix of the compartment signals divided by the sum of all eigenvalues.

Statistics

All suitable animals available at the time of experimentation took part in this study. Accordingly, there was no pre-selection nor restriction for group allocation. No data was excluded for analysis. Sample sizes could not be predetermined statistically in the absence of a prior literature reporting relevant expected effect sizes; instead we adopted sample sizes similar to those reported in previous publications detailing (interventional) macaque fMRI studies (Chau et al., 2015; O'Reilly et al., 2013; Papageorgiou et al., 2017). Data collection and analysis were not performed blind to the conditions of the experiments. We used a group PCA approach to define the centre of group rs-fMRI effects (see Macaque rs-fMRI analyses). For all other effects the centre of the group is defined as the mean of the individual subject effects.

To report on the full extent of the TUS effects and the simple effects that drive any observed differences we reproduce the whole-brain functional connectivity maps unthresholded for the control, SMA TUS, and FPC TUS conditions separately. In order to make a statistical comparison of the functional coupling of the SMA and FPC in the control and TUS conditions it is problematic to compare coupling at each and every other point in the brain because there is a risk of false positive effects if multiple comparisons are made. Given the limited sample sizes possible with non-human primate experiments, however, there is a risk of false negative results if stringent correction for multiple comparisons is undertaken at the whole-brain level. Indeed, here we avoid these pitfalls and make inference on a single statistic that combines information from a planned limited set of regions defined by coordinates from previous studies (Mars et al., 2011; Neubert et al., 2015; Neubert et al., 2014; Sallet et al., 2013).

Importantly, rather than examining activity coupling between the seed area and each of the fingerprint ROIs in turn and risking potential false positive results, we compared the overall pattern of coupling using the method devised by Mars and colleagues (Mars et al., 2016); non-parametric permutation tests were performed on cosine similarity metrics summarizing pairs of fingerprints (SMA TUS versus control and FPC TUS versus control). Each connectional fingerprint can be represented as a multidimensional vector, with the number of dimensions corresponding to the number of target ROIs in the fingerprint. The cosine similarity metric quantifies the angle between two of such multi-dimensional vectors. As such, it takes into account the shape of the fingerprint irrespective of its mean amplitude and results in a single metric per pair of fingerprints, negating the necessity for correcting for multiple comparisons across fingerprint targets. To assess the likelihood of observing this cosine similarity metric under the null-hypothesis of no difference between conditions, it needs to be compared against a distribution of metrics obtained from a random sample. To avoid non-compliance to the requirements for a conventional parametric approach (e.g. independence of sampling and adherence to a Gaussian normal distribution) we applied a non-parametric permutation approach, in which the parent distribution is empirically drawn by repeatedly (randomly) shuffling the group-membership of data points and re-calculating the metric based on the shuffled labelling (e.g. data points acquired in the control state might be randomly assigned to the intervention condition, and vice versa). In this way the empirical distribution under the null-hypothesis does not rely on assumptions about the shape of the distribution but will acknowledge dependencies between target ROIs in the fingerprint; as such this approach will avoid inflation of type I error (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007; Winkler et al., 2014). The relatively small number of TUS sites, animals, and fMRI runs allowed us to exhaustively test all possible permutations (24309) to obtain the true probability of rejecting the null hypothesis.

All other statistical inferences were drawn in the context of generalized linear mixed-effects (GLME) models that considered the intercept and factorial design (including interactions where appropriate) as fixed effects and the intercept and slope grouped per animal as random effects with possible correlation between them (as implemented in MATLAB, Mathworks, Natick, USA). The models were assumed to adhere to a normal distribution of the data (not formally tested) and were fitted using Maximum-Pseudo-Likelihood estimation methods where the covariance of the random effects was approximated using Cholesky parameterization. Statistical significance was set at α = 0.05, two-tailed, and estimated using conventional analyses of variance (ANOVA). For all parametric tests we report the test statistic (F), probability estimate (p), effect size (Cohen’s d), and the lower and upper limits of the 95% confidence interval (CI). For all plots, the central tendency across individual animals is derived directly from the group-PCA approach(Smith et al., 2014) or described as the mean across all animals (n = 3). Dispersion is described as the standard-error of the mean (SE).

Histology

In experiment 4, prior to histological examination, animals were anaesthetized with sodium pentorbarbitone and perfused with 90% saline and 10% formalin. A post-mortem examination of the surface of the brain was conducted prior to the brain extraction. The brains were then removed and placed in 10% sucrose formalin. The brains were blocked in the coronal plane at the level of the lunate sulcus. Each brain was cut in 50 μm coronal sections. Every tenth section, two adjacent sections were retained for analysis whereby one was stained with Cresyl Violet (Nissl body staining) and the other with Haemotoxylin and Eosin (H and E staining), in line with previous studies (Dallapiazza et al., 2018; Lee et al., 2016b).

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Greg Daubney for his assistance with the histological preparation and analysis, and Dr. Constantin Coussios and Dr. Robin Cleveland for their technical support. Funding for this research was provided by a Wellcome Trust UK Henry Dale fellowship (105651/Z/14/Z), Wellcome Trust Senior Investigator Award (WT100973AIA), Medical Research Council UK (MRC) programme grant (MR/P024955/1), MRC programme grant (G0902373), Wellcome Trust UK Grant (105238/Z/14/Z), Wellcome Trust Sir Henry Wellcome fellowship (103184/Z/13/Z), Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council UK (BBSRC) grant (BB/N019814/1), The Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO) grant (452-13-015), The Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging (203139/Z/16/Z), Bettencourt Schueller Foundation, and the Agence Nationale de la Recherche under the Future Investments program (ANR-10-EQPX-15).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Lennart Verhagen, Email: lennart.verhagen@psy.ox.ac.uk.

Joshua I Gold, University of Pennsylvania, United States.

Sven Bestmann, University College London, United Kingdom.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Wellcome 203139/Z/16/Z to Lennart Verhagen, Davide Folloni, Daria EA Jensen, Léa Roumazeilles, Miriam C Klein-Flügge, Rogier B Mars, Matthew FS Rushworth, Jerome Sallet.

  • Wellcome WT100973AIA to Lennart Verhagen, Miriam C Klein-Flügge, Matthew FS Rushworth.

  • Bettencourt Schueller Foundation to Cécile Gallea, Charlotte Constans, Harry Ahnine, Mathieu Santin, Stéphane Lehericy, Pierre Pouget, Jean-François Aubry.

  • Agence Nationale de la Recherche ANR-10-EQPX-15 to Cécile Gallea, Charlotte Constans, Harry Ahnine, Mathieu Santin, Stéphane Lehericy, Pierre Pouget, Jean-François Aubry.

  • Wellcome 105238/Z/14/Z to Davide Folloni.

  • Medical Research Council MR/P024955/1 to Davide Folloni, Miriam C Klein-Flügge, Matthew FS Rushworth, Jerome Sallet.

  • Wellcome 103184/Z/13/Z to Miriam C Klein-Flügge.

  • Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council BB/N019814/1 to Rogier B Mars.

  • Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek 452-13-015 to Rogier B Mars.

  • Medical Research Council G0902373 to Matthew FS Rushworth, Jerome Sallet.

  • Wellcome 105651/Z/14/Z to Jerome Sallet.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Writing—review and editing.

Resources, Writing—review and editing.

Software, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Resources, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Software, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Resources, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All procedures were conducted under project licenses from the United Kingdom (UK) Home Office in accordance with The Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986. In all cases they complied with the European Union guidelines (EU Directive 2010/63/EU).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.40541.014

Data availability

FSL can be downloaded from https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki. HCP Workbench can be downloaded from https://www. humanconnectome.org/software/connectome-workbench. For any information regarding MrCat please see http://www.rbmars.dds.nl/lab/toolbox.html; further inquiries can be directed to Lennart Verhagen (lennart.verhagen@psy.ox.ac.uk). All dedicated software tools are available at https://github.com/neuroecology/MrCat. Data contributing to this work have been publicly deposited at the Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging (https://git.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/lverhagen/offlinetus). For any inquiries regarding the data, please contact Jérôme Sallet (jerome.sallet@psy.ox.ac.uk).

The following dataset was generated:

Lennart Verhagen. 2019. offlineTUS. Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging. offlinetus

References

  1. Ahnine H, Constans C, Wattiez N, Tanter M, Aubry J-F, Pouget P. Long-Lasting modulation of visuomotor activity by repetitive focused ultrasound stimulation of frontal CortexInternational society for therapeutic ultrasound. Presented at the the 18th International Symposium on Therapeutic Ultrasound.2018. [Google Scholar]
  2. Airan RD, Butts Pauly K. Hearing out ultrasound neuromodulation. Neuron. 2018;98:875–877. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.05.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith S. FMRIB Technial Report TR07JA2. Oxford: FMRIB Centre; 2007. Non-Linear Registration Aka Spatial Normalisation. [Google Scholar]
  4. Aubry JF, Tanter M, Pernot M, Thomas JL, Fink M. Experimental demonstration of noninvasive transskull adaptive focusing based on prior computed tomography scans. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2003;113:84–93. doi: 10.1121/1.1529663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bates JF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Prefrontal connections of medial motor areas in the rhesus monkey. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1993;336:211–228. doi: 10.1002/cne.903360205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bestmann S, Walsh V. Transcranial electrical stimulation. Current Biology. 2017;27:R1258–R1262. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blackmore J, Shrivastava S, Sallet J, Butler CR. Ultrasound neuromodulation; a review of results, mechanisms and safety. Ultrasound in Medicine & Biology. 2018 doi: 10.1016/j.ultrasmedbio.2018.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Caspari N, Arsenault JT, Vandenberghe R, Vanduffel W. Functional similarity of medial superior parietal areas for Shift-Selective attention signals in humans and monkeys. Cerebral Cortex. 2018;28:2085–2099. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chau BK, Sallet J, Papageorgiou GK, Noonan MP, Bell AH, Walton ME, Rushworth MF. Contrasting roles for orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala in credit assignment and learning in macaques. Neuron. 2015;87:1106–1118. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Constans C, Deffieux T, Pouget P, Tanter M, Aubry JF. A 200-1380-kHz quadrifrequency focused ultrasound transducer for neurostimulation in rodents and primates: transcranial in vitro calibration and numerical study of the influence of skull cavity. IEEE Transactions on Ultrasonics, Ferroelectrics, and Frequency Control. 2017;64:717–724. doi: 10.1109/TUFFC.2017.2651648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Constans C, Mateo P, Tanter M, Aubry JF. Potential impact of thermal effects during ultrasonic neurostimulation: retrospective numerical estimation of temperature elevation in seven rodent setups. Physics in Medicine & Biology. 2018;63:025003. doi: 10.1088/1361-6560/aaa15c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cox BT, Kara S, Arridge SR, Beard PC. k-space propagation models for acoustically heterogeneous media: application to biomedical photoacoustics. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2007;121:3453–3464. doi: 10.1121/1.2717409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dallapiazza RF, Timbie KF, Holmberg S, Gatesman J, Lopes MB, Price RJ, Miller GW, Elias WJ. Noninvasive neuromodulation and thalamic mapping with low-intensity focused ultrasound. Journal of Neurosurgery. 2018;128:875–884. doi: 10.3171/2016.11.JNS16976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dayan E, Censor N, Buch ER, Sandrini M, Cohen LG. Noninvasive brain stimulation: from physiology to network dynamics and back. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:838–844. doi: 10.1038/nn.3422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Deffieux T, Younan Y, Wattiez N, Tanter M, Pouget P, Aubry JF. Low-intensity focused ultrasound modulates monkey visuomotor behavior. Current Biology. 2013;23:2430–2433. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Duck FA. Physical Properties of Tissues: A Comprehensive Reference Book. Academic Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  17. Dum RP, Strick PL. Frontal lobe inputs to the digit representations of the motor areas on the lateral surface of the hemisphere. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:1375–1386. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3902-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Elias WJ, Khaled M, Hilliard JD, Aubry JF, Frysinger RC, Sheehan JP, Wintermark M, Lopes MB. A magnetic resonance imaging, histological, and dose modeling comparison of focused ultrasound, radiofrequency, and gamma knife radiosurgery lesions in swine thalamus. Journal of Neurosurgery. 2013;119:307–317. doi: 10.3171/2013.5.JNS122327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Folloni D, Verhagen L, Mars RB, Fouragnan E, Constans C, Aubry J-F, Rushworth MFS, Sallet J. Manipulation of subcortical and deep cortical activity in the primate brain using transcranial focused ultrasound stimulation. Neuron. 2019 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fomenko A, Neudorfer C, Dallapiazza RF, Kalia SK, Lozano AM. Low-intensity ultrasound neuromodulation: an overview of mechanisms and emerging human applications. Brain Stimulation. 2018;11:1209–1217. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fouragnan E, Chau BKH, Folloni D, Kolling N, Verhagen L, Klein-Flügge M, Tankelevitch L, Papageorgiou GK, Aubry J-F, Sallet J, Rushworth MFS. The macaque anterior cingulate cortex translates counterfactual choice value into actual behavioral change. Nature Neuroscience. 2019 doi: 10.1101/336917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Galashan FO, Rempel HC, Meyer A, Gruber-Dujardin E, Kreiter AK, Wegener D. A new type of recording chamber with an easy-to-exchange microdrive array for chronic recordings in macaque monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;105:3092–3105. doi: 10.1152/jn.00508.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Galvan A, Caiola MJ, Albaugh DL. Advances in Optogenetic and chemogenetic methods to study brain circuits in non-human primates. Journal of Neural Transmission. 2018;125:547–563. doi: 10.1007/s00702-017-1697-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Geyer S, Matelli M, Luppino G, Zilles K. Functional neuroanatomy of the primate isocortical motor system. Anatomy and Embryology. 2000;202:443–474. doi: 10.1007/s004290000127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ghahremani M, Hutchison RM, Menon RS, Everling S. Frontoparietal functional connectivity in the common marmoset. Cerebral Cortex. 2017;27:3890–3905. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Glasser MF, Sotiropoulos SN, Wilson JA, Coalson TS, Fischl B, Andersson JL, Xu J, Jbabdi S, Webster M, Polimeni JR, Van Essen DC, Jenkinson M, WU-Minn HCP Consortium The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage. 2013;80:105–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Goss SA, Frizzell LA, Dunn F. Ultrasonic absorption and attenuation in mammalian tissues. Ultrasound in Medicine & Biology. 1979;5:181–186. doi: 10.1016/0301-5629(79)90086-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Guo H, Hamilton M, Offutt SJ, Gloeckner CD, Li T, Kim Y, Legon W, Alford JK, Lim HH. Ultrasound produces extensive brain activation via a cochlear pathway. Neuron. 2018;98:1020–1030. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Huang YZ, Edwards MJ, Rounis E, Bhatia KP, Rothwell JC. Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron. 2005;45:201–206. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage. 2002;17:825–841. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. NeuroImage. 2012;62:782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jenkinson M, Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Medical Image Analysis. 2001;5:143–156. doi: 10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Johansen-Berg H, Behrens TE, Robson MD, Drobnjak I, Rushworth MF, Brady JM, Smith SM, Higham DJ, Matthews PM. Changes in connectivity profiles define functionally distinct regions in human medial frontal cortex. PNAS. 2004;101:13335–13340. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403743101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kim H, Park MY, Lee SD, Lee W, Chiu A, Yoo SS. Suppression of EEG visual-evoked potentials in rats through neuromodulatory focused ultrasound. NeuroReport. 2015;26:211–215. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Krug K, Salzman CD, Waddell S. Understanding the brain by controlling neural activity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2015;370:20140201. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kubanek J, Shi J, Marsh J, Chen D, Deng C, Cui J. Ultrasound modulates ion channel currents. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:24170. doi: 10.1038/srep24170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kubanek J, Shukla P, Das A, Baccus SA, Goodman MB. Ultrasound elicits behavioral responses through mechanical effects on neurons and ion channels in a simple nervous system. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:3081–3091. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1458-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lee W, Kim H, Jung Y, Song IU, Chung YA, Yoo SS. Image-guided transcranial focused ultrasound stimulates human primary somatosensory cortex. Scientific Reports. 2015;5:8743. doi: 10.1038/srep08743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lee W, Kim HC, Jung Y, Chung YA, Song IU, Lee JH, Yoo SS. Transcranial focused ultrasound stimulation of human primary visual cortex. Scientific Reports. 2016a;6:34026. doi: 10.1038/srep34026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lee W, Lee SD, Park MY, Foley L, Purcell-Estabrook E, Kim H, Fischer K, Maeng LS, Yoo SS. Image-Guided focused Ultrasound-Mediated regional brain stimulation in sheep. Ultrasound in Medicine & Biology. 2016b;42:459–470. doi: 10.1016/j.ultrasmedbio.2015.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Legon W, Sato TF, Opitz A, Mueller J, Barbour A, Williams A, Tyler WJ. Transcranial focused ultrasound modulates the activity of primary somatosensory cortex in humans. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:322–329. doi: 10.1038/nn.3620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Legon W, Ai L, Bansal P, Mueller JK. Neuromodulation with single-element transcranial focused ultrasound in human thalamus. Human Brain Mapping. 2018;39:1995–2006. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lu MT, Preston JB, Strick PL. Interconnections between the prefrontal cortex and the premotor areas in the frontal lobe. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1994;341:375–392. doi: 10.1002/cne.903410308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Margulies DS, Vincent JL, Kelly C, Lohmann G, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Villringer A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Petrides M. Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. PNAS. 2009;106:20069–20074. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905314106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Margulies DS, Ghosh SS, Goulas A, Falkiewicz M, Huntenburg JM, Langs G, Bezgin G, Eickhoff SB, Castellanos FX, Petrides M, Jefferies E, Smallwood J. Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. PNAS. 2016;113:12574–12579. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608282113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Maris E, Oostenveld R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2007;164:177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mars RB, Jbabdi S, Sallet J, O'Reilly JX, Croxson PL, Olivier E, Noonan MP, Bergmann C, Mitchell AS, Baxter MG, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Tomassini V, Miller KL, Rushworth MF. Diffusion-weighted imaging tractography-based parcellation of the human parietal cortex and comparison with human and macaque resting-state functional connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:4087–4100. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5102-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mars RB, Sallet J, Neubert FX, Rushworth MF. Connectivity profiles reveal the relationship between brain areas for social cognition in human and monkey temporoparietal cortex. PNAS. 2013;110:10806–10811. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302956110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mars RB, Verhagen L, Gladwin TE, Neubert FX, Sallet J, Rushworth MF. Comparing brains by matching connectivity profiles. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;60:90–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. McLaren DG, Kosmatka KJ, Oakes TR, Kroenke CD, Kohama SG, Matochik JA, Ingram DK, Johnson SC. A population-average MRI-based atlas collection of the rhesus macaque. NeuroImage. 2009;45:52–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Munoz F, Aurup C, Konofagou EE, Ferrera VP. Modulation of brain function and behavior by focused ultrasound. Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports. 2018;5:153–164. doi: 10.1007/s40473-018-0156-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Naor O, Krupa S, Shoham S. Ultrasonic neuromodulation. Journal of Neural Engineering. 2016;13:031003. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/13/3/031003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Neubert FX, Mars RB, Thomas AG, Sallet J, Rushworth MF. Comparison of human ventral frontal cortex areas for cognitive control and language with areas in monkey frontal cortex. Neuron. 2014;81:700–713. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Neubert FX, Mars RB, Sallet J, Rushworth MF. Connectivity reveals relationship of brain areas for reward-guided learning and decision making in human and monkey frontal cortex. PNAS. 2015;112:E2695–E2704. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1410767112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. O'Reilly JX, Croxson PL, Jbabdi S, Sallet J, Noonan MP, Mars RB, Browning PG, Wilson CR, Mitchell AS, Miller KL, Rushworth MF, Baxter MG. Causal effect of disconnection lesions on interhemispheric functional connectivity in rhesus monkeys. PNAS. 2013;110:13982–13987. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1305062110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. O'Shea J, Johansen-Berg H, Trief D, Göbel S, Rushworth MF. Functionally specific reorganization in human premotor cortex. Neuron. 2007;54:479–490. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.04.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Papageorgiou GK, Sallet J, Wittmann MK, Chau BKH, Schüffelgen U, Buckley MJ, Rushworth MFS. Inverted activity patterns in ventromedial prefrontal cortex during value-guided decision-making in a less-is-more task. Nature Communications. 2017;8:1886. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01833-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Passingham RE, Stephan KE, Kötter R. The anatomical basis of functional localization in the cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2002;3:606–616. doi: 10.1038/nrn893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pennes HH. Analysis of tissue and arterial blood temperatures in the resting human forearm. Journal of Applied Physiology. 1948;1:93–122. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1948.1.2.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Petrides M, Pandya DN. Efferent association pathways from the rostral prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:11573–11586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2419-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pinton G, Aubry JF, Bossy E, Muller M, Pernot M, Tanter M. Attenuation, scattering, and absorption of ultrasound in the skull bone. Medical Physics. 2012;39:299–307. doi: 10.1118/1.3668316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Polanía R, Nitsche MA, Ruff CC. Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:174–187. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0054-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Preuss TM. Do rats have prefrontal cortex? the rose-woolsey-akert program reconsidered. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1995;7:1–24. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Prieto ML, Ömer O, Khuri-Yakub BT, Maduke MC. Dynamic response of model lipid membranes to ultrasonic radiation force. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e77115. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Pulkkinen A, Huang Y, Song J, Hynynen K. Simulations and measurements of transcranial low-frequency ultrasound therapy: skull-base heating and effective area of treatment. Physics in Medicine and Biology. 2011;56:4661–4683. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/56/15/003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Reveley C, Gruslys A, Ye FQ, Glen D, Samaha J, E Russ B, Saad Z, K Seth A, Leopold DA, Saleem KS. Three-Dimensional digital template atlas of the macaque brain. Cerebral Cortex. 2017;27:4463–4477. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sallet J, Mars RB, Noonan MP, Neubert FX, Jbabdi S, O'Reilly JX, Filippini N, Thomas AG, Rushworth MF. The organization of dorsal frontal cortex in humans and macaques. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:12255–12274. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5108-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Sato T, Shapiro MG, Tsao DY. Ultrasonic Neuromodulation Causes Widespread Cortical Activation via an Indirect Auditory Mechanism. Neuron. 2018;98:1031–1041. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Shen HH. Core concept: resting-state connectivity. PNAS. 2015;112:14115–14116. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1518785112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Shen K, Hutchison RM, Bezgin G, Everling S, McIntosh AR. Network structure shapes spontaneous functional connectivity dynamics. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:5579–5588. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4903-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human Brain Mapping. 2002;17:143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Smith SM, Hyvärinen A, Varoquaux G, Miller KL, Beckmann CF. Group-PCA for very large fMRI datasets. NeuroImage. 2014;101:738–749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Sternson SM, Roth BL. Chemogenetic tools to interrogate brain functions. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2014;37:387–407. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Strick PL, Dum RP, Picard N. Motor areas on the medial wall of the hemisphere. Novartis Foundation Symposium. 1998;218:64–75. doi: 10.1002/9780470515563.ch5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tufail Y, Matyushov A, Baldwin N, Tauchmann ML, Georges J, Yoshihiro A, Tillery SI, Tyler WJ. Transcranial pulsed ultrasound stimulates intact brain circuits. Neuron. 2010;66:681–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Tufail Y, Yoshihiro A, Pati S, Li MM, Tyler WJ. Ultrasonic neuromodulation by brain stimulation with transcranial ultrasound. Nature Protocols. 2011;6:1453–1470. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2011.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Tyler WJ, Lani SW, Hwang GM. Ultrasonic modulation of neural circuit activity. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2018;50:222–231. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2018.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Van Essen DC, Dierker DL. Surface-based and probabilistic atlases of primate cerebral cortex. Neuron. 2007;56:209–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Vanduffel W. Micro-, Meso- and Macro-Connectomics of the Brain. Springer; 2016. In-Vivo Connectivity in MonkeysMicro-, Meso- and Macro-Connectomics of the Brain, Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences. [Google Scholar]
  80. Verhagen L. How to Grasp a Ripe Tomato. Utrecht University; Donders Institute, Radboud University; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  81. Verhagen L. Github; 2019. https://github.com/neuroecology/MrCat [Google Scholar]
  82. Vincent JL, Patel GH, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Baker JT, Van Essen DC, Zempel JM, Snyder LH, Corbetta M, Raichle ME. Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature. 2007;447:83–86. doi: 10.1038/nature05758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Wang H, Kim M, Normoyle KP, Llano D. Thermal regulation of the Brain-An anatomical and physiological review for clinical neuroscientists. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2015;9 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Wattiez N, Constans C, Deffieux T, Daye PM, Tanter M, Aubry JF, Pouget P. Transcranial ultrasonic stimulation modulates single-neuron discharge in macaques performing an antisaccade task. Brain Stimulation. 2017;10:1024–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Winkler AM, Ridgway GR, Webster MA, Smith SM, Nichols TE. Permutation inference for the general linear model. NeuroImage. 2014;92:381–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Wise SP. Forward frontal fields: phylogeny and fundamental function. Trends in Neurosciences. 2008;31:599–608. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Yoo SS, Bystritsky A, Lee JH, Zhang Y, Fischer K, Min BK, McDannold NJ, Pascual-Leone A, Jolesz FA. Focused ultrasound modulates region-specific brain activity. NeuroImage. 2011;56:1267–1275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Younan Y, Deffieux T, Larrat B, Fink M, Tanter M, Aubry JF. Influence of the pressure field distribution in transcranial ultrasonic neurostimulation. Medical Physics. 2013;40:082902. doi: 10.1118/1.4812423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Zhang Y, Brady M, Smith S. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging. 2001;20:45–57. doi: 10.1109/42.906424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Sven Bestmann1
Reviewed by: Stefan Everling2, Jacek P Dmochowski3

