Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 Jan 25;8:e42138. doi: 10.7554/eLife.42138

Motor context dominates output from purkinje cell functional regions during reflexive visuomotor behaviours

Laura D Knogler 1, Andreas M Kist 1, Ruben Portugues 1,
Editors: Indira M Raman2, Ronald L Calabrese3
PMCID: PMC6374073  PMID: 30681408

Abstract

The cerebellum integrates sensory stimuli and motor actions to enable smooth coordination and motor learning. Here we harness the innate behavioral repertoire of the larval zebrafish to characterize the spatiotemporal dynamics of feature coding across the entire Purkinje cell population during visual stimuli and the reflexive behaviors that they elicit. Population imaging reveals three spatially-clustered regions of Purkinje cell activity along the rostrocaudal axis. Complementary single-cell electrophysiological recordings assign these Purkinje cells to one of three functional phenotypes that encode a specific visual, and not motor, signal via complex spikes. In contrast, simple spike output of most Purkinje cells is strongly driven by motor-related tail and eye signals. Interactions between complex and simple spikes show heterogeneous modulation patterns across different Purkinje cells, which become temporally restricted during swimming episodes. Our findings reveal how sensorimotor information is encoded by individual Purkinje cells and organized into behavioral modules across the entire cerebellum.

Research organism: Zebrafish

Introduction

Decades of influential anatomical (Eccles et al., 1967; Palay and Chan-Palay, 1974), theoretical (Marr, 1969; Albus, 1971; Ito, 1972) and experimental work (see Ito, 2006 for review) have led to our current knowledge highlighting the cerebellum as a major brain region for the control of motor behaviors. This ability to coordinate motor control and learning relies critically on the integration of sensory and motor-related signals in Purkinje cells, as these neurons constitute the main computational units and output of the cerebellum. In order to understand the detailed operations of the cerebellum, it is therefore of fundamental importance to characterize the physiology of cerebellar neurons, especially the Purkinje cells, during sensorimotor behaviors.

Purkinje cells receive two excitatory input streams, via parallel fibers from granule cells and a single climbing fiber from the inferior olive, that differentially modulate their spike output. Across vertebrate species, climbing fibers from inferior olivary neurons drive complex spikes in Purkinje cells at a spontaneous rate of ~0.5–2 Hz whereas parallel fiber inputs modulate intrinsic simple spike activity at much higher rates (from tens of Hz in larval zebrafish up to hundreds of Hz in mammals; Hsieh et al., 2014; Eccles et al., 1967; Raman and Bean, 1997). Simple spike output can furthermore be biased to burst or pause by the arrival of a complex spike (Mathews et al., 2012; Badura et al., 2013; Sengupta and Thirumalai, 2015), though the precise nature of this relationship varies across Purkinje cells (Zhou et al., 2014; Zhou et al., 2015; Xiao et al., 2014). In addition, inhibitory interneurons may also exert considerable control over simple spike rates (Dizon and Khodakhah, 2011; ten Brinke et al., 2015; Jelitai et al., 2016). Characterizing the type of information carried by these different input streams at the population level and disentangling their relative contributions to Purkinje cell output has been challenging due to the large number of Purkinje cells in the mammalian cerebellum (>300,000 in the rat cerebellum) receiving convergent input from 100,000 to 200,000 parallel fibers (Harvey and Napper, 1991).

Due to this complicated physiology, the anatomy of climbing fiber projections onto Purkinje cells has primarily been used to characterize the organization of the mammalian cerebellum. Four transverse zones along the rostrocaudal axis have been described (Ozol et al., 1999) that can be further subdivided into longitudinal zones and microzones defined by additional anatomical, physiological and molecular features (see Apps and Hawkes, 2009 for review). This organization is thought to produce functional modules that each participate in the control of a certain set of behaviors (Cerminara and Apps, 2011). However, since these regions have been largely defined in terms of anatomical rather than physiological properties, the behavioral relevance of cerebellar modules is not well understood. Purkinje cells are at the center of cerebellar circuits, integrating climbing fiber and parallel fiber input. A detailed description of the flow of sensory and motor information within both individual and groups of Purkinje cells is important to understand the functional significance of these proposed behavioral modules.

A fundamental first step is therefore a population-level investigation of Purkinje cell activity during a simple set of sensorimotor behaviors with single-cell resolution of simple and complex spikes. In this study, we took advantage of the larval zebrafish to study how sensorimotor variables are encoded in Purkinje cell output during reflexive, visually-driven motor behaviors. The larval zebrafish cerebellum is anatomically organized in a typical vertebrate tri-layered configuration, with a population of fewer than 500 Purkinje cells, each receiving inputs from many parallel fibers and likely just one climbing fiber (Bae et al., 2009; Hashimoto and Hibi, 2012; Hsieh et al., 2014; Hamling et al., 2015). Several studies have demonstrated a functional role for the cerebellum in the larval zebrafish relating to motor coordination, adaptation, and learning (Aizenberg and Schuman, 2011; Ahrens et al., 2012; Matsui et al., 2014; Portugues et al., 2014; Harmon et al., 2017). The behavioral repertoire of the larval zebrafish includes robust but variable swimming and eye movements to drifting gratings and rotating stimuli (the optomotor and optokinetic response, respectively). These visual stimuli are particularly useful because they elicit graded, episodic swim bouts and eye movements that vary across trials, allowing us to disambiguate clearly between sensory and motor responses. We are furthermore able to extract many different features from both the visual stimuli and motor behaviors (i.e. onset, direction, velocity) to pinpoint how Purkinje cell activity correlates with particular features of visual stimuli at a fine temporal scale.

Using this approach, in this study we investigated three main questions: (1) how motor and sensory information is encoded in individual Purkinje cells from different input pathways, (2) how the temporal dynamics of these different information streams are encoded in Purkinje cell output, and (3) how responses are spatially organized across the entire cerebellum. Calcium imaging across the whole cerebellum to the same set of visual stimuli in tandem with tail-free and eye-free behavior revealed considerable spatial segregation in Purkinje responses. We supplemented calcium imaging data with direct electrophysiological recordings in order to examine complex and simple spikes directly under conditions of fictive or eye-free behavior. In agreement with our imaging data, we uncovered a consistent and striking organization of the Purkinje cell population into three functional regions along the rostrocaudal axis that encode visual information with respect to either directional motion onset, rotational motion velocity, or changes in luminance. The fine temporal resolution of our electrophysiological recordings together with our ability to disentangle different sensorimotor variables revealed that these regions receive similar motor-related parallel fiber input but are strongly differentiated by sensory complex spike responses that encode distinct visual features with unique temporal dynamics. We relate these findings to other work in the field to propose an overarching organization of the larval zebrafish cerebellum into cerebellar modules underlying innate and flexible visually-driven behaviors.

Results

Activity in the cerebellum is arranged into functionally-defined and anatomically-clustered symmetrical regions of Purkinje cells

Anatomical, physiological, and genetic studies of the mammalian cerebellum across species show that the cerebellar cortex is organized into spatially-restricted regions of Purkinje cells, where a given region has a specific set of inputs and outputs and is thought to control the coordination and adaptation of a different set of sensorimotor behaviors (Apps and Hawkes, 2009; Witter and De Zeeuw, 2015). In order to describe the organization of Purkinje cell responses across the entire cerebellum with high spatial resolution, we performed two-photon calcium imaging across the complete population of Purkinje cells while presenting a variety of visual stimuli that drive variable, reflexive sensorimotor behaviors (Easter and Nicola, 1996) to awake, head-embedded larval zebrafish whose eyes and tail were freed and could move (Figure 1a,b; see Video 1 for an animation of visual stimuli as presented to the fish during two-photon imaging experiments).

Figure 1. Using population imaging and multilinear regression to describe feature responses across the Purkinje population during visuomotor behaviors.

(a) Cartoon of the embedded zebrafish preparation under the two-photon microscope with freely-moving eyes and tail. (b) Overview of the visual stimuli presented to the awake, behaving zebrafish during volumetric two-photon calcium imaging. See Materials and methods for further details. The mean swimming activity and eye position for a representative fish across an entire experiment is shown (N = 100 trials). (c) Composite bright field image of a seven dpf zebrafish larva from a dorsal view showing Purkinje cells expressing GCaMP6s driven by a ca8 enhancer element. Scale bar = 100 microns. (d) Overview of the multilinear regression analysis. See Materials and methods for additional details and see Figure 1—figure supplement 1 for full list of regressors. (e) Left panels, example calcium signal from a Purkinje cell across two planes (black trace) can be well recapitulated through multilinear regression (MLR, grey trace; R2 = 0.77). The regressors with the seven largest coefficients (β) are shown below scaled in height and colored by their β value (blue = positive, red = negative). The asterisk for regressor four refers to a negative value of β which results in an inverted regressor. Right, a bar graph quantifying the normalized β values for all regressors for this cell with the regressors shown at left labelled. See also Figure 1—figure supplements 1 and 2.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Functional imaging anatomy and full regressor list.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(a) Single imaging planes showing PC:GCaMP6s fluorescence as obtained from confocal imaging (upper panel) and during two-photon experiments (middle panel). Lower panel, a single confocal imaging plane from a PC:NLS-GCaMP6s fish where GCaMP is restricted to the nucleus. Red arrowheads indicate example Purkinje cell somata. Scale car = 25 microns. (b) Quantification of Purkinje cells in the entire cerebellum at seven dpf as counted in the PC:NLS-GCaMP6s line. N = 3 fish. (c) The complete set of regressors used in analysis of calcium imaging data. Individual regressors fall into one of five categories (three sensory and two motor), as indicated by the categories at right. Tail and eye motor regressors are calculated for each imaging plane based on the motor activity during that trial, therefore a representative example from one trial in the dataset is shown here. See also Videos 1 and 2 for example imaging trials with the sequence of visual stimuli displayed. (d) Projections of the first ten principal components of Purkinje cell activity in response to experimental stimuli across all fish (N = 6; see Materials and methods), ordered in increasing variance explained. Components that show a high degree of anatomical clustering are colored. Colors are arbitrarily chosen.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Calcium signals report complex spikes reliably but can also report simple spike bursts.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(a) Example cell-attached electrophysiological recording (ephys, black trace) and simultaneously recorded fluorescence trace (green) from a Purkinje cell expressing GCaMP6s under the Purkinje cell-specific ca8 enhancer. All complex spikes (orange dots) are accompanied by an increase in fluorescence as shown as a deflection in the fluorescence trace that accounts for every peak in the complex spike regressor (orange trace, spike rate convolved with GCaMP kernel). In contrast, only high frequency bursts of simple spikes (blue dots) influence the fluorescence signal (indicated by blue arrowheads). (b) The mean spike-triggered fluorescence signal and standard deviation is plotted for the example cell from a) for complex and simple spike bursts (N = 25 each). (c) A composite epifluorescent image showing a bright field dorsal view of the cerebellum together with single-cell GCaMP expression in the Purkinje cell from the previous panels and the rhodamine-filled electrode contacting this cell. The outline and midline of the cerebellum is indicated by the dashed white line. Scale bar = 50 microns. (d) The mean spike-triggered fluorescence signal and standard error is plotted for eight cells (N = 6 fish) for complex and simple spike bursts. (e) The relative contribution of the complex spike (CS) and simple spike (SS) regressors (spike rates convolved with the GCaMP kernel) to the fluorescence signal in each cell as determined by least squares regression (see Materials and methods) across the eight cells. The example cell from a) is indicated. (f) The location of all example cells, color-coded by relative SS regressor contribution. (g) Overview of i) the morphology of a singly-labelled Purkinje cell and the subcellular regions of interest (ROIs) with ii) corresponding calcium signals obtained from high resolution two-photon imaging (see Materials and methods). Scale bars = 20 microns. (h) Quantification of the correlation coefficient between the calcium signal from the most distal dendritic segment and the soma. N = 5 cells from three fish.

Video 1. Z-projection map of GCaMP6s responses (max dF/F) in Purkinje cells to visual stimuli.

Download video file (838.7KB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42138.004

Related to Figure 2.

Figure 2. Purkinje cell activity is functionally clustered across the cerebellum.

Figure 2.

(a) Heatmaps of the z-projected mean voxelwise correlation coefficients from multilinear regression (MLR) with example sensory and motor regressors for a representative fish (see Materials and methods). Scale bar = 50 microns. (b) Voxels from the example fish in a) are colored according to whether the best regressor for correlated sensory stimuli and motor events (including i) swimming and ii) eye movement) are sensory (magenta), motor (green), or equal/uncorrelated (white). (c) Left, quantification of principal component analysis, clustering, and stereotypy of Purkinje cell responses. Left axis, index values across the first ten principal components with respect to the anatomical clustering of principal components within a fish (red line) and the stereotypy of these clusters across fish (blue line). Dotted black line shows an index value of 1 (equivalent to chance). Right axis, total variance explained across principal components. Right panel, mean spatial mapping of the four principal components with the highest index values for anatomical clustering and stereotypy as individual maps (above) and composite (below). Colors are arbitrarily chosen.

We observed frequent eye and tail movements that varied across visual stimuli and across trials (Figure 1b). Whole-field gratings moving in the four cardinal directions elicited reflexive but variable optomotor swimming responses. Swimming episodes (bouts) were evoked in a probabilistic manner that was modulated by the direction and speed of the visual motion (i.e. no swim response to gratings in the reverse direction). A windmill pattern centered on the larva’s head rotating with a sinusoidal velocity elicited a reflexive optokinetic response of the eyes that also showed some behavioral variability across trials. Moderate intensity whole-field flashes were included to provide stimuli that evoke no acute behavioral response (Figure 1b) but that could nonetheless contribute to ethological behaviors over longer timescales, for example relating to circadian rhythms (Burgess and Granato, 2007). The visual stimuli were presented in open loop (i.e. with no updating of the visual stimuli in response to behavior) in order to clearly dissociate the sensory stimuli and any behavioral response. It should be noted that visually-driven motor behaviors are robust on average but episodic and variable across trials, allowing us to clearly disambiguate sensory and motor contributions to neuronal activity when we examine the correlations between Purkinje cell activity and eye or tail motor activity on a trial by trial basis.

We used two-photon calcium imaging to image neural activity in 7 days-post fertilization (dpf) zebrafish larvae expressing GCaMP6s in all Purkinje cells (Figure 1c, Figure 1—figure supplement 1a). This strategy allowed us to measure the entire Purkinje cell population in response to this set of stimuli with high spatial resolution while tracking eye and tail movements. Neural responses to these stimuli showed considerable temporal and spatial structure across the cerebellum, as visualized by the z-projection map of average calcium responses (max dF/F) in the entire Purkinje cell population across the trial (Video 1) as well as in the activity from single imaging planes (Video 2). We estimated the number of Purkinje cells in the larval zebrafish to be 433 ± 19 (mean ± std, N = 3) by identifying spherical nuclei in confocal stacks of a transgenic line that expresses nuclear-localized GCaMP6s using 3D template matching (Figure 1—figure supplement 1; see Materials and methods). This number is higher than the previously reported range of 180–360 Purkinje cells at seven dpf (N = 6; Hamling et al., 2015).

Video 2. Single plane at −35 microns depth from the dorsal surface showing GCaMP6s responses (max dF/F) in Purkinje cells to visual stimuli.

Download video file (1.1MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42138.005

Related to Figure 2.

In order to quantify how different features of the visual stimuli and the tail and eye behaviors contribute to Purkinje cell activity, we performed multilinear regression on voxelwise calcium signals obtained across the Purkinje cell population (Figure 1d, Figure 1—figure supplement 1; see Materials and methods for detailed description). Multilinear regression is advantageous for two reasons in particular. First, it allows the identification of multiple visual and/or motor features that may contribute to a single calcium signal. Second, we can distinguish between regressors that may be moderately correlated in our experiments, such as forward moving gratings and the variable swim bouts that these stimuli elicit. Zebrafish swim in episodic bursts of swimming that last just hundreds of milliseconds, separated by rest periods lasting seconds, whereas the visual stimuli driving these swim bouts were presented for many seconds. As a result, motor regressors for eye or tail movements look very different from visual sensory regressors (Figure 1d,e, Figure 1—figure supplement 1) and their respective contributions to calcium signals can be determined.

Our analysis showed that Purkinje cell activity is functionally segregated across the cerebellum with respect to different visual and motor features (Figure 2a, Video 3). Responses to whole-field flashes were enriched in a bilaterally symmetric central region of the cerebellar cortex, whereas responses to clockwise and counterclockwise rotational motion had an asymmetric localization within the left and right hemisphere of the caudolateral cerebellum, respectively. Purkinje cell responses to motor activity, including eye and tail motion, were generally broad and showed strong, uniform correlations across most of the cerebellar cortex.

Video 3. Upper left, anatomical stack of Purkinje cell anatomy (upper left) showing the depth in microns of the plane from the dorsal surface.

Download video file (1.8MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42138.008

Other panels, the corresponding plane from the stack of regressor coefficient weights (labelled for regressor type) for all Purkinje cells as quantified with multilinear regression (see Materials and methods). Related to Figure 2.

Next, to disambiguate between visual and motor responses we explicitly visualized the sensory/motor preference across the Purkinje cell layer for two visuomotor behaviors: swimming driven by forward-moving gratings, and left/right eye movements driven by rotational windmill motion. Figure 2b shows a z-projection of the cerebellum for each of these visuomotor behaviors, with areas colored magenta or green based on whether the relevant visual or motor feature was significantly better in explaining the activity in that region (see Materials and methods). As Figure 2bi shows, the activity of Purkinje cells distributed across a broad region of the cerebellum correlated highly with tail movement during swimming and accounted for the modulation of calcium activity to a much greater extent that sensory grating motion. In contrast, Figure 2bii shows that a large dense bilateral area of the caudolateral cerebellum had activity that was more strongly related to sensory rotational motion while the remaining area of the rostral and medial cerebellum showed a stronger correlation with eye movements. These results indicate that locomotor activity of the tail and eyes is broadly encoded in Purkinje cell activity across the cerebellum whereas sensory responses to visual features are more anatomically clustered.

Finally, in order to identify groups of Purkinje cells whose activity was similarly modulated during the experiment, regardless of which feature drove the response, we performed principal component analysis on the coefficient weights for all cells across all fish (N = 10; see Materials and methods). This analysis again revealed considerable spatial structure and stereotypy in Purkinje cell responses, with most functional clusters being also both anatomically clustered within fish and similarly located across fish (Figure 2c, Figure 1—figure supplement 1d). Four functional clusters emerged that were particularly spatially-clustered and tiled the cerebellum across the rostrocaudal axis of each hemisphere (Figure 2c). Together, these results suggest a clear spatial organization of Purkinje cells into functional regions along the rostrocaudal axis of the zebrafish cerebellum.

Calcium signals in Purkinje cells report complex spikes with high fidelity with lesser contributions from simple spikes

Since Purkinje cells receive excitatory inputs from both climbing fibers and parallel fibers that drive different types of spiking, it is critical to understand exactly what the calcium signals described above represent in terms of the underlying spike identity and structure. Climbing fiber inputs driving complex spikes have been shown to reliably produce large dendritic calcium signals in mammalian Purkinje cells with little to no signal in the soma (Lev-Ram et al., 1992). In contrast, parallel fiber inputs may contribute to small, local calcium signals at dendritic spines or branchlets (see Kitamura and Kano, 2013 for review) while changes in sodium-dependent simple spike rates may be read out from somatic calcium signals (Ramirez and Stell, 2016). We performed in vivo cell-attached electrophysiological recordings of spontaneous activity from single Purkinje cells expressing GCaMP6s in order to show how the signals obtained during calcium imaging relate to complex and simple spike output in larval zebrafish Purkinje cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 2).

As expected, we found that every complex spike elicited a peak in the calcium signal of a Purkinje cell’s dendrites (Figure 1—figure supplement 2a,b). However, we also found that isolated bursts of simple spikes correlated with widespread increases in the dendritic calcium signal. Aligning the calcium signal to the onset of simple spike bursts and single complex spike events showed consistent simple spike-triggered calcium transients that were of smaller amplitude but similar duration to complex spike-triggered transients (Figure 1—figure supplement 2b,d). We used multilinear regression methods to determine the relative contribution from the activity of complex and simple spikes to the calcium signals we measured in different Purkinje cells across fish. We find that although the majority of the signal is driven by the occurrence of a complex spike, simple spikes also contribute to a varying degree across cells and can account for up to half of the calcium signal (mean percentage contribution from complex spikes = 78.4 ± 6.8%, N = 8 cells from eight fish; Figure 1—figure supplement 2e,f).

These findings reveal that both complex spikes and simple spike bursts can contribute to the dendritic fluorescence signals obtained by calcium imaging in larval zebrafish Purkinje cells. The observation above that many visual and motor features can contribute to the calcium signal from a single Purkinje cell (Figure 1e) is therefore unsurprising if this signal represents not only complex spikes but also simple spike responses modulated by the convergent input from many parallel fibers. We furthermore observed that somatic signals and dendritic signals were highly correlated with each other (mean correlation = 0.87 ± 0.2, N = 5 cells from three fish; Figure 1—figure supplement 2g,h), suggesting that the contribution from these different input streams may not be as spatially segregated in these Purkinje cells as shown in other systems and therefore cannot be isolated by subcellular imaging. In summary, calcium signals across Purkinje cells report both complex spikes and high frequency simple spiking and care must therefore be taken when interpreting the underlying activity patterns of Purkinje cells measured with functional imaging.

Electrophysiological recordings from Purkinje cells reveal distinct complex spike responses that can be grouped into three primary visual response phenotypes

In order to overcome the mixed contribution of complex spikes and simple spike bursts to calcium signals and to record Purkinje cell spiking activity in greater detail, we turned to single-cell electrophysiology. We performed cell-attached Purkinje cell electrophysiological recordings at different locations across the cerebellum in the awake, paralyzed larval zebrafish while presenting visual stimuli as for the functional imaging experiments described above (N = 61 cells from 61 fish). Complex spikes and simple spikes can be clearly distinguished in these recordings with automated thresholding by amplitude (Figure 3a) and converted to a spike rate (Figure 3b; see Materials and methods). Simultaneous fictive recordings of locomotor activity were obtained from a ventral root extracellular electrode (Figure 3a) as previously described (Masino and Fetcho, 2005) and used to extract information about fictive swim bouts (see Materiials and methods). The high temporal resolution of electrophysiological recordings further enhances our ability to separate feature components. For example, we find that swimming activity is only moderately correlated with forward visual motion on a trial by trial basis (mean correlation = 0.31 ± 0.2, Figure 3c, Figure 3—figure supplement 2).

Figure 3. Electrophysiological recordings from Purkinje cells reveal distinct complex spike responses that can be grouped into four primary response types corresponding to sensory or motor features.

(a) Cartoon of the embedded, paralyzed zebrafish preparation used for simultaneous Purkinje cell (PC) electrophysiology with fictive swimming patterns extracted from the ventral root (VR). (b) Example single trial from a cell-attached Purkinje cell (PC) recording (upper trace, black) with simultaneous ventral root recording (lower trace, gray, shown as a moving standard deviation). Complex spikes in the PC are indicated by orange dots above the trace and simple spikes are indicated by blue dots below the trace. Stimuli are color-coded as before (see Figure 1 and Materials and methods for more details). (b) Left, the mean simple spike (SS) and complex spike (CS) rate for the cell shown in (a) across five trials. Right, the correlation coefficients of forward, left and rightward grating motion with the trial by trial fictive swim activity for all fish. (c) Plot of the correlation coefficient for each fish between the regressor for concatenated swimming activity during moving forward, left, and right gratings across all trials and the summed sensory regressor for forward, left, and right grating motion. The mean is indicated by the black bar. (d) Example mean complex spike rate extracts from three different Purkinje cells showing the temporal similarity of firing dynamics with visual feature regressors. (e) Above, heatmap of coefficient weights for the complex spike firing rates of 61 cells from z-scored least-squares multilinear regression (MLR) with a full set of 24 stimulus- and motor-related variables (see Materials and methods). Below, histogram showing the distribution of cells’ highest regressor weight. (f) Location of these cells across all fish mapped onto a reference cerebellum (dorsal view). The color indicates the highest MLR coefficient weight for that cell while the size indicates the degree to which that coefficient contributes to the overall firing rate respective to the others, where the biggest circles = 100%. Scale bar = 50 microns. (g) Left, heatmap of complex spike rates for all 61 cells clustered according to the category of their highest MLR coefficient weight (e.g. luminance, rotational motion, swimming). Colored bars at right indicate complex spike category as indicated in previous panels. Right, the mean z-scored complex spike rate from each cluster. See also Figure 3—figure supplements 1 and 2.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Sensory and motor regressors used for multilinear least-squares regression with electrophysiological recordings Top left, cartoon of recording setup.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Top center, description of stimuli used in the electrophysiological experiments (see Materials and methods and Figure 2 for details). Gratings speeds are 10 mm/s with additional slow (Fslow, 3 mm/s) and fast (Ffast, 30 mm/s) speeds for forward grating stimuli. The windmill stimulus rotated at sinusoidal velocities in the clockwise (CW) and counter-clockwise (CCW) directions with a frequency of 0.2 Hz. Six total periods were shown, with the first two periods being whole-field windmills, the second two periods restricted to the left visual field only, and the final two periods restricted to the right visual field only. Below, the complete set of regressors used in analysis of electrophysiological data. Individual regressors fall into one of five categories (four visual or one motor), as indicated by the colored bars and category names at the right, pertaining to either sensory or motor features as categorized at left. Regressors 19–21 are calculated for each cell based on the motor activity in that trial, therefore a representative example from one trial in the dataset is shown here.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Visually-evoked swimming responses to forward gratings are episodic, vary across trials, and are clearly resolvable from visual responses.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(a) Shown is the mean simple spike rate (upper trace, blue), complex spike rate (middle trace, orange), and swim bout vigor (lower trace, grey) across trials for an example fish. The compressed time scale and trial averaging suggests that visual and motor responses to forward-moving gratings may be correlated with each other as well as with both simple and complex spike rates. Multilinear analysis (summarized in text at right) finds however that the coefficient weights for motor regressors are large for simple spike activity across trials while they are zero for complex spike activity. Conversely, direction motion onset regressors for visual motion contribute to the majority of the complex spike activity and to less than 5% of simple spike activity. (b) Upper traces, the boxed area in (a) is shown on an expanded timescale and for five individual trials (numbered at left) in order to better show the temporal structure of neural and behavioral responses to visual stimuli. Excerpts from cell-attached recordings from this Purkinje cell (PC) and simultaneous ventral root (VR) show reliable complex spikes elicited at visual motion onset and swim bouts of varying durations and strength evoked at different latencies (up to two seconds) from visual stimulus onset. Note that trials occur where the visual stimulus can fail to elicit a complex spike (red asterisks) or a bout (purple asterisk). Lower traces, the average traces from these stimuli are also shown on an expanded timescale to drive home the point that although average activity may look correlated, the variability of visually-evoked behaviors across trials allows multilinear regression to clearly separate visual and motor responses in simple spike and complex spike activity. The forward motion onset regressor, which captures spiking responses in the 500 ms window following visual stimulus onset, is also shown for comparison.

In an approach similar to that used to analyze the functional imaging results presented above, we built regressors to capture the most salient features of the visual and motor stimuli (see Figure 3d for examples and Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for the full regressor list). The high temporal resolution of electrophysiology allows us to resolve transient changes in simple spike firing rate as well as single complex spikes, therefore we added regressors for the visual and motor regressors that would capture spiking responses to a more specific set of visual stimulus and behavioral features such as visual motion onset, duration, velocity, swim onset, and graded swim strength. The window chosen for stimulus onset covered 500 milliseconds from actual stimulus onset (e.g. motion onset of forward gratings) in order to account for the inherent synaptic delays for visual information to arrive in the cerebellar input layer, on the order of 100–200 milliseconds (Knogler et al., 2017). Preliminary assessments of spike rates with visual and motor feature regressors further confirmed that these regressors appropriately captured the temporal dynamics of Purkinje cell spiking (Figure 3d, Figure 3—figure supplement 2). We employed a variant of multilinear regression with elastic net optimization that includes regularization terms to help sparsify the number of features that are used to reconstruct the signal, as well as variable selection and parameter optimization to overcome the minor degree of correlation between some regressors (Figure 3e and Figure 3—figure supplement 1; see Materials and methods).

Since the complex spikes and simple spikes of Purkinje cells are modulated by climbing fiber and parallel fiber input streams, respectively, we independently assessed these responses across the population of cells (Figure 3e). We will first address the complex spike responses, as these provided a useful classification of Purkinje cell groups within the population in line with the functional and spatial organization seen during functional imaging.

We observed that complex spikes were generally evoked by a narrow subset of stimuli. Only a few visual or motor features provided a significant contribution to each cell’s complex spike rate (mean number of nonzero coefficients = 6.0 ± 0.4 out of 22, N = 61), and in many cases a single feature was very dominant (Figure 3e). Mixed complex spike responses to multiple stimuli are possible due to mixed selectivity in neurons of the inferior olive (Ohmae and Medina, 2015; Ju et al., 2018) or residual multiple climbing fiber input (Crepel et al., 1976). We found little evidence however that individual Purkinje cells encode multiple types of visual stimuli or both visual and motor features in their complex spike responses. The current results do not rule out the likely possibility that information from other sensory modalities than vision are also encoded in the complex spikes of these cells.

A survey of the best regressor category for each cell from this dataset revealed that Purkinje cell complex spike responses were strongly enriched for visual information (Figure 3e), specifically the onset of direction-specific translational motion (N = 31/61) and direction-specific rotational velocity (N = 14/61). The remaining Purkinje cells were categorized as having complex spikes that best responded to changes in whole-field luminance, to fictive motor activity, or to the duration of translational motion. Notably, sensory responses across visual features are far better represented than motor responses in the complex spike responses of Purkinje cells (Figure 3e). This was not due to a paucity of motor activity, as bouts of swimming behavior were consistently elicited across trials. Only 8/61 cells had the biggest contribution to complex spike rates from motor activity, and across the remaining cells the average contribution from motor regressors was less than 5% (3.7 ± 1%, N = 53). Of the eight cells whose best regressor was motor-related, there were nonetheless significant responses to visual features present as determined by non-zero sensory coefficient weights accounting for 10–40% of the complex spike activity (mean contribution = 20 ± 5%). As a result, we made the surprising observation that all but one of the Purkinje cells that we recorded from across the entire cerebellum could be unambiguously assigned to one of three visual complex spike ‘phenotypes’ corresponding to a response to directionally-selective translational motion onset, directionally-selective rotational velocity, or changes in luminance.

We hypothesized that these three different visual complex spike phenotypes could underlie the spatial clustering of Purkinje cell population activity that we observed with functional imaging (Figure 2c). Mapping the coordinates of all Purkinje cells onto a reference cerebellum revealed a spatial organization of complex spike sensory response phenotypes similar to our functional imaging data (Figure 3f). In particular, we observed a rostromedial cluster of cells responsive to the onset of directional motion in the visual stimulus and a caudolateral cluster of cells responsive to rotational stimulus velocity. Luminance responses were more scattered but generally occupied the central zone between these regions.

Together with our functional imaging data, these results suggest that zebrafish Purkinje cells contribute to the formation of three distinct spatial regions across each cerebellar hemisphere through visual complex spike profiles encoding either directionally-selective translational motion onset, directionally-selective rotational velocity, or changes in luminance. These regions bear a striking resemblance to the anatomically clustered activity patterns identified by principal component analysis in our imaging data (Figure 2c), suggesting that the visual complex spike response phenotype is an important parameter that can be used to understand the spatial and functional organization of Purkinje cells across the cerebellum.

Purkinje cells in different regions receive feature-specific climbing fiber input and project to different downstream regions

From the three major visual complex spike phenotypes we observed across the Purkinje cell population, we observed that further subdivisions could be made based on the specific type of response to a given visual stimulus. For example, direction-selective motion onset-responsive Purkinje cells differ in their directional tuning, and luminance-responsive cells can prefer either increases or decreases in luminance, or bidirectional changes (Figure 3d,g). Therefore, we next performed further detailed analyses of Purkinje cell complex spike activity in combination with additional anatomical experiments in order to quantify precisely how visual features such as directionality are encoded by different Purkinje cells with the same visual phenotype and to also identify the projection patterns of Purkinje cells across phenotypes.

The largest group of Purkinje cells showed a phenotype for strong, direction-selective responses to the onset of translational motion (N = 33/61 cells). These responses typically spanned two of the four cardinal directions tested, producing on average just one complex spike at the onset of motion in the preferred directions (1.2 ± 0.6 spikes/stimulus; Figure 4a). The occurrence of a complex spike was not dependent on the behavioral response since visually-evoked complex spikes occurred with equal probability whether there was a swimming response or not (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). In the clearest example, reverse visual motion evokes no swimming but is equally well-represented by a complex spike response at motion onset as the directions that do drive swimming (Figures 3g and 4a, Figure 3—figure supplement 2a).

Figure 4. Purkinje cells in different regions show complex spike responses that encode different visual features and one group sends outputs to a different downstream region.

(a) Raster plot (upper left panels) and histogram (lower left panels, 500 ms bins) of complex spikes occurring across trials during translational whole-field motion of black and white bars in all four cardinal directions for two example Purkinje cells (PC). Numbers assigned to PCs for this and panels b-c are arbitrary. (b) Raster plot (upper left panels) and histogram (lower left panels, 100 ms bins) of complex spikes occurring across trials during whole- and half-field bidirectional rotational motion of a black and white windmill for an example cell. The dashed lines over the histogram show the velocity of the stimulus in each direction across the trial. (c) Raster plot (upper left panels) and histogram (lower left panels, 100 ms bins) of complex spikes occurring across trials during whole-field light/dark flashes for two example cells, (i) and ii). (d) A box plot of complex spike firing rates during blank trials (no visual stimuli) for cells grouped by their sensory or motor complex spike category (see Figure 2). N = 31, 14, 5, 8. Asterisks indicate significance (one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc correction, p<0.001). j (i) The location of cells colored by complex spike phenotype are plotted onto a flattened dorsal view of the cerebellum with all coordinates flipped to the right half of the cerebellum. e (ii) Three example maximum projection images of traced axonal morphology from stochastically-labelled, Fyn-mClover3-expressing Purkinje cells for which electrophysiological recordings were also obtained. Labels for each cell refer to the electrophysiological traces in panels a-c. The asterisk for cell a) indicates that these coordinates were flipped to the right half of the cerebellum. Scale bar = 50 microns. e (iii) Categorical grouping of complex spike phenotypes for internal versus caudal axonal projections. N = 17 cells from 17 fish. (f) Morphed Purkinje cell axonal morphologies from single-cell labelling across fish (N = 50 cells) can be grouped into two populations based on axonal projection (as for e iii). N = 27 cells with internal axons, N = 23 cells with caudal axons. See also Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Complex spike responses encode specific aspects of visual features.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(a) Left, polar plot of all Purkinje cells with a significant contribution to complex spike activity from the onset of translational motion in a given direction (N = 33/61 cells). The tuning of the cells is indicated by the polar coordinates as well as the marker color, with 0° indicating forward motion. The distance from the center indicates the direction selectivity index of the cell (see Materials and methods). Dashed areas indicate the four quadrants used for binning. Right, the location of all cells with this complex spike phenotype within the quadrants are plotted onto a flattened dorsal view of the cerebellum (example cells i and ii from Figure 3a are indicated and outlined in black). The overview shows the zoomed-in region of the rostromedial cerebellum. Colors indicate tuning preference as shown at left. Dotted ellipses indicate the boundary for the mean location and SEM for each group of similarly tuned cells. Scale bar = 100 microns for overview, 500 microns for cropped zoom. (b) Upper plots, the mean complex spike firing rate (normalized to baseline, dotted black line) of all Purkinje cells with significant coefficient weights for rotational motion regressors (N = 11) is shown for both the duration of rotational motion in a given direction (left plot) and the duration of leftward and rightward translational motion (right plot). Two distinct groups are clearly seen that prefer either clockwise and rightward motion (orange lines) or counter-clockwise and leftward motion (blue lines). Lower panel, the location of all cells with this complex spike phenotype are plotted onto a flattened dorsal view of the cerebellum. Colors indicate rotational motion preference. Scale bar = 50 microns. (c) Left, all luminance-responsive cells as determined by significant autocorrelation values for whole-field flashes for the 2 s lag (N = 25/61) are plotted in a heat map sorted by maximum autocorrelation value for the 2 s lag. Cells are ordered by peak autocorrelation at 2 s. (d) Z-scored complex spike firing rates for all luminance-responsive Purkinje cells averaged across flash repetitions and sorted by the timing of their peak firing rate are shown as a heatmap. Black lines mark the transition from dark to light and back again as indicated by the grey bars above. Example cells from Figure 3c are indicated. N = 25 cells. (e) The location of all cells with a luminance complex spike phenotype are plotted onto a flattened dorsal view of the cerebellum with all coordinates flipped to the right half of the cerebellum. Colors indicate the preference for light or dark flashes (or both). Scale bar = 50 microns. (f) Four example Purkinje cell mean complex spike firing rates in response to whole-field flashes (left) and three directions of moving gratings (right) show different responses to global versus local luminance changes. (g) Additional recordings from a luminance-responsive Purkinje cell (see Figure 3cii) during the presentation of whole-field black (here shown as dark grey) and white flashes of various durations (50–5000 ms) from a baseline intermediate luminance (light grey). Upper panel, raster plot of complex spikes across trials (N = 11). Lower panel, complex spike count histogram. This cell produces has a clear sustained increase in complex spike activity during darkness whereas complex spike activity is nearly absent during bright flashes. (h) Quantification of the baseline complex spike firing rate of the cell in g) in the absence of changing visual stimuli for periods of tens of minutes for three different whole-field luminance levels. Three asterisks indicate p<0.001 and two indicate p<0.01 as calculated by one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc correction.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Purkinje cell dendrites show a mostly planar morphology.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(a) Four example Purkinje cell morphologies obtained by single-cell labeling (see Materials and methods) are shown with their soma and axon in black and dendrites in orange. Asterisks indicate a truncated axon. (b) Quantification of dendritic morphology as measured by determining the principal axes (see Materials and methods) shows that dendrites are significantly more planar than chance (p<0.01, Wilcoxon signed rank test).
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Motor-related complex spikes are rare.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

(a) Upper plot, the mean bout-triggered complex spike rate with shaded SEM for this cell for all swim bouts during the blank recordings (no stimuli presented, orange trace) and during trials with visual stimuli (red trace). N = 16 bouts (blanks), 76 bouts (stimuli). Lower traces, example excerpt from a blank recording from this Purkinje cell (PC, black trace) with simultaneous ventral root recording (VR, gray trace, shown as a moving standard deviation). Complex spikes are indicated by orange dots above the trace. (b) Upper traces, a subset of bouts are plotted aligned to bout onset for swim episodes during which a complex spike (orange dot) occurred. Below, a normalized histogram for all CS-positive bouts in this recording show that the majority of the complex spikes are triggered in the period 100–150 ms following bout onset (N = 34/76 CS-positive bouts). (c) Upper plot, the mean bout off-triggered complex spike rate with shaded SEM for this cell for blank and visual stimuli trials. N = 12 bouts (blanks), 468 bouts (stimuli). Lower traces, example excerpt from a blank electrophysiological recording from this cell. (d) Heatmap of bout on- and off-triggered mean complex spike rates for all cells with significant motor coefficients arranged by peak CS firing rate from bout onset. The lower three rows correspond to cells that have a decrease in CS activity during bouts which increases following bout offset. The example cells from a) and c) are indicated. (e) The locations of these Purkinje cells with CS activity correlated with bout onset (green) or bout offset (black) are plotted on the right lobe of a reference cerebellum (some coordinates were flipped from left to right). The example cells from a) and c) are indicated. Scale bar = 50 microns. (f) The 12 eye motor regressors used for multilinear least squares regression (MLR) of electrophysiological data with eye movements in the semi-paralyzed zebrafish (see Materials and methods for details; see Figure 1—figure supplement 2 for the description of sensory regressors). All eye motor regressors are calculated for each cell based on the motor activity of each eye (tracked independently) in that trial. A representative set of regressors computed from eye movement in one trial in the dataset is shown here. (g) Heatmap of all 30 regressor coefficient weights (18 sensory and 12 eye motor) for the complex spike (left) and simple spike (right) firing rates of 13 cells (N = 11 fish). The sensory regressors with the largest coefficient weights for complex spike rates are indicated. For complex spike phenotypes, 11/13 Purkinje cells have a stronger ‘sensory’ phenotype, whereas 13/13 Purkinje cell have a simple spike ‘motor’ phenotype. The two remaining Purkinje cells with a motor complex spike phenotype are indicated as ci and cii (arrowheads). (h) Location of all cells, color-coded for complex spike phenotype as determined by MLR and additional analyses (see subsequent panels). Scale bar = 50 microns. (i) Left, mean activity and SEM for the complex spike rate and best eye movement regressor excerpted from the rotational stimulus portion of the experiment for Purkinje cell two as indicated in g) and classified as having a motor complex spike phenotype. The single correlation coefficient between the best motor and sensory regressors across trials are very high (r = 0.60 across the full trial). Right, mean activity and SEM for the complex spike rate and best eye movement regressor excerpted from the rotational stimulus and flash portion of the experiment for Purkinje cell three as indicated in b) and the only other cell classified as having a ‘motor’ complex spike phenotype. The single correlation coefficient values for the complex spike rate with the indicated regressors across trials for just the rotational stimulus period or just the luminance period are shown. (j) Heatmap of eye movement (left eye, nasal) and complex spike rates across all trials of an experiment for a representative Purkinje cell in the left caudolateral cerebellum (cell seven as indicated in g,h). Note the variability of the eye movement across trials (left) compared to the complex spike rate (right). Clockwise velocity is indicated for reference. (k) The best motor regressors for each eye and the best sensory regressor are plotted against the complex spike rate of the cell in j) for the first (left) and last (right) trial of the experiment. Single correlation coefficient values are shown between each regressor and the complex spike rate for this trial. Time scale is same as for j).

The direction selectivity index (see Materials and methods) of these cells ranged from 0.2 to 0.9 (Figure 4—figure supplement 1a), and cells typically responded to two of the four cardinal directions tested (Figure 4a). No cells were found that responded significantly to motion onset in opposing directions. Although the Purkinje cell somata displaying this complex spike phenotype were closely clustered in the most rostromedial part of the cerebellum (Figure 3f), the lateralization of Purkinje cells was biased such that cells in the left cerebellar hemisphere preferred either forward motion to the right (0 to 90°, N = 7) or reverse motion to the left (−90 to −180°, N = 5; Figure 4—figure supplement 1a). Conversely, Purkinje cells in the right cerebellar hemisphere preferred either forward motion to the left (0 to −90°, N = 5) or reverse motion to the right (90 to 180°, N = 5; Figure 4—figure supplement 1a). The reliable, phasic nature of these complex spike responses suggests that these Purkinje cells encode acute, directional changes in the visual field.

The second group of Purkinje cells, located in the caudolateral cerebellum, showed a phenotype for large, directionally-selective increase in complex spikes during either clockwise or counter-clockwise rotational motion that unlike the previous group persisted throughout the duration of movement (Figure 4b; N = 12/61 cells). During rotational motion in the preferred direction, complex spike firing rates in these cells were two to five times higher than baseline (mean rate increase = 340 ± 40%, N = 12). In contrast, complex spike rates during motion in the non-preferred direction fell to nearly zero, well below the baseline rate (mean rate non-preferred direction = 0.32 ± 0.1 Hz versus 0.87 ± 0.1 Hz at baseline; Figure 4—figure supplement 1b). Consistent with our functional imaging data (Figure 2), these complex spike responses to rotational motion were highly lateralized such that all Purkinje cells (10/10) that preferred clockwise rotational motion were located in the caudolateral region of the left cerebellar hemisphere while the only two Purkinje cells that preferred counter-clockwise motion were located in the mirror symmetric region of the right cerebellar hemisphere (Figure 4—figure supplement 1b). Additional experiments in the semi-paralyzed animal (see ‘Motor-related complex spikes are rare for tail and eye movements’, below) confirmed this laterality (N = 11; Figure 4—figure supplement 3h).

These Purkinje cells also showed an increase in complex spiking for the duration of translational motion in a preferred lateral direction, determined to be rightwards motion for clockwise motion-preferring cells and vice versa (mean rate increase above baseline = 280 ± 20%; Figure 4—figure supplement 1b), suggesting that these cells respond to motion over a large area situated in the front half of the visual field. Finally, we observed an apparent homeostatic regulation firing in these cells where spontaneous complex spike rates were strongly depressed for several seconds following the robust complex spike responses elicited by rotational motion (normalized mean rate for one second following rotational stimuli = 37 ± 16% of spontaneous firing rates). Thus a high complex spike rate for the preferred direction of rotational motion may come at the expense of stochastic complex spikes. A homeostatic regulation of complex spike rates has also recently been observed in the mammalian cerebellum (Ju et al., 2018), though the underlying mechanism is not known.

The third prominent group of Purkinje cells had complex spike responses correlated with changes in whole-field luminance that were surprisingly heterogeneous in feature encoding compared to the notably stereotyped responses seen for the previous two groups (N = 25/61 cells; Figure 4c and Figure 4—figure supplement 1c–e; see Materials and methods). Purkinje cells with this luminance phenotype had complex spike responses that encoded either luminance increases (9/25) or decreases (11/25) or both (5/25; Figure 4c and Figure 4—figure supplement 1d) and the location of cells with different luminance response types was mixed across the central region of the cerebellum (Figure 4—figure supplement 1e). The latency from the onset of the preferred luminance transition to the first complex spike occurred for each cell with very little jitter, but the latency itself varied across cells (Figure 4—figure supplement 1d). Most whole-field luminance responses were transient such that cells fired just one complex spike for the preferred luminance change (mean = 0.80 ± 0.02 spikes). We did however observe, in two cells, different complex firing rates as a function of the ambient luminance presented that did not adapt over tens of minutes and therefore appear to encode ambient luminance through their complex spike rate (Figure 4cii, one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc correction, p<0.01; Figure 4—figure supplement 1g,h). Luminance-responsive Purkinje cells furthermore showed differing patterns of complex spike responses to local luminance changes during the translational motion of gratings (Figure 4—figure supplement 1f), suggesting that these cells have a wide range of receptive field sizes over which they integrate luminance.

In additional to having qualitative and quantitative differences in visual feature encoding, the three different types of Purkinje cell visual phenotypes described thus far also had notable differences in spontaneous complex spike rates (Figure 4d). Purkinje cells responding to rotational motion velocity had a significantly higher baseline firing rate than those with directionally selective motion onset responses (0.77 ± 0.1 Hz and 0.20 ± 0.02 Hz, respectively, p<0.001, Bonferroni post hoc correction). Purkinje cells responding most strongly to luminance or motor activity had intermediate baseline complex spike rates (0.28 ± 0.1 Hz and 0.34 ± 0.1 Hz, respectively).

Mapping the coordinates of Purkinje cell somata belonging to these three visual complex spike phenotypes supports a regional division of the cerebellum along the rostrocaudal axis where each of the three regions within the cerebellar hemisphere receives inputs from the same or similar inferior olive neurons carrying visual information (Figure 4ei). To examine the corresponding outputs of Purkinje cells from these groups, we performed cell-attached electrophysiological recordings in combination with morphological reconstructions via stochastic single-cell labelling of Purkinje cells to visualize the axonal projections (N = 17 cells from 17 fish; Figure 4eii). Unlike the mammalian cerebellum, where all Purkinje cell axons project outside of the cerebellar cortex, zebrafish Purkinje cells can be divided into two anatomical populations - one with internally-projecting axons that contact eurydendroid cells (the equivalent of mammalian cerebellar nuclei neurons) and the other whose somata are more lateral and who have externally-projecting caudal axons that contact neurons in the vestibular nuclei (Bae et al., 2009; Matsui et al., 2014).

Strikingly, 6/7 Purkinje cells with caudally projecting axons exhibited a clear complex spike phenotype for directional rotational motion, whereas only 1/10 cells with an internal axon had this same phenotype (Figure 4e). We further reconstructed and aligned 50 singly-labelled Purkinje cell morphologies across fish to a reference brain. Although the somata of Purkinje cells with caudal (N = 23/50) and internal (N = 27/50) axons partially overlap (Figure 4f), the segregation of rotational motion responses with caudal axon anatomies in this dataset further support our definition of this functional region of Purkinje cells. We also found that Purkinje cell dendrites generally had a classic albeit simplified morphology with mostly planar dendrites (Figure 4—figure supplement 2) oriented orthogonally to the axis of parallel fiber extension across the cerebellum (Knogler et al., 2017), as seen in mammalian cerebellum (Eccles et al., 1967). Together, these results define three functional groups of Purkinje cells residing in different regions across the cerebellum. These groups operate with different complex spike frequencies and encode distinct visual features related to visuomotor behaviors, and one group also sends the majority of its projections to a different downstream area than the others.

Motor-related complex spikes are rare for tail and eye movements

As discussed above, motor regressors did not significantly contribute to complex spike activity in the majority of Purkinje cells (N = 49/61) despite an abundance of visually-evoked fictive behavior and the use of multiple motor regressors to capture different motor features. We nonetheless used this small group of Purkinje cells with motor-related complex spike responses to examine motor feature encoding (Figure 4—figure supplement 3).

We analyzed complex spike responses from Purkinje cells during spontaneously-evoked swimming in blank trials as well as in trials where visual stimuli elicited swimming and confirmed that some complex spikes were indeed correlated with swimming activity even in the absence of visual stimuli (Figure 4—figure supplement 3a). Swim-related responses were however unreliable such that a complex spike occurred on fewer than half of swim bouts on average for these cells (mean probability = 0.38 ± 0.07 for stimuli trials, N = 9 cells; mean probability for blank trials = 0.42 ± 0.07, N = 5 cells; p<0.05, Wilcoxon signed rank test). Aligning the subset of swim bouts that were positive for motor-related complex spikes showed that the latency from bout onset to the occurrence of a complex spike in blank trials varied considerably for an individual cell, in contrast to the fixed latencies for most visual-driven complex spikes (Figure 4—figure supplement 3b, compare with Figure 4 and Figure 3—figure supplement 2). This is consistent with observations that complex spikes do not show phase-locking with stereotyped locomotor movements (Apps, 1999). Some Purkinje cells showed a decrease in complex spikes during swim bouts with a subsequent increase following bout offset (mean probability of a complex spike during bout <0.02, N = 3 cells; Figure 4—figure supplement 3c) however this was rarer than those with motor-related increases (Figure 4—figure supplement 3d). Unlike the spatial mapping seen for Purkinje cells with visual complex spike responses, Purkinje cells with motor-related complex signals were distributed across the cerebellum with no apparent clustering (Figure 4—figure supplement 3e).

We observed that both translational and rotational visual motion induced frequent bouts of fictive swimming in fish (Figure 3b); however the complex spike responses during these visual stimuli in most Purkinje cells did not correlate well on a trial-by-trial basis with swim bouts (Figure 3b, Figure 3—figure supplement 2) and were thus classified from multilinear regression analysis as sensory (visual), as described above. Rotational windmill stimuli are however known to evoke stereotyped eye movements known as the optokinetic reflex (Easter and Nicola, 1996), therefore complex spike responses to rotational visual motion could relate to the activation of eye rather than tail muscles. Studies of the cerebellar control of eye movements have shown evidence that climbing fibers provide eye motor error signals, which could account for the prominent complex spike signals observed in Purkinje cells in the caudolateral cerebellum during rotational windmill motion. In order to examine the potential contribution of eye movements to complex spikes in this group of Purkinje cells, we performed cell-attached recordings from Purkinje cells in the caudolateral cerebellum in the semi-paralyzed zebrafish, where the eyes were free to move and were tracked with a high-speed camera (see Materials and methods). The independent movement of each eye was then used to build a set of twelve regressors corresponding to eye position and velocity in different directions (Figure 4—figure supplement 3f).

Least-squares multilinear regression was used to analyze the complex spike activity of all cells with the existing set of sensory regressors for visual features and the twelve new eye motor regressors. We once again observed a clear bias for a visual complex spike phenotypes across Purkinje cells (N = 11/13), with only two cells whose best regressor related to eye movement (Figure 4—figure supplement 3g,h). A further analysis of the complex spike phenotypes of these latter two cells showed that in one case, the eye movement was exceptionally well-correlated with one visual feature (directional rotational velocity) that it was hard to disambiguate the true sensory vs motor nature of the complex spike response (Figure 4—figure supplement 3i, cell 2). For the other cell (Figure 4—figure supplement 3i, cell 3), a strong luminance (sensory) complex spike phenotype was identified through additional autocorrelation analyses (p<0.001 from Ljung-Box Q-test; r = 0.67 for correlation with the luminance regressor) in addition to the moderate correlation with eye movement (r = 0.32 for correlation with eye motor regressor). Nonetheless, the majority of Purkinje cells in this region could be clearly assigned to a visual complex spike phenotype since these cells showed very stereotyped complex spike responses to directional rotational stimuli that did not correlate well with the variable eye movements observed across trials (Figure 4—figure supplement 3j,k). We conclude from these data that the complex spike responses during rotational visual motion are predominantly sensory rather than motor. It is furthermore important to note that these responses are equally prominent in electrophysiological recordings in the paralyzed fish, where the eyes cannot move, as in the electrophysiological and functional imaging experiments where the eyes are free and track the stimulus (compare Figure 2, Figure 3f–g, and Figure 4—figure supplement 3).

Simple spike responses across the Purkinje cell population are highly modulated by motor efference copies during fictive swimming

Having observed that Purkinje cells can be clustered into functional regions defined by their visual complex spike responses and anatomical features, we next wanted to understand how simple spike responses were organized across the cerebellum. Multilinear regression showed that many visual and motor features contributed to simple spike responses in individual Purkinje cells, such that response phenotypes were broader than those observed for complex spike activity (Figure 5a and Figure 3e), as expected in a circuit where many parallel fibers converge on a single Purkinje cell (De Zeeuw et al., 2011). Although simple spike rates were modulated to some extent by many of the visual stimuli presented, motor activity significantly modulated simple spike activity in nearly all Purkinje cells across the cerebellum (N = 60/61) and was in fact the main contributor to modulating simple spike activity in the majority of cells (N = 44/61; Figure 5a).

Figure 5. Simple spike rates in most Purkinje cells are increased during fictive swimming.

Figure 5.

(a) Above, heatmap of coefficient weights for the simple spike firing rates of 61 cells from least-squares regression with a full set of 24 stimulus- and motor-related variables (see Materials and methods for more details). Below, histogram showing the distribution of cells’ highest regressor weight and the associated sensory/motor categories. (b) Upper panel, heatmap of z-scored simple spike rates for all 61 cells sorted by decreasing motor coefficient weight. Lower panel, the mean simple spike rate for the ten cells with the highest (upper trace) and lowest (lower trace) motor coefficient weights. (c) Left panel, example cell-attached Purkinje cell recording (PC, upper trace, black) from a blank trial (no stimuli) with simultaneous ventral root recording (VR, lower trace, gray, shown as a moving standard deviation). The simple spike rate is also shown (SSrate, middle trace, purple). Right, the bouts highlighted in green on an expanded timescale show the close timing of fictive bout onset and simple spike activity. (d) The bout on- and off-triggered mean simple spike firing rates for the cell in c) during blank recordings (purple) and stimulus trials (pink). (e) Z-scored heatmap of bout on- and off-triggered mean simple spike firing rates across all Purkinje cells sorted by mean firing rate in the 300 ms following bout onset. (f) Mean autocorrelation heatmap for simple spikes (SS, upper panel) and for ventral root recordings (VR, lower panel) for all Purkinje cells that showed spontaneous swimming bouts during blank trials (N = 30 cells from 30 fish), sorted by time to first peak in the VR autocorrelation. Right, the first significant peak in the VR autocorrelation for each recording is plotted to give the mean fictive swim frequency for each fish.

Different motor regressors accounted for various motor features including swim onset, offset, duration, and the continuous quantitative readout of swim strength, termed vigor (calculated from the standard deviation of the ventral root signal). Simple spike firing rates for these cells had consistently larger contributions to their activity from swim vigor than from bout duration or any other motor regressor, suggesting that fictive swimming activity is encoded in a graded manner by simple spike output. Mean simple spike firing rates across the population were on average twice as high during a bout as during the rest of the trial (mean rate during a bout = 14.5 ± 1.5 Hz vs 7.6 ± 0.8 Hz at rest; p<0.001, Wilcoxon signed rank test). Trial-averaged simple spike responses across the population appeared as a continuum rather than as clusters (Figure 5b), suggesting that the organization of parallel fiber inputs does not follow the same regional specificity as climbing fiber inputs across the cerebellum. Our analyses furthermore revealed that translational and rotational motion of visual stimuli, regardless of direction, was the most prominent sensory feature encoded by simple spike activity (Figure 5a). These findings suggest that Purkinje cells are integrating inputs from motion responsive granule cells with different directional tuning (Knogler et al., 2017).

In order to rule out potential sensory contributions to simple spike rates during visually-evoked behaviors, we analyzed simple spike activity during additional blank trials where no visual stimuli was presented (Figure 5c). Purkinje cells exhibit considerable spontaneous simple spike firing in the absence of any sensory stimuli or motor activity (Hsieh et al., 2014; Sengupta and Thirumalai, 2015); however fictive swim bouts consistently increased simple spike firing well above baseline levels (Figure 5c–e) and to an ever greater extent for spontaneous bouts in the absence of visual stimuli (p<0.01, Wilcoxon signed rank test). Aligning the mean bout-triggered simple spike rates for all Purkinje cells at bout onset and offset confirmed that the majority of cells have consistent motor-related increases in simple spike activity that begin at bout onset and return to baseline following bout offset (Figure 5e) although a small number of Purkinje cells instead show bout-triggered decreases in simple spike firing rates (Figure 5e), as observed elsewhere (Scalise et al., 2016). As expected for rhythmic locomotor output, the ventral root signal was highly autocorrelated for all fish with a mean autocorrelation frequency across fish of 26.7 ± 0.7 Hz (N = 30 fish; Figure 5f), consistent with the slow swim bout frequency reported for restrained zebrafish larvae (Severi et al., 2014). The autocorrelation analyses of simple spike firing for each Purkinje cell during a spontaneous fictive bout revealed however no significant autocorrelations for simple spikes at any frequency. Unlike the modulation of simple spike firing rate seen during step phase in locomoting rats (Sauerbrei et al., 2015), simple spikes in zebrafish Purkinje cells do not appear to be modulated in phase with rhythmic swimming activity but are nonetheless graded by swim strength. This suggests that individual Purkinje cell firing does not encode the activation of individual muscles involved in rhythmic swimming.

Motor activity is broadly represented in granule cell signals

The timing and reliability of swim-related simple spike activity is consistent with motor efference signals from spinal locomotor circuits during fictive swimming that arrive along mossy fibers to the granule cell layer and subsequently to Purkinje cells. A disynaptic pathway from spinal premotor interneurons to the granule cells via the lateral reticular nucleus was recently found that would convey information about ongoing network activity in the spinal cord (Pivetta et al., 2014). There is growing evidence across species that granule cells are strongly driven by ongoing locomotor activity (Ozden et al., 2012; Powell et al., 2015; Jelitai et al., 2016; Giovannucci et al., 2017; Knogler et al., 2017). Furthermore, extensive electrophysiological recordings from the granule cells of the cerebellum-like circuit of the electric organ in the electric fish revealed that an overwhelming majority (>90%) of granule cells receive depolarizing motor efference signals during electric organ discharge, although this translated into spiking in only ~20% of granule cells (Kennedy et al., 2014).

In order to characterize motor-related granule cell activity and its potential contribution to motor-related excitation in Purkinje cells across the cerebellum, we imaged responses in the granule cell population to the same set of visual stimuli while tracking tail and eye movement (Figure 6a). Multilinear regression was once again used to disambiguate responses to sensory stimuli and motor activity. Across fish (N = 7), we observed that granule cell activity was strong and widespread during swimming activity, both in the somatic layer and across the parallel fiber layer (Figure 6a). Granule cell activity relating to eye movements was weaker but also widespread (Figure 6a). These findings suggest that a large number of granule cells receive mossy fiber inputs relaying motor efference copies that drive them to fire, and they in turn drive broad motor-related activation of simple spikes in Purkinje cells (Figure 2a,b). These findings show more widespread motor-related representations in comparison to previous population-wide analyses of granule cell activity (Knogler et al., 2017) due to the abundance of behavior elicited by the current set of visual stimuli.

Figure 6. Granule cells across the cerebellum code for motor activity with high fidelity.

(a) Heatmaps of the z-projected mean voxelwise correlation coefficients of two-photon granule cell GCaMP6s signals from multilinear regression with example sensory and motor regressors averaged across seven fish (see Materials and methods). Scale bar = 50 microns. Upper right, cartoon of experimental set-up. (b) Left, cartoon of experimental set-up. Right, upper panel, example cell-attached recording from a granule cell (gc, upper trace, black) from a blank trial with simultaneous ventral root recording (VR, lower trace, gray). The granule cell firing rate is also shown (spike rate, middle trace, orange). The bout highlighted in green (i) is shown below on an expanded timescale. (c) The bout on- (left) and off- (right) triggered mean firing rates for this granule cell during blank recordings (orange) and stimulus trials (red). (d) Z-scored heatmaps of bout on- (left) and off- (right) triggered mean firing rates in all granule cells sorted by mean firing rate in the 300 ms following bout onset (N = 8 cells from eight fish). (e) Mean autocorrelation heatmap for spikes (upper panel) and ventral root recordings (VR, lower panel) for all granule cells from d), sorted by time to first peak in the VR autocorrelation. The red arrowheads signify granule cells with significant spike autocorrelations during fictive swim bouts (N = 3; p<0.001, Ljung-Box Q-test; see Materials and methods). Right, the first significant peak in the VR autocorrelation for each recording is plotted to give the mean fictive swim frequency for each fish. The red circles are the mean spike autocorrelation frequency obtained from the three significantly autocorrelated granule cells. (f) An example bout from the cell indicated in e), which was located ipsilateral to the ventral root recording. The smoothed spike rate (red) is in antiphase with the ipsilateral fictive tail contractions (grey). See also Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Many granule cells show significant modulation of their firing rates during fictive swimming bouts.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(a) Left, matrix of multilinear regressor coefficients for granule cell firing rates from all 10 cell-attached electrophysiological recordings with simultaneous fictive behavior in response to the same sensory stimuli as shown in Figure 2a. Right, histogram of cell counts for each best regressor category (as color-coded at left). (b) Location of granule cells across all fish mapped onto a reference cerebellum (dorsal view) and colored according to their motor phenotype. All coordinates are flipped onto the right hemisphere. Scale bar = 50 microns. (c) Left, heatmap of z-scored mean firing rates for all granule cells sorted by decreasing motor regressor coefficient. Colored bars at right indicate cells whose firing rate is positively modulated by bout duration (green), by bout offset (black), or by neither (grey). Right, cluster mean granule cell firing rates (black) and mean fictive bout vigor (grey).

In order to understand the temporal patterning of swim-related motor signals in the granule cells layer, we performed additional electrophysiological recordings from randomly targeted granule cells across the cerebellum while simultaneously recording ventral root activity to identify fictive swim episodes. These recordings revealed several granule cells with negligible firing rates in the absence of motor activity but that showed large, significant increases in their spike rates during fictive bouts (N = 6/8 cells; mean firing rate at rest = 1.3 ± 0.3 Hz vs 25.7 ± 7.6 Hz during a bout; p<0.005, Wilcoxon signed rank test; Figure 6b–d; Figure 6—figure supplement 1). These granule cells had graded responses correlated with swim strength and could reach high instantaneous firing frequencies of up to 150 Hz during a fictive bout, similar to the burst firing seen in mammalian granule cells during locomotion (Powell et al., 2015) or whisker stimulation (van Beugen et al., 2013). Half of these motor-excited granule cells (N = 3/6) also showed significant autocorrelations in their spiking activity during fictive swimming (p<0.001, Ljung-Box Q-test; Figure 6e). The frequency of the spike autocorrelations for these cells was comparable to the fictive swim frequency obtained from the ventral root (mean difference in frequency = 1.3 ± 0.6 Hz, N = 3), suggesting that the periodicity of granule cell spiking is related to the swimming activity (Figure 6e). The phase of the granule cell spiking with respect to the ipsilateral ventral root activity varied however across cells, arriving either in phase, with a lag, or in antiphase (Figure 6f).

Together, these results suggest that motor efference copies are relayed along mossy fibers to many granule cells to drive burst firing during swimming bouts, whether fictive or real. In turn, parallel fibers deliver graded swim-related excitation to nearly all cerebellar Purkinje cells. We are confident that these are true efference signals and not motor-related sensory input from proprioception or the lateral line since the fish is paralyzed and the muscles are not moving during these electrophysiological experiments. The widespread increases in Purkinje cell calcium signals observed in the behaving animal during swimming (Figure 2a,b) therefore are likely to reflect simple spike bursts in Purkinje cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 2) driven by the high frequency firing of one or more presynaptic granule cells carrying motor efference information.

Purkinje cells combine sensory and motor information from distinct inputs

Our data suggests that as a population, Purkinje cells preferentially encode visual features in their complex spike activity whereas swimming activity, arriving in the form of motor efference copies, is predominantly encoded by simple spikes. Breaking this down by group, we find that Purkinje cells belonging to the three different visual complex spike phenotypes described above have simple spike activity that is correlated most strongly with motor activity (fraction of total signal from motor regressors = 0.65 ± 0.05, 0.54 ± 0.07, 0.76 ± 0.02 for the three visual complex spike phenotypes; Figure 7—figure supplement 1a). In contrast, for the small group of Purkinje cells with dominant motor-related complex spike phenotypes, the contribution of motor activity to simple spike activity is relatively low (0.35 ± 0.12) and simple spikes are instead broadly influenced by a combination of sensory and motor features (Figure 7—figure supplement 1a). This relationship holds true for individual Purkinje cells as well (Figure 7—figure supplement 1b). Together, these data suggest that sensory and motor information is preferentially combined in Purkinje cells from distinct sources.

Motor context alters the relationship between complex spike and simple spike activity

It is well-established that the occurrence of a complex spike can alter simple spike activity in a Purkinje cell both acutely and across longer timescales (De Zeeuw et al., 2011). On a short timescale, complex spikes typically cause a brief pause of tens of milliseconds in simple spike firing that can be followed by an increase or decrease in simple spikes lasting hundreds of milliseconds. The particular complex spike-triggered change in simple spiking is robust for a given Purkinje cell but varies considerably across cells (Zhou et al., 2014; Zhou et al., 2015; Xiao et al., 2014). Similar to previous findings, we observed heterogeneity in the relationship between complex and simple spikes across Purkinje cell recordings (Figure 7a). At the most extreme end, we observed complex spike-induced pauses or increases in simple spike rates in different cells that took several hundred milliseconds to return to baseline. These pauses or increases in simple spiking may be attributable to a toggling action of the complex spike to shift the Purkinje cell between ‘up’ and ‘down’ states (Loewenstein et al., 2005; Sengupta and Thirumalai, 2015). Several cells had brief pauses (tens of milliseconds) following a complex spike which left simple spikes otherwise unchanged, whereas others showed a brief increase in simple spike firing. Previous studies have suggested that the modulation of simple spike firing by a complex spike is related to the cell’s location within the cerebellum (Zhou et al., 2014; Zhou et al., 2015). We did not, however, observe any clear spatial organization of the complex spike-simple spike relationship in this dataset (Figure 7a).

Figure 7. Purkinje cells modify their simple spike output in a complex spike- and motor context-dependent way.

(a) Heatmap of complex spike-driven simple spike (CS:SS) counts for each cell normalized to the mean over 100 ms preceding a complex spike. Cells are sorted by decreasing simple spike pause and increasing excitation. The inset shows the location of these cells colored by the normalized difference in simple spiking in the 50 ms following the CS. (b) The mean complex spike-triggered simple spike count (10 ms bins) is shown for five example cells (as indicated in a) for five different contexts. Left (green box), in the presence (‘motor’) versus absence (‘non-motor’) of fictive swimming episodes. Under non-motor conditions these different Purkinje cells show, respectively, a CS-induced i) long SS pause, ii) short SS pause with rebound increase, iii) no change in SS, iv) short SS increase, and finally a v) long SS increase. Green arrows highlight changed patterns during motor context. Middle (magenta box), CS:SS relationships across preferred versus all other sensory contexts (only non-motor periods included). Right (grey box), the CS:SS relationship during blank trials (no stimuli, only non-motor periods). Vertical scale bar indicates the rate conversion for 0.2 spikes/10 ms bin (20 Hz). (c) Green markers show the mean normalized simple spike rates (calculated from 10 ms bins) for all Purkinje cells centered on the occurrence of a complex spike during a fictive bout minus those occurring at any other point (N = 51 cells). Data are mean ± SEM. Grey markers, simple spike rates centered on the occurrence of a complex spike during all sensory stimuli minus those occurring during blank trials (N = 53 cells). The dashed black line indicates zero difference between conditions. Inset, the window around complex spike onset shown on an expanded timescale. Asterisks indicate p<0.05 for motor minus nonmotor conditions (green markers) as computed by the Wilcoxon signed rank test. Grey markers, no significant differences. (d) Heat maps are shown for individual Purkinje cell binned simple spike counts over the three different 50 ms periods as indicated in e). Complex-spike triggered simple spike counts are separated for each cell for those complex spikes occurring during a fictive bout (left column of heatmaps, outlined in green) or at any other time (right column of heatmaps, outlined in black).

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Individual Purkinje cells preferentially combine sensory and motor information.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(a) The mean fraction of the simple spike (SS) response contributed by each regressor computed for each of the four Purkinje cell complex spike (CS) groups. (b) Left, scatterplot of the fraction of complex spike versus simple spike activity accounted for by motor regressors. Right, the fraction of simple spike activity accounted for by motor regressors versus the fraction of complex spike activity accounted for by all sensory regressors.

The current behavioral state of the animal should provide important contextual information for cerebellar circuits, therefore we hypothesized that the modulation of simple spikes by a complex spike might be altered in different sensory and motor contexts. In periods during which the fish was at rest (no fictive swimming), the relationship between a complex spike and the simple spike firing rate was similar whether or not visual stimuli were being presented (Figure 7b, ‘non-motor’ versus ‘blank trials’). When the fish was performing a fictive swim bout however, the effect of a complex spike on simple spike output appeared diminished (Figure 7b, ‘non-motor’ versus ‘motor’), which was not the case for complex spikes occurring during a cell’s preferred complex spike sensory stimulus versus those occurring during all other periods (Figure 7b, ‘pref. sensory’ versus ‘all other periods’).

The unique effect of motor context on this relationship is likely related to the finding that many Purkinje cells have simple spike rates that are strongly excited by motor activity (Figure 5e), therefore a complex spike stochastically occurring during a bout would be faced with simple spikes rates that are significantly higher than baseline. Upon closer examination of the temporal window around the occurrence of a complex spike, we observed that the acute effect of a complex spike to modulate simple spike rates was identical between motor and non-motor periods for only a 50 millisecond period following the complex spike, after which time simple spiking returned to high levels correlated with ongoing behavior (Figure 7c,d). This temporal window was the same across cells regardless of whether the baseline modulation by a complex spike was to pause or facilitate simple spike firing. These findings suggest that the acute effect of a complex spike to change simple spike output in a Purkinje cell is temporally restricted by the behavioral state of the animal and that plasticity mechanisms relying on coincident complex spike and simple spike activity will have a unique dependence on motor context (see discussion).

Discussion

In this study, we have taken advantage of an innate set of visually-driven motor behaviors in the larval zebrafish to comprehensively interrogate how Purkinje cells encode sensory and motor features relating to these behaviors at high spatial and temporal resolution across the cerebellum. Our population imaging data across both the Purkinje cell and granule cell populations are supported by single cell electrophysiological recordings that elucidate complex spike and simple spikes. We furthermore show the robustness and specificity of these patterns across behavioral conditions regardless of whether the tail and/or eyes are freely moving or paralyzed. We show that Purkinje cells fall into anatomically clustered regions that are functionally defined by complex spike responses that convey mostly sensory information. On the other hand, simple spikes convey mostly motor-related information about tail and eye movement. During visuomotor behaviors, these two input streams converge on Purkinje cells in specific regions of the cerebellum and communicate the presence of distinct visual features together with motor context. Each of these regions therefore likely represents a behavioral module whose neural computations are used to guide sensorimotor integration and motor learning in the cerebellum.

Anatomical and functional organization of cerebellar regions

The three distinct regions formed along the rostrocaudal axis of the larval zebrafish cerebellum we define here based on distinct Purkinje cells sensory complex spike phenotypes to visual stimuli should receive topographically-specific climbing fiber inputs from the inferior olive (Figure 8; Ozol et al., 1999). The presence of these discrete complex spike response phenotypes across Purkinje cells suggests that zebrafish climbing fiber inputs from the inferior olive have undergone refinement by seven dpf to innervate only one Purkinje cell, in support of other findings (Hsieh et al., 2014; Hsieh and Papazian, 2018). Ongoing work characterizing the physiology and anatomy of inferior olive neurons and their climbing fibers (unpublished observations) supports this regional characterization and, together with studies of Purkinje cell output to eurydendroid cells, will further our understanding of these anatomical regions.

Figure 8. Organization of the larval zebrafish cerebellum Granule cells (GCs) send long parallel fibers (grey lines) that contact Purkinje cells (PCs) across the cerebellum and broadly relay motor efference copies of locomotor activity (swimming).

Figure 8.

Sensory information relating to different visual features are sent by climbing fibers of inferior olive neurons (IO) to stereotyped regions of the contralateral Purkinje cell layer. These visual stimuli contribute to several reflexive behaviors; rotational motion drives the optokinetic reflex of the eyes, translational forward motion drives the optomotor swimming reflex while others, such as luminance, may drive behavior over longer (e.g. circadian) timescales. The three distinct functional regions in the zebrafish cerebellum defined by Purkinje cell complex spike sensory responses that encode these different visual features represent putative behavioral modules. Information about the onset of directional translational motion is preferentially sent to PCs in the rostromedial region of the cerebellum (cyan) and would be important for coordinating turning and swimming, while information about the direction and velocity of rotational motion as would be needed for coordinating eye and body movements is sent to the caudolateral region (blue). The central region (red) receives information about luminance and may provide a substrate for learned sensorimotor associations. Axons from PCs (black dashed lines) of the rostromedial and central regions have mostly internal axons that contact eurydendroid cells (EC) within the cerebellar cortex. Axons from PCs in the caudolateral region have mostly external axons that exit the cerebellum and contact neurons in the caudally-located ipsilateral vestibular nucleus.

Differences in developmental timing (e.g. birthdate) are known to contribute to the formation of a topographic functional map in the cerebellum across species (Hashimoto and Hibi, 2012). In zebrafish, Purkinje cell development occurs in waves that map onto the same regions we describe here, beginning with a large rostromedial cluster and a smaller, caudolateral cluster and later filling in the central region to form a continuous layer (Hamling et al., 2015). Just like in mammals, all climbing fibers cross the midline after leaving the inferior olive and contact the somata or proximal dendrites Purkinje cells in the contralateral hemisphere of the zebrafish cerebellum (Takeuchi et al., 2015). The topography of early afferent climbing fiber connectivity onto Purkinje cells is likely hard-wired, as in mammals it is guided by chemical cues and does not depend on developmental activity (see Apps and Hawkes, 2009 for review). Although all ipsilateral climbing fibers enter the cerebellar cortex as one bundle at larval stages, in the adult, additional fiber bundles are visible (Takeuchi et al., 2015), suggesting that other routes or types of information are added for communication between the inferior olive and cerebellar cortex at later stages. Regional differences in cytoarchitecture and patterns of molecular markers such as zebrin have also been useful for identifying related Purkinje cells into groups in the mammalian cerebellum (see Cerminara et al., 2015 for review). Although in larval zebrafish all Purkinje cells are zebrin-positive (Bae et al., 2009), many other genes are expressed in restricted patterns in the zebrafish (Takeuchi et al., 2017) and mammalian cerebellum (Hawkes, 2014) that may help define the subdivision of Purkinje cells into clearly-defined subregions within the cerebellum.

The organization of the cerebellum is thought to impart distinct functional roles across regions, such that each group of Purkinje cells processes sensorimotor information relating to a different behavioral component (Witter and De Zeeuw, 2015). Although we only probed one sensory modality to drive behavior, zebrafish are highly visual animals that perform robust visuomotor behaviors at the larval stage, including prey capture, optokinetic and optomotor responses, and associative learning with a conditioned visual stimulus (Easter and Nicola, 1996; Budick and O'Malley, 2000; Aizenberg and Schuman, 2011; Harmon et al., 2017). Visual information is therefore a highly salient sensory modality at this age and in accordance with this strong ethological relevance we find that the complex spike sensory responses to visual stimuli provide an overarching organization of the Purkinje cell layer into putative behavioral modules (Figure 8). Previous studies have used confocal imaging and optogenetics to identify general regions of the cerebellum that are important for optomotor and optokinetic responses (Matsui et al., 2014). Our current results build on these maps with an expanded set of visual stimuli, high-resolution two-photon population imaging, and single-cell electrophysiology, to comprehensively describe visual and motor feature coding from the level of single spikes to population activity.

We see little evidence for the encoding of multiple visual features in these complex spike responses, however we expect to find representations of features from multiple sensory modalities in individual Purkinje cells arising from the multimodality of inferior olive neurons (Ohmae and Medina, 2015; Ju et al., 2018). It will be of great interest to see if the same spatial mapping by complex spike phenotype is conserved across other sensory modalities. Many other sensory systems are active at this age and provide salient stimuli for larval zebrafish as demonstrated in behavioral studies. Larval zebrafish show innate behavioral startle responses to loud auditory cues across a range of frequencies (Bhandiwad et al., 2013), however functional imaging across the brain suggests that the neural coding of auditory stimuli at this stage is generic and underdeveloped in zebrafish compared to the visual modality (Vanwalleghem et al., 2017). In contrast, the activity of many neurons across the brain including the cerebellum are differentially modulated by vestibular inputs at larval stages (Favre-Bulle et al., 2018; Migault et al., 2018). Given the pronounced complex spike responses we observe in response to rotational motion in the Purkinje cells with axonal output to the vestibular nucleus, it is likely that the coordination of vestibular and visual inputs during rolling movements (the vestibulo-ocular reflex), critically engages the cerebellum of the larval zebrafish. Although the larval zebrafish exhibits a broad repertoire of innate behavioral responses to many other stimulus modalities including touch, the lateral line, and olfaction (see Fero et al., 2011 for review), there is a lack of physiological data to understand how these signals are encoded in the central nervous system and the cerebellum in particular. It is furthermore conceivable that other sensory systems become more important at later developmental stages, for examples olfactory processing of cues for kin recognition and social behaviors in the juvenile fish (Dreosti et al., 2015) or lateral line-mediated schooling behaviors in the adult (Miller and Gerlai, 2012).

In support of the spatial division of the cerebellum by visual complex spike phenotypes that we show in the current study, previous findings from a completely different behavioral paradigm in larval zebrafish also found three complex spike regions with a similar organization. Harmon et al., 2017 developed an associative learning task to pair visual stimuli with an electric shock to elicit conditioned swimming responses. They found, using single-cell electrophysiological recordings, that Purkinje cell complex spike patterns in their conditioning paradigm were spatially and functionally clustered into three regions along the rostrocaudal axis that overlap well with the regions described here. These complementary findings suggest that this regionalization is of fundamental importance across modalities and behaviors. However, experimental attempts at associative learning using auditory stimuli have been unsuccessful at this stage and even in the 6 week-old larva (Thompson, 2016), suggesting a prioritization of visual information for the earliest motor learning in larval zebrafish. Future studies are needed to examine how the function and organization of the zebrafish cerebellum across regions may change to reflect an increasing complexity and repertoire of sensorimotor behaviors at later developmental stages.

Complex spikes use different temporal bases to encode specific visual features important for the animal’s behavioral repertoire

Our results show that that majority of Purkinje cells across the cerebellum encode visual and not motor information in their complex spike activity. We observed a remarkably discrete and complete classification of nearly all Purkinje cells (>90%) for a specific visual complex spike phenotype whose sensory nature was clearly distinguishable from motor-related signals of eye and tail behavior. These visual complex spike phenotypes were distinct between the three groups of Purkinje cells in different rostrocaudal regions. Below, we discuss how the representation of these different visual features may serve as behavioral modules that relate to the particular behavioral repertoire of the larval zebrafish and to findings from the mammalian literature.

Transient changes in the direction of translational visual motion convey information critical for driving locomotion and turning behaviors, or in the case of visual reafference, for evaluating the success of a directed behavior. In the larval zebrafish, Purkinje cells of the rostromedial cerebellum reliably encode acute, directional changes of motion in the visual field with a preferred directional tuning. During the optomotor response, fish swim to stabilize their position with respect to the visual field. Larval zebrafish also perform a variety of low and high-angle turns at this stage while exploring, performing escape maneuvers, and hunting prey, therefore complex spike signals updating the brain about a transient change in motion in the visual field have strong ethological relevance. This population of Purkinje cells whose complex spikes encode directionally-selective motion onset are reminiscent of the directionally-tuned Purkinje cells in the oculomotor vermis of posterior lobes VI and VII in primates, where complex spike tuning organizes the cells into functional groups whose simple spikes encode real-time eye motion (Soetedjo and Fuchs, 2006; Herzfeld et al., 2015).

In the caudolateral region of the cerebellum we find Purkinje cells with strong complex spike responses to unidirectional rotational motion and axons that project primarily to the octaval (vestibular) nuclei in zebrafish (Matsui et al., 2014). These Purkinje cells fire complex spikes during visual motion in a temporal to nasal direction presented to the ipsilateral eye (with respect to the anatomical location of the Purkinje cell), resulting in a tonically elevated complex spike rate during visual motion in the preferred direction. This caudal region is likely the vestibulocerebellum, homologous to the mammalian flocculonodular lobe where Purkinje cells receive climbing fiber input conveying information about ongoing, opposing directional visual and rotational head motion that is used for vestibulo-ocular coordination (Simpson and Alley, 1974; Ito, 1982). Complementary imaging studies in larval zebrafish show strong, directionally-tuned responses in the activity of undefined cerebellar neurons in this same region (Favre-Bulle et al., 2018; Migault et al., 2018). Larval zebrafish perform slow steering maneuvers of the tail while navigating and also produce smooth eye movements while engaging in activities such as prey tracking (McElligott and O'malley, 2005), both of which result in slow changes in the visual field. In addition, zebrafish can control their eyes independently from each other, so these signals are likely to be integrated in the brain together with vestibular and body axis information to achieve coordinated movements. Notably, these zebrafish vestibulocerebellar Purkinje cells show complex spike responses not only to rotational but also to translational moving fields, which is not seen in mammals but has been observed in pigeons (Wylie and Frost, 1991). This finding may relate to the additional complexity of optic flow that arises during navigation in a three-dimensional world for birds and fish.

We furthermore observed that Purkinje cells in the caudolateral region have spontaneous complex spike rates an order of magnitude higher than those in the rostromedial region described above and show sustained high complex spike firing in response to their preferred stimulus. This could allow for increased temporal precision in order to generate fast and precise firing patterns as would be required when generating sensorimotor associations or coordinating smooth movements (Porrill et al., 2013; Suvrathan et al., 2016). The computation itself may in fact be different in this region since complex spikes could use conventional rate coding to encode the speed and direction of ongoing, slow movements of the visual field during behavior, as proposed by Simpson et al., 1996 based on observations in the mammalian flocculonodular lobe across species. These findings challenge the assumption that the computations being performed across the cerebellum all follow the same rules and that the occurrence of a discrete event, rather than information about an ongoing event, is transmitted by complex spikes.

The heterogeneity of sensory complex spike coding of luminance in the intermediate region of the zebrafish cerebellum sets this group of Purkinje cells apart from the other two visual phenotypes We see both many differences in responses to luminance changes, including light/dark preference, tonic/phasic responses, latency from stimulus onset to complex spike, and receptive field size. We propose that these Purkinje cells are therefore well-suited to modulate a diversity of light-mediated behaviors in the larval zebrafish. Although the luminance stimuli in the current experiments were titrated to be moderate and thus not evoke acute behavioral responses, sudden strong decreases in luminance induce re-orienting navigational turns (Burgess and Granato, 2007) or escapes (Temizer et al., 2015) in zebrafish larvae and transient startle responses in the adult (Easter and Nicola, 1996), the latter two representing likely predator avoidance responses. With respect to these fast behaviors, a transient encoding of luminance change could serve to modulate these response circuits. Luminance increases spontaneous locomotor activity in larval zebrafish over longer timescales as well, which is used as a cue to regulate circadian rhythms and motivate feeding and exploratory behavior in the daytime (Burgess and Granato, 2007). There is also an innate preference for larval zebrafish to be in lighter areas of their environment, a behavior known as phototaxis (Brockerhoff et al., 1995). These latter behaviors would more likely make use of rate coding of ambient luminance, as observed in the complex spike output of some Purkinje cells, to provide sensory integration over long timescales (tens of minutes).

The differing luminance preferences and temporal dynamics across this group may furthermore be useful for learning novel associations. Indeed, a recent report by Harmon et al., 2017 found that Purkinje cells in this central area of the larval zebrafish cerebellum (termed ‘multiple complex spike cells’ in this study) preferentially acquired complex spike responses to a conditioned visual stimulus during associative learning. As mentioned above, Purkinje cells in this central region are also born slightly later in development compared to the groups described above (Hamling et al., 2015), findings that together suggest this region may preferentially contribute to flexible or learned sensorimotor behaviors. This region may be similar to areas in the central zone (posterior lobes VI and VII) of the cerebellum in mammals, which support a wide range of behavioral functions (Koziol et al., 2014; Stoodley et al., 2012).

What signals are complex spikes encoding?

There is great debate about whether climbing fiber signals convey error, predictive, or novelty signals (see Simpson et al., 1996 and Streng et al., 2018 for reviews). The error hypothesis would suggest that the visually-evoked responses we observe here signal unexpected events or ‘negative sensory events to be avoided’ such as retinal slip (Lang et al., 2017). However, these signals are not necessarily a classical error signal (Ito, 2013), because in the current study we find that stimulus-evoked complex spikes are equally prominent in paralyzed fish as in experiments where the eyes and tail are free and track the stimulus. Furthermore, many complex spikes are robustly elicited by visual stimuli that do not acutely drive behavior, such as reverse motion or luminance changes.

Other work suggests that climbing fibers carry instructional signals for upcoming motor actions in a learned behavior, in the context of a reinforcement learning signal (Ohmae and Medina, 2015; ten Brinke et al., 2015; Heffley et al., 2018), or could provide the corrective drive used to initiate locomotion (Ozden et al., 2012). While we are not testing predictive signals in this study, it is nonetheless clear that for the complex spikes elicited during visual stimuli in our experiments do not signal an upcoming motor event. In cases when complex spikes are driven by the onset of directional translational motion, they occur with a consistently short latency (approximately 200 ms) whereas the latency to swim bout onset is much longer and more variable, and the presence of absence of these visual complex spikes across trials do not predict the occurrence of a swim bout.

Other hypotheses suggest that climbing fibers may encode novelty or salience signals related to sensory stimuli, although in fact these hypotheses do not exclude the previous ones since climbing fibers may be able to carry different types of signals by multiplexing (Ohmae and Medina, 2015). The complex spike responses we observe in this study do not encode all novel or salient visual stimuli as we see that responses are selective for certain visual features. In our experiments, complex spikes do not habituate but are consistently elicited by visual stimuli, across many trials and many hours, in contrast to what might be expected if complex spikes encoded novelty. It remains however to be seen how robust these responses are over longer timescales, as previous work has suggested the complex spike response to a novel sensory stimulus is subject to habituation only with repeated exposure across many days (Ohmae and Medina, 2015).

Since the above hypotheses were mostly developed with respect to observations in the context of cerebellar learning, the role of complex spikes may be different for innate feature coding. Our results suggest an innate coding of sensory features in climbing fiber signals in the naïve animal, consistent with observations of visual and multimodal sensory responses carried by climbing fibers in other studies in zebrafish (Hsieh et al., 2014; Sengupta and Thirumalai, 2015; Scalise et al., 2016; Harmon et al., 2017) and mammals (Ohmae and Medina, 2015; Ju et al., 2018). The complex spikes resulting from climbing fibers tuned to specific sensory features could subsequently drive the learning that underlies novel associations, including predictions, as arises when an animal experiences the repeated pairing of a complex spike-evoking stimulus and a motor event (Ito, 2001; Harmon et al., 2017). In this context, the sensory complex spike signal could be interpreted as a sensory prediction error that drives associative learning. Additional work is needed to determine how the complex spike responses encode different sensory modalities both in the naïve animal and throughout the course of learning.

Motor efference copies in the cerebellum

Our population-wide imaging and extensive electrophysiological recordings show that most Purkinje cells across the cerebellum encode the current behavioral state (motor context) of the animal through a pronounced increase in simple spikes during locomotor behaviors. We observed strong swim-related signals in granule cells and Purkinje cells during both active and fictive swimming, where zebrafish were awake but paralyzed, therefore this activity is more consistent with motor efference copy signals than proprioceptive or lateral line activation. Moreover, we found that simple spike output correlated best with the strength of ongoing swimming rather than reporting a phasic or binary locomotor state, supporting previous findings that motor parameters are linearly coded in the cerebellum (Raymond and Medina, 2018). Only a small minority of Purkinje cells showed a motor-related decrease in simple spiking, which may reflect the relatively small contribution of feed-forward inhibition via molecular layer interneurons. The increases in simple spike output that we observe are far less heterogeneous than the effects of locomotion on mammalian Purkinje cell simple spike firing (Jelitai et al., 2016; Sauerbrei et al., 2015) and build on previous electrophysiological samplings of Purkinje cell activity that showed increases in membrane depolarization and simple spike output during fictive swimming in zebrafish (Sengupta and Thirumalai, 2015; Scalise et al., 2016).

These results suggest that motor efference signals during whole-body locomotion (swimming) drive simple spike output in nearly all cerebellar Purkinje cells in the larval zebrafish. Our current granule cell population imaging and electrophysiological recordings in zebrafish together with other recordings and optogenetic experiments in zebrafish and mice (Ozden et al., 2012; Powell et al., 2015; Jelitai et al., 2016; Giovannucci et al., 2017; Knogler et al., 2017; Albergaria et al., 2018) provide strong evidence that the cerebellum broadly encodes intended locomotor output or signals related to it in the input layer (Figure 6). These findings suggest an enrichment of motor signals across parallel fiber inputs, though some regional specialization of signals in limbed vertebrates may be needed to coordinate different limb networks. Future work is required to investigate the origin of mossy fibers carrying eye and tail motor efference copies to the zebrafish cerebellum and how these signals are transformed by subsequent processing stages in cerebellar circuits.

Complex spike - simple spike relationships

We observed that the dominant action of motor activity on simple spiking acutely changes how complex spikes and simple spikes interact in Purkinje cells. During non-locomotor periods, complex spikes have the ability to consistently increase or pause simple spiking for several hundreds of milliseconds in different Purkinje cells. Under motor-driven conditions of high simple spike rates, however, a complex spike resets simple spike activity for only a brief (<50 ms) window in all Purkinje cells before simple spikes return to their previous high rate. This is likely due to the overwhelming excitatory influence of locomotor activity carried by parallel fibers that drives simple spiking across the Purkinje cell population at high rates. When faced with these high simple spike rates, a complex spike arriving during motor activity therefore has a limited influence over simple spike output. The narrowing of this temporal window may serve to make finer adjustments of motor activity through very acute perturbations in network activity.

Across longer timescales, sensorimotor behaviors needs to be adjusted during development, experience, and learning, so that an animal can adapt to suit a changing environment or context. In a developmental context, the cerebellum may be actively engaged in refining and maintaining sensorimotor behaviors as the physiology of neural circuits, muscles, and sensory appendages matures. In the context of supervised cerebellar learning, classical theories predict that the coincident activation of a climbing fiber input and parallel fiber synapses drives long-term depression at the active parallel fiber to Purkinje cell synapses, leading to motor learning (Ito, 2001; but see Bouvier et al., 2018). Synaptic plasticity mechanisms both at other synapses and involving other cerebellar neurons (e.g. interneurons) are also likely to contribute (see Gao et al., 2012 for review). We propose that motor efference signals during swimming and eye movements are widely broadcasted across the cerebellum to Purkinje cells because these are the most relevant signals not only for coordinating ongoing behaviors but also for driving plasticity. The enrichment of motor-related activity across the granule cell layer and subsequent broad excitation of Purkinje cells would support learned associations between motor behaviors and any relevant sensory information carried by regionally-specially climbing fiber input. Indeed, recent work by Albergaria et al., 2018 supports this idea by showing that a generalized increase in granule cell excitation during either locomotion or optogenetic stimulation enhances cerebellar learning in a paradigm for eyeblink conditioning. The amenability of the zebrafish cerebellum to in vivo physiological and behavioral recordings together with the hypotheses raised by this study should make it an attractive system to study the rules of cerebellar plasticity and learning in the future.

Outlook

Our results reveal a strong spatial organization of visual feature encoding in the Purkinje cell population into three rostrocaudal functional regions receiving different climbing fiber inputs. These regions are each involved in processing visual information relating to distinct motor behaviors and as such exhibit unique temporal features in sensory coding. Broad excitation from granule cells is layered on these regions during locomotor activity as a contextual signal. We believe that the system of granule cells and Purkinje cells together thus forms the substrate for cerebellar modules modulating innate and learned motor behaviors. These and other recent findings (Matsui et al., 2014; Harmon et al., 2017) provide a promising outlook for using the zebrafish as a model organism for understanding motor control and learning in the cerebellum.

Materials and methods

Experimental model and subject details

Zebrafish (Danio rerio) were maintained at 28 ˚C on a 14 hr light/10 hr dark cycle using standard protocols. All animal procedures were performed in accordance with approved protocols set by the Max Planck Society and the Regierung von Oberbayern (TVA 55-2-1-54-2532-82-2016). All experiments were performed using larvae at 6–8 dpf of as yet undetermined sex.

To label Purkinje cells specifically, we made use of the aldoca promoter and the carbonic anhydrase 8 (ca8) enhancer element as published previously (Takeuchi et al., 2015; Matsui et al., 2014). For electrophysiological recordings in Purkinje cells, aldoca:GFF;mn7GFF;UAS:GFP fish were used (Takeuchi et al., 2015; Asakawa et al., 2008; Asakawa et al., 2013), with Tg(gSAIzGFFM765B); UAS:GFP and Tg(gSAG6A); UAS:GFP fish additionally used for granule cell recordings (Takeuchi et al., 2015). For calcium imaging experiments with granule cells, Tg(gSA2AzGFF152B); UAS:GCaMP6s fish were used (Takeuchi et al., 2015; Thiele et al., 2014). For calcium imaging experiments in Purkinje cells, we cloned GCaMP6s (Chen et al., 2013) downstream of the ca8 enhancer with an E1b minimal promoter referred hereafter as PC:GCaMP6s. We injected PC:GCaMP6s together with tol2 mRNA in one cell stage embryos (25 ng/µl each), screened at six dpf for expression in the cerebellum, and raised strong positive fish to adulthood. Positive F1 progeny were used for all imaging experiments. For simultaneous electrophysiological and imaging experiments, we injected PC:GCaMP6s without tol2 mRNA to achieve sparse, single-cell labelling. For anatomical experiments, we created a construct harboring a bright GFP variant mClover3 (Bajar et al., 2016) tagged with a membrane targeting signal (Fyn). This construct is termed PC:Fyn-mClover3. Injections were done as described for sparse GCaMP6s labelling in fish expressing aldoca:gap43-mCherry to allow registration across fish. For Purkinje cell counting, we created a stable transgenic line as described above where a nuclear localization signal (NLS) is fused to the N-terminus of GCaMP6s (PC:NLS-GCaMP6s) to restrict GCaMP6s to the nucleus.

Visual stimuli

For functional imaging experiments, trials were presented that consisted of the following stimuli, in non-randomized order: Black and white whole-field gratings were presented with motion in the forward direction at slow, medium, and fast speeds (3, 10, and 30 mm/s, respectively), for five seconds each with a pause of five seconds between stimuli, followed by reverse, leftward, and rightward moving gratings of the same duration and at medium speed. Grating remained static between stimuli. Black and white windmill patterns were rotated at 0.2 Hz with changing velocity that followed a sine function. Windmill patterns were presented across the whole field as well as for each half of the visual field. Flashes covered the whole visual field and switched between maximum luminance and darkness. For electrophysiological recordings, stimuli were similar as for functional imaging with the exception that the stimulus set had shorter pauses between stimuli and that fewer repetitions of the rotating windmill stimulus were presented. Blank trials consisting of static gratings were also interspersed with stimuli trials to obtain baseline responses. For one experiment (Figure 4—figure supplement 1g) the fish was also presented with a series of whole-field black or white flashes of various durations (50–5000 ms) against a baseline intermediate luminance.

Functional population imaging

Volumetric functional imaging in the larval zebrafish cerebellum was performed as previously described in Knogler et al., 2017. Briefly. 6–8 dpf nacre (mitfa -/-) transgenic zebrafish larvae with GCaMP6s expressed in Purkinje cells were embedded in 1.5–2.5% agarose prior to imaging. Neural activity was recorded with a custom-built two-photon microscope. A Ti- Sapphire laser (Spectra Physics Mai Tai) tuned to 905 nm was used for excitation. Larval brains were systematically imaged while presenting visual stimuli (see below) at 60 frames per second using a Telefunken microprojector controlled by custom Python software and filtered (Kodak Wratten No.25) to allow for simultaneous imaging and visual stimulation. We acquired the total cerebellar volume by sampling each plane at ~5 Hz. After all stimuli were shown in one plane, the focal plane was shifted ventrally by 1 µm and the process was repeated. Tail and eye movement was tracked throughout with 850 nm infrared illumination and customized, automated tracking software. Behavior was imaged at up to 200 frames per second using an infrared-sensitive charge-coupled device camera (Pike F032B, Allied Vision Technologies) and custom written software in Python.

Image processing

Image analysis was performed with MATLAB (MathWorks) and Python similar to Knogler et al., 2017. Python analysis used scikit-learn and scikit-image (Pedregosa et al., 2012; van der Walt et al., 2014). Volumetrically-acquired two-photon data was aligned first within a plane then across planes to ensure that stacks were aligned to each other with subpixel precision. Any experiments during which the fish drifted significantly in z were stopped and the data discarded. The boundary of the cerebellum was manually masked to remove external signals such as skin autofluoresence. All signals from all planes were extracted for voxelwise analysis (mean of approximately 350 billion ± 10 billion for 5 fish with 100 planes with an additional 118 billion for a sixth fish with only 34 planes). Purkinje cell ROI activity traces were extracted using automated algorithms based on local signal correlations between pixels (see Portugues et al., 2014 for details) and used for principal component analysis (see Materials and methods below). Tail activity during imaging experiments was processed to yield a vigor measurement (standard deviation of a 50 ms rolling buffer of the tail trace) that was greater than zero when the fish is moving. Independent left and right eye position and velocity were obtained from eye tracking data.

Single cell Purkinje cell imaging

Sparse labelled Purkinje cells expressing GCaMP6s were used to perform two-photon imaging as described above to identify any signal compartmentalization (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Visual stimuli consisting of reverse and forward moving gratings were probed to evoke signals in Purkinje cells. For five Purkinje cells across three fish, ROIs for soma and parts of the dendrite were drawn manually and Calcium traces were extracted using custom-written software in Python. The most distal dendritic ROI was correlated with somatic ROI to determine the correlation coefficient for each cell.

Electrophysiological neural recordings

Cell-attached electrophysiological recordings were performed in 6–8 dpf zebrafish as previously described (Knogler et al., 2017) using an Axopatch Multiclamp 700B amplifier, a Digidata series 1550 Digitizer, and pClamp nine software (Axon Instruments, Molecular Devices). Data were acquired at 8.3 kHz using Clampex 10.2. Wild-type or transgenic zebrafish larvae with GFP-positive Purkinje cells and motor neurons were used for most recordings (see subject details above).

Larvae were paralyzed in bath-applied buffered 1 mg/ml alpha-bungarotoxin (Cayman Scientific, Concord, CA) and embedded in 1.5% low melting point agarose in a 35 mm petri dish. External solution was composed of Evans solution (134 mM NaCl, 2.9 mM KCl, 2.1 mM CaCl2, 1.2 mM MgCl2, 10 mM glucose, 10 mM HEPES, pH 7.8 with NaOH). Electrodes for neuron recordings (6–12 MΩ) were pulled from thick-walled borosilicate glass with filament and were filled with the following intracellular solution (in mM): 105 D-gluconic acid, 16 KCl, 2 MgCl2, 10 HEPES, and 10 EGTA, adjusted to pH 7.2, 290 mOsm (Drapeau et al., 1999). Sulforhodamine B (0.1%) was included in the intracellular solution to visualize the electrode. The skin overlying the cerebellum was carefully removed with a glass electrode prior to recording. Post-recording fluorescent images of GFP-positive Purkinje cells and the recording electrode (visualized with an RFP filter) as well as bright-field images to confirm cell identity and map somatic location were acquired with an epifluorescent ThorLabs camera controlled by Micromanager.

Electrophysiological data were analyzed offline with Clampfit 10.2 software (Molecular Devices) and Matlab (Mathworks, Natick MA). Cell-attached traces were high-pass filtered at 1–10 Hz and complex spikes and simple spike were automatically extracted by setting a threshold for each type of spike in that recording. A 2.5 ms period was blanked following each complex spike so that the complex spike waveform did not cross the simple spike threshold. Baseline firing rates were calculated from blank trials where no visual stimuli were presented or from the two second period at the beginning of each trial prior to the first stimulus onset if no blanks were obtained.

For experiments with simultaneous calcium imaging, stochastically-labeled single Purkinje cells expressing GCaMP6s were recorded with an epifluorescent backlit-CMOS camera (Photometrix Prime 95B) at 11.5 fps controlled by Micromanager and triggered by pClamp software during electrophysiological recordings. No visual stimuli were shown in these experiments. Fluorescent Purkinje cell activity was processed by manual ROI extraction. Extracted complex spike and simple spike rates from simultaneous electrophysiology traces were convolved with a GCaMP6 kernel for comparison with the fluorescent signal.

For electrophysiological recordings in the semi-paralyzed animal, larval zebrafish were embedded in 2% low-melting point agarose and injected with 0.5 mg/ml alpha-bungarotoxin in the caudal tip of the tail. This method reduces the trunk contractions during swimming but preserved full eye movement. The agarose around the eyes was removed and the fish was lit from below with 850 nm infrared illumination to allow for good image contrast of the eyes. Eye movement was recorded during simultaneous electrophysiological recordings and tracked offline with customized, automated software to extract independent trajectories for each eye.

Ventral root recordings

To obtain extracellular ventral root recordings, a thin-walled borosilicate glass electrode with a large opening (approximately a quarter of the width of a somite) was first used to remove a small section of skin overlying the horizontal myotomes of the spinal cord around the fifth spinal somite. The electrode was then cleared with positive pressure and positioned over the terminals of the ventral root with gentle suction to ensure good signal to noise.

Motor activity was extracted as a moving standard deviation of the ventral root trace. A threshold was then applied to identify ventral root activity that would correspond to motor output on the side of the animal ipsilateral to the recording electrode. To extract a binarized trace of swimming bouts, ventral root activity separated by an interval of less than 100 ms was considered to be part of the same bout. The vigor trace was median filtered to extrapolate vigor information across the entire bout. Peaks in the lag of the autocorrelation analysis of the thresholded, binarized signal was used to extract fictive swim frequency.

Multilinear regression

Briefly, this analysis involves three steps. First, we processed and extracted physiological signals from imaging data and electrophysiological recordings (see above). Second, we used each different feature of the visual stimulus or motor behavior, such as rotational clockwise visual motion velocity, or the strength of the swimming bouts across a trial, to build a vector of values for each trial convolved with the temporal dynamics of the signal (calcium signal or firing rate). These feature vectors are termed regressors. Third, we performed multilinear regression to quantify the contribution of these different features to the signal of interest. This step included parameter validation to ensure that the results of the analysis are robust. Following this process, each signal is assigned a vector of coefficient weights that can be multiplied by the set of regressors to best recapitulate the activity of that signal.

Motor regressors were computed for each trial from the behavioral parameters obtained from eye and tail motor information in imaging and electrophysiology experiments (see above). Motor regressors for swimming were created to capture various features including bout onset, offset, duration, and vigor. Eye motor regressors captured directional velocity of each eye independently. Sensory regressors for each type of experiment were the same for all cells and were created using features including the duration, direction, and velocity of moving stimuli as well as luminance (see Figure 1—figure supplement 2 and Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for full regressor lists for imaging and electrophysiology).

For functional imaging data, regressors were convolved with a GCaMP6s kernel, modeled as a single exponential function with time decay constant tau = 1600 ms. The tau for this kernel was derived from the average single exponential fit of the fluorescence peak produced by a single complex spike as ascertained by simultaneously recorded GCaMP6s and electrophysiological signals (Figure 1—figure supplement 2, N = 8 cells). Regressors were normalized and passed to the scikit-learn function LinearRegression to compute the mulilinear regression coefficients, which was sufficient to accurately recapitulate the calcium traces (mean coefficient of multiple determination = 0.46 ± 0.02).

The higher sampling rates of electrophysiological recordings (8.3 KHz) allowed us to create additional regressors that captured more subtle features in the visual stimuli, for example the onset of translational motion in a given direction. The window for these regressors spanned a 500 ms period beginning at stimulus onset. Our previous electrophysiological recordings in granule cells have suggested a latency of ~100–200 ms for visual input to arrive at the input layer of the cerebellum (Knogler et al., 2017) similar to the mean latency of 126 ms reported for visual responses in the mouse inferior olive (Ju et al., 2018). Since most sensory stimuli were presented for longer periods (gratings for 5 s, windmill stimuli for 10 s, flashes for 1 s), this short window was designed to be sufficiently long to capture onset-related signals that face synaptic delays, but also clearly distinguish between responses that are transient at stimulus onset or last for the duration of the stimulus. Windmill stimuli had sinusoidal velocity and smoothly changed direction, thus multiple regressors were built for these stimuli that represented graded velocity, binary motion in a given direction, as well as change of direction. These motor and visual regressors were then convolved with a 20 ms filter to match the convolution of spiking into firing rates.

In order to best analyze our electrophysiological data with this extended set of regressors, we implemented a variant of lasso regression known as elastic net regularization using the function lasso from MATLAB. This is a useful fitting method for linear regression using generalized penalties that has been shown to be robust and gives sparse coefficient weight distributions such that in practice many regressor coefficients are zero (Zou and Hastie, 2005; Tibshirani, 2011; Dean et al., 2015).

Documentation from MATLAB (r2018b) gives the following formulation:

‘Elastic net solves the problem

minβ0.β12Ni=1Nyi-β0-xiTβ2+λPaβ, where

Paβ=1-α2β22+αβ1=j=1p1-α2βj2+αβj.

  • N is the number of observations.

  • yi is the response at observation i.

  • xi is data, a vector of length p at observation i.

  • λ is a nonnegative regularization parameter corresponding to one value of Lambda.

  • The parameters β0 and β are a scalar and a vector of length p, respectively.

  • The penalty term Paβ interpolates between the L1 norm of β and the squared L2 norm of β.’

Alpha values of 0.2 were used which represent an elastic net optimization with only modest sparsification, approaching ridge regression. Increasing the alpha parameter to move closer to an elastic net optimization did not significantly alter the main regressor weights. As the regularization coefficient Lambda increases, the number of nonzero components for regressor weights decreases. Lambda was selected by assessing the lowest total root mean squared error across the dataset following iterative regression with different parameter values: 0.9 for complex spike analyses and 0.8 for simple spike analyses. Both alpha and Lambda parameters were robust across a range of values for the distribution of coefficient weights. The same procedure was used to obtain both Purkinje cell and granule cell coefficient weights.

For analyses of both imaging and electrophysiological data, multilinear regression produced a vector of coefficient weights for all regressors for the activity of each cell/voxel. In the latter case, a separate set of coefficient weights was obtained for complex spikes and simple spikes. The estimated weights for each regressor for a given cell/voxel can take positive or negative values (or zero). Negative weights are interpretable as a relay through inhibitory neurons.

Purkinje cell maps (Figure 2a) shows mean z-projections of the regressor coefficients from a representative fish. Granule cell maps (Figure 6a) are means of seven morphed fish and were manually masked either for parallel fibers and granule cell somata to show potential differences in the signal topography. To further dissociate motor and sensory responses for sensory stimuli that strongly drive a particular behavior (translational motion and swimming, or rotational motion and left/right eye velocities), we used a maximum intensity projection of respective sensory and motor regressor maps and colored a pixel depending on whether sensory (magenta) or motor (green) regressors explain this pixel better with a given minimum distance. Differences that are below that minimum distance or are uncorrelated are colored white. Despite the slow time constant for the calcium signal decay, the variability of tail and eye movements across trials, including their onset, duration, and presence/absence, was sufficient to assign clear sensory or motor origins to the majority of these voxels.

For detailed electrophysiological analyses of the different classes of visual complex spike responses for Purkinje cells, we included for analyses all cells for which that regressor coefficient weight was significant. To determine which Purkinje cells showed significant responses to luminance, we used autocorrelation analyses of complex spike rates during whole-field flashes only and assessed significance using the Ljung-Box Q-test. For analysis of complex spikes and motor activity, we analyzed all cells with significant, nonzero motor regressor weights for complex spike activity. When examining the relationship between complex spikes and simple spike rates in individual Purkinje cells, cells with less than ten complex spikes for any condition (e.g. motor versus non-motor) were excluded from analysis.

Our multilinear regression analyses were carefully chosen in place of a series of separate simple regressions which would not provide useful or even correct insight into the question of which features these neurons are encoding. Multilinear regression is therefore preferred statistical method when considering which of multiple features contribute to a given signal and to what degree. However, as with any analysis, one must acknowledge the potential caveats or considerations when using this method (see Slinker and Glantz, 2008 for review). For example, although multilinear regression assumes a simple addition of the regressor multiplied by the coefficient values, different sensory and/or motor features could interact nonlinearly to influence a cell’s firing rate. Models do exist that incorporate nonlinearity (interaction terms), however these terms will highly correlate with each of the variables used to create the product and artificially introduce multicollinearity. Therefore since the R2 values of the linear fits were reasonable, we did not explore these models. The complete set of regressors used for electrophysiological analysis nonetheless face the consideration that even in a linear model some regressors will be correlated with each other (for example, stimulus onset and duration, or swim strength and duration). We addressed this concern in two ways. First, we explored a wide range of possible regressors, both quantitative and categorical, then we dropped unnecessary and redundant regressors that consistently gave small or zero coefficient values. This was done through variable selection methods to select the optimal pool of regressors. Second, we used the elastic net optimization of lasso with low alpha values that approach ridge regression, which specifically helps to sparsify the coefficients rather than split coefficient weights between correlated regressors.

Principal component analysis (PCA)

We performed PCA on the vector of correlations with all regressors for all automatically segmented ROIs and all fish. This correlation vector representation was clustered in the PC space in 10 clusters using k-means. This number was chosen because 10 PCs already explained ~90% of the variance. All voxels were then colored in according to the cluster they belonged to.

The anatomical clustering and stereotypy indices were calculated as follows. For the anatomical clustering index, the average distance between ROIs of the same cluster within a fish was compared against the average distance between an ROI from that cluster and a randomly chosen ROI from that fish. The inverse ratio of these two quantities is the anatomical clustering index. The stereotypy index is computed similarly. In this case, the average distance between an ROI from a particular cluster and fish and other ROIs from that same cluster but other fish is compared against the distance between an ROI from that same cluster and fish and other ROIs from other clusters and fish. Again, the inverse ratio of these two quantities is the stereotypy index. To summarize, the index is a comparison of average distance within a condition to average distance without the restraint of that condition.

Purkinje cell morphology

Sparsely labelled Purkinje cells were imaged using a 20x water immersion objective with 1 NA (Zeiss) on a confocal microscope (LSM 700, Carl Zeiss, Germany). High resolution stacks of Purkinje cells were deconvolved using Richardson-Lucy algorithm and artifacts were removed manually. Purkinje cell axonal projections were traced using NeuTube (Feng et al., 2015) and the Simple Neurite Tracer plugin for FIJI (Longair et al., 2011). SWC files were converted to line stacks and post-processed using custom written software in Python. Individual axonal projections were morphed together to a reference brain using aldoca:gap43-mCherry as a reference and CMTK as morphing tool (Rohlfing and Maurer, 2003). Dendritic planarity was assessed by performing principal component analysis on binarized dendritic morphologies. The ratio of the third principal component to the second was used to determine planarity (planar dendrites have ratios approaching 0, whereas nonplanar dendrites have ratios approaching 1).

Purkinje cell counting

We imaged three individual PC:NLS-GCaMP6s transgenic fish line at seven dpf using confocal microscopy as described for morphological experiments above. In this line, GCaMP6s is restricted to the nucleus and approximates a sphere. Consequently, we used 3D template matching using a 3D (spherical) Gaussian to find individual nuclei using custom written software in Python. False positives were removed and missed cells were added manually.

Quantification and statistical analysis

Data were analyzed in MATLAB and Python with custom software (Knogler, 2019; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/Knogler_etal_2019_eLife).

Values given in the text are mean ± standard error of the mean. Baseline complex spike firing rates for groups of Purkinje cells sorted by complex spike phenotype were compared by one-way ANOVA, followed by pairwise post hoc analyses using Bonferroni post hoc correction. The nonparametric Wilcoxon signed rank test was used on paired nonparametric datasets. Details of statistical analyses are found in the text and figure legends.

Data/resource sharing

Example electrophysiological datasets are available at https://zenodo.org/record/1494071. An example imaging dataset is available at https://zenodo.org/record/1638807. Further information and requests for data, resources, and reagents should be directed to Ruben Portugues (rportugues@neuro.mpg.de).

Acknowledgements

We thank Reinhard Köster for kindly providing the ca8 backbone used for transgenic line development. We thank Herwig Baier for providing mn7GFF;UAS:GFP and UAS:GCaMP6s fish. We thank Oliver Griesbeck for use of a Photometrics Prime 95B camera. We thank David Herzfeld, Vilim Štih, and Éliane Proulx for helpful comments on the manuscript. We thank the Reviewing Editor Indira Raman for useful advice. LDK was funded by the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation, the Carl von Siemens Foundation, the Fonds de recherche du Québec - Santé, and the Max Planck Gesellschaft (MPG). AMK was funded by the International Max Planck Research School for Life Sciences (IMPRS-LS), a Joachim-Herz fellowship, and the MPG. RP was funded by the MPG. This research was also partly funded by the DFG (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft - German Research Foundation) through grant PO 2105/2–1. LDK would like to dedicate this paper to the memory of her colleague and friend, Dr. Sean E Low.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Ruben Portugues, Email: rportugues@neuro.mpg.de.

Indira M Raman, Northwestern University, United States.

Ronald L Calabrese, Emory University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Alexander von Humboldt-Stiftung to Laura D Knogler.

  • Carl Friedrich von Siemens Foundation to Laura D Knogler.

  • Fonds de Recherche du Québec - Santé to Laura D Knogler.

  • Max-Planck-Gesellschaft Open-access funding to Laura D Knogler, Andreas M Kist, Ruben Portugues.

  • International Max Planck Research School for Life Sciences to Andreas M Kist.

  • Joachim Herz Stiftung to Andreas M Kist.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft PO 2105/2-1 to Laura D Knogler, Andreas M Kist, Ruben Portugues.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft.

Resources, Software, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All procedures involving animals were in accordance with the Max Planck Society guidelines and approved by the Regierung von Oberbayern (TVA# 55-2-1-54-2532-82-2016).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42138.022

Data availability

Example electrophysiological datasets are available at https://zenodo.org/record/1494071. An example imaging dataset is available at https://zenodo.org/record/1638807. MATLAB code for electrophysiological analysis available via GitHub (https://github.com/portugueslab/Knogler_etal_2019_eLife; copy archived athttps://github.com/elifesciences-publications/Knogler_etal_2019_eLife).

References

  1. Ahrens MB, Li JM, Orger MB, Robson DN, Schier AF, Engert F, Portugues R. Brain-wide neuronal dynamics during motor adaptation in zebrafish. Nature. 2012;485:471–477. doi: 10.1038/nature11057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aizenberg M, Schuman EM. Cerebellar-dependent learning in larval zebrafish. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:8708–8712. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6565-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Albergaria C, Silva NT, Pritchett DL, Carey MR. Locomotor activity modulates associative learning in mouse cerebellum. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:725–735. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0129-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Albus JS. A theory of cerebellar function. Mathematical Biosciences. 1971;10:25–61. doi: 10.1016/0025-5564(71)90051-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Apps R. Movement-related gating of climbing fibre input to cerebellar cortical zones. Progress in Neurobiology. 1999;57:537–562. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00068-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Apps R, Hawkes R. Cerebellar cortical organization: a one-map hypothesis. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:670–681. doi: 10.1038/nrn2698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Asakawa K, Suster ML, Mizusawa K, Nagayoshi S, Kotani T, Urasaki A, Kishimoto Y, Hibi M, Kawakami K. Genetic dissection of neural circuits by Tol2 transposon-mediated Gal4 gene and enhancer trapping in zebrafish. PNAS. 2008;105:1255–1260. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704963105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Asakawa K, Abe G, Kawakami K. Cellular dissection of the spinal cord motor column by BAC transgenesis and gene trapping in zebrafish. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2013;7:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Badura A, Schonewille M, Voges K, Galliano E, Renier N, Gao Z, Witter L, Hoebeek FE, Chédotal A, De Zeeuw CI. Climbing fiber input shapes reciprocity of purkinje cell firing. Neuron. 2013;78:700–713. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bae YK, Kani S, Shimizu T, Tanabe K, Nojima H, Kimura Y, Higashijima S, Hibi M. Anatomy of zebrafish cerebellum and screen for mutations affecting its development. Developmental Biology. 2009;330:406–426. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2009.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bajar BT, Wang ES, Lam AJ, Kim BB, Jacobs CL, Howe ES, Davidson MW, Lin MZ, Chu J. Improving brightness and photostability of green and red fluorescent proteins for live cell imaging and FRET reporting. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:1–12. doi: 10.1038/srep20889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bhandiwad AA, Zeddies DG, Raible DW, Rubel EW, Sisneros JA. Auditory sensitivity of larval zebrafish (Danio rerio) measured using a behavioral prepulse inhibition assay. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2013;216:3504–3513. doi: 10.1242/jeb.087635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bouvier G, Aljadeff J, Clopath C, Bimbard C, Ranft J, Blot A, Nadal J-P, Brunel N, Hakim V, Barbour B. Cerebellar learning using perturbations. eLife. 2018;7:53785. doi: 10.7554/eLife.31599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brockerhoff SE, Hurleyt JB, Janssen-bienholdt U, Neuhauss SCF, Driever W, Dowling JE. System defects. Neurobiology. 1995;92:10545–10549. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.23.10545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Budick SA, O'Malley DM. Locomotor repertoire of the larval zebrafish: swimming, turning and prey capture. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2000;203:2565–2579. doi: 10.1242/jeb.203.17.2565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Burgess HA, Granato M. Modulation of locomotor activity in larval zebrafish during light adaptation. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2007;210:2526–2539. doi: 10.1242/jeb.003939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cerminara NL, Lang EJ, Sillitoe RV, Apps R. Redefining the cerebellar cortex as an assembly of non-uniform purkinje cell microcircuits. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2015;16:79–93. doi: 10.1038/nrn3886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cerminara NL, Apps R. Behavioural significance of cerebellar modules. The Cerebellum. 2011;10:484–494. doi: 10.1007/s12311-010-0209-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chen TW, Wardill TJ, Sun Y, Pulver SR, Renninger SL, Baohan A, Schreiter ER, Kerr RA, Orger MB, Jayaraman V, Looger LL, Svoboda K, Kim DS. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature. 2013;499:295–300. doi: 10.1038/nature12354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Crepel F, Mariani J, Delhaye-Bouchaud N. Evidence for a multiple innervation of purkinje cells by climbing fibers in the immature rat cerebellum. Journal of Neurobiology. 1976;7:567–578. doi: 10.1002/neu.480070609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. De Zeeuw CI, Hoebeek FE, Bosman LW, Schonewille M, Witter L, Koekkoek SK. Spatiotemporal firing patterns in the cerebellum. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2011;12:327–344. doi: 10.1038/nrn3011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dean P, Porrill J, Kingdom U. At the edge of chaos : how cerebellar granular layer network dynamics can provide the basis for temporal filters. PLoS Computational Biology. 2015:1–28. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dizon MJ, Khodakhah K. The role of interneurons in shaping purkinje cell responses in the cerebellar cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:10463–10473. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1350-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Drapeau P, Ali DW, Buss RR, Saint-Amant L. In vivo recording from identifiable neurons of the locomotor network in the developing zebrafish. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 1999;88:1–13. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(99)00008-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dreosti E, Lopes G, Kampff AR, Wilson SW. Development of social behavior in young zebrafish. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2015;9 doi: 10.3389/fncir.2015.00039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Easter SS, Nicola GN. The development of vision in the zebrafish (Danio rerio) Developmental Biology. 1996;180:646–663. doi: 10.1006/dbio.1996.0335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Eccles JC, Ito M, Szentagothai J. The Cerebellum as a Neuronal Machine. springer; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  28. Favre-Bulle IA, Vanwalleghem G, Taylor MA, Rubinsztein-Dunlop H, Scott EK. Cellular-Resolution imaging of vestibular processing across the larval zebrafish brain. Current Biology. 2018;28:3711–3722. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.09.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Feng L, Zhao T, Kim J. neuTube 1.0: a new design for efficient neuron reconstruction software based on the SWC format. eNeuro. 2015;2 doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0049-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fero K, Yokogawa T, Burgess HA. The Behavioral Repertoire of Larval Zebrafish. In: Kalueff A, Cachat J. M, editors. Zebrafish Models in Neurobehavioral Research. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press; 2011. pp. 249–291. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gao Z, van Beugen BJ, De Zeeuw CI. Distributed synergistic plasticity and cerebellar learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13:619–635. doi: 10.1038/nrn3312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Giovannucci A, Badura A, Deverett B, Najafi F, Pereira TD, Gao Z, Ozden I, Kloth AD, Pnevmatikakis E, Paninski L, De Zeeuw CI, Medina JF, Wang SS. Cerebellar granule cells acquire a widespread predictive feedback signal during motor learning. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:727–734. doi: 10.1038/nn.4531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hamling KR, Tobias ZJ, Weissman TA. Mapping the development of cerebellar purkinje cells in zebrafish. Developmental Neurobiology. 2015;75:1174–1188. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Harmon TC, Magaram U, McLean DL, Raman IM. Distinct responses of purkinje neurons and roles of simple spikes during associative motor learning in larval zebrafish. eLife. 2017;6:e22537. doi: 10.7554/eLife.22537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Harvey RJ, Napper RM. Quantitative studies on the mammalian cerebellum. Progress in Neurobiology. 1991;36:437–463. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(91)90012-P. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hashimoto M, Hibi M. Development and evolution of cerebellar neural circuits. Development, Growth & Differentiation. 2012;54:373–389. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-169X.2012.01348.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hawkes R. Purkinje cell stripes and long-term depression at the parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapse. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2014;8:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Heffley W, Song EY, Xu Z, Taylor B, Hughes MA, McKinney A, Joshua M, Hull C. Coordinated cerebellar climbing fiber activity is gated by behavioral context in a voluntary motor regime. Nature Neuroscience. 2018 doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0228-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Herzfeld DJ, Kojima Y, Soetedjo R, Shadmehr R. Encoding of action by the purkinje cells of the cerebellum. Nature. 2015;526:439–442. doi: 10.1038/nature15693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hsieh JY, Ulrich B, Issa FA, Wan J, Papazian DM. Rapid development of purkinje cell excitability, functional cerebellar circuit, and afferent sensory input to cerebellum in zebrafish. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2014;8:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2014.00147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hsieh J-Y, Papazian DM. Extracellular Loose-Patch Recording of Purkinje Cell Activity in Awake Zebrafish and Emergence of Functional Cerebellar Circuit. In: Sillitoe R. V, editor. Extracellular Recording Approaches. New York, NY: Springer; 2018. pp. 207–224. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ito M. Neural design of the cerebellar motor control system. Brain Research. 1972;40:81–84. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90110-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ito M. Cerebellar control of the vestibulo-ocular reflex--around the flocculus hypothesis. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1982;5:275–297. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.05.030182.001423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ito M. Cerebellar long-term depression: characterization, signal transduction, and functional roles. Physiological Reviews. 2001;81:1143–1195. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.3.1143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Ito M. Cerebellar circuitry as a neuronal machine. Progress in Neurobiology. 2006;78:272–303. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ito M. Error detection and representation in the olivo-cerebellar system. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2013;7:1. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Jelitai M, Puggioni P, Ishikawa T, Rinaldi A, Duguid I. Dendritic excitation–inhibition balance shapes cerebellar output during motor behaviour. Nature Communications. 2016;7:1–13. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Ju C, Bosman LWJ, Hoogland TM, Velauthapillai A, Murugesan P, Warnaar P, Negrello M, De ZCI. Neurons of the inferior olive respond to broad classes of sensory input while subject to homeostatic control. bioRxiv. 2018 doi: 10.1101/379149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  49. Kennedy A, Wayne G, Kaifosh P, Alviña K, Abbott LF, Sawtell NB. A temporal basis for predicting the sensory consequences of motor commands in an electric fish. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:416–422. doi: 10.1038/nn.3650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kitamura K, Kano M. Dendritic calcium signaling in cerebellar purkinje cell. Neural Networks. 2013;47:11–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Knogler LD, Markov DA, Dragomir EI, Štih V, Portugues R. Sensorimotor representations in cerebellar granule cells in larval zebrafish are dense, spatially organized, and Non-temporally patterned. Current Biology. 2017;27:1288–1302. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.03.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Knogler LD. Github; 2019. https://github.com/portugueslab/Knogler_etal_2019_eLife [Google Scholar]
  53. Koziol LF, Budding D, Andreasen N, D’Arrigo S, Bulgheroni S, Imamizu H, Ito M, Manto M, Marvel C, Parker K, Pezzulo G, Ramnani N, Riva D, Schmahmann J, Vandervert L, Yamazaki T. Consensus paper: the cerebellum's Role in Movement and Cognition. The Cerebellum. 2014;13:151–177. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0511-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Lang EJ, Apps R, Bengtsson F, Cerminara NL, De Zeeuw CI, Ebner TJ, Heck DH, Jaeger D, Jörntell H, Kawato M, Otis TS, Ozyildirim O, Popa LS, Reeves AM, Schweighofer N, Sugihara I, Xiao J. The roles of the olivocerebellar pathway in motor learning and motor control. A consensus paper. The Cerebellum. 2017;16:230–252. doi: 10.1007/s12311-016-0787-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lev-Ram V, Miyakawa H, Lasser-Ross N, Ross WN. Calcium transients in cerebellar purkinje neurons evoked by intracellular stimulation. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1992;68:1167–1177. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.4.1167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Loewenstein Y, Mahon S, Chadderton P, Kitamura K, Sompolinsky H, Yarom Y, Häusser M. Bistability of cerebellar purkinje cells modulated by sensory stimulation. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:202–211. doi: 10.1038/nn1393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Longair MH, Baker DA, Armstrong JD. Simple neurite tracer: open source software for reconstruction, visualization and analysis of neuronal processes. Bioinformatics. 2011;27:2453–2454. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Marr D. A theory of cerebellar cortex. The Journal of Physiology. 1969;202:437–470. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1969.sp008820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Masino MA, Fetcho JR. Fictive swimming motor patterns in wild type and mutant larval zebrafish. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;93:3177–3188. doi: 10.1152/jn.01248.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Mathews PJ, Lee KH, Peng Z, Houser CR, Otis TS. Effects of climbing fiber driven inhibition on purkinje neuron spiking. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:17988–17997. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3916-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Matsui H, Namikawa K, Babaryka A, Köster RW. Functional regionalization of the teleost cerebellum analyzed in vivo. PNAS. 2014;111:11846–11851. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1403105111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. McElligott MB, O'malley DM. Prey tracking by larval zebrafish: axial kinematics and visual control. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2005;66:177–196. doi: 10.1159/000087158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Migault G, van der Plas TL, Trentesaux H, Panier T, Candelier R, Proville R, Englitz B, Debrégeas G, Bormuth V. Whole-Brain calcium imaging during physiological vestibular stimulation in larval zebrafish. Current Biology. 2018;28:3723–3735. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Miller N, Gerlai R. From schooling to shoaling: patterns of collective motion in zebrafish (Danio rerio) PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e48865. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Ohmae S, Medina JF. Climbing fibers encode a temporal-difference prediction error during cerebellar learning in mice. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:1798–1803. doi: 10.1038/nn.4167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Ozden I, Dombeck DA, Hoogland TM, Tank DW, Wang SS. Widespread state-dependent shifts in cerebellar activity in locomoting mice. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e42650. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ozol K, Hayden JM, Oberdick J, Hawkes R. Transverse zones in the vermis of the mouse cerebellum. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1999;412:95–111. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990913)412:1&#x0003c;95::AID-CNE7&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Palay SL, Chan-Palay V. Cerebellar Cortex: Cytology and Organization. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1974. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. Pedregosa F, Varoquaux G, Gramfort A, Michel V, Thirion B, Grisel O, Blondel M, Prettenhofer P, Weiss R, Dubourg V, Vanderplas J, Passos A, Cournapeau D, Brucher M, Perrot M, É D. Scikit-learn: machine learning in python. Journal of Machine Learning Research. 2012;12:2825–2830. [Google Scholar]
  70. Pivetta C, Esposito MS, Sigrist M, Arber S. Motor-circuit communication matrix from spinal cord to brainstem neurons revealed by developmental origin. Cell. 2014;156:537–548. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Porrill J, Dean P, Anderson SR. Adaptive filters and internal models: multilevel description of cerebellar function. Neural Networks. 2013;47:134–149. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2012.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Portugues R, Feierstein CE, Engert F, Orger MB. Whole-brain activity maps reveal stereotyped, distributed networks for visuomotor behavior. Neuron. 2014;81:1328–1343. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Powell K, Mathy A, Duguid I, Häusser M. Synaptic representation of locomotion in single cerebellar granule cells. eLife. 2015;4:e07292. doi: 10.7554/eLife.07290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Raman IM, Bean BP. Resurgent sodium current and action potential formation in dissociated cerebellar purkinje neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:4517–4526. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-12-04517.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ramirez JE, Stell BM. Calcium imaging reveals coordinated simple spike pauses in populations of cerebellar purkinje cells. Cell Reports. 2016;17:3125–3132. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.11.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Raymond JL, Medina JF. Computational principles of supervised learning in the cerebellum. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2018;41:233–253. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-061948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Rohlfing T, Maurer CR. Nonrigid image registration in shared-memory multiprocessor environments with application to brains, breasts, and bees. IEEE Transactions on Information Technology in Biomedicine. 2003;7:16–25. doi: 10.1109/TITB.2003.808506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sauerbrei BA, Lubenov EV, Siapas AG. Structured variability in purkinje cell activity during locomotion. Neuron. 2015;87:840–852. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Scalise K, Shimizu T, Hibi M, Sawtell NB. Responses of cerebellar purkinje cells during fictive optomotor behavior in larval zebrafish. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2016;116:2067–2080. doi: 10.1152/jn.00042.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Sengupta M, Thirumalai V. AMPA receptor mediated synaptic excitation drives state-dependent bursting in purkinje neurons of zebrafish larvae. eLife. 2015;4:e09158. doi: 10.7554/eLife.09158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Severi KE, Portugues R, Marques JC, O'Malley DM, Orger MB, Engert F. Neural control and modulation of swimming speed in the larval zebrafish. Neuron. 2014;83:692–707. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Simpson JI, Wylie DR, De Zeeuw CI. On climbing fiber signals and their consequence(s) Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1996;19:384–398. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00081486. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  83. Simpson JI, Alley KE. Visual climbing fiber input to rabbit vestibulo-cerebellum: a source of direction-specific information. Brain Research. 1974;82:302–308. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90610-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Slinker BK, Glantz SA. Multiple linear regression: accounting for multiple simultaneous determinants of a continuous dependent variable. Circulation. 2008;117:1732–1737. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.106.654376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Soetedjo R, Fuchs AF. Complex spike activity of purkinje cells in the oculomotor vermis during behavioral adaptation of monkey saccades. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:7741–7755. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4658-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Stoodley CJ, Valera EM, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography of the cerebellum for motor and cognitive tasks: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2012;59:1560–1570. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Streng ML, Popa LS, Ebner TJ. Complex spike wars: a new hope. The Cerebellum. 2018;17:735–746. doi: 10.1007/s12311-018-0960-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Suvrathan A, Payne HL, Raymond JL. Timing Rules for Synaptic Plasticity Matched to Behavioral Function. Neuron. 2016;92:959–967. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Takeuchi M, Matsuda K, Yamaguchi S, Asakawa K, Miyasaka N, Lal P, Yoshihara Y, Koga A, Kawakami K, Shimizu T, Hibi M. Establishment of Gal4 transgenic zebrafish lines for analysis of development of cerebellar neural circuitry. Developmental Biology. 2015;397:1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2014.09.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Takeuchi M, Yamaguchi S, Sakakibara Y, Hayashi T, Matsuda K, Hara Y, Tanegashima C, Shimizu T, Kuraku S, Hibi M. Gene expression profiling of granule cells and purkinje cells in the zebrafish cerebellum. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2017;525:1558–1585. doi: 10.1002/cne.24114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Temizer I, Donovan JC, Baier H, Semmelhack JL. A visual pathway for Looming-Evoked escape in larval zebrafish. Current Biology. 2015;25:1823–1834. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. ten Brinke MM, Boele HJ, Spanke JK, Potters JW, Kornysheva K, Wulff P, IJpelaar AC, Koekkoek SK, De Zeeuw CI. Evolving models of pavlovian conditioning: cerebellar cortical dynamics in awake behaving mice. Cell Reports. 2015;13:1977–1988. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.10.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Thiele TR, Donovan JC, Baier H. Descending control of swim posture by a midbrain nucleus in zebrafish. Neuron. 2014;83:679–691. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Thompson AW. Library Search; 2016. PhD Thesis, University of Queensland.https://espace.library.uq.edu.au/view/UQ:376894 [Google Scholar]
  95. Tibshirani R. Regression shrinkage and selection via the lasso: a retrospective. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B. 2011;73:273–282. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9868.2011.00771.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  96. van Beugen BJ, Gao Z, Boele HJ, Hoebeek F, De Zeeuw CI. High frequency burst firing of granule cells ensures transmission at the parallel fiber to purkinje cell synapse at the cost of temporal coding. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2013;7:95. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. van der Walt S, Schönberger JL, Nunez-Iglesias J, Boulogne F, Warner JD, Yager N, Gouillart E, Yu T, scikit-image contributors scikit-image: image processing in Python. PeerJ. 2014;2:e453. doi: 10.7717/peerj.453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Vanwalleghem G, Heap LA, Scott EK. A profile of auditory-responsive neurons in the larval zebrafish brain. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2017;525:3031–3043. doi: 10.1002/cne.24258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Witter L, De Zeeuw CI. Regional functionality of the cerebellum. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2015;33:150–155. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Wylie DR, Frost BJ. Purkinje cells in the vestibulocerebellum of the pigeon respond best to either translational or rotational wholefield visual motion. Experimental Brain Research. 1991;86:229–232. doi: 10.1007/BF00231059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Xiao J, Cerminara NL, Kotsurovskyy Y, Aoki H, Burroughs A, Wise AK, Luo Y, Marshall SP, Sugihara I, Apps R, Lang EJ. Systematic regional variations in purkinje cell spiking patterns. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e105633. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Zhou H, Lin Z, Voges K, Ju C, Gao Z, Bosman LW, Ruigrok TJ, Hoebeek FE, De Zeeuw CI, Schonewille M. Cerebellar modules operate at different frequencies. eLife. 2014;3:e02536. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Zhou H, Voges K, Lin Z, Ju C, Schonewille M. Differential purkinje cell simple spike activity and pausing behavior related to cerebellar modules. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2015;113:2524–2536. doi: 10.1152/jn.00925.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Zou H, Hastie T. Regularization and variable selection via the elastic net. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B. 2005;67:301–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9868.2005.00503.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Indira M Raman1
Reviewed by: Eva Naumann2, David L McLean3

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

[Editors’ note: this article was originally rejected after discussions between the reviewers, but the authors were invited to resubmit after an appeal against the decision.]

Thank you for submitting your work entitled "Motor context dominates output from Purkinje cell functional regions during reflexive visuomotor behaviors" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and a Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Eva Naumann (Reviewer #1); David L McLean (Reviewer #2).

Our decision has been reached after extensive consultation among the reviewers and editors. Based on these discussions and the individual reviews below, we regret to inform you that your work in its present form will not be considered further for publication in eLife.

Because the depth of the discussion is not fully represented in the reviews, we provide a fuller than usual explanation for the decision, quoting and paraphrasing from the discussion (including editor's comments), in the hope that it will be of value to you as you proceed with your work. As you will see in the reviews, the reviewers found a great deal of value in the work from the perspective of the technical achievement of documenting the climbing fiber responses of Purkinje cells of the full zebrafish cerebellum in the context of visuomotor behaviors. There were, however, some important concerns, as follows:

1) Relating to approach and analysis: there is much potentially valuable data, but analyses are opaque and not clearly validated. Most simply, we are not actually shown the relationships between the data and the transformations sufficiently to be assured of their validity. We quickly are taken to derived measures, but methods are not always clear, critical tests of the results are absent, considerations of context and alternatives are not given, caveats are not presented, and many key elements of the data are too microscopic or on too slow of a time base to be intelligible. A few examples: a 1600 ms GCaMP convolution kernel for the imaging is long; how do we know that the correlations are valid without seeing what happens in this transformation? The windows of 'sensory' and 'motor' for the relation of complex spikes to one or the other category aren't clearly defined; since complex spikes often occur singly, it is hard to associate them with a sensory or motor event without some temporal criterion; and the swimming bouts are impossible to see on the time scales shown, and so how was a criterion defined to be associated with ‘motor’? Similar points arise for most figures. Likewise, panels are not always called out correctly, and descriptions in the text are often difficult to see reflected in the data that are illustrated.

2) Because the experiments only entail correlations within one modality, the conclusions are limited to providing additional support for attributes of cerebellar physiology for which there is already substantial accumulating evidence (e.g., complex spikes can relay sensory inputs, while granule motor-related information can come in through the granule cells; the notion of state-dependence in Figure 5 is interesting, but the presented result can simply be inferred from the idea that there is more parallel fiber drive during swimming, and so the illustration does not actually provide any further insight). In this context, attention was drawn by the reviewers to the fact that optogenetic tools that might be useful for manipulations are described in the Materials and methods but are not actually used in the paper.

3) Given that a single modality was studied, the conclusions are rather over-generalized without consideration of what the cerebellum might do in other behaviors and the final model offers little further insight., e.g., the claim in the discussion that complex spikes don't encode predictive motor signals. Nothing studied here actually tests anything about prediction. Here a relevant point might be the link to the associative learning study of Harmon et al., which found different patterns of complex and simple spike activity in a different set of behaviors, yet evidence for a topographic segregation into three classes is nevertheless still present. More experiments and/or more consideration of published work would be necessary to make the model sophisticated and flexible enough to be useful.

A strong case was made by some reviewers that perhaps more data can be gathered to conduct more investigative tests and generate some more definitive conclusions and/or a more in-depth model, which could strengthen the manuscript. If this is the case, we encourage you to address all the comments herein and appeal to resubmit the manuscript; however, the charge of the evaluation is to judge the data presented and so we cannot return a favorable decision based on the conjecture that such experiments might be forthcoming.

Reviewer #1:

This manuscript presents new insights on how the entire vertebrate cerebellum activity is related to multiple visuomotor transformations, particularly its relation to visual motion cues and eye and body movements. The cerebellum is thought to integrate sensory and motor-related information in Purkinje cells (PC), the main computational components that disseminate information to the output nuclei and enable motor learning.

The significance of Knogler et al.'s research lies in examining the complex relationship between sensorimotor states and PC activity (both simple spikes (SSs) and complex spikes (CSs)). They did this using an interesting multilinear regression approach that allowed them to disentangle the encoding of sensory and motor variables, even when correlated. They harness the experimental power of the zebrafish to comprehensively describe all PCs within individuals to reveal the functional topography, and intricately demonstrate how the zebrafish cerebellum segregates into functional modules, such that PCs in different regions collect visual input via climbing fibers; they also present evidence that CSs can change SS rates in a behavior-state dependent fashion.

Starting with two-photon calcium imaging, they use transgenic fish expressing fluorescent calcium sensors only in PCs to holistically describe PC function across the entire cerebellum. They reveal the response profiles of the entire set of these particular cell types. They demonstrate that anatomical functional clustering exists along behavioral dimensions, sorting PCs into three regions that handle different classes of sensory information. In a second phase of experiments, they use electrophysiology to better parse the CS and SS correlations with sensorimotor variables, and one another. The context-dependence of the CS/SS interactions was a very interesting and provocative result, with the potential to open up whole new lines of research. The inclusion of eye-movement signals as a regressor was an important control. Together, the methodologies and tools that were developed in this study are a significant step to investigate how PC function in these different zones affects learning.

The research appears rigorous, well executed and presented data are extensive and appropriately analyzed with relevant statistical methods, including appropriate treatment of individual animals. The figures are relatively easy to absorb, and while the legends are clear they could be shortened significantly. The presented supplementary data answers for many concerns and provides controls. And the Materials and methods section is clear but seems to have an unnecessary section discussing optogenetics. It seems like the authors generated (PC:ChR2(H134R)-tagRFP) under ca8 promoter control but we are not shown any data.

Despite the many strengths of the current manuscript, some questions remain and require revisions or discussion in the paper:

1) The main experimental weakness is that they focused on correlations only, without doing experimental perturbations for causality. For future directions the authors might use laser ablations or optogenetic perturbations of these different sectors or functional cell types. Evidently, one strength of their zebrafish preparation is that they can actually control (almost) all the cells in a population of interest and look at causality by measuring motor output. Since the authors seemed to have generated the optogenetic tools for this project ca8 (PC:ChR2(H134R)-tagRFP), why was no data presented? Please explain why this section was included but none of the results.

2) The authors could have done more to explain the 'voxel/multilinear regression' analysis method and it is confusing to call the imaging volumetric without explanation in the main text. In particular, it would help if they discussed the multilinear regression method in more detail: for instance, when sensory and motor variables are highly correlated (which they clearly are), how does the multilinear regression serve to separate out these effects, and does it take into account interaction terms? Also, it would be nice if they did some kind of internal validation of this technique, for instance fixing the stimulus (e.g., leftward motion) and examining how well the model predicted behavior, or at least explained why such a validation procedure was not used.

3) It would be great if they had expanded on the broader view in the literature that CSs encode error. As is, the reader learns almost nothing about how they interpret these results.

4) Another limitation is that the authors focused only on one modality without discussing relevance to other modalities and motor output. It would be better if this were discussed in more detail in both the main text and the discussion. And, in the future, it would be great to see how the same set of PCs processes information from other modalities to guide behavior.

5) Given the presented data, the model in Figure 6 is somewhat disappointing. A cell-type specific model that would illustrate the predictions for perturbation experiments would be preferred.

6) Since much of the authors' interpretation is concerned with a difference in motor and sensory signals, a different analysis than these simplified motor regressor would be favored. It would be good to show continuous quantitative readout of behavior as a regressor. Perhaps some measure like the rectified ratio of left and right outputs of the ventral root traces (as in Dunn et al., 2016). Their relatively simple measures of behavior could bias the results to favor a sensory coding scheme.

7) It would be interesting to see a map as in Figure 2F for all recorded PC neurons by using the information from electrophysiologically recorded neurons.

If these points would be addressed, the presented research is a good fit for eLife. Both the insights into vertebrate cerebellar function and technical execution are of high quality, justifying publication in this journal.

Reviewer #2:

The work by Knogler and colleagues uses sophisticated microscopic and analytical methods to examine the functional organization of Purkinje cells in the larval zebrafish cerebellum. Contrary to the authors assertions (Results section first paragraph), this is not the first population level study of the cerebellum (Aizenberg and Schuman, 2011; Matsui et al., 2014). Also, the discovery of caudolateral Purkinje cells that are associated with eye movements and more rostromedial neurons associated with tail movements has been reported previously (Matsui et al., 2014). Regression and principal component analysis has been used to dissect relative sensory and motor contributions to responses, however the there is insufficient detail describing the methodology in the main text or Materials and methods section for non-computational neuroscientists to decipher what is going on. In Figure 1C, for example, the 'for motion' regressors have three peaks that align well with three of the largest z-score deviations, but many of the other deviations appear to be ignored. Why? Unfortunately, this acts to undermine much of the paper, which is further exacerbated by occasions where the authors either call out figure panels with statements that lack supporting evidence (see Results section, third paragraph and Figure 1D – reverse grating motion and luminance responses look identical to me; subsection “Purkinje cells in different regions receive climbing fiber input related to different, specific visual features and send outputs to different downstream regions” and Figure 2D,G – I'm not sure where to find the analysis of depression here; subsection “Motor activity is broadly represented in granule cell signals” and Figure 4I – not seeing any periodicity to speak of) or are too small to make any meaningful interpretation (see the same section and Figure 4K – not sure how you are supposed to see phase differences at this magnification). From reading the manuscript, you would think that the zebrafish cerebellum is only involved in encoding visuomotor behaviors (subsections “Purkinje cells combine sensory and motor information from distinct inputs” and “Regional functionality of the zebrafish and mammalian cerebellum”). Auditory, tactile, thermosensory modalities are also highly salient sensory modalities, but are not explored here. Since these modalities were not tested, it is not clear how the pattern generalizes. The authors admit themselves that patterns can change based on how the fish is stimulated (subsection “Motor activity is broadly represented in granule cell signals”). It is also not clear how these data fit with recent descriptions of regionalization also based on complex spike activity in zebrafish (Harmon et al., 2017). Overall, I wanted to like this paper, but the lack of work made for a rather frustrating read. The authors need to do a better job of placing their work in the context of previous discoveries, focus more on what is new here, and do a more thorough job of describing it.

Reviewer #3:

Reading the manuscript "Motor context dominates output from Purkinje cell functional regions during reflexive visuomotor behaviors", Knogler et al. present large datasets from whole population Purkinje cell Ca2+-imaging and single cell patch recording in the zebrafish larval cerebellum during visually driven reflexive behaviors. While complex spikes are identified to be associated with sensory correlates, simple spike firing instead represents motor variables. Complex spikes seem to represent sensory climbing fiber input relating to visual cues and these complex spikes can modulate simple spike firing extensively, while the zebrafish larvae rest, whereas during swimming, during which simple spike firing increases gradually in frequency, modulation of simple spike firing by complex spikes is temporally more restricted to about 50 milliseconds across the Purkinje cell population. Gradually increasing simple spike firing correlates with increased swim strength but is not in phase with the latter, arguing for simple spike firing to represent motor afferent copies transmitted via granule cells rather than proprioceptive or lateral line derived input. In addition, the analysis of complex spike firing simultaneous to monitoring behavior reveals that complex spike firing precedes swim behavior but does not seem to predict motor behavior as spike firing remains unchanged albeit the stimuli-elicited swim bouts vary. Moreover, topographical analysis of complex and simple spike firing reveals a topographical organization of the Purkinje cell layer being subdivided into three functional regions along its rostro-caudal axis with respect to sensory climbing fiber input-stimulated complex firing in Purkinje cells. In contrast, motor-related simple spike firing occurs across the Purkinje cell layer without obvious topographical organization in functional regions.

Thus, in summary, this manuscript presents a huge amount of novel highly interesting and exciting findings, the analysis is performed with large care, the data is presented beautifully and manuscript clearly and convincingly written. Not being an expert in mathematics, computation and electrophysiology I cannot comment extensively on this part of the manuscript and have to leave this to my colleague reviewers. From the point of neuroanatomical organization of the differentiating zebrafish cerebellum some points remain that could in my opinion make this manuscript even nicer:

1) The authors suggest that the Purkinje cell population receives a topographically organized input from climbing fibers stimulating complex firing localized to three different regions along the rostro-caudal axis of the Purkinje cell layer (e.g. in paragraph five of subsection “Purkinje cells in different regions receive climbing fiber input related to different, specific visual features and send outputs to different downstream regions”). Climbing fiber projections in zebrafish have been demonstrated but are only poorly characterized at last. I understand that the authors are working on this and a full neuroanatomical and physiological analysis of climbing fiber projection and connectivity will exceed the purpose and focus of this manuscript. Nevertheless, a few studies supporting the suggested topographic climbing fiber organization should be provided.

2) In addition, it remained unclear to me whether climbing fibers synapse with Purkinje cell somata or along their dendritic trees or both. Is there a preferential location of PC-CF synapses?

3) The authors state the zebrafish contain two different types of anatomically different Purkinje cells according to their projections that occur either internally to eurydendroid cells or externally to the vestibular nuclei. Does this model also include Purkinje cell collaterals projecting to Purkinje cells nearby or do the authors think that such PC-PC connectivities do not exist in zebrafish?

4) The authors performed whole cell recordings in the caudolateral region of the Purkinje cell layer during rotational windmill motion evoked optokinetic reflex. Were these Purkinje cells internally of externally projecting PCs?

eLife. 2019 Jan 25;8:e42138. doi: 10.7554/eLife.42138.025

Author response


Because the depth of the discussion is not fully represented in the reviews, we provide a fuller than usual explanation for the decision, quoting and paraphrasing from the discussion (including editor's comments), in the hope that it will be of value to you as you proceed with your work. As you will see in the reviews, the reviewers found a great deal of value in the work from the perspective of the technical achievement of documenting the climbing fiber responses of Purkinje cells of the full zebrafish cerebellum in the context of visuomotor behaviors. There were, however, some important concerns, as follows:

1) Relating to approach and analysis: there is much potentially valuable data, but analyses are opaque and not clearly validated. Most simply, we are not actually shown the relationships between the data and the transformations sufficiently to be assured of their validity. We quickly are taken to derived measures, but methods are not always clear, critical tests of the results are absent, considerations of context and alternatives are not given, caveats are not presented, and many key elements of the data are too microscopic or on too slow of a time base to be intelligible. A few examples: a 1600 ms GCaMP convolution kernel for the imaging is long; how do we know that the correlations are valid without seeing what happens in this transformation? The windows of 'sensory' and 'motor' for the relation of complex spikes to one or the other category aren't clearly defined; since complex spikes often occur singly, it is hard to associate them with a sensory or motor event without some temporal criterion; and the swimming bouts are impossible to see on the time scales shown, and so how was a criterion defined to be associated with ‘motor’? Similar points arise for most figures. Likewise, panels are not always called out correctly, and descriptions in the text are often difficult to see reflected in the data that are illustrated.

We apologize that the regression analysis was not explained in sufficiently comprehensive detail. We wrongfully assumed that people would be familiar with this approach and we regret that this made the manuscript so difficult to follow.

We have significantly expanded the details in the Materials and methods section to better explain the analysis and parameter validation. The main text also offers a better introduction to the analysis and context for the choice of analysis. References have been added for these analyses as applied to other relevant datasets. We specifically mention caveats and other considerations for these analyses as a paragraph in the Materials and methods, including the discussion of interaction terms, variable selection methods, and elastic net optimization.

This expansion of the analysis explanation in the main text furthermore includes a revision of the figures such that Figure 1 is now an overview of the experiment and regression methodology. In particular, Figure 1D now shows an overview of the regression process, while Figure 1E shows an example calcium signal that is recapitulated by the summation of several regressors scaled by their coefficient weights. This latter panel also helps show how sensory and motor signals can be clearly separated.

We now provide the full set of regressors for both imaging (Figure 1—figure supplement 1) and electrophysiology (Figure 3—figure supplement 1) as these are slightly different (as explained in the Materials and methods). A new figure supplement (Figure 3—figure supplement 2) also further shows how visual and motor activity can be resolved with electrophysiological signals.

In the Materials and methods, we better explain our reasons for choosing the regressors and time windows. We are confident that the temporal time windows are reasonably chosen despite the relatively low complex spike rate because we clearly find complex responses associated with visual features including motion onset and luminance, for which a 500 ms bin at stimulus onset captures these responses very well. Swim bouts are on the order of 500 ms in duration, therefore this should be sufficiently long to capture a bout-related complex spike response, yet these are very rare despite looking for a relationship to the graded swim strength, swim duration, or the phase of spiking activity in relation to swim frequencies. We also look for bout-related responses preceding or following a bout, with another window, thereby providing comprehensive coverage around a bout for a motor-related complex spike to be found.

Finally, we have been careful to revise the figures and text throughout to ensure that all panels are sufficiently large to be readable, and that the text calls these panels correctly and in a way that is easy to follow.

2) Because the experiments only entail correlations within one modality, the conclusions are limited to providing additional support for attributes of cerebellar physiology for which there is already substantial accumulating evidence (e.g., complex spikes can relay sensory inputs, while granule motor-related information can come in through the granule cells; the notion of state-dependence in Figure 5 is interesting, but the presented result can simply be inferred from the idea that there is more parallel fiber drive during swimming, and so the illustration does not actually provide any further insight). In this context, attention was drawn by the reviewers to the fact that optogenetic tools that might be useful for manipulations are described in the Materials and methods but are not actually used in the paper.

Although our findings only pertain to the visual modality, we provide a comprehensive mapping of this modality across the entire Purkinje cell population. This has not been achieved in mammalian cerebellum for any modality. Furthermore, because of the nature of the visual stimuli we used and the variable eye and tail behaviors that are elicited, there are many components to the visual stimuli and motor responses. As a result, we are not limited to a simple first-order classification of complex spikes as sensory or motor (which many people have done previously in various contexts) but instead we can say which particular aspect of the sensory stimulus or motor response they relate to. For instance, we see graded complex spike responses to rotational velocity for some cells, and a transient response to stimulus onset for others. Our findings therefore show important qualitative and quantitative differences in the complex coding of visual features across regions of Purkinje cells that can now be used as a foundation for future manipulations (e.g. optogenetic experiments).

With respect to optogenetics in particular, it was our mistake to leave the mention of optogenetic tools in the Materials and methods. We have indeed developed some genetic tools for Purkinje cell optogenetics. However, the effect of channelrhodopsin in Purkinje cells is complex and many more controls are needed to understand and/or optimize these experiments. For example, due to the bistability of Purkinje cells (sensitivity to ionic balance), channelrhodopsin can easily lead to depolarization block of simple spikes in Purkinje cells rather than driving simple spikes, and the levels of power need to be carefully titrated for individual cells. We and no doubt others will follow up this work with experiments to probe manipulations of activity in these regions and with respect to behavior, however this work is beyond the scope of the current paper. We therefore decided, in consultation with the editors, not to add data but to instead clarify what we already have, as these findings already represent a large amount of data and analyses for the reader to take away from the current manuscript.

With regards to the comment that “the notion of state-dependence in Figure 5 is interesting, but the presented result can simply be inferred from the idea that there is more parallel fiber drive during swimming,” we would like to point out that these findings are not necessarily what is expected from the literature. Sengupta and Thirumalai, 2015, working in larval zebrafish, suggested that a spontaneous complex spike “toggles” the simple spike rate of a Purkinje cell, such that an “up state” (i.e. high simple spike rate) is ended by a complex spike and sent to the “down state,” and vice versa. We in fact do not see this effect during visuomotor behaviors, suggesting that the interaction of a complex spike with simple in a motor context has different network/plasticity effects as we discuss in the text.

Finally, in further support of the notion that the interplay between complex spikes and behavioral context is an important and relevant feature for understanding cerebellar function, we highlight in the discussion recent work from the Carey lab in mouse cerebellum showing that reflexive locomotor activity or a broad but mild optogenetic activation of granule cells enhances cerebellar learning.

3) Given that a single modality was studied, the conclusions are rather over-generalized without consideration of what the cerebellum might do in other behaviors and the final model offers little further insight., e.g., the claim in the discussion that complex spikes don't encode predictive motor signals. Nothing studied here actually tests anything about prediction. Here a relevant point might be the link to the associative learning study of Harmon et al., which found different patterns of complex and simple spike activity in a different set of behaviors, yet evidence for a topographic segregation into three classes is nevertheless still present. More experiments and/or more consideration of published work would be necessary to make the model sophisticated and flexible enough to be useful.

We regret that we over-generalized the conclusions from this singular modality. We have altered the text in several ways to better address these concerns.

Firstly, we now discuss other modalities including auditory, vestibular, olfactory and lateral line. In particular, we discuss evidence for cerebellar coding of these modalities from calcium imaging studies and we cite data showing that cerebellar-dependent associative learning with the auditory modality in unsuccessful at this stage (and even absent in the 6 week-old larva). This in particular suggests that the visual modality is more developed than audition at this stage. We also cite very recent findings from two studies published this month that show cerebellar activity strongly modulated by vestibular inputs, and relate this together with our findings for the visual coding of rotational motion to the known mammalian cerebellar-dependent vestibulo-ocular reflex.

Next, the findings of the Harmon et al. study using a different sensorimotor paradigm are now better utilized and integrated into our results and discussed in multiple contexts. Briefly, we make direct links to the similar regionality of Purkinje cell complex spike phenotypes in their associative learning paradigm. We also comment on the finding from their study that the central region of the cerebellar cortex appears to acquire novel complex spike signals during conditioning that complements the distinctively more flexible encoding of innate features in this region in our current study.

We have confidence that the visual organization of the cerebellum we observe is important. Although only the visual modality was tested, the degree to which nearly all Purkinje cells fit into these three observed complex spike visual phenotypes is striking. Nevertheless, in addition to the considerations above, we explicitly state that other modalities certainly need to be tested, to determine if/how they map onto these regions, and that the observed mapping may also change with development.

Additionally, we have rephrased the text discussing our interpretation of the meaning of these complex spikes. When we originally stated they were not predictive, we meant this in the sense that a complex spike occurring for a visual stimulus does not predict an upcoming episode of behavior in response to that stimulus. We have now clarified this in an expanded discussion that furthermore talks about the “error” and “novelty” hypotheses for complex spikes.

Reviewer #1:

[…]

Despite the many strengths of the current manuscript, some questions remain and require revisions or discussion in the paper:

1) The main experimental weakness is that they focused on correlations only, without doing experimental perturbations for causality. For future directions the authors might use laser ablations or optogenetic perturbations of these different sectors or functional cell types. Evidently, one strength of their zebrafish preparation is that they can actually control (almost) all the cells in a population of interest and look at causality by measuring motor output. Since the authors seemed to have generated the optogenetic tools for this project ca8 (PC:ChR2(H134R)-tagRFP), why was no data presented? Please explain why this section was included but none of the results.

We apologize for having mistakenly included this transgenic line in the Materials and methods section. As discussed above, this tool is not yet sufficiently optimized for manipulation experiments. Purkinje cells are a rather special type of neuron and although in the future we are planning experimental manipulations including ablations and optogenetic perturbations to probe the role of simple spikes and complex spikes in these behaviors, there is a considerable amount of groundwork to do before this is possible. The editors support our decision to instead focus on the current dataset. Although the data does not directly probe causality, it nonetheless provides a wealth of new knowledge and hypotheses for future work in the zebrafish cerebellum as well as insight into the general principles of feature encoding and organization that should extend to other species.

2) The authors could have done more to explain the 'voxel/multilinear regression' analysis method and it is confusing to call the imaging volumetric without explanation in the main text. In particular, it would help if they discussed the multilinear regression method in more detail: for instance, when sensory and motor variables are highly correlated (which they clearly are), how does the multilinear regression serve to separate out these effects, and does it take into account interaction terms? Also, it would be nice if they did some kind of internal validation of this technique, for instance fixing the stimulus (e.g., leftward motion) and examining how well the model predicted behavior, or at least explained why such a validation procedure was not used.

The term volumetric in the context of two-photon imaging is often used to refer to two-photon imaging at successive planes in a sample to yield a volume (Renninger and Orger, 2013; Portugues et al., 2014). In the Materials and methods under ‘Functional population imaging’ we nonetheless made this explicit by saying “We acquired the total cerebellar volume by sampling each plane at ~ 5 Hz. After all stimuli were shown in one plane, the focal plane was shifted ventrally by 1 μm and the process was repeated.” We now also mention these details in the main Results text for clarity. We moreover would like to note that this method (compared to e.g. confocal or light sheet microscopy) allows for very high, single-cell resolution. The additional of Figure 1—figure supplement 1 panels, showing an anatomical stack along with planewise regressor maps, and Video 2 showing the activity from a single two-photon plane, should also clarify this for readers.

The lack of details given the multilinear regression were a major issue for all reviewers. Please see the earlier Reviewing Editor comments, point 1, for our response to this issue.

Please also note that visual and motor features are not highly correlated – see additional text, Materials and methods, and figures including Figure 1E and Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

We do not include interaction terms in the multilinear regression model as the model without them had good fits and introducing these complicated additional terms can produce misleading results that would be very difficult to interpret. This is clarified in the Materials and methods.

In response to your last comment regarding an internal validation, we would like to say that we are not trying to predict behavior from any model and are simply using the regression to describe the encoding of stimulus and motor variables in the Purkinje cell activity. We hope that the expanded section on regression clarifies this.

3) It would be great if they had expanded on the broader view in the literature that CSs encode error. As is, the reader learns almost nothing about how they interpret these results.

The Discussion has been edited considerably and now contains a section titled ‘What signals are complex spikes encoding?’ in order to better explain our interpretation of the complex spike results in relation to several current hypotheses including the “error” hypothesis. We would furthermore like to mention that one of the strengths of our study is that we see consistent signals across experimental paradigms including when the fish is fully paralyzed, trunk-embedded with eyes freed, and fully eye and tail-freed. Thus, we are able for example to directly evaluate and reject the interpretation of complex spikes during rotational motion as an “error” signal caused by retinal slip.

4) Another limitation is that the authors focused only on one modality without discussing relevance to other modalities and motor output. It would be better if this were discussed in more detail in both the main text and the discussion. And, in the future. it would be great to see how the same set of PCs processes information from other modalities to guide behavior.

The discussion of other modalities was an important point raised by many reviewers and we agree that we are interested in exploring this in the future. Please see the earlier Reviewing Editor comments, point 3, for our response to this issue.

5) Given the presented data, the model in Figure 6 is somewhat disappointing. A cell-type specific model that would illustrate the predictions for perturbation experiments would be preferred.

We provided the schematic in this final figure as a summary figure rather than a predictive model. Since the data presented in this manuscript is dense, we thought that such a summary would be a helpful graphic overview of our findings. Without any results from perturbation experiments, a predictive model would be highly speculative.

6) Since much of the authors' interpretation is concerned with a difference in motor and sensory signals, a different analysis than these simplified motor regressor would be favored. It would be good to show continuous quantitative readout of behavior as a regressor. Perhaps some measure like the rectified ratio of left and right outputs of the ventral root traces (as in Dunn et al., 2016). Their relatively simple measures of behavior could bias the results to favor a sensory coding scheme.

A continuous quantitative readout of behavior (“vigor”) was already a motor regressor (as shown in updated Figure 1—figure supplement 1, Figure 3—figure supplement 1 and 2 and mentioned in the text and Materials and methods). We have greatly expanded the explanation of both the sensory and motor regressors and the analysis in the text, Materials and methods, and figures to ensure that this is clearer for the readers.

During functional imaging experiments, the tail movement is used to extract these motor features. For electrophysiology, it is the ventral root recording. We cannot provide the rectified ratio of left and right ventral root traces as suggested because we perform unilateral recordings from the left ventral root only. However, we find no rhythmicity in the Purkinje cell complex spikes or simple spikes that suggest a patterning of input in line with swimming frequencies (autocorrelation analyses, updated Figure 5F). We do however compute an integrated quantitative readout of swim strength from the single ventral root recording in order to obtain the readout of bilateral swim vigor. We furthermore are satisfied that this captures well the behavioral dynamics since the simple spike rates are highly correlated with this continuous vigor regressor.

7) It would be interesting to see a map as in Figure 2F for all recorded PC neurons by using the information from electrophysiologically recorded neurons.

The map in Figure 3F (previously Figure 2F) does indeed show the location of the 61 Purkinje cells recorded by electrophysiology.

Reviewer #2:

The work by Knogler and colleagues uses sophisticated microscopic and analytical methods to examine the functional organization of Purkinje cells in the larval zebrafish cerebellum. Contrary to the authors assertions (Results section first paragraph), this is not the first population level study of the cerebellum (Aizenberg and Schuman, 2011; Matsui et al., 2014). Also, the discovery of caudolateral Purkinje cells that are associated with eye movements and more rostromedial neurons associated with tail movements has been reported previously (Matsui et al., 2014).

We have toned down this assertion however we wish to emphasize that our work is not replicating the previous studies you cite.

1) The first reference, Aizenberg and Schuman, use in their study a bolus injection of organic calcium indicators to image local groups of mixed neuronal cell types including eurydendroid cells, Purkinje cells, and molecular layer interneurons. They themselves state that this is a mixed population. The use of the injection method and the fact that they furthermore only imaged five focal planes means that these imaging results are neither comprehensive nor exclusive across the Purkinje cell population. We nonetheless appreciate the insight from this work and cite this work in the current manuscript.

2) The imaging for the Matsui study was done on a confocal microscope with the pinhole maximally open, therefore the resolution was very low. Signals are also much weaker in the GCaMP5g transgenic line than in the GCaMP6s line both in general and specifically in Purkinje cells (personal observations and communications). Our imaging results therefore provide a large improvement in resolution that significantly expand and refine these regions. We also have an expanded set of visual stimuli that is significant for the novel insights we present in our current study regarding the overarching visual organization of the cerebellum.

Furthermore, without electrophysiological data to support the calcium imaging, no conclusions could be made from Matsui et al. regarding the origin of the calcium signals from simple spikes or complex spikes. We believe their findings represent a mix of sensory complex spike and motor simple spike contributions that we only now disambiguate with our new results.

Our findings in particular show not only new but also substantially different results from the study indicated:

1) We report that caudolateral Purkinje cell complex spikes are not associated with eye movements but instead sensory (visual) motion of the grating stimulus

2) We report that the simple spike activity of nearly all Purkinje cells are associated with tail movements while almost no complex spikes report motor information. It is telling that nowhere in the Matsui et al. paper is the term “complex spikes” or “simple spike” mentioned, therefore mammalian cerebellar researchers would likely incorrectly attribute these signals to complex spikes.

3) We report that rostromedial Purkinje cell complex spikes are associated with directional onset of sensory motion and that central Purkinje cells heterogeneously encode luminance.

Our ability to not only properly disambiguate between not only visual and motor responses, but also attribute a complex spike or simple spike origin to individual Purkinje cell signals, is of major importance for the interpretation of cerebellar function.

Regression and principal component analysis has been used to dissect relative sensory and motor contributions to responses, however the there is insufficient detail describing the methodology in the main text or Materials and methods section for non-computational neuroscientists to decipher what is going on. In Figure 1C, for example, the 'for motion' regressors have three peaks that align well with three of the largest z-score deviations, but many of the other deviations appear to be ignored. Why?

We apologize that insufficient details were provided initially to allow for a clear reading and interpretation of the results. Please see the earlier Reviewing Editor comments, point 1, for our response to this issue.

On a more specific note, the example mentioned for what was previously Figure 1C appears to be a misunderstanding. As we stated in the main text accompanying the reference to this figure panel, “Each different feature of the sensory stimulus or motor behavior, such as translational motion in a certain direction, or the duration of the swimming bouts across a trial, were used to build a vector of values for the trial duration.”. It is for precisely the reason that more than one regressor may contribute to the calcium activity that we use the complete set of all single sensory and motor regressors in the multilinear regression. The other deviations in these example calcium traces would therefore have nonzero coefficients for other regressors beyond the one that is shown. This is now made more explicit in revised Figure 1D,E and the text and Materials and methods.

Unfortunately, this acts to undermine much of the paper, which is further exacerbated by occasions where the authors either call out figure panels with statements that lack supporting evidence (see Results section, third paragraph and Figure 1D – reverse grating motion and luminance responses look identical to me; subsection “Purkinje cells in different regions receive climbing fiber input related to different, specific visual features and send outputs to different downstream regions” and Figure 2D,G – I'm not sure where to find the analysis of depression here; subsection “Motor activity is broadly represented in granule cell signals” and Figure 4I – not seeing any periodicity to speak of) or are too small to make any meaningful interpretation (see the same section and Figure 4K – not sure how you are supposed to see phase differences at this magnification).

The figure panels are now called more carefully. For example, the depression was not obvious from the example shown in Figure 4I as you mentioned, therefore it is now quantified in the results and does not refer to a figure panel. Our apologies that some panels were indeed too small; figures have been split up and/or adjusted to allow for all panels and text to be of adequate size.

From reading the manuscript, you would think that the zebrafish cerebellum is only involved in encoding visuomotor behaviors (subsections “Purkinje cells combine sensory and motor information from distinct inputs” and “Regional functionality of the zebrafish and mammalian cerebellum”). Auditory, tactile, thermosensory modalities are also highly salient sensory modalities, but are not explored here. Since these modalities were not tested, it is not clear how the pattern generalizes.

Please see the Reviewing Editor comments, point 3, for our response to this issue.

Briefly, we discuss these points in regards to other modalities and behavioral paradigms (including that of Harmon et al.) to better place these findings in context.

The authors admit themselves that patterns can change based on how the fish is stimulated (subsection “Motor activity is broadly represented in granule cell signals”).

We do not claim that these patterns change based on how the fish is stimulated – we do not see that the responses change. What this statement meant is that our 2017 study elicited swimming behavior so rarely (<10% probability of swimming during forward gratings, due to very low contrast settings, see Figure 4A in Knogler et al., 2017) that these current responses are not directly comparable because the previous study essentially represented a no-swimming paradigm (and shock-related “escapes” are very different).

It is also not clear how these data fit with recent descriptions of regionalization also based on complex spike activity in zebrafish (Harmon et al., 2017).

Please see the Reviewing Editor comments, point 3, for comments related to this issue.

We would also like to mention to this reviewer that in the first submission we did in fact cite this work several times and drew positive parallels between it and the current study. We are however using different paradigms to answer different questions – we are trying to understand the innate feature coding for visual stimuli and motor activity while Harmon et al. focused on conditioned responses that emerged over time.

We did however fail to adequately make use of the similarity in findings regarding the regionalization of Purkinje cell responses (albeit it to different features) in the original submission and in the revised manuscript we better highlight this work and the parallels that arise with respect to complex spike regionalization.

Overall, I wanted to like this paper, but the lack of work made for a rather frustrating read. The authors need to do a better job of placing their work in the context of previous discoveries, focus more on what is new here, and do a more thorough job of describing it.

We have significantly edited the text to address these concerns. We hope that this reviewer will find the new version much clearer and more insightful.

Reviewer #3:

[…]

1) The authors suggest that the Purkinje cell population receives a topographically organized input from climbing fibers stimulating complex firing localized to three different regions along the rostro-caudal axis of the Purkinje cell layer (e.g. in paragraph five of subsection “Purkinje cells in different regions receive climbing fiber input related to different, specific visual features and send outputs to different downstream regions”). Climbing fiber projections in zebrafish have been demonstrated but are only poorly characterized at last. I understand that the authors are working on this and a full neuroanatomical and physiological analysis of climbing fiber projection and connectivity will exceed the purpose and focus of this manuscript. Nevertheless, a few studies supporting the suggested topographic climbing fiber organization should be provided.

The work of Bae et al. (2009) is cited in regard to the conservation of climbing fiber properties in the Introduction. We have now explicitly stated that (like in mammals) all climbing fibers cross the midline after leaving the inferior olive and thus contact Purkinje cells in the contralateral hemisphere of the cerebellum (Takeuchi et al., 2015). We furthermore added the interesting finding that although only one bundle of ipsilateral climbing fibers in seen at larval stages, in the adult other fiber bundles are visible (Takeuchi et al., 2015), suggesting that other routes or types of information are added for communication between the inferior olive and cerebellar cortex at later stages. Finally, we cite the review of Apps and Hawkes (2009) for information about the hardwired topography of climbing fiber projections to Purkinje cells in the mammalian cerebellum.

2) In addition, it remained unclear to me whether climbing fibers synapse with Purkinje cell somata or along their dendritic trees or both. Is there a preferential location of PC-CF synapses?

Takeuchi et al. (2015) find in their study that climbing fibers project onto the somata or proximal dendrites of Purkinje cells in larvae and adult animals. This has now been mentioned in the text.

3) The authors state the zebrafish contain two different types of anatomically different Purkinje cells according to their projections that occur either internally to eurydendroid cells or externally to the vestibular nuclei. Does this model also include Purkinje cell collaterals projecting to Purkinje cells nearby or do the authors think that such PC-PC connectivities do not exist in zebrafish?

The experimental techniques in our current study did not provide insight into this particular neuroanatomical question. The finding that Purkinje cells that are located very closely to each other have similar complex spike and simple spike responses, however, would suggest that even if these PC-PC collaterals exist, they are not sufficient to provide mutual spike inhibition. We will not however discuss this in the text as this interpretation is speculative.

4) The authors performed whole cell recordings in the caudolateral region of the Purkinje cell layer during rotational windmill motion evoked optokinetic reflex. Were these Purkinje cells internally of externally projecting PCs?

The partial answer to this question can be found in Figure 4E, in the revised manuscript (previously Figure 3E) and in the results. Of 17 Purkinje cells for which we had complete morphology as well as single-cell electrophysiological recordings, 6/7 Purkinje cells with externally-projecting axons (referred to as “caudal axonal projection” in the figure panel) showed these responses to rotational windmill motion (and only 1/10 Purkinje cells with internal axons had this phenotype). In addition, there is indirect evidence to support this at the population level, based on the spatial overlap of Purkinje cells with externally-projecting axons and the complex spike phenotype for rotational windmill motion (compare Figure 2B, Figure 3F, Figure 4F, Figure 4—figure supplement 1H).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42138.022

    Data Availability Statement

    Example electrophysiological datasets are available at https://zenodo.org/record/1494071. An example imaging dataset is available at https://zenodo.org/record/1638807. MATLAB code for electrophysiological analysis available via GitHub (https://github.com/portugueslab/Knogler_etal_2019_eLife; copy archived athttps://github.com/elifesciences-publications/Knogler_etal_2019_eLife).


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES