Abstract
BACKGROUND:
Substance use is prevalent and is associated with academic performance among adolescents. Few studies have examined the association between abstinence from all substances and academic achievement.
METHODS:
Data from a nationally representative sample of 9,578 twelfth graders from the 2015 Monitoring the Future survey were analyzed to examine relationships between abstinence from substance use and four academic variables: skipping school, grades, academic self-efficacy, and emotional academic engagement. Participants were categorized as lifetime non-users, former users, and past-year users based on the use of 14 substances.
RESULTS:
Approximately one-quarter of participants had never used cigarettes, alcohol, or other drugs during their lifetime, and 8%wt used at least one substance during their lifetime but not during the past year. Adjusting for demographic variables, past-year substance users had 2.71 greater odds of skipping school during the past month than lifetime non-users and 1.74 greater odds of having low grades. Lifetime non-users reported greater academic self-efficacy and emotional academic engagement than past-year users.
CONCLUSIONS:
Many twelfth graders have abstained from all substance use during their lifetime, and these adolescents experience better academic outcomes than their substance-using peers. Substance use prevention programs should be evaluated as a way to promote academic achievement.
Keywords: academic engagement, academic performance, adolescents, substance use, truancy
In the United States, approximately two-thirds of adolescents have consumed alcohol by their senior year of high school and about half have tried an illicit drug.1 Substance use behaviors arise from a complex interplay between numerous risk and protective factors at the individual, family, social/peer, and environmental levels.2,3 Some examples of risk factors include temperament characteristics such as impulsivity, sensation-seeking, being male, affiliating with peers who use substances, low religiosity, perceived approval of use by family members, child maltreatment, and living in affluent suburban neighborhoods.3–6
Whereas the health effects of substance use are well established,7–10 comparatively less attention has been directed at the association between various forms of substance use and academic achievement. Several cross-sectional and prospective studies focusing on marijuana use have found an association with lower grades, lower scores on measures of academic functioning, a greater likelihood of skipping school and dropping out of high school, and a lower likelihood of enrolling in college.11–16 For instance, Ellickson, Martino, and Collins15 found that individuals who used marijuana fewer than ten times per year throughout adolescence had lower levels of educational attainment at age 29 compared with those who never used marijuana.
Other studies have examined the associations between past-month and past-year use of multiple substances and academic outcomes.16–19 Henry18 found that the past-month use of cigarettes, alcohol, and marijuana were each associated with an increased likelihood of skipping school among eighth and tenth grade students. Similarly, Cox et al.20 observed that, among a sample of students in ninth through twelfth grades, having low average grades was significantly associated with binge drinking and using marijuana during the past month. Substance use has also been operationalized using continuous measures. Henry21 computed a polysubstance use score (the mean of items to reflect tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana use), and found that it was significantly associated with poorer academic performance. Among students whose grades decreased during three years of junior high school, significant increases were seen in the polysubstance use score.
Most researchers would agree that substance use and academic performance are most likely are related bidirectionally, such that substance use both influences and is influenced by academic performance.22 Bachman et al.22 concluded that academic experiences predict substance use more strongly than use predicts performance. However, significant findings linking substance use to subsequent academic outcomes have also been observed. Tobacco smoking at age 14 was associated with an increased likelihood of dropping out of high school by age 18, and smoking at age 18 was associated with lower educational attainment by age 22.
Complicating this bidirectional relationship are a number of common risk factors that affect risk for both substance use and academic difficulties. For example, boys are more likely than girls to both use substances and have lower grades in high school.3,23 Externalizing disorders such as attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder also increase risk for both substance use and decreased academic achievement.24–26 However, studies controlling for such factors suggest that substance use is negatively associated with academic outcomes beyond the influence of shared risk factors.27–29
A compelling argument for the influence of substance use on academic achievement comes from evidence that substance use is associated with learning and memory deficits that can impede academic performance.30–34 Brain imaging studies have found that substance use negatively affects structure and performance in the prefrontal cortex, which is responsible for decision-making, working memory, and self-control.35–39 Studies comparing alcohol-dependent adolescents with non-dependent adolescents have also noted decreased performance on neuropsychological tests as well as lower volume of the hippocampus, which subserves long-term memory.30,31
Reward perception might also play a role in the relationship between substance use and academic performance.40 Research has shown that the immediate rewarding effects of substance use creates an attentional bias among substance users, such that heightened attention is paid to drug-related cues. This heightened attention leads to cravings for the drug, which in turn increases the likelihood of repeated drug use.41 This suggests that as adolescents become involved in substance use, their attention is shifted toward its immediate rewarding effects rather than longer-term rewards associated with academic endeavors. These academic endeavors might be de-prioritized and academic performance might suffer as a result. The notion that impaired reward perception might underlie the relationship between substance use and academic performance is supported by studies that have found an association between substance use and decreased academic engagement.42,43 One longitudinal study using a school-based sample found that higher levels of school bonding, a dimension of academic engagement that refers to having a positive emotional connection to school, in fifth grade was associated with delayed initiation of alcohol use, which in turn predicted lower levels of alcohol misuse in twelfth grade.42 A follow-up study found that students whose school bonding increased between seventh grade and twelfth grade were less likely to have used cigarettes or alcohol during their lifetime by twelfth grade.43 School bonding in twelfth grade was also negatively associated with lifetime marijuana and other drug use.
This study extends prior research on the association between substance use and academic performance in three important ways. First, we utilize a contemporary nationally representative secondary data source, the 2015 Monitoring the Future (MTF) survey. Second, the research question focuses on whether abstinence from all forms of substance use either in the students’ lifetime or during the past year was associated with academic variables. Third, we examine academic engagement in a new way, using a multidimensional construct derived from items asked on the MTF survey. In summary, this study aimed to compare skipping school, grades, and academic engagement among three groups of high school seniors: (1) adolescents who had never used any substance in their lifetime; (2) adolescents who had used at least one substance during their lifetime, but zero substances during the past year; and (3) adolescents who had used at least one substance during the past year. We hypothesized that adolescents who have never used alcohol, cigarettes, illicit drugs, and prescription medications nonmedically would be more academically engaged, less likely to skip school, and have higher grades than students who have used substances during their lifetime.
METHODS
Procedures
This is a cross-sectional secondary analysis from the 2015 MTF study,44 an ongoing nationally-representative, classroom-based study of students at public and private schools in the contiguous United States.45 Questionnaires are self-administered to a sample of students (or all students, in smaller schools) in eighth, tenth, and twelfth grades at schools selected through multistage random sampling. Further detail regarding the study design and methods is available elsewhere.1,45
Participants
The analytic sample consisted of 9,578 twelfth graders with non-missing data for lifetime and past-year substance use. Twelfth grade was chosen because of the availability of data on academic engagement, which is not assessed for younger students.
Instrumentation
Substance use.
Lifetime and past-year use was assessed for 14 substances, including cigarettes, alcohol, eight illicit drugs [marijuana/hashish, cocaine, heroin, inhalants, LSD or “acid,” hallucinogens other than LSD, methamphetamine, and 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA or “ecstasy”)], and four classes of prescription drugs used nonmedically (amphetamines, narcotics, sedatives, and tranquilizers) with the questions “On how many occasions (if any) have you used [substance]: a: in your lifetime? b: during the last 12 months?” Responses were provided on a nine-point scale with options ranging from “0 occasions” to “40 or more,” and later dichotomized as “use” or “non-use.” Nonmedical use was defined as using a prescription medication “on your own—that is, without a doctor telling you to take them.”
A variable was computed to categorize participants into one of three mutually exclusive categories: (1) lifetime non-users of any substance; (2) former users (used at least one substance during their lifetime but no past-year use); and (3) users of at least one substance during the past year. Due to missing data for methamphetamine, MDMA, and inhalant use, participants were first categorized based on their use of the other eleven substances. Participants were only included in the analytic sample if they had non-missing data for lifetime and past-year use of these eleven substances. They were then reclassified as necessary based on responses for methamphetamine, MDMA, and inhalant use. Very few individuals used these three substances exclusively (N = 22 for lifetime use, N = 3 for past-year use). MTF does not assess past-year cigarette use; past 30-day use was substituted when computing this variable.
Skipping school.
Skipping school was assessed via the question “During the last four weeks, how many whole days of school have you missed because you skipped or ‘cut’?” Responses were provided on a seven-point scale ranging from “None” to “11 or more days” and later dichotomized as “None” or “One or more days.”
Grades.
To assess high school grades, students were asked, “Which of the following best describes your average grade so far in high school?” with nine response options ranging from “D” to “A.” These responses were dichotomized as “Low grades” (C+ or lower) or “High grades” (B- or higher).
Academic engagement.
Nine items assessed aspects of academic engagement. Table 1 lists these items and their response options. Because there are several recognized dimensions of academic engagement,46,47 a principal components analysis (PCA) was conducted to determine whether more than one dimension was present among these nine items and to identify items that could be excluded. Factors with eigenvalues >1.0 were retained. Two items with factor loadings <0.6 were dropped. The resulting PCA identified two factors. The first factor was comprised of two items, both related to academic self-efficacy (ASE). These items were averaged to create an ASE score, with higher scores indicating greater self-efficacy in academic abilities. The second factor was comprised of five items related to emotional academic engagement (EAE), which is the positive and negative feelings a student has toward academic experiences.46,47 These items were averaged to create an EAE score. Higher EAE scores indicated more positive feelings toward school.
Table 1.
Variable | Question | Response Options | M (SD) | PCA Factor Loadings |
Final Result | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Factor 1 | Factor 2 | |||||
Intelligence | Compared with others your age throughout the country, how do you rate yourself on school ability? | 1 (Far below average) to 7 (Far above average) |
4.99 (1.21) | 0.922 | - | Retained in academic self-efficacy score |
Ability in school | How intelligent do you think you are compared with others your age? | 1 (Far below average) to 7 (Far above average) |
4.88 (1.20) | 0.908 | - | Retained in academic self-efficacy score |
Interest in courses | How interesting are most of your courses to you? | 1 (Very dull) to 5 (Very exciting and stimulating) |
2.87 (0.99) | - | .781 | Retained in emotional academic engagement score |
Likes going to school | How much do you agree or disagree with each statement below? Going to school has been an enjoyable experience for me. | 1 (Disagree) to 5 (Agree) |
3.06 (1.03) | - | .754 | Retained in emotional academic engagement score |
Importance of school topics for later life | How important do you think the things you are learning in school are going to be for your later life? | 1 (Not important) to 5 (Very important) |
2.96 (1.18) | - | .733 | Retained in emotional academic engagement score |
School work is meaningful | How often do you feel that the school work you are assigned in meaningful and important? | 1 (Never) to 5 (Almost always) |
2.99 (1.07) | - | .731 | Retained in emotional academic engagement score |
Enjoyment of school experience | Some people like school very much. Others don’t. How do you feel about going to school? | 1 (I don’t like it at all) to 5 (I like it very much) |
3.39 (1.23) | - | .639 | Retained in emotional academic engagement score |
Satisfaction with educational experiences | How satisfied are you with your educational experiences? | 1 (Completely dissatisfied) to 7 (Completely satisfied) |
5.07 (1.59) | Dropped | ||
Importance of doing well in school for getting a good job | How much do you agree or disagree with each statement below? Doing well in school is important for getting a good job. | 1 (Disagree) to 5 (Agree) |
4.23 (1.01) | Dropped |
Seven of these items were form-specific and were not asked of all participants. The PCA and subsequent academic engagement analyses were restricted to participants with non-missing data for these items (N = 1536). Only factor loadings > |0.6| are displayed.
M, mean; SD, standard deviation.
Covariates.
Seven covariates were included due to their association with either substance use and/or academic performance: sex, age, race/ethnicity, highest level of parental education, hours worked per week during school year, geographic region of the participant’s school, and type of high school program. With the exception of geographic region, all covariates were self-reported. Race/ethnicity is a categorical variable computed by MTF based on self-report. Participants selected one or more responses from a list of nine race/ethnicity categories. The responses were recoded to include non-Hispanic black or African American, non-Hispanic white, or Hispanic. Participants who indicated other races or who indicated more than one race were coded by MTF as “Missing” for this variable. Participants originally coded as Missing were recoded as Other and included in these analyses.
Data Analysis
Data were analyzed using SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics Version 24.0, IBM Corporation). Means for academic engagement items and prevalence estimates for the substance use groups, covariates, skipping school, and grades were computed using sampling weights provided by MTF. All variables were examined for differences between the three groups of interest using z-tests to compare proportions for categorical variables. Logistic regression models for skipping school and high school grades were developed, and linear regression models were developed for the ASE and EAE scores. Unweighted data were used for the regression modeling. All regression models were adjusted for covariates. Sex, age, and race/ethnicity were retained in these models regardless of statistical significance. Estimated marginal means measuring the likelihood of skipping school, likelihood of getting low grades, mean ASE score, and mean EAE score (adjusted for covariates) were obtained from the regression models.
RESULTS
As shown in Table 2, approximately one-quarter (28.8%wt) of the sample had never used any substances during their lifetime. Former substance use was less common (7.8%wt), and the majority (63.4%wt) used at least one substance during the past year. Half of the sample was male (46.9%wt) and self-identified as non-Hispanic white (54.5%wt). Approximately one-quarter (28.3%wt) of the sample skipped at least one day of school during the past four weeks. High average grades (B- or higher) were reported by 85.1%wt of the sample. Non-Hispanic white adolescents, adolescents from the Northeast, and adolescents who worked during the school year were overrepresented among the past-year users, relative to lifetime non-users. A greater proportion of lifetime non-users had at least one parent with a college degree compared with former users and past-year users.
Table 2.
Overall (N = 9,578) |
Lifetime Non-users (N = 2,758; 28.8%wt) |
Former Users (N = 747; 7.8% wt) |
Past-year Users (N = 6,073; 63.4% wt) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
%wt | N | %wt | N | %wt | N | %wt | N | |
Sex (% male) | 46.9 | 4262 | 47.9a | 1242 | 50.0a | 357 | 46.1a | 2663 |
Age (% 18 years or older) | 56.0 | 5322 | 55.4a | 1517 | 58.3a | 433 | 56.0 a | 3372 |
Race/ethnicity | ||||||||
Non-Hispanic white | 54.5 | 5217 | 51.2a | 1412 | 43.5b | 325 | 57.3c | 3479 |
Non-Hispanic black | 13.0 | 1245 | 16.8a | 463 | 18.5a | 138 | 10.6b | 643 |
Hispanic | 16.4 | 1568 | 14.4a | 397 | 20.5b | 153 | 16.8c | 1018 |
Other/biracial/missing | 16.2 | 1548 | 17.6a | 485 | 17.5a,b | 130 | 15.4b | 932 |
Highest level of education completed by a parent | ||||||||
Less than a high school degree | 19.6 | 1786 | 17.1a | 440 | 23.9b | 164 | 20.3b | 1181 |
High school degree | 32.2 | 2930 | 29.9a | 771 | 32.9a,b | 226 | 33.2b | 1933 |
Some college | 19.9 | 1806 | 21.1a | 544 | 19.7a | 136 | 19.3a | 1126 |
College degree or higher | 28.3 | 2569 | 32.0a | 827 | 23.5b | 162 | 27.2b | 1580 |
Hours worked per week during school year | ||||||||
0 hours | 39.7 | 3659 | 50.2a | 1332 | 42.4b | 301 | 34.7c | 2027 |
Up to 10 hours | 19.0 | 1745 | 20.6a | 546 | 17.2a | 122 | 18.4a | 1077 |
11 to 20 hours | 20.6 | 1894 | 16.0a | 425 | 20.8b | 147 | 22.6b | 1321 |
More than 20 hours | 20.8 | 1911 | 13.1a | 349 | 19.6b | 139 | 24.3c | 1424 |
Geographic region of school | ||||||||
Northeast | 17.0 | 1631 | 13.3a | 367 | 12.2a | 91 | 19.3b | 1173 |
North Central | 21.2 | 2029 | 22.7a | 627 | 21.0a | 157 | 20.5a | 1245 |
Southeast | 38.5 | 3691 | 41.5a | 1143 | 45.3a | 338 | 36.4b | 2210 |
West | 23.3 | 2227 | 22.5a | 621 | 21.6a | 161 | 23.8a | 1445 |
High school program | ||||||||
College preparatory | 51.9 | 4853 | 55.9a | 1505 | 45.9b | 331 | 50.9c | 3017 |
General | 35.2 | 3292 | 30.8a | 828 | 36.9b | 267 | 37.0b | 2197 |
Vocational/technical | 3.0 | 279 | 2.1a | 56 | 3.5a,b | 25 | 3.3b | 198 |
Other/don’t know | 9.9 | 921 | 11.2a | 302 | 13.7a | 99 | 8.8b | 519 |
Skipped school (% skipped at least one day during the past four weeks) | 28.3 | 2548 | 14.9a | 384 | 21.2b | 150 | 35.2c | 2014 |
Average high school grade (% B- or higher) | 85.1 | 7900 | 89.5a | 2387 | 84.5b | 615 | 83.1b | 4898 |
Cells within the same row not sharing a common superscript are significantly different at p < .05.
Monitoring the Future does not assess past-year cigarette use; past-30 day use was substituted when categorizing participants into the substance use groups.
Descriptive Results of Academic Engagement Items
Means and standard deviations for the academic engagement items are presented in Table 1. In general, high levels of perceived academic ability were observed as were satisfaction with educational experiences and enjoyment of the school experience. Lower ratings were observed for items relating to school courses, such as interest in courses, importance of courses for later life, and meaningfulness of schoolwork.
Skipping School
Table 3 presents the findings of the regression models. As hypothesized, past-year users had nearly three times greater odds of skipping school than lifetime non-users (adjusted odds ratio [AOR]=2.71; 95% confidence interval [CI]=2.39–3.07). Former users were also significantly more likely to skip school than lifetime non-users (AOR=1.39; 95%CI=1.11–1.73). All results were robust to the inclusion of covariates. As presented in Figure 1, 16.7% of lifetime non-users skipped school during the past month, compared with 21.7% of former users and 35.1% of past-year users.
Table 3.
Skipping school | Low average grades |
Academic self- efficacy score |
Emotional academic engagement score |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | |
Sex (Ref = Male) | 1.07 (0.97, 1.18) | 0.70 (0.61, 0.80)** | 0.79 (0.76, 0.83)** | 1.00 (0.92, 1.08) |
Race (Ref = Non-Hispanic white) | ||||
Non-Hispanic black | 0.83 (0.70, 0.98)* | 2.25 (1.84, 2.76)** | 0.92 (0.86, 0.99)* | 1.18 (1.04, 1.34)* |
Hispanic | 1.27 (1.10, 1.45)** | 1.26 (1.04, 1.53)* | 0.88 (0.72, 0.82)** | 1.11 (0.99, 1.24) |
Other/missing | 1.12 (0.97, 1.29) | 1.03 (0.84, 1.27) | 1.00 (0.94, 1.07) | 1.03 (0.92, 1.16) |
Age (Ref = Less than 18 years old) | 1.13 (1.02, 1.25)* | 0.96 (0.84, 1.10) | 0.96 (0.92, 1.01) | 1.08 (1.00, 1.17) |
Parents’ highest level of education completed (Ref = Less than a high school degree) |
||||
High school degree | -- | 0.81 (0.68, 0.97)* | 1.13 (1.50, 1.72)** | -- |
Some college | -- | 0.61 (0.49, 0.75)** | 1.30 (1.21, 1.40)** | -- |
College degree or higher | -- | 0.40 (0.32, 0.50)** | 1.61 (1.06, 1.20)** | -- |
Hours worked per week during school year (Ref = 0 hours) |
||||
Up to 10 hours | 1.12 (0.98, 1.30) | 0.71 (0.58, 0.88)** | -- | 1.05 (0.94, 1.17) |
11 to 20 hours | 1.19 (1.04, 1.36)* | 0.78 (0.65, 0.95)* | -- | 0.86 (0.78, 0.96)** |
More than 20 hours | 1.53 (1.34, 1.74)** | 1.04 (0.87, 1.23) | -- | 0.91 (0.82, 1.02) |
Geographic region (Ref = Northeast) | ||||
North Central | 0.81 (0.69, 0.94)** | 1.65 (1.33, 2.05)** | 0.99 (0.90, 1.04) | -- |
Southeast | 1.12 (0.97, 1.28) | 0.85 (0.69, 1.05) | 1.08 (1.02, 1.15)* | -- |
West | 1.11 (0.95, 1.30) | 2.05 (1.64, 2.56)** | 0.97 (0.92, 1.06) | -- |
High school program (Ref = College preparatory) | ||||
General | 1.34 (1.20, 1.49)** | 2.88 (2.47, 3.35)** | 0.61 (0.58, 0.64)** | 0.85 (0.78, 0.93)** |
Vocational/technical | 1.17 (0.88, 1.57) | 3.49 (2.49, 4.89)** | 0.52 (0.45, 0.59)** | 0.57 (0.45, 0.71)** |
Other/don’t know | 1.02 (0.86, 1.22) | 4.15 (3.36, 5.14)** | 0.47 (0.44, 0.51)** | 0.86 (0.75, 1.00)* |
Substance use (Ref = Lifetime non-users) | ||||
Former user | 1.39 (1.11, 1.73)** | 1.31 (0.99, 1.73) | 0.97 (0.89, 1.06) | 0.97 (0.84, 1.12) |
Past-year user | 2.71 (2.39, 3.07)** | 1.74 (1.47, 2.06)** | 0.94 (0.90, 0.99)* | 0.72 (0.66, 0.79)** |
CI, confidence interval; AOR, adjusted odds ratio.
p < .05
p < .01
-- Non-significant variables were dropped from the final models. Age, sex, and race were retained regardless of significance.
High School Grades
As hypothesized, past-year users had significantly greater odds of getting low grades compared with lifetime non-users (prevalence 20.1% compared with 12.6%; AOR=1.74; 95%CI=1.47–2.06), even after adjusting for covariates. Former users were not significantly different from lifetime non-users or past-year users. All covariates, with the exception of age, were significantly associated with grades in this model.
Academic Self-Efficacy and Emotional Academic Engagement Scores
Compared with past-year users, lifetime non-users had significantly higher scores for both measures of academic engagement, after adjustment for covariates. As shown in Figure 2, former-users had significantly higher EAE scores than past-year users but did not differ from lifetime non-users, and the former users did not differ from either group on ASE scores.
DISCUSSION
Utilizing a large, nationally representative sample of high school seniors, we examined the associations between abstinence from substance use and academic variables. Although most of the sample had used at least one substance during the past year, a sizeable minority (28.8%wt) had abstained from all forms of substance use during their lifetime. The results provide support for the hypothesized association between lifetime abstinence from substance use and academic variables. Specifically, lifetime abstinence was significantly associated with a decreased likelihood of skipping school during the past four weeks and an increased likelihood of having average grades of B- or higher. The current findings confirm previous evidence for an association between substance use and poorer grades,48–51 as well as increased risk for skipping school among adolescents.18
This study extends the existing literature by demonstrating differences in academic engagement between substance users and non-users. Specifically, lifetime non-users of alcohol and all other drugs had greater self-efficacy in their academic abilities and more positive feelings toward their education relative to past-year substance users, even after adjustment for a number of demographic variables. This finding extends previous research observing similar relationships in relation to illicit drug use.52 To our knowledge, the present study is the first to evaluate the relationship between substance use and a multi-dimensional measure of academic engagement using a nationally representative sample of youth.
Interestingly, enjoyment of school was rated generally high, whereas engagement with schoolwork was comparatively low. Given the positive relationship between school bonding and academic performance,53 the development of interventions to increase school bonding could be informed by further research on how aspects of engagement affect enjoyment of school.
The present findings fit within the context of Problem Behavior Theory (PBT),54,55 which posits that problem behaviors such as skipping school and substance use tend to cluster together as a result of a general tendency to avoid engagement in prosocial or conventional behaviors.56 The current findings support the notion that substance use and poor academic outcomes, namely skipping school and disengagement, “cluster” among adolescents. Interestingly, skipping class was the only academic variable measured that was different between former substance users and lifetime abstainers. Skipping class could be seen as more closely related to the problem behavior syndrome, as described by Jessor,54 than academic engagement and grades. For these latter variables, former users of substances and students who were lifetime abstainers could not be distinguished, lending support to the notion that substance use per se might be an important influence on academic performance, over and above associated risk factors for substance use. This finding is consistent with previous studies utilizing addiction treatment samples of youth that found improvement in academic outcomes, namely school attendance and high school completion, after abstaining from substance use.57–59 However, because the present data are cross-sectional, we cannot make conclusions regarding the temporality of the association between substance use and academic performance.
Although “experimentation” with substance use is considered to be a common behavior during adolescence,60 cessation of substance use was less common among this sample than persistence (7.8%wt vs. 63.4%wt). Risk factors that contribute to both substance use and academic performance, beyond those that were controlled for in the present study, might differentiate the former substance users from current or lifetime non-users. Given the uniqueness of the former users, future research is warranted to further describe this group with respect to the risk and protective factors that differentiate them from the past-year users. The former users subsample might reflect adolescents who ceased using substances following treatment for a substance use disorder or those who naturally stopped using substances. It is also plausible that this group stopped using substances after experiencing consequences, such as decreased academic performance or being caught by their parents or school officials. These formers users could have also engaged in transient substance use resulting from peer pressure. Characterizing the nature and reasons for persistence and cessation of substance use involvement during adolescence is an area worthy of future exploration. Qualitative methods might be especially appropriate for this purpose.
In a similar way, research to describe motivations for not using any substances is warranted to inform efforts to prevent adolescent substance use. Concerns about psychological and physical harms were paramount among the reasons cited by high school seniors for abstaining from marijuana use.61 Research is needed to understand whether or not messages about the impact of substance use on academic achievement would resonate with adolescents.
Shifting attitudes towards marijuana use and changes in the nonmedical use of prescription medications represent a new “drug landscape” for high school students in the United States.1 This study included nonmedical use of four types of prescription drugs, including prescription stimulants in the definition of substance use. In contrast to the common perception that nonmedical use of prescription stimulants can increase cognitive performance,62,63 studies of college students have found that this behavior is not associated with a higher GPA, in part because of the overlap between nonmedical use of prescription drugs and multiple other substances.64,65 Given that high school students engage in nonmedical prescription stimulant use to get high as well as for studying,66 the current findings that substance use is associated with poorer grades is especially relevant.
The finding that lifetime users differed from former users and past-year users on demographic characteristics aligns with known risk factors for substance use during adolescence.2,3 However, significant differences in academic outcomes remained even when controlling for these characteristics. Future studies should examine the potential moderating effects of these demographic characteristics. Understanding the role that race, sex, and socioeconomic status play in the relationship between substance use and academic achievement will allow schools to better tailor prevention and intervention efforts to their student population. Additionally, given the number of environmental factors that increase risk for substance use, future research could also evaluate the interrelationships between social context, school environment, substance use, and academic achievement. Furthermore, additional research is needed to evaluate factors that might mediate the relationship between substance use and academic outcomes. In particular, peer and parental factors should be considered, as they have been shown to affect both risk for substance use and academic performance. Affiliation with deviant peers is a risk factor for substance use during adolescence,67 and association with these peers might negatively affect academic engagement.68 By contrast, positive family relationships and parental monitoring during high school appear to be protective factors related to substance use.67,69–71
Given the implications of academic performance during adolescence for degree attainment, earning potential, and later health status, these findings underscore the importance of preventing adolescent substance use.72–74 As described below, strengthening the capacity for schools to prevent use and intervene with students who are at risk for escalation of substance use problems should be viewed as a way to support and promote students’ academic success.
Limitations
Several limitations of the present study must be noted. First, we are unable to infer causality in differences in academic outcomes by substance use group due to the cross-sectional nature of the data. Future research should utilize longitudinal designs to examine temporality in the relationship between substance use and subsequent changes in academic engagement and academic outcomes. Second, self-report data might be influenced by recall or social desirability bias. Third, we had to make inferences regarding group membership if a student had missing data on MDMA, methamphetamine, or inhalant use. However, it is unlikely that this decision impacted our results given that the use of illicit drugs other than marijuana rarely occurs in the absence of alcohol, cigarette, or marijuana use.75,76 In order to answer our particular research question, the current study compared groups of adolescents based on their abstinence from any form of substance use. Using more continuous measures such as the number of illicit drugs used during the lifetime is an alternative way of operationalizing substance use severity. Although we controlled for a variety of covariates, we were unable to account for the effects of externalizing behaviors and conduct problems, which are more common among substance users than non-users and are negatively associated with academic achievement.26,77 To ease interpretation of estimates from the regression models, dichotomized variables were used for skipping school and grades. It is possible that using continuous variables for these measures could alter the interpretation of the results.
Finally, adolescents who dropped out of high school or were not present in class on the day of data collection are not included among this sample. Adolescents who skip school are more likely to be substance users than students who do not skip,16,78 and high school students who have a substance use disorder or experience serious academic failure might drop out prior to their senior year. Therefore, these results might underestimate the prevalence of substance use, skipping school, and low grades. Further research on this topic utilizing community-based samples, rather than school-based, is needed to avoid possible bias from absenteeism and dropout.
Conclusions
A sizeable minority of high school seniors have abstained from all substance use during their lifetime. Our findings suggest that lifetime non-users are less likely to skip school or get low grades and have higher levels of academic engagement relative to current substance users. Interventions to prevent substance use during adolescence might be useful in promoting academic achievement. Further research is needed to describe the mechanisms that might explain the observed relationship between substance use, academic engagement, and academic achievement.
IMPLICATIONS FOR SCHOOL HEALTH.
Students might not perform well academically for a number of health-related reasons, and substance use appears to be one possible contributing factor. School health programs, therefore, play an essential role is promoting academic performance by addressing multiple reasons for academic difficulties, including substance use. A comprehensive approach to addressing substance use requires a continuum of strategies, includes education, screening, intervention, and making referrals and should be seen as a priority for schools, to promote not only student health but also academic success.79 The following suggestions are offered as starting points for developing an overall strategy:
Resources to support school-based substance use prevention programs, including funding and staff time are essential. Emphasizing that there is evidence for a link between academic achievement and substance use can be a compelling argument to allocate new resources for prevention and intervention efforts. Leveraging the expertise of local public health and substance use prevention practitioners might help make this case to policymakers who are responsible for funding decisions.
Ongoing training for teachers and other educational professionals regarding the identification of early warning signs of substance use allows for interventions of at-risk students to occur at the earliest point possible, before problems escalate.
Teachers, school administrators, and school health professionals should coordinate to ensure that messaging from the school about substance use and relevant policies are implemented consistently and comprehensively throughout at student’s academic experience. A “no tolerance” message is recommended.80
School-based prevention programs are effective at preventing substance use during adolescence,81–83 and programs as early as first and second grade are associated with less substance use during late adolescence.84 Programs that address social influences, norms, and commitment not to use, in addition to using peer leaders and interactive delivery methods, are the most effective.81
Substance use screening can be adapted for administration in a variety of settings, such as visits to the school nurse, meetings with school counselors, or through athletics. In particular, school-based health centers are well-positioned to provide screening and other substance use services.85 The American Academy of Pediatrics recommends using the CRAFFT, which takes less than five minutes to administer, to screen adolescents for alcohol and drug use,72,86 and previous studies have recommended the CRAFFT as an efficient, valid tool for use in high schools.87
Developing relationships with community-based resources and local health care professionals who specialize in the care and management of adolescent substance use problems is essential so that students can be appropriately referred when there is a need for additional assessment or treatment.79
Schools can utilize the six strategies outlined by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention as a guide for implementing programs to foster school connectedness, an important component of academic engagement.53 Increasing school connectedness can protect against health risk behaviors, including substance use.
Educational messaging to parents about the consequences of substance use should specifically include reference to possible benefits of abstaining from substance use on promoting student academic performance and engagement.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This article was prepared as the first author’s Master’s thesis. We thank Kevin O’Grady and Kimberly Caldeira for their valuable advice on the statistical analyses. Funding for this study was provided by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01DA014845, Dr. Arria, PI). The Monitoring the Future survey is funded by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01DA001411–41, Dr. Johnston, PI).
Footnotes
Human Subjects Approval Statement
Monitoring the Future is approved by the University of Michigan Institutional Review Board (IRB). The IRB at the University of Maryland determined that this secondary data analysis was exempt from IRB review.
Contributor Information
Brittany A. Bugbee, Center for Young Adult Health and Development, Department of Behavioral and Community Health, University of Maryland School of Public Health, 1234 School of Public Health, College Park, Maryland 20742, Phone: (301) 405-9748, Fax: (301) 314-9167, bbugbee@umd.edu.
Kenneth H. Beck, Department of Behavioral and Community Health, 1234 School of Public Health, College Park, Maryland 20742, Phone: (301) 405-2527, Fax: (301) 314-9167, kbeck1@umd.edu.
Craig S. Fryer, Department of Behavioral and Community Health, 1234 School of Public Health, College Park, Maryland 20742, Phone: (301) 405-0818, Fax: (301) 314-9167, csfryer@umd.edu.
Amelia M. Arria, Center for Young Adult Health and Development, Department of Behavioral and Community Health, University of Maryland School of Public Health, 1234 School of Public Health, College Park, Maryland 20742, Phone: (301) 405-9795, Fax: (301) 314-9167, aarria@umd.edu.
REFERENCES
- 1.Miech RA, Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future: national survey results on drug use, 1975–2015: volume I: secondary school students. 2016. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan; Available at: http://www.monitoringthefuture.org/pubs/monographs/mtf-vol1_2015.pdf. Accessed June 29, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Connell CM, Gilreath TD, Aklin WM, Brex RA. Social-ecological influences on patterns of substance use among non-metropolitan high school students. Am J Community Psychol. 2010;45(1–2):36–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Belcher HM, Shinitzky HE. Substance abuse in children: prediction, protection, and prevention. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1998;152(10):952–960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Luthar SS, D’Avanzo K. Contextual factors in substance use: a study of suburban and inner-city adolescents. Dev Psychopathol. 1999;11(4):845–867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Castellanos-Ryan N, O’Leary-Barrett M, Conrod PJ. Substance-use in childhood and adolescence: a brief overview of developmental processes and their clinical implications. J Can Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;22(1):41–46. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Nash SG, McQueen A, Bray JH. Pathways to adolescent alcohol use: family environment, peer influence, and parental expectations. J Adolesc Health. 2005;37(1):19–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Grant BF, Dawson DA. Age at onset of alcohol use and its association with DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J Subst Abuse. 1997;9:103–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hingson RW, Heeren T, Jamanka A, Howland J. Age of drinking onset and unintentional injury involvement after drinking. JAMA. 2000;284(12):1527–1533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gryczynski J, Schwartz RP, O’Grady KE, Restivo L, Mitchell SG, Jaffe JH. Understanding patterns of high-cost health care use across different substance user groups. Health Aff. 2016;35(1):12–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Duncan SC, Strycker LA, Duncan TE. Exploring associations in developmental trends of adolescent substance use and risky sexual behavior in a high-risk population. J Behav Med. 1999;22(1):21–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Jiang Y, Mermin J, Perry DK, Hesser JE. The relationship of multiple, simultaneously occurring health risk behaviors to academic performance of high school students. J Behav Health. 2013;2(1):44–51. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bray JW, Zarkin GA, Ringwalt CL, Qi J. The relationship between marijuana initiation and dropping out of high school. Health Econ. 2000;9(1):9–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Brook JS, Stimmel MA, Zhang C, Brook DW. The association between earlier substance use and subsequent academic achievement and health problems: a longitudinal study. Am J Addict. 2008;17(2):155–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Homel J, Thompson K, Leadbeater B. Trajectories of marijuana use in youth ages 15–25: implications for postsecondary education experiences. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2014;75(4):674–683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ellickson PL, Martino SC, Collins RL. Marijuana use from adolescence to young adulthood: multiple developmental trajectories and their associated outcomes. Health Psychol. 2004;23(3):299–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hallfors D, Vevea JL, Iritani B, Cho H, Khatapoush S, Saxe L. Truancy, grade point average, and sexual activity: a meta-analysis of risk indicators for youth substance use. J Sch Health. 2002;72(5):205–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Patte KA, Qian W, Leatherdale ST. Marijuana and alcohol use as predictors of academic achievement: a longitudinal analysis among youth in the COMPASS study. J Sch Health. 2017;87(5):310–318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Henry KL. Who’s skipping school: characteristics of truants in 8th and 10th grade. J Sch Health. 2007;77(1):29–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Patrick ME, Schulenberg JE, O’Malley PM. High school substance use as a predictor of college attendance, completion, and dropout: a national multicohort longitudinal study. Youth Soc. 2016;48(3):425–447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Cox RG, Zhang L, Johnson WD, Bender DR. Academic performance and substance use: findings from a state survey of public high school students. J Sch Health. 2007;77(3):109–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Henry KL. Academic achievement and adolescent drug use: an examination of reciprocal effects and correlated growth trajectories. J Sch Health. 2010;80(1):38–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bachman JG, O’Malley PM, Schulenberg JE, Johnston LD, Freedman-Doan P, Messersmith EE. The Education-Drug Use Connection: How Successes and Failures in School Relate to Adolescent Smoking, Drinking, Drug Use, and Delinquency. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates/Taylor & Francis; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Fortin NM, Oreopoulos P, Phipps S. Leaving boys behind: gender disparities in high academic achievement. J Hum Resour. 2015;50(3):549–579. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lee SS, Humphreys KL, Flory K, Liu R, Glass K. Prospective association of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and substance use and abuse/dependence: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31(3):328–341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Loe IM, Feldman HM. Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32(6):643–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Englund MM, Egeland B, Oliva EM, Collins WA. Childhood and adolescent predictors of heavy drinking and alcohol use disorders in early adulthood: a longitudinal developmental analysis. Addiction. 2008;103(Suppl 1):23–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Maggs JL, Staff J, Kloska DD, Patrick ME, O’Malley PM, Schulenberg J. Predicting young adult degree attainment by late adolescent marijuana use. J Adolesc Health. 2015;57(2):205–211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bryant AL, Schulenberg JE, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Johnston LD. How academic achievement, attitudes, and behaviors relate to the course of substance use during adolescence: a 6-year, multiwave national longitudinal study. J Res Adolesc. 2003;13(3):361–397. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Brière FN, Fallu J-S, Morizot J, Janosz M. Adolescent illicit drug use and subsequent academic and psychosocial adjustment: an examination of socially-mediated pathways. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;135:45–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brown SA, Tapert SF, Granholm E, Delis DC. Neurocognitive functioning of adolescents: effects of protracted alcohol use. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2000;24(2):164–171. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.De Bellis MD, Clark DB, Beers SR, Soloff PH, Boring AM, Hall J, Kersh A, Keshavan MS. Hippocampal volume in adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(5):737–744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Jacobus J, Tapert SF. Neurotoxic effects of alcohol in adolescence. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9:703–721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Crean RD, Crane NA, Mason BJ. An evidence based review of acute and long-term effects of cannabis use on executive cognitive functions. J Addict Med. 2011;5(1):1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Cuzen NL, Koopowitz S-M, Ferrett HL, Stein DJ, Yurgelun-Todd D. Methamphetamine and cannabis abuse in adolescence: a quasi-experimental study on specific and long-term neurocognitive effects. BMJ Open. 2015;5(1). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Meruelo AD, Castro N, Cota CI, Tapert SF. Cannabis and alcohol use, and the developing brain. Behav Brain Res. 2017;325(Pt A):44–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Battistella G, Fornari E, Annoni J-M, Chtioui H, Dao K, Fabritius M, Favrat B, Mall J- F, Maeder P, Giroud C. Long-term effects of cannabis on brain structure. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(9):2041–2048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Medina KL, McQueeny T, Nagel BJ, Hanson KL, Schweinsburg AD, Tapert SF. Prefrontal cortex volumes in adolescents with alcohol use disorders: unique gender effects. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32(3):386–394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Medina KL, McQueeny T, Nagel BJ, Hanson KL, Yang TT, Tapert SF. Prefrontal cortex morphometry in abstinent adolescent marijuana users: subtle gender effects. Addic Biol. 2009;14(4):457–468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Tapert SF, Schweinsburg AD, Drummond SP, Paulus MP, Brown SA, Yang TT, Frank LR. Functional MRI of inhibitory processing in abstinent adolescent marijuana users. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2007;194(2):173–183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Arria AM, Caldeira KM, Bugbee BA, Vincent KB, O’Grady KE. The academic opportunity costs of substance use during college. 2013. College Park, MD: Center on Young Adult Health and Development; Available at: www.cls.umd.edu/docs/AcadOppCosts.pdf. Accessed June 29, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 41.van Hemel-Ruiter ME, de Jong PJ, Oldehinkel AJ, Ostafin BD. Reward-related attentional biases and adolescent substance use: the TRAILS study. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(1):142–150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hawkins JD, Graham JW, Maguin E, Abbott R, Hill KG, Catalano RF. Exploring the effects of age of alcohol use initiation and psychosocial risk factors on subsequent alcohol misuse. J Stud Alcohol. 1997;58(3):280–290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hawkins JD, Guo J, Hill KG, Battin-Pearson S, Abbott RD. Long-term effects of the Seattle Social Development Intervention on school bonding trajectories. Appl Dev Sci. 2001;5(4):225–236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research. National Addiction and HIV Data Archive Program. 2017; http://www.icpsr.umich.edu/icpsrweb/NAHDAP/. Accessed June 5, 2017.
- 45.Bachman JG, Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Schulenberg JE, Miech RA. The Monitoring the Future project after four decades: design and procedures Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan; Occasional paper 82. 2015. Available at: http://monitoringthefuture.org/pubs/occpapers/mtf-occ82.pdf. Accessed November 1, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Appleton JJ, Christenson SL, Furlong MJ. Student engagement with school: critical conceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Psychol Sch. 2008;45(5):369–386. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Fredricks JA, Blumenfeld PC, Paris AH. School engagement: potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Rev Educ Res. 2004;74(1):59–109. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Crosnoe R, Benner AD, Schneider B. Drinking, socioemotional functioning, and academic progress in secondary school. J Health Soc Behav. 2012;53(2):150–164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Balsa AI, Giuliano LM, French MT. The effects of alcohol use on academic achievement in high school. Econ Educ Rev. 2011;30(1):1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Clark TT, Belgrave FZ, Nasim A. Risk and protective factors for substance use among urban African American adolescents considered high-risk. J Ethn Subst Abuse. 2008;7(3):292–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Martins SS, Alexandre PK. The association of ecstasy use and academic achievement among adolescents in two U.S. national surveys. Addict Behav. 2009;34(1):9–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Li Y, Lerner RM. Trajectories of school engagement during adolescence: implications for grades, depression, delinquency, and substance use. Dev Psychol. 2011;47(1):233–247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. School connectedness: strategies for increasing protective factors among youth. 2009. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/healthyyouth/protective/pdf/connectedness.pdf. Accessed December 7, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Jessor R Problem-behavior theory, psychosocial development, and adolescent problem drinking. Br J Addict. 1987;82(4):331–342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Donovan JE, Jessor R. Structure of problem behavior in adolescence and young adulthood. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1985;53(6):890–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Bartlett R, Holditch-Davis D, Belyea M. Cluster of problem behaviors in adolescents. Res Nurs Health. 2005;28(3):230–239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Balsa AI, Homer JF, French MT, Weisner CM. Substance use, education, employment, and criminal activity outcomes of adolescents in outpatient chemical dependency programs. J Behav Health Serv Res. 2009;36(1):75–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Engberg J, Morral AR. Reducing substance use improves adolescents’ school attendance. Addiction. 2006;101(12):1741–1751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Anderson KG, Ramo DE, Cummins KM, Brown SA. Alcohol and drug involvement after adolescent treatment and functioning during emerging adulthood. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2010;107(2–3):171–181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Griffin KW, Botvin GJ. Evidence-based interventions for preventing substance use disorders in adolescents. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2010;19(3):505–526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Terry-McElrath YM, O’Malley PM, Johnston LD. Saying no to marijuana: why American youth report quitting or abstaining. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2008;69(6):796–805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Forlini C, Racine E. Disagreements with implications: diverging discourses on the ethics of non-medical use of methylphenidate for performance enhancement. BMC Med Ethics. 2009;10(1):1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Partridge BJ, Bell SK, Lucke JC, Yeates S, Hall WD. Smart drugs “as common as coffee”: media hype about neuroenhancement. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(11):e28416–e28416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Arria AM, Caldeira KM, Vincent KB, O’Grady KE, Cimini MD, Geisner IM, Fossos-Wong N, Kilmer JR, Larimer ME. Do college students improve their grades by using prescription stimulants nonmedically? Addict Behav. 2017;65:245–249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Arria AM, Wilcox HC, Caldeira KM, Vincent KB, Garnier-Dykstra LM, O’Grady KE. Dispelling the myth of “smart drugs”: cannabis and alcohol use problems predict nonmedical use of prescription stimulants for studying. Addict Behav. 2013;38(3):1643–1650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Boyd CJ, McCabe SE, Cranford JA, Young A. Adolescents’ motivations to abuse prescription medications. Pediatrics. 2006;118(6):2472–2480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Van Ryzin MJ, Fosco GM, Dishion TJ. Family and peer predictors of substance use from early adolescence to early adulthood: an 11-year prospective analysis. Addict Behav. 2012;37(12):1314–1324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Stanard P, Belgrave FZ, Corneille MA, Wilson KD, Owens K. Promoting academic achievement: the role of peers and family in the academic engagement of African American adolescents. J Prev Interv Community. 2010;38(3):198–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Branstetter S, Furman W. Buffering effect of parental monitoring knowledge and parent-adolescent relationships on consequences of adolescent substance use. J Child Fam Stud. 2013;22(2):192–198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Clark HK, Shamblen SR, Ringwalt CL, Hanley S. Predicting high risk adolescents’ substance use over time: the role of parental monitoring. J Prim Prev. 2012;33(2–3):67–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Arria AM, Kuhn V, Caldeira KM, O’Grady KE, Vincent KB, Wish ED. High school drinking mediates the relationship between parental monitoring and college drinking: a longitudinal analysis. Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy. 2008;3(6):1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Committee on Substance Abuse. Policy statement: substance use screening, brief intervention, and referral to treatment for pediatricians. Pediatrics. 2011;128(5):e1330–e1340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Beaton A, Shubkin CD, Chapman S. Addressing substance misuse in adolescents: a review of the literature on the screening, brief intervention, and referral to treatment model. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2016;28(2):258–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Levy SJL, Williams JF, Ryan SA, Gonzalez PK, Patrick SW, Quigley J, Siqueira L, Smith VC, Walker LR. Substance use screening, brief intervention, and referral to treatment. Pediatrics. 2016;138(1):e20161211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Conway KP, Vullo GC, Nichter B, Wang J, Compton WM, Iannotti RJ, Simons-Morton B. Prevalence and patterns of polysubstance use in a nationally representative sample of 10th graders in the United States. J Adolesc Health. 2013;52(6):716–723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.O’Grady KE, Arria AM, Fitzelle DB, Wish ED. Heavy drinking and polydrug use among college students. J Drug Issues. 2008;39(2):445–466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Hinshaw SP. Externalizing behavior problems and academic underachievement in childhood and adolescence: causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychol Bull. 1992;111(1):127–155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Henry K Skipping school and using drugs: a brief report. Drugs Educ Prev Polic. 2010;17(5):650–657. [Google Scholar]
- 79.Council on School Health and Committee on Substance Abuse. The role of schools in combating illicit substance abuse. Pediatrics. 2007;120(6):1379–1384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Evans-Whipp TJ, Plenty SM, Catalano RF, Herrenkohl TI, Toumbourou JW. Longitudinal effects of school drug policies on student marijuana use in Washington State and Victoria, Australia. Am J Public Health. 2015;105(5):994–1000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Faggiano F, Minozzi S, Versino E, Buscemi D. Universal school-based prevention for illicit drug use. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2014(12). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Carney T, Myers B. Effectiveness of early interventions for substance-using adolescents: findings from a systematic review and meta-analysis. Subst Abuse Treat Prev Policy. 2012;7:25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Cuijpers P Effective ingredients of school-based drug prevention programs: a systematic review. Addict Behav. 2002;27(6):1009–1023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Kellam SG, Wang W, Mackenzie AC, Brown CH, Ompad DC, Or F, Ialongo NS, Poduska JM, Windham A. The impact of the Good Behavior Game, a universal classroom-based preventive intervention in first and second grades, on high-risk sexual behaviors and drug abuse and dependence disorders into young adulthood. Prev Sci. 2014;15(Suppl 1):6–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Harris B, Shaw B, Lawson H, Sherman B. Barriers to addressing adolescent substance use: perceptions of New York school-based health center providers. J Sch Health. 2016;86(2):96–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Knight JR, Sherritt L, Shrier LA, Harris SK, Chang G. Validity of the CRAFFT Substance Abuse Screening Test among adolescent clinic patients. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2002;156(6):607–614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Falck RS, Nahhas RW, Li L, Carlson RG. Surveying teens in school to assess the prevalence of problematic drug use. J Sch Health. 2012;82(5):217–224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]