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

[Editors’ note: the authors were asked to provide a plan for revisions before the editors issued a final decision. What follows is the editors’ letter requesting such plan.]

Thank you for sending your article entitled "Offline impact of transcranial focused ultrasound on cortical activation in primates" for peer review at eLife. Your article is being evaluated by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation is being overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Joshua Gold as the Senior Editor.

Given the list of essential revisions, including new experiments, the editors and reviewers invite you to respond within the next two weeks with an action plan and timetable for the completion of the additional work. We plan to share your responses with the reviewers and then issue a binding recommendation.

The consensus was clear that this is potentially important work, very well conducted overall and potentially of high impact to the field of non-invasive brain stimulation. However, there were some potentially more serious concerns, which are detailed below. Therefore, before making the final recommendation, we suggest possible actions that you could take to remediate these concerns.

While this seems an unusual step, eLife fortunately allows for such a more informal route. We think this will help to discuss a realistic plan for a revision, and to clarify any misunderstandings prior to making an editorial decision (wrt the points below).

Please note that while we outline possible actions below, there may well be other ways to address these concerns, hopefully within a realistic timeline. We wouldn't sweat over the usual two months timeline should you feel that more time was needed.

Comments:

If our understanding is correct, then the main concern relates to the experimental design and disambiguating the effect of TUS with inter-animal differences in functional connectivity.

The comparison of brain connectivity of the stimulated animals with the brain connectivity of other non-stimulated animals raises concern about the contribution of inter-individual variability in the connectivity as a potential confound to the findings. Differences between stimulated and non-stimulated animals would seem to reveal variability comprising both the effect of TUS as well as any across-individual differences. The concern is that the latter can be substantial. Related, in the case of the TUS conditions, fMRI data collection began only after completion of the TUS train.

Specifically, our concern arises from the description of the procedures, stating that 'three animals participated in experiments 1 and 2, including the control condition (all males, mean age and weight at time of scan: control, 5.5 years, 10.2 kg; SMA TUS, 5.6 years, 10.7 kg; FPC TUS, 6.1 years, 11.8 kg).' This implies that each animal took part in one condition. Yet it is also stated that the same animals 'took part in experiments 1 and 2', and 'The same three animals participated in experiments 1 and 2, and a control experiment conducted in the absence of TUS.' At the same time, it is stated that 'while experiments 1 and 2 followed a within-subject design, for experiment 3 we conducted a between-subject analysis where the subjects in the two experiments differed in age.' – this would imply that in E1 and E2 a within-subject comparison was done (though it was not clear whether the control comparison was within or between animals). If you could provide a clearer description of the exact procedures and comparisons, this would greatly help with the discussion.

Therefore, to which degree could the results arise from significant across-individual differences (as opposed to TUS + across-subject variability)? One observation that may point to this being at least a possibility is that the connectivity of the stimulated animals does not seem to be converging to that of the non-stimulated animals even after 1.5 hours.

It would seem that a within-subject comparison of the functional connectivity before, during (if possible) and after TUS is necessary. This procedure could then be repeated for non-stimulated animals to control for the effect of time on connectivity, i.e. compare pre-TUS FC maps for each animal and then post-TUS or post control period maps, for the two target sites. This would address whether the effects that were observed were significantly larger than those that would be expected from inter-animal differences.

However, we are aware that repeating the study in full with a within-subject design might be unfeasible, and would therefore discuss a realistic way forward given everyone agreed about the relevance of your work and its likely impact. We would not necessarily expect another large dataset, and a small set of animals may be able to provide reassurance regarding the point above.

And there may also be other ways to address the concerns. Would a 'normative' dataset, to which your group might have access to, provide an alternative route, e.g. resting state datasets from other monkey MRI projects? We discussed whether comparison of your animals with such a dataset potentially address the concerns, e.g. by showing how each animal differs from such a normative dataset? As mentioned above, we're happy to hear suggestions given the more substantial expertise on these type of data in your group.

There would be other comments should we proceed, but we first wanted to hear your thoughts and how you may be able to address the concerns.

[Editors’ note: formal revisions were requested, following approval of the authors’ plan of action.]

Thank you for sending your article entitled "Offline impact of transcranial focused ultrasound on cortical activation in primates" for peer review at eLife. Your article is being evaluated by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation is being overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Joshua Gold as the Senior Editor. Two of the reviewers have agreed to share their identity: Stefan Everling (Reviewer 1) and Jacek Dmochowski (Reviewer 3).

Thanks for the quick reply, which has greatly clarified things. We're happy to invite a revision based on these, and are confident you can provide these within the usual timeframe.

Perhaps most critically then would be to clarify the procedures throughout (after all, five people could not confidently work this out), leaving no ambiguity to exactly what was done. The key point of reviewer 3 (and related reviewer 1 and the two editors) can otherwise be ignored. Elsewhere, I am confident that you will find the requested revisions are straightforward to address.

Other points you should address:

In addition, we'd like you to provide additional detail on the procedures, as raised by reviewer 3.

Effect sizes remained a concern (see comments by reviewer 1 and reviewer 3). These may be resolved by revising the figures, but addressing the concern about the procedures for determining and analysing connectivity fingerprints would be helpful. Please also clarify the concern regarding the reporting of the statistical procedures.

Some additional detail on exactly how the fingerprints of stimulated animals were compared to non-stimulated animals (i.e., how statistical significance of the fingerprints was computed) would be helpful. For example, the Materials and methods section merely refers to a paper (Mars et al., 2016), which refers to another paper (Nichols and Holmes, 2007) on the important issue of how the permutations were performed.

For some analyses multiple tests were conducted. What procedures for corrections for multiple comparisons were in place throughout?

The sizes of the correlation vectors being compared do not seem to be provided either in the text or figure captions, but would be useful to see.

The choice of frontal polar cortex seems valid as a control because it has a very distinct functional connectivity from SMA. Is the SNR comparable for these two regions? To rule out the possibility that differences between FPC and SMA are simply related to SNR differences, could you should show temporal SNR maps and quantify these differences, if any?

It is virtually impossible to appreciate the differences in the FC maps in Figure 2. The addition of difference maps might help here (e.g. TUA over SMA minus control).

Regarding the acoustic parameter, 30 ms burst in every 100 ms (i.e. pulse repetition of 10 Hz; i.e. duty cycle of 30%), seems unconventional, even though they appear to be effective here. 10 Hz is well below the audible frequency, which helped to disregard effects from auditory involvement. However, an additional rationale for the choice of parameter (or brief summary of the literature) will be helpful.

With regard to the acoustic and Thermal modelling, the conservative approach taken here may over-estimate the actual temperature rise in the skull and the brain. Although additional experiments are not necessary, realistic estimation of temperature rise using phantoms would be desirable. If access to such data exist we would encourage you to include these.

In the histological analysis, a rather limited staining approach is used (i.e. Cresyl Violet and H&E) to evaluate the sonicated brain tissue. Although not absolutely needed in present study, there are other types of histological analyses to assess the presence of tissue damages (such as apoptotic activities or ischemic damages). At the very least, please reflect on these limitations in the Discussion.

Transcranial focused ultrasound was abridged to 'TUS'. As the method utilized the focused version (i.e. FUS), the change in acronym to tFUS might be more intuitive.

Inclusion of some relevant published work may be helpful, at your discretion: (1) fMRI-tFUS human study(ies) and (2) recent findings regarding the presence of long-term neuromodulatory effects of tFUS in rodents.

Reviewer 1:

Verhagen and colleague used resting-state fMRI in macaque monkeys to investigate the effects of transcranial focused ultrasound (FUS) on functional brain connectivity. The study appears to be carefully conducted and it reveals some important aspects of FUS. First it shows that FUS increases local functional connectivity and mainly decreases distant functional connectivity. Second it demonstrates that the effects last for at least 2 hours. Overall the effects appear to quite small but they seem to be consistent across animals (Fg. 3c). I have a few major points that need to be addressed.

1) I assume the authors picked frontal polar cortex because it has a very distinct functional connectivity from SMA. Based on my experience, the SNR can be quite low for this anterior area. To rule out the possibility that differences between FPC and SMA are simply related to SNR differences, the authors should show temporal SNR maps.

2) I found it virtually impossible to see any differences in the FC maps in Figure 2. The addition of difference maps might help here (e.g. TUA over SMA minus control).

3) It is not clear from the paper how often each animal was actually scanned. Did the authors only perform one SMA and one FPC stimulation session per monkey or did they conduct multiple sessions?

Reviewer 2:

Verhagan and colleagues reported neuromodulatory effects that are present beyond the transcranial FUS (tFUS)-mediated brain stimulation in supplementary motor area and frontal polar cortex using functional MRI on non-human primate (NHP) model. This is the first study reported in NHP that shows the presence of long-term, neuromodulatory effects conferred by tFUS. In addition, the study found that the effects are entirely independent from the confounding effects from potential acoustic startle response (ARS), which is allegedly believed to be a part of the ultrasound-mediated brain stimulation (at least in rodent studies). Due to these two major findings, along with very robust and rigorous methods used in this study (including the PCA analysis regarding the signals originating from the meningeal compartment), the data and their interpretation are convincing. Publication of this report would be timely for scientific community and will provide new perspectives regarding the acoustic neuromodulation. This study also provides strong early evidence that the tFUS, given at the level, did not induce apparent damages to the brain tissue. The report deserves expedited publication.

Here are a few suggestions, with moderate importance, which may improve the manuscript.

1) Regarding the acoustic parameter, 30 ms burst in every 100 ms (i.e. pulse repetition of 10 Hz; i.e. duty cycle of 30%), is bit unconventional, but appeared to be effective. 10 Hz is well below the audible frequency, which helped the authors to disregard effects from auditory involvement. Additional rationale for the choice of parameter or brief review on literatures on the parameters in the Discussion section will be helpful.

2) In regard to the acoustic and Thermal modeling, the study seems to take conservative approach, which may over-estimate the actual temperature rise in the skull and the brain. Although additional experiments are not necessary, realistic estimation of temperature rise using phantoms will help to ascertain the non-thermal effects of tFUS.

3) In histological analysis, authors performed a rather limited staining (i.e. Cresyl Violet and H&E) to evaluate the sonicated brain tissue. Although not absolutely needed in present study, there are other types of histological analyses to assess the presence of tissue damages (such as apoptotic activities or ischemic damages), and authors are advised to reflect these limitations in the Discussion.

4) Transcranial focused ultrasound's acronym was defined as 'TUS'. As the method utilized the focused version (i.e. FUS), the change in acronym to tFUS is strongly suggested.

5) Although the detailed references are given in the technical methods section, surprisingly, only a few previous investigations on the matter of main subjects (fMRI and long-term effects of tFUS) are given. Inclusion of (1) fMRI-tFUS study(ies) among humans and (2) recent findings regarding the presence of long-term neuromodulatory effects of tFUS on rodents (a simple Pubmed search will do) is advised for general audience.

Reviewer 3:

Summary:

In this paper the authors investigate a relatively new form of neuromodulation using ultrasound (Transcranial Ultrasound Stimulation or TUS). TUS was applied over the supplementary motor area (SMA) and frontal polar cortex (FPC) in primates, and resting-state functional connectivity was assessed at least 20 minutes following the stimulation. Control animals were scanned but without first receiving TUS. The authors argue that the "connectivity fingerprint", a measure of the connection strength between the stimulated area (SMA or FPC) and other brain regions was altered by TUS. In particular, they argue that regions with which the seed connects to strongly have their connections increased, while regions weakly connected are further weakened. They perform a replication study and also argue for changes in the meningeal (CSF) compartments of the brain.

Major concerns:

Inadequacy of control condition. In my view, the paper suffers from a suboptimally chosen control. Namely, the authors compare the brain connectivity of the stimulated animals with the brain connectivity of other non-stimulated animals. This selection means that inter-individual variability in the connectivity is not controlled for and has the potential to confound the findings. Given two animals A and B, animal A receives stimulation and animal B does not. Comparing A and B on any outcome measure leads to total variability comprising both the effect of TUS as well as any across-individual differences (sorry if I am stating what is obvious, I just want to be clear). The nature of functional connectivity and its shaping by experience could seemingly lead to significant across-individual differences which have not been examined. The authors have not provided data arguing that the observed differences (i.e., from TUS + across-subject variability) are significantly larger than those from across-individual variability alone. One observation that emphasizes this concern is that the connectivity of the stimulated animals does not seem to be converging to that of the non-stimulated animals even after 1.5 hours (see Figure 3F, where the difference between red and blue over M1-SPL-MCC is actually larger than at 1 hour).

Lack of rigour in statistical procedures. There is a significant absence of necessary detail regarding how exactly the fingerprints of stimulated animals were compared to non-stimulated animals (i.e., how statistical significance of the fingerprints was computed). The Materials and methods section refers to a paper (Mars et al., 2016), which refers to another paper (Nichols and Holmes, 2007) on the important issue of how the permutations were performed. Another issue is the apparent lack of correction for multiple comparisons, even though it seems that multiple tests were conducted. The sizes of the correlation vectors being compared are not provided either in the text or figure captions. I also have concerns about how the areas that comprise the fingerprint of a given region were selected, and how this selection influences the results of the statistical tests.

Magnitude of the effect on connectivity fingerprint does not appear to be very large. I list this as a major concern because eLife aims to publish research of the "highest scientific standards and importance." Combined with the issues described above, it is my opinion that, taken as a whole, the data is not convincing enough to justify the authors' conclusions that TUS altered functional connectivity in the macaque brain following stimulation.

I believe that a more suitable approach would have been to compare functional connectivity before, during (if possible) and after TUS within an animal. This procedure would then be repeated for non-stimulated animals to control for the effect of time on connectivity. It would also have been more convincing to observe effects when comparing the entire functional connectivity matrices (instead of just the pruned fingerprints, especially since little information was provided as to how the pruning was achieved).

In summary, I commend the authors for undertaking what were clearly technically challenging and involved experiments. They have also done a good job in communicating the results and preparing nice figures. While I believe that TUS has promise as a technique for a neuromodulation, I do not feel that the data presented here are strong enough to warrant publication in eLife. I am sorry that I cannot be more positive in my review.

eLife. 2019 Feb 12;8:e40541. doi: 10.7554/eLife.40541.019

Author response


[Editors’ note: what follows is the authors’ plan to address the revisions.]

Comments:

If our understanding is correct, then the main concern relates to the experimental design and disambiguating the effect of TUS with inter-animal differences in functional connectivity.

The comparison of brain connectivity of the stimulated animals with the brain connectivity of other non-stimulated animals raises concern about the contribution of inter-individual variability in the connectivity as a potential confound to the findings. Differences between stimulated and non-stimulated animals would seem to reveal variability comprising both the effect of TUS as well as any across-individual differences. The concern is that the latter can be substantial. Related, in the case of the TUS conditions, fMRI data collection began only after completion of the TUS train.

Specifically, our concern arises from the description of the procedures, stating that 'three animals participated in experiments 1 and 2, including the control condition (all males, mean age and weight at time of scan: control, 5.5 years, 10.2 kg; SMA TUS, 5.6 years, 10.7 kg; FPC TUS, 6.1 years, 11.8 kg).' This implies that each animal took part in one condition. Yet it is also stated that the same animals 'took part in experiments 1 and 2', and 'The same three animals participated in experiments 1 and 2, and a control experiment conducted in the absence of TUS.' At the same time, it is stated that 'while experiments 1 and 2 followed a within-subject design, for experiment 3 we conducted a between-subject analysis where the subjects in the two experiments differed in age.' – this would imply that in E1 and E2 a within-subject comparison was done (though it was not clear whether the control comparison was within or between animals). If you could provide a clearer description of the exact procedures and comparisons, this would greatly help with the discussion.

Therefore, to which degree could the results arise from significant across-individual differences (as opposed to TUS + across-subject variability)? One observation that may point to this being at least a possibility is that the connectivity of the stimulated animals does not seem to be converging to that of the non-stimulated animals even after 1.5 hours.

It would seem that a within-subject comparison of the functional connectivity before, during (if possible) and after TUS is necessary. This procedure could then be repeated for non-stimulated animals to control for the effect of time on connectivity, i.e. compare pre-TUS FC maps for each animal and then post-TUS or post control period maps, for the two target sites. This would address whether the effects that were observed were significantly larger than those that would be expected from inter-animal differences.

However, we are aware that repeating the study in full with a within-subject design might be unfeasible, and would therefore discuss a realistic way forward given everyone agreed about the relevance of your work and its likely impact. We would not necessarily expect another large dataset, and a small set of animals may be able to provide reassurance regarding the point above.

And there may also be other ways to address the concerns. Would a 'normative' dataset, to which your group might have access to, provide an alternative route, e.g. resting state datasets from other monkey MRI projects? We discussed whether comparison of your animals with such a dataset potentially address the concerns, e.g. by showing how each animal differs from such a normative dataset? As mentioned above, we're happy to hear suggestions given the more substantial expertise on these type of data in your group.

There would be other comments should we proceed, but we first wanted to hear your thoughts and how you may be able to address the concerns.

Thank you very much for considering and reviewing our manuscript. We truly appreciate the opportunity to respond to one of the main comments before a binding recommendation can be made.

We understand that a critical sentence concerning this comment is our statement that: "three animals participated in experiments 1 and 2, including the control condition”. We understand that our phrasing might have given the impression that each animal participated in only one condition. In fact, each of the three animals participated in all three of the conditions. If we name the three animals A, B, and C, we could state: “Three animals (A, B, and C) participated in experiment 1. The same three animals (A, B, and C) participated in experiment 2. Lastly, the same three animals (A, B, and C) were scanned following the control conditions against which the results of experiment 1 and 2 are compared. Accordingly, experiments 1 and 2 are within-subject designs with n=3.”

One suggestion arising from the reviews would be to acquire data before and after TUS. In fact, that is exactly what we have done. We first acquired the control condition, after which the animals underwent TUS over SMA (experiment 1), followed by TUS over FPC at least one week later (experiment 2). This also entails that while the effects of TUS did not converge back to baseline within 2 hours, they were not permanent either, as evidenced by the fact that effects observed immediately following TUS over SMA did not carry over to the next session.

In summary, this means that individual differences cannot be mediating apparent effects of interest. For example on the in Figure 2, we compare the effect, on SMA, of TUS to SMA or the control site FPC. The data come from the same individuals but only the stimulated area exhibits the effect of interest. On the right side of Figure 2, however, everything is swapped round and now the only effect of interest is from the FPC when FPC is stimulated. Figure 3 is organized to a similar principle.

Further evidence that effects are not due to individual differences comes from experiment 3) in which we show that we can reproduce the same FPC effects in a second group of animals (let’s call these animals D, E, and F).

In the same light, it was suggested to acquire data not only before and after TUS, as we have done, but perhaps also during TUS, if possible. This is indeed technically feasible. However, placing the TUS transducer and coupling cone close to the head and stimulating while inside the scanner bore introduces MR artefacts. There is also a possibility that the TUS protocol induces audible skull resonance at frequencies caused by low-frequency modulations of the ultrasonic sound wave. To exclude these artefacts completely, we have opted for an offline approach.

For completeness, we would like to highlight that our validation experiment (experiment 3, with TUS over FPC), is indeed a between-subject comparison: three new animals (D, E, and F) underwent TUS over FPC and their results are compared to the control condition of animals A, B, and C. In essence, here the control condition of animals A, B, and C acts as a normative dataset against which D, E, and F are compared.

To conclude, we apologise for any misunderstanding that our wording might have caused. However, we would like to highlight that we have in fact performed a full within-subject comparison, as suggested by the reviews, and that inter-animal differences could therefore not have contributed to the TUS-induced differences that we observed.

[Editors’ notes: the authors’ response after being formally invited to submit a revised submission follows.]

Thanks for the quick reply, which has greatly clarified things. We're happy to invite a revision based on these, and are confident you can provide these within the usual timeframe.

Thank you for asking for this additional information in such a straightforward manner. We hope we have addressed any remaining queries but if there is anything that is still unclear please get in touch. In the responses below we have attempted to address each of the remaining concerns expressed by the reviewers.

In addition to noting some of the points that you raised in your previous letter we also noted in the reviewer comments several points dealing with the pitfalls of undertaking multiple statistical comparisons and a suggestion that this may have occurred when we examined connectivity changes across conditions. Although we reply to these points in much more detail below, it may be worth stating briefly here that we avoid such potential problems by using single indices – cosine similarity metrics – to summarize entire fingerprints of connectivity. It is these single indices that we employ for statistical inference.

Perhaps most critically then would be to clarify the procedures throughout (after all, five people could not confidently work this out), leaving no ambiguity to exactly what was done. The key point of reviewer3 (and related reviewer1 and the two editors) can otherwise be ignored. Elsewhere, I am confident that you will find the requested revisions are straightforward to address.

We would like to thank you for helping us clarify a misunderstanding. To re-iterate, for the main analyses in this study we employed a within-subject design whereby all three animals participated in all conditions. In fact, only in experiment 3, where we perform a validation of experiment 2 in a new set of animals, and in experiment 4, where we perform terminal experiments on the animals, did we employ between-subject comparisons.

To further clarify our study design, experimental timeline, pre-processing pipeline, and analyses strategies we have now included a new figure in the revised manuscript, Figure 1.

We have further clarified our experimental design in the revised manuscript as follows:

Abstract

“In a within-subject design, we observe regionally specific TUS effects for two medial frontal brain regions – supplementary motor area and frontal polar cortex. Independently of these site-specific effects, TUS also induced signal changes in the meningeal compartment.”

Introduction

“Here we focused on the effects of TUS outlasting the stimulation period, investigating the impact of 40 s trains of TUS on measurements of neural activity in three macaque monkeys provided by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) up to 2 hours after stimulation (Figure 1, top panel). […]We also validated the results of experiment 2 in a new set of three different animals (experiment 3).”

Figure 3 legend. “Coupling of activity between stimulated areas and the rest of the brain in experiments 1 (SMA) and 2 (FPC). The left panels show activity coupling between SMA and the rest of the brain in the control state (a), after SMA TUS (b), and after FPC TUS (c). […] Connectivity seed regions are indicated with black asterisks. Key anatomical features are labelled in panel (a): pos, parieto-occipital sulcus; cal, calcarine sulcus; cgs, cingulate sulcus; ps, principal sulcus; as, arcuate sulcus; cs, central sulcus; ips, intraparietal sulcus; sts, superior temporal sulcus; ls, lunate sulcus.”

Discussion

“In experiments 1 and 2, adopting a within-subject design with three animals, we found that TUS application produced different effects when applied to different brain regions: the SMA, a part of motor association cortex, and FPC a part of granular prefrontal cortex (Figure 3, 4).”

Materials and methods

“Subject details

For this study, six healthy male macaques (Macaca mulatta) were stimulated with transcranial focused ultrasound and scanned to acquire resting state functional magnetic resonance (rs-fMRI) and anatomical MR images. […] In all cases they complied with the European Union guidelines (EU Directive 2010/63/EU).”

Figure 3—figure supplement 1 legend. “Specific patterns of change in the coupling of activity between stimulated areas and the rest of the brain were replicated in experiments 2 and 3. […] All conventions as in Figure 3. Lighter coloured error bands indicate the standard-error of the mean across individual animals.”

Other points you should address:

In addition, we'd like you to provide additional detail on the procedures, as raised by reviewer 3.

Effect sizes remained a concern (see comments by reviewer 1 and reviewer 3). These may be resolved by revising the figures, but addressing the concern about the procedures for determining and analysing connectivity fingerprints would be helpful. Please also clarify the concern regarding the reporting of the statistical procedures.

Some additional detail on exactly how the fingerprints of stimulated animals were compared to non-stimulated animals (i.e., how statistical significance of the fingerprints was computed) would be helpful. For example, the Materials and methods section merely refers to a paper (Mars et al., 2016), which refers to another paper (Nichols and Holmes, 2007) on the important issue of how the permutations were performed.

In the revised manuscript we have attempted to make our analysis approach clearer and we have given particular emphasis to the statistical inference drawn on the connectional fingerprints.

Materials and methods

“Importantly, rather than examining activity coupling between the seed area and each of the fingerprint ROIs in turn and risking potential false positive results, we compared the overall pattern of coupling using the method devised by Mars and colleagues (Mars et al., 2016); non-parametric permutation tests were performed on cosine similarity metrics summarizing pairs of fingerprints (SMA TUS versus control and FPC TUS versus control). […] The relatively small number of TUS sites, animals, and fMRI runs allowed us to exhaustively test all possible permutations (24309) to obtain the true probability of rejecting the null hypothesis.”

In addition, we have clarified the origin of the ROIs used and the rationale for selecting them. The revised text reads as follows:

Materials and methods

“We report on the whole-brain functional coupling of the stimulation sites in the control state and after TUS, adopting a seed-based correlation analysis approach. […] Lastly, to test for specificity of the TUS effects, we included SMA as a target for the FPC fingerprint and FPC as a target for the SMA fingerprint.”

To construct regions-of-interest (ROI) for SMA and FPC circles of 4 mm radius were drawn on the cortical surface around the point closest to the average stimulation coordinate (Figure 2), in both the left and the right hemisphere. The same procedure was used to define other bilateral cortical regions of interest, based on literature coordinates (Mars et al., 2011; Neubert et al., 2015, 2014; Sallet et al., 2013), to serve as seeds for connectivity analyses (A1, Figure 4G-I; POp, Figure 6, Figure 6—figure supplement 1) or targets for the fingerprint analyses (Figure 4). Apart from the stimulation sites and primary auditory cortex, all ROIs were selected based on their known anatomical connectivity, of relevance because they are known to be distinctively strongly or distinctively weakly connected with the stimulation sites.

For some analyses multiple tests were conducted. What procedures for corrections for multiple comparisons were in place throughout?

In the revised manuscript we put additional emphasis on the manner in which our statistical approach avoids the potential pitfalls associated with making multiple comparisons. The revised text reads as follows:

Materials and methods

“To report on the full extent of the TUS effects and the simple effects that drive any observed differences we reproduce the whole-brain functional connectivity maps unthresholded for the control, SMA TUS, and FPC TUS conditions separately. […]Importantly, rather than examining activity coupling between the seed area and each of the fingerprint ROIs in turn and risking potential false positive results, we compared the overall pattern of coupling using the method devised by Mars and colleagues (Mars et al., 2016); non-parametric permutation tests were performed on cosine similarity metrics summarizing pairs of fingerprints (SMA TUS versus control and FPC TUS versus control).”

Again we note, as we have noted above, how the precise nature of the approach used precludes multiple comparisons. The cosine metric provides a single index summarizing each fingerprint. Comparisons between fingerprints are not based on multiple comparisons of each point on each fingerprint but instead by a single comparison between the cosine metrics. We note the relevant section of the manuscript again below:

“Importantly, rather than examining activity coupling between the seed area and each of the fingerprint ROIs in turn and risking potential false positive results, we compared the overall pattern of coupling using the method devised by Mars and colleagues (Mars et al., 2016); non-parametric permutation tests were performed on cosine similarity metrics summarizing pairs of fingerprints (SMA TUS versus control and FPC TUS versus control). Each connectional fingerprint can be represented as a multidimensional vector, with the number of dimensions corresponding to the number of target ROIs in the fingerprint. The cosine similarity metric quantifies the angle between two of such multi-dimensional vectors. As such, it takes into account the shape of the fingerprint irrespective of its mean amplitude and results in a single metric per pair of fingerprints, negating the necessity for correcting for multiple comparisons across fingerprint targets.”

The sizes of the correlation vectors being compared do not seem to be provided either in the text or figure captions, but would be useful to see.

In the revised manuscript we now specify the exact length of the fMRI timeseries (correlation vectors) in volumes.

Materials and methods

“To represent subject effects, the timeseries from the three runs were concatenated to create a single timeseries of 3×800-5=2385 volumes per animal per intervention (control, SMA TUS, FPC TUS). To represent group effects the run-concatenated timeseries of all animals were combined using a group-PCA approach resulting in a series of 200 volumes representing the principal components (Smith et al., 2014).”

The choice of frontal polar cortex seems valid as a control because it has a very distinct functional connectivity from SMA. Is the SNR comparable for these two regions? To rule out the possibility that differences between FPC and SMA are simply related to SNR differences, could you should show temporal SNR maps and quantify these differences, if any?

The temporal signal-to-noise of the BOLD signal measured from a given brain area could impact the estimated coupling strength (Pearson’s r) of that area with other brain regions. This is an undesirable and common confound in functional connectivity analyses. Nonetheless, main differences in tSNR, either between brain areas or between control and TUS, cannot straightforwardly drive the TUS effects observed in our study. Namely, the observed effects are not only specific to the stimulation condition (control vs. TUS), but also to the site (SMA or FPC). As such, any potential effect on tSNR would have to exhibit an interaction effect of TUS and site. For example, we don’t just compare the impact of SMA TUS on SMA’s connectivity with FPC TUS’s impact on FPC’s connectivity; we also compare SMA TUS’s impact on SMA connectivity with FPC TUS’s impact on SMA connectivity and examine SMA connectivity at baseline. In the case of FPC, we compare FPC TUS’s impact on FPC connectivity with SMA TUS’s impact on FPC connectivity and FPC connectivity at baseline.

Moreover, even in the hypothetical case that such an interaction were present, this would result in global change of the correlation values estimated for each stimulation site. It would not be able to explain how after TUS the coupling of a seed region might be enhanced with some areas but disrupted with others.

Accordingly, to exclude the possibility of a three-way interaction of stimulation condition x site x other-brain-areas we quantified the temporal variability of the BOLD signal at every point in the brain, including the SMA and FPC, in all three TUS conditions (control, SMA, FPC). Please note that the mean offset of the signal is largely uninformative in BOLD fMRI and detrimental for correlational analyses. We have therefore quantified the temporal standard deviation of the signal instead. This reveals a pattern of variation across the brain that reflects basic properties of the data acquisition and signal (e.g. proximity to the RF receive coils) but importantly this pattern is not modulated by TUS and is not meaningfully different between SMA and FPC regions.

We have included these new analyses in the revised manuscript in the Results section and as a figure supplement:

Results

“While BOLD fMRI cannot provide an absolute measure of neural activity, we can characterize how homogeneous the activation signal is within the stimulated region, as quantified by the coupling strength of the signal at each point in the stimulated region of interest to all other points in that region. […]This suggests that TUS leaves intact basic haemodynamics and neurophysiology and instead has a circumscribed and specific impact on the coupling of the stimulated region with the rest of the brain.”

It is virtually impossible to appreciate the differences in the FC maps in Figure 2. The addition of difference maps might help here (e.g. TUA over SMA minus control).

In the revised manuscript we have now included an additional figure (Figure 5) showing the difference maps for SMA TUS minus control and for FPC TUS minus control. In this figure we have also drawn our ROIs as used in the fingerprint analyses. This figure shows the direction and extent of the TUS induced changes to whole-brain coupling and highlights that the effects were neither limited to nor maximal in the ROIs selected for the fingerprint analyses.

The revised text reads as follows:

Results

“Following ultrasound stimulation, SMA changed its coupling with the sensorimotor system, anterior and posterior cingulate, anterior temporal, inferior parietal, and prefrontal cortex (Figure 3B). […]This pattern not only emerges from the fingerprint analyses, but constitutes a principle evident across the brain, as illustrated by whole-brain differential SMA-connectivity maps of the effect of SMA TUS (Figure 5).”

Experiment 2, TUS modulation of FPC connectivity

[…]

“These results are apparent in the whole brain functional connectivity maps for the FPC region (Figure 3, compare panels D and F, representative changes highlighted by dashed black circles) and on the whole brain differential connectivity maps in Figure 5 (panels D and F).”

Regarding the acoustic parameter, 30 ms burst in every 100 ms (i.e. pulse repetition of 10 Hz; i.e. duty cycle of 30%), seems unconventional, even though they appear to be effective here. 10 Hz is well below the audible frequency, which helped to disregard effects from auditory involvement. However, an additional rationale for the choice of parameter (or brief summary of the literature) will be helpful.

The sonication protocol we employed in this study was based on a preliminary work on sustained ultrasonic neuromodulation (Ahnine et al., 2018). This protocol was similar to the protocol used by Dallapiazza and colleagues when observing short-lived offline effects following ultrasound targeted at swine thalamus (Dallapiazza et al., 2017). This protocol is characterised by a repetitive structure with a relatively high rate of acoustic energy deposition. Previous work has suggested that ultrasound protocols with a higher acoustic intensity (Isppa > 5 W/cm2) and a higher duty cycle (> 5%) might be most likely to elicit neuromodulation outlasting the sonication period (Kim et al., 2015; Yoo et al., 2011). We have taken this into consideration when choosing our parameters, while aiming to maintain safe stimulation intensities that do not lead to excessive thermal modulation or microstructural changes.

We have now included these considerations in the revised text:

Introduction

“To date ultrasonic applications are primarily focused on direct ‘online’ effects. […] We have built on such protocols when designing the current experiment.”

With regard to the acoustic and Thermal modelling, the conservative approach taken here may over-estimate the actual temperature rise in the skull and the brain. Although additional experiments are not necessary, realistic estimation of temperature rise using phantoms would be desirable. If access to such data exist we would encourage you to include these.

In this study the thermal modelling serves to provide an informed estimate whether the employed sonication protocol might have caused thermal damage and whether thermal effects could potentially underlie the observed neuromodulatory effects. We feel that for these purposes a rigorous and perhaps somewhat conservative approach might be most appropriate. While at the time of the study we did not have access to realistic phantoms of macaque heads we agree that this would be a worthwhile endeavour to further characterise the current protocol.

We have revised the text as follows:

Discussion

“Similarly, the thermal modelling approach we adopted here was designed to estimate an informed upper-bound on potential thermal effects, but when further developing this protocol additional simulation validations – for example based on phantom measurements – might be informative.”

In the histological analysis, a rather limited staining approach is used (i.e. Cresyl Violet and H&E) to evaluate the sonicated brain tissue. Although not absolutely needed in present study, there are other types of histological analyses to assess the presence of tissue damages (such as apoptotic activities or ischemic damages). At the very least, please reflect on these limitations in the Discussion.

Our histological analyses were based on previous publications that employed NISSL H&E staining to identify microhaemorrhages following either high-intensity or repetitive low-intensity ultrasound stimulation (Dallapiazza et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2016b). By conducting a similar analysis, we were able to show that no similar tissue damage was associated with our 40s stimulation protocol. To further characterize potential confounding effects, we inspected T1w images for possible sign of oedema. T1w images were collected immediately after the 3 runs of resting-state data used for the main analysis of this paper. We did not observe any sign of oedema 2h post-stimulation.

Finally, we agree with the reviewer that future histological analyses will need to be conducted to further characterized the impact of TUS on the brain (see for example Tufail et al., 2010).

We have amended the revised text as follows:

Results

“Experiment 4, meso- and micro-structural analyses

Some higher intensity ultrasound stimulation protocols, distinct from those used here, have been shown to induce thermal lesions or haemorrhage following cavitation (Elias et al., 2013). […]Neither were any signs of neuronal alteration or haemorrhage observed in histological analyses of three macaques following pre-SMA TUS (Figure 8).”

Discussion

“Some caution might also be warranted in relation to the potential of ultrasound to cause microstructural damage, especially when stimulating at higher intensities, longer durations, or for more repetitions. While our structural MRI and histological analyses did not reveal any evidence of damage and were comparable to previous studies (Dallapiazza et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2016b), it would be of interest in the future to include additional histological indices of apoptotic or inflammatory processes (Tufail et al., 2010) to further assess the safety of TUS.”

Materials and methods

“Histology

In experiment 4, prior to histological examination, animals were anaesthetized with sodium pentorbarbitone and perfused with 90% saline and 10% formalin. […] Every tenth section, two adjacent sections were retained for analysis whereby one was stained with Cresyl Violet (Nissl body staining) and the other with Haemotoxylin and Eosin (H&E staining), in line with previous studies (Dallapiazza et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2016b).”

Transcranial focused ultrasound was abridged to 'TUS'. As the method utilized the focused version (i.e. FUS), the change in acronym to tFUS might be more intuitive.

We acknowledge that there are indeed several acronyms currently in use in the literature, all referring to the same approach. We feel that in such circumstances it would be helpful to aim for consistency. In one way, one could argue that it would be consistent to adopt the ‘tFUS’ acronym used by several US-based labs. In another way, one could argue that it would be consistent to adopt the ‘TUS’ acronym we have used in previous publications employing the same technique.

We have expressed a preference for the ‘TUS’ acronym, here and previously in our work. Namely, this acronym avoids any redundant or implicit aspects of the technique: virtually all reversible ultrasound stimulation protocols to date use focused, pulsed, low-intensity, low-frequency ultrasound. At the same time, this acronym maintains its reference to the core features: it passes through the skull (signifying it is targeted at the brain and is therefore minimally invasive), it utilises ultrasound to deliver energy, and it is a form of stimulation (rather than imaging). In this way the acronym ‘TUS’ also provides an easily recognizable complement to other non-invasive brain stimulation approaches: TMS, TCS, and TES.

Inclusion of some relevant published work may be helpful, at your discretion: (1) fMRI-tFUS human study(ies) and (2) recent findings regarding the presence of long-term neuromodulatory effects of tFUS in rodents.

In the revised manuscript we have tried to give a more thorough overview of the relevant literature. In addition, we now also refer to recent review publications that provide a more comprehensive overview of the field than we are able to do in this original research report. We have adapted the manuscript as follows:

Introduction

“Here we report on a particular protocol of low intensity pulsed transcranial focused ultrasound stimulation (TUS) that we show induces a sustained period of neuromodulation in primates without inducing structural damage. […] We have built on such protocols when designing the current experiment.”

Reviewer 1:

Verhagen and colleague used resting-state fMRI in macaque monkeys to investigate the effects of transcranial focused ultrasound (FUS) on functional brain connectivity. The study appears to be carefully conducted and it reveals some important aspects of FUS. First it shows that FUS increases local functional connectivity and mainly decreases distant functional connectivity. Second it demonstrates that the effects last for at least 2 hours. Overall the effects appear to quite small but they seem to be consistent across animals (Fg. 3c). I have a few major points that need to be addressed.

1) I assume the authors picked frontal polar cortex because it has a very distinct functional connectivity from SMA. Based on my experience, the SNR can be quite low for this anterior area. To rule out the possibility that differences between FPC and SMA are simply related to SNR differences, the authors should show temporal SNR maps.

Please see above responses.

2) I found it virtually impossible to see any differences in the FC maps in Figure 2. The addition of difference maps might help here (e.g. TUA over SMA minus control).

Please see above responses.

3) It is not clear from the paper how often each animal was actually scanned. Did the authors only perform one SMA and one FPC stimulation session per monkey or did they conduct multiple sessions?

We performed one session per monkey per condition. In total we performed three sessions per animal across experiments 1 and 2, and one session per animal in experiment 3. Each session consisted of three consecutive runs. This is further clarified in the new Figure 1 and in the revised text:

Introduction

“In a within-subject design, the same three animals participated in experiments 1 and 2, and a control experiment conducted in the absence of TUS (Figure 1). As such, fMRI was acquired in three conditions for all animals: following SMA TUS, following FPC TUS, and in a control state. In turn, each of the MRI sessions consisted of three consecutive runs.”

Materials and methods

Macaque MRI acquisition

We acquired one MRI session per monkey per condition: in total we performed three sessions per animal across experiments 1 and 2, and one session per animal in experiment 3.”

Reviewer 2:

[…]

Here are a few suggestions, with moderate importance, which may improve the

manuscript.

1. Regarding the acoustic parameter, 30 ms burst in every 100 ms (i.e. pulse repetition of 10 Hz; i.e. duty cycle of 30%), is bit unconventional, but appeared to be effective. 10 Hz is well below the audible frequency, which helped the authors to disregard effects from auditory involvement. Additional rationale for the choice of parameter or brief review on literatures on the parameters in the Discussion section will be helpful.

Please see above responses.

2. In regard to the acoustic and Thermal modeling, the study seems to take conservative approach, which may over-estimate the actual temperature rise in the skull and the brain. Although additional experiments are not necessary, realistic estimation of temperature rise using phantoms will help to ascertain the non-thermal effects of tFUS.

Please see above responses.

3. In histological analysis, authors performed a rather limited staining (i.e. Cresyl Violet and H&E) to evaluate the sonicated brain tissue. Although not absolutely needed in present study, there are other types of histological analyses to assess the presence of tissue damages (such as apoptotic activities or ischemic damages), and authors are advised to reflect these limitations in the Discussion.

Please see above responses.

4. Transcranial focused ultrasound's acronym was defined as 'TUS'. As the method utilized the focused version (i.e. FUS), the change in acronym to tFUS is strongly suggested.

Please see above responses.

5. Although the detailed references are given in the technical methods section,

surprisingly, only a few previous investigations on the matter of main subjects (fMRI and long-term effects of tFUS) are given. Inclusion of (1) fMRI-tFUS study(ies) among humans and (2) recent findings regarding the presence of long-term neuromodulatory effects of tFUS on rodents (a simple Pubmed search will do) is advised for general audience.

Please see above responses.

Reviewer 3:

[…]

I believe that a more suitable approach would have been to compare functional connectivity before, during (if possible) and after TUS within an animal. This procedure would then be repeated for non-stimulated animals to control for the effect of time on connectivity. It would also have been more convincing to observe effects when comparing the entire functional connectivity matrices (instead of just the pruned fingerprints, especially since little information was provided as to how the pruning was achieved).

We hope we have now clarified that we have performed within-subject comparisons in multiple animals unconfounded by effects of order and time. This constitutes an even more powerful experimental design than the one proposed by the reviewer. Indeed, a comparison between animals of a change in functional connectivity over time, contrasting TUS animals with control animals, would require one to test a mixed-level interaction effect with somewhat reduced statistical power. Similarly, a statistical comparison of whole-brain functional connectivity would likely suffer from either a distractingly high false-positive rate, or, when stringently correcting for multiple comparisons, from a detrimentally high false-negative rate. The fingerprint analysis is designed specifically to avoid these pitfalls, allowing one to leverage a-priori knowledge of neuroanatomy.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Lennart Verhagen. 2019. offlineTUS. Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging. offlinetus

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.40541.014

    Data Availability Statement

    FSL can be downloaded from https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki. HCP Workbench can be downloaded from https://www. humanconnectome.org/software/connectome-workbench. For any information regarding MrCat please see http://www.rbmars.dds.nl/lab/toolbox.html; further inquiries can be directed to Lennart Verhagen (lennart.verhagen@psy.ox.ac.uk). All dedicated software tools are available at https://github.com/neuroecology/MrCat. Data contributing to this work have been publicly deposited at the Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging (https://git.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/lverhagen/offlinetus). For any inquiries regarding the data, please contact Jérôme Sallet (jerome.sallet@psy.ox.ac.uk).

    The following dataset was generated:

    Lennart Verhagen. 2019. offlineTUS. Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging. offlinetus


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES