Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Lancet Psychiatry. 2018 Sep 20;5(10):771–773. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30356-0

Shared genetic factors, fetal programming, and the transmission of depression

Claudia Lugo-Candelas 1, Catherine Monk 1, Cristiane S Duarte 1, Seonjoo Lee 1, Jonathan Posner 1
PMCID: PMC6374113  NIHMSID: NIHMS1010149  PMID: 30245186

Interest in prenatal exposures shaping later life has increased considerably in the past two decades. Set forth by Barker in the 1980s, the fetal programing hypothesis posits that in-utero exposures affect the early development of organs and tissues in ways that persist into adulthood.1 The study of fetal programming has extended to neurodevelopment, with a growing amount of scientific literature of human and animal studies suggesting that prenatal maternal depression places offspring at increased risk for mental disorders.2,3 Although the need to account for genetic and shared environmental confounding has been previously argued,4 the field has not consistently obliged. The study by Laurie Hannigan and colleagues5 will most certainly change that.

Hannigan and colleagues leverage an impressive cohort of twin (178 monozygotic and 104 dizygotic), sibling, and singleton mothers (n=21 913), and their offspring (n=35 229) to examine transmission mechanisms of familial depression, including shared genetic factors and exposure to prenatal and postnatal maternal depression. Using a children-of-twins study design and structural equation modelling, the authors test mechanisms of transmission accounting for associations between self-reported maternal prenatal depression (weeks 17 and 30) and offspring internalising and externalising problems (maternal report at 18, 36, and 60 months, when maternal depression was also assessed). Shared genetic factors accounted for the largest portion of offspring internalising (41%, 95% CI 36–46) and externalising (37%, 30–44) problems. Postnatal exposure to maternal depression had a smaller, but significant role for internalising disorders. Surprisingly, no support was found for fetal programming effects for either internalising or externalising disorders. This study reinforces the importance of shared genetic influences, providing a cautionary note with respect to the recent enthusiasm that the fetal programming hypothesis has garnered. In addition to its sample size, the study findings based on prospectively collected data allow the authors to make a compelling argument in support of genetic transmission. Also worth commending is the authors’ consideration of the clinical implications of their work: even if intergenerational transmission is largely genetic, there are still substantial benefits to the mother and offspring in reducing exposure to maternal postnatal depression.

Despite the study’s significant strengths, methodological limitations should be considered in the interpretation of the results. The field could also benefit from contextualising study findings within a broader theoretical understanding of the original fetal programming hypothesis.

Whereas the study sample is no doubt large, the levels of depressive symptoms are very low. Many mothers—quantification of data is difficult to ascertain but data were noted to be “excessively skewed”5 and transformed mean was 0—report an absence of symptoms, and there is significant attrition of the most depressed mothers. This finding raises two concerns. First, low scores in mother and child will inherently correlate to each other which, given the assumptions underlying the study design, as well as potential differential selection or attrition of monozygotic versus dizygotic twins, risks overestimating the impact of genetic factors. Second, fetal programming might arise primarily in the context of more severe depression: the putative biochemical or hormonal inutero exposures proposed to underlie fetal programming might occur only in mothers with considerable distress.

It also is worthwhile to review how fetal programming is being conceptualised and tested. As proposed by Barker,1 in response to prenatal exposures, the fetus (and placenta) makes iterative adaptations, preparing the fetus for the postnatal environment and fostering survival and procreation. Thus, broad assessments of psychopathology starting as late as at 18 months might not capture subtle, evolving developmental divergences that might only loosely correspond to internalising or externalising problems. For example, 5-week-old infants of prenatally depressed mothers show alterations in brain structure and function that suggest accelerated maturation of limbic substrates.6 In this example, the hypothesised adaptation could serve to prepare the child to monitor effectively a less responsive postnatal environment but would not necessarily translate to frank behavioural disturbances at 18 months.7 Adaptations are probably not static but instead interact with both genes and subsequent environmental exposures, requiring longitudinal, in-depth evaluation of risk trajectories. Finally, Hannigan and colleagues did not examine offspring sex differences. Extensive literature documents sex differences in risk for childhood disorders, and more specifically, in fetal programming.810 It is thus possible that by examining male and female offspring together, some effects went undetected.11,12

In summary, Hannigan and colleagues’ work marks an important moment in developmental neuroscience research, reinforcing the need for rigour in accounting for genetic transmission when investigating fetal programming hypotheses. However, a coda is necessary: discarding the possibility of fetal programming effects in future work is not yet warranted. Polarising opinions (eg, nature vs nurture) are unlikely to do justice to the complexity of human development and historically have not served our field well.

Acknowledgments

JP receives research support from Shire Pharmaceuticals and Aevi Genomic Medicine. The other authors declare no competing interests.

References

  • 1.Barker DJ. The Wellcome Foundation Lecture, 1994. The fetal origins of adult disease. Proc R Soc Lond B 1995; 262: 37–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kim DR, Bale TL, Epperson CN. Prenatal programming of mental illness: current understanding of relationship and mechanisms. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2015; 17: 5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.O’Donnell KJ, Glover V, Barker ED, O’Connor TG. The persisting effect of maternal mood in pregnancy on childhood psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 2014; 26: 393–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Babineau V, Green CG, Jolicoeur-Martineau A, et al. Prenatal depression and 5-HTTLPR interact to predict dysregulation from 3 to 36 months—a differential susceptibility model. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2015; 56: 21–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hannigan LJ, Eilertsen EM, Gjerde LC, et al. Maternal prenatal depressive symptoms and risk for early-life psychopathology in offspring: genetic analyses in the Norwegian Mother and Child Birth Cohort Study. Lancet Psychiatry 2018; published online Sept 20. 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30225-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Posner J, Cha J, Roy AK, et al. Alterations in amygdala-prefrontal circuits in infants exposed to prenatal maternal depression. Transl Psychiatry 2016; 6: e935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Granat A, Gadassi R, Gilboa-Schechtman E, Feldman R. Maternal depression and anxiety, social synchrony, and infant regulation of negative and positive emotions. Emotion 2017; 17: 11–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Dean DC Iii, Planalp EM, Wooten W, et al. Association of prenatal maternal depression and anxiety symptoms with infant white matter microstructure. JAMA Pediatrics 2018; published online Aug 27. DOI: 10.1001/jamapediatrics20182132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Glover V, Hill J. Sex differences in the programming effects of prenatal stress on psychopathology and stress responses: an evolutionary perspective. Physiol Behav 2012; 106: 736–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bale TL. The placenta and neurodevelopment: sex differences in prenatal vulnerability. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2016; 18: 459–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gilman SE, Cherkerzian S, Buka SL, Hahn J, Hornig M, Goldstein JM. Prenatal immune programming of the sex-dependent risk for major depression. Transl Psychiatry 2016; 6: e822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Braithwaite EC, Hill J, Pickles A, Glover V, O’Donnell K, Sharp H. Associations between maternal prenatal cortisol and fetal growth are specific to infant sex: findings from the Wirral Child Health and Development Study. J Dev Orig Health Dis 2018; 9: 425–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES