Abstract
Objective
Psychotic experiences (PE) are dimensional phenomena in the general population that resemble psychotic symptoms, such as paranoia and hallucinations. This is the first twin study to explore the degree to which tobacco use and PE share genetic or environmental influences. Previous studies on the association between adolescent tobacco use and PE have not considered PE dimensionally, included negative symptoms, or accounted for confounding by sleep disturbance and stressful life events.
Method
An unselected adolescent twin sample (N = 3,787 pairs; mean age = 16.16 years) reported on PE (paranoia, hallucinations, cognitive disorganization, grandiosity, and anhedonia) and regularity of tobacco use. Parents rated the twins’ negative symptoms. Regression analyses were conducted while adjusted for sociodemographic characteristics, prenatal maternal smoking, cannabis use, sleep disturbance, and stressful life events. Bivariate twin modeling was used to estimate the degree of genetic and common and unique environmental influences shared between tobacco use and PE.
Results
Regular smokers were significantly more likely to experience paranoia, hallucinations, cognitive disorganization, and negative symptoms (β = 0.17−0.34), but not grandiosity or anhedonia, than nonsmokers, after adjustment for confounders. Paranoia, hallucinations, and cognitive disorganization correlated ≥0.15 with tobacco use (r = 0.15−0.21, all p < .001). Significant genetic correlations (rA=0.37−0.45) were found. Genetic influences accounted for most of the association between tobacco use and paranoia (84%) and cognitive disorganization (81%). Familial influences accounted for 80% of the association between tobacco use and hallucinations.
Conclusion
Tobacco use and PE during adolescence were associated after adjustment for confounders. They appear to co-occur largely because of shared genetic influences.
Key words: psychotic-like experiences, youth, smoking, cigarette, heritability
Tobacco use, a modifiable behavior, is associated with psychotic experiences (PE).1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9 PE are common dimensional phenomena in the general population that range in severity and resemble psychotic symptoms at the extreme, such as paranoia and hallucinations. Those who experience PE, especially if they persist, are at higher risk for developing psychotic and other psychiatric disorders.10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15 Longitudinal evidence that adolescent tobacco use precedes PE suggests a possible role of smoking in the development of PE.3, 8 In the United Kingdom, 35% of 15-year-olds have smoked cigarettes at least once,16 and in the United States, 21% of 15- to 17-year-olds,17 with 3% to 5.5% smoking daily.18 Understanding the nature of the association between adolescent PE and tobacco use is therefore important. There is a lack of evidence regarding whether PE and tobacco use share genetic or environmental influences. Furthermore, although previous studies considered confounders such as cannabis use and sociodemographic characteristics on the association between PE and smoking, factors such as sleep disturbance and stressful life events have been ignored.
Previous studies on tobacco use and PE, mostly conducted on adults,1, 2, 4, 6, 7 found that regular tobacco use increased the odds of experiencing PE by 20% to 47% after accounting for sociodemographic characteristics, cannabis use, and comorbid mental and physical disorders. These studies measured only positive PE, such as hallucinations and delusions, defined categorically. In the studies that assessed age of smoking initiation, individuals who started smoking during or prior to adolescence were more likely to report PE as adults.2, 6
Other factors associated with PE during adolescence include cannabis use,9, 19, 20, 21 stressful life events,22, 23 illicit drug use,3, 14 sleep disturbance,24, 25 and prenatal maternal smoking.26 Because these factors may also be associated with tobacco use, it is sensible to investigate the contribution of tobacco use while accounting for these factors, some of which were not considered previously.
Although tobacco might be considered an environmental exposure, twin studies estimate significant heritability of tobacco use during adolescence (36%−60%).27, 28, 29, 30, 31 It is therefore important to consider both genetic and environmental influences when investigating the association between adolescent PE and tobacco use. Twin studies on PE indicate genetic and unique environmental influences as important; common environmental influences are small or negligible.32
We aimed to investigate whether associations between tobacco use and PE exist in adolescence and whether they remain after adjusting for sex, age, ethnicity, socio-economic status, cannabis use, stressful life events, sleep disturbance, and prenatal maternal smoking. Our second aim was to test whether shared genetic and environmental influences underlie adolescent PE and tobacco smoking. We consider a range of PE including paranoia, hallucinations, cognitive disorganization, grandiosity, anhedonia, and parent-rated negative symptoms (such as flattened affect and low motivation), assessed as dimensional traits.
Method
Sample
Research participants came from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), a UK-based community sample of twins born between 1994 and 199633 who, along with their parents, participated in the Longitudinal Experiences and Perceptions Study (LEAP)34 at age 16 years. Of the 8,534 families invited to report on PE and substance use, 3,941 (46.2%) participated. Table S1, available online, compares participating and nonparticipating families.
For regression analyses, one twin per family was chosen randomly to select a sample of unrelated individuals. After exclusions (missing zygosity data, no consent information, severe medical conditions, or perinatal complications), the sample included 3,787 individuals.
For twin analyses, zygosity was determined using a parent-rated measure and confirmed using DNA testing in ambivalent cases. The sample, after exclusions, consisted of 1,342 monozygotic (MZ; 43.8% male) and 1,219 same-sex dizygotic (DZ) pairs (44.5% male). Opposite-sex DZ pairs (1,208) were not included because of limited power in bivariate twin models to test for qualitative sex differences in categorical data.
The Institute of Psychiatry Ethics Committee, King’s College London, granted ethical approval for TEDS. All research participants granted informed consent. The Birkbeck Department of Psychological Sciences’ Ethics Committee and the core TEDS team approved this study.
Measures
Psychotic Experiences
The Specific Psychotic Experiences Questionnaire (SPEQ)34 included three subscales measuring “positive” PE (paranoia, hallucinations, and grandiosity), two measuring “negative” PE (parent-rated negative symptoms and anhedonia), and one cognitive disorganization subscale. Subscales consisted of eight to 15 items and asked about frequency or severity of recent PE. Subscales showed good-to-excellent internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.77–0.93) and test–retest reliability over 9 months (r = 0.65–0.74). Negative symptoms were parent-rated; other subscales were self-rated.
Untransformed SPEQ scores were used in descriptive statistics. Subscales with a positive skew (paranoia, hallucinations, grandiosity, and parent-rated negative symptoms) were square root transformed. PE were standardized to have a mean of 0 and a standard deviation (SD) of 1.
Tobacco Use
A tobacco use variable was created with three levels. Nonsmokers were those who answered “no” to the question “Have you ever smoked a cigarette (including roll-ups)?” Occasional and regular smokers were defined based on the item “How many cigarettes have you smoked, in total, in your lifetime?” Occasional smokers were those who had smoked <50 cigarettes and regular smokers ≥50. Those who had not smoked in the past year were considered nonsmokers. The threshold of 50 cigarettes (rather than a higher threshold) was chosen because adolescents have had less access to resources and opportunity to smoke than adults. For smoking frequency by group, see Table S2, available online.
Confounding Variables
Variables adjusted for during multiple regression included sex, age, ethnicity, socioeconomic status (SES), prenatal maternal smoking, and self-rated measures (at age 16 years) of cannabis use, sleep disturbance measured by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI),35 and stressful life events (SLE) measured by an abbreviated Coddington Life Events Record.36
Cannabis use was determined from “yes” responses to “Have you ever tried cannabis?” Those who indicated no use during the past year were considered non−cannabis-users. SES was a standardized score derived from parent qualifications and employment and mother’s age at birth of her first child (ascertained at first contact when the twins were aged 18 months). Prenatal maternal smoking was determined at first contact from “yes” responses to “Did you smoke cigarettes while pregnant?” SLE scores were square root transformed to reduce skewness. Variables were standardized for regression analyses.
Data Analyses
Differences among nonsmokers, occasional smokers, and regular smokers on SPEQ scores and covariates were tested using one-way analyses of variance (for continuous variables) and χ2 tests (categorical variables). Pairwise comparisons were performed using Bonferroni-corrected p values.
Regression Analyses
Regression analyses were conducted using the lm() function in R.37 PE were the outcome variables and tobacco use the exposure. Missing data were omitted on a pairwise basis. Linear regression between PE and tobacco use was run to estimate unadjusted models. In adjusted models, predictor variables were entered simultaneously. The presence of multicollinearity was assessed by computing variance inflation factors (VIF), where VIF > 4 indicates multicollinearity. Significance thresholds were set at p < .05 and adjusted R2 values reported to account for the number of predictors in models.
Structural Equation Twin Modeling
The twin design enables individual differences to be decomposed into genetic and environmental components. If a trait is heritable, MZ within-pair similarity is higher than DZ similarity. Shared environmental influences make children growing up in the same family similar as indexed by DZ within-pair similarity being greater than half MZ within-pair similarity. Nonshared environmental influences make children growing up in the same family different, present where MZ within-pair similarity is less than unity.
Structural equation twin modeling was conducted in OpenMx 2.038 for R37. The effects of sex and age were regressed out of SPEQ scales after normalization. Twin models were fitted if the correlation between PE and tobacco use was >0.15 to allow for enough covariance to be decomposed into genetic and environmental influences. Liability-threshold models, which assume an underlying normal distribution to ordinal data, were fitted for univariate tobacco use models. For bivariate analyses, joint ordinal−continuous twin models were fitted that modeled liability to tobacco use and variation in PE.
Twin correlations were calculated using intraclass correlations. Saturated models, constraining means, thresholds, and phenotypic correlations across twin order, were run between PE and tobacco use to calculate cross-twin cross-trait (CTCT) and phenotypic polyserial correlations. Saturated models provide a full description of the data prior to decomposing variance/covariance into genetic (A), common environmental (C), and unique environmental (E) influences. ACE models were compared to saturated models. Subsequently, statistical significance of the variance components was tested by systematically fixing each to zero in submodels (while retaining E because it contains residual error) and comparing to ACE models. The most parsimonious model is preferred and was identified if a likelihood-ratio test at p < .05 indicated a not significantly worse fit compared to full models and based on the lowest Akaike Information Criterion (AIC).
The extent to which the same genes or environments influence PE and tobacco use was estimated from the genetic correlations (rA), shared environmental correlations (rC), and unique environmental correlations (rE). The bivariate heritability (biva2) and equivalent bivariate values for shared (bivc2) and unique environments (bive2) provided estimates of the degree of covariation between tobacco use and PE explained by A, C, or E. These values were divided by the phenotypic correlations to calculate the proportion of the covariance explained by genetic or environmental influences.
Results
In the current sample (N = 3,787; 45.9% male; mean 16.16 years, SD = 0.68), 31.4% reported ever having smoked cigarettes. Of the 3,610 adolescents who provided information on the regularity of their tobacco use in the past year, 2,985 (82.7%) were nonsmokers, 436 (12.1%) occasional smokers, and 189 (5.2%) regular smokers.
The PE scores were significantly different across tobacco use groups (Table 1). Pairwise comparisons indicated that occasional smokers scored significantly higher than nonsmokers on paranoia (p < .001), hallucinations (p = .002), and cognitive disorganization (p < .001) and lower on anhedonia (p = .023), with no significant differences on grandiosity (p = 1.00) and parent-rated negative symptoms (p = .078). Compared to nonsmokers, regular smokers scored significantly higher on paranoia (p < .001), hallucinations (p < .001), cognitive disorganization (p < .001), grandiosity (p = .002), and negative symptoms (p < .001), but not on anhedonia (p = .061). Compared to occasional smokers, regular smokers scored significantly higher on all six scales including paranoia (p = .006), hallucinations (p < .001), cognitive disorganization (p < .001), grandiosity (p = .018), anhedonia (p = .001), and negative symptoms (p < .001).
Table 1.
Characteristic | Range | Total Sample |
Nonsmokers |
Occasional Smokers |
Regular Smokers |
F | p | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | Mean (SD) | n | Mean (SD) | n | Mean (SD) | n | Mean (SD) | ||||
Psychotic experiences (SPEQ) | |||||||||||
Paranoia | 0–75 | 3,603 | 11.70 (10.54) | 2,978 | 11.12 (10.31) | 436 | 13.43 (10.56) | 189 | 16.25 (12.81) | 30.33 | <.001 |
Hallucinations | 0–45 | 3,607 | 4.56 (6.07) | 2,983 | 4.28 (5.81) | 436 | 5.33 (6.62) | 188 | 7.35 (8.16) | 25.61 | <.001 |
Cognitive disorganization | 0–11 | 3,606 | 3.90 (2.83) | 2,982 | 3.72 (2.79) | 436 | 4.27 (2.75) | 188 | 5.64 (2.95) | 46.86 | <.001 |
Grandiosity | 0–24 | 3,606 | 5.41 (4.52) | 2,981 | 5.34 (4.47) | 436 | 5.39 (4.28) | 189 | 6.47 (5.32) | 4.52 | .011 |
Anhedonia | 0–50 | 3,604 | 16.31 (7.79) | 2,979 | 16.36 (7.84) | 436 | 15.29 (7.14) | 189 | 17.71 (8.58) | 6.86 | .001 |
Negative symptoms | 0–30 | 3,583 | 2.86 (3.88) | 2,965 | 2.74 (3.69) | 431 | 2.52 (3.64) | 187 | 4.57 (5.26) | 19.49 | <.001 |
Covariates (continuous) | |||||||||||
Age | 14.9–18.7 | 3,610 | 16.16 (.68) | 2,985 | 16.12 (.68) | 436 | 16.35 (.65) | 189 | 16.38 (.62) | 33.44 | <.001 |
SES | –2.6 to 2.6 | 3,430 | 0.25 (.98) | 2,840 | 0.26 (.97) | 412 | .36 (1.02) | 178 | −0.07 (1.01) | 12.06 | <.001 |
Sleep disruption | 0–21 | 3,603 | 5.48 (2.69) | 2,980 | 5.32 (2.64) | 434 | 5.92 (2.79) | 189 | 6.72 (3.04) | 31.52 | <.001 |
Stressful life events | 0–20 | 3,236 | 2.31 (1.74) | 2,672 | 2.12 (1.61) | 392 | 2.79 (1.72) | 172 | 3.92 (2.42) | 79.98 | <.001 |
Covariates (categorical) | n | n | % | n | % | n | % | χ2 | p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sex | 0.07 | .967 | |||||||||
Males | 1,660 | 1,370 | 82.53% | 203 | 12.23% | 87 | 5.24% | ||||
Females | 1,950 | 1,615 | 82.82% | 233 | 11.95% | 102 | 5.23% | ||||
Ethnicity | 8.31 | .016 | |||||||||
White | 3,376 | 2,778 | 82.29% | 420 | 12.44% | 178 | 5.27% | ||||
Other | 225 | 201 | 89.33% | 14 | 6.22% | 10 | 4.44% | ||||
Cannabis use | 1099.90 | <.001 | |||||||||
No | 3,159 | 2,666 | 84.39% | 372 | 11.78% | 121 | 3.83% | ||||
Yes | 438 | 307 | 70.09% | 63 | 14.38% | 68 | 15.53% | ||||
Maternal smoking during pregnancy | 111.81 | <.001 | |||||||||
No | 3,159 | 2,666 | 84.39% | 372 | 11.78% | 121 | 3.83% | ||||
Yes | 438 | 307 | 70.09% | 63 | 14.38% | 68 | 15.53% |
Note: SPEQ = Specific Psychotic Experiences Questionnaire.
Regression Models
The VIF for all predictors ranged between 1.07 and 1.51, which indicated no multicollinearity between predictor variables (see Table S3, available online, for correlations between variables). Unadjusted models (Table 2) indicated that regular smoking, compared to not smoking, significantly (p < .05) predicted higher scores on all PE subscales. Occasional smoking, compared to not smoking, significantly predicted higher scores for paranoia, hallucinations, and cognitive disorganization and lower scores for anhedonia, and did not predict grandiosity and parent-rated negative symptoms. Models accounted for 0.3% to 2.5% of variance in PE scores, most for cognitive disorganization, paranoia, and hallucinations.
Table 2.
Unadjusted Model |
Adjusted Model |
Unadjusted Model |
Adjusted Model |
||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
R2 | β | 95% CI | R2 | β | 95% CI | R2 | β | 95% CI | R2 | β | 95% CI | ||
Paranoia | .017 | .160 | Grandiosity | .003 | .046 | ||||||||
Tobacco use | Tobacco use | ||||||||||||
Occasional smokers | 0.27 | 0.17–0.37 | 0.13 | 0.02–0.23 | Occasional smokers | 0.05 | –0.05–0.15 | 0.05 | –0.07–0.17 | ||||
Regular smokers | 0.46 | 0.31–0.60 | 0.17 | 0.00–0.34 | Regular smokers | 0.22 | 0.07–0.37 | 0.17 | –0.01–0.35 | ||||
Sex | –0.04 | –0.11–0.02 | Sex | 0.26 | 0.19–0.33 | ||||||||
Age | –0.08 | –0.13 to –0.03 | Age | –0.02 | –0.08–0.03 | ||||||||
Ethnicity | –0.07 | –0.21–0.07 | Ethnicity | 0.36 | 0.21–0.51 | ||||||||
Cannabis use | 0.10 | –0.04–0.24 | Cannabis use | –0.04 | –0.19–0.12 | ||||||||
Sleep disturbance | 0.34 | 0.31–0.38 | Sleep disturbance | –0.02 | –0.05–0.02 | ||||||||
Significant life events | 0.10 | 0.07–0.14 | Significant life events | 0.15 | 0.12–0.19 | ||||||||
Socio-economic status | 0.08 | 0.05–0.12 | Socio-economic status | 0.01 | –0.02–0.05 | ||||||||
Prenatal maternal smoking | 0.01 | –0.10–0.11 | Prenatal maternal smoking | 0.00 | –0.12–0.11 | ||||||||
Hallucinations | 0.014 | .145 | Anhedonia | .004 | .083 | ||||||||
Tobacco use | Tobacco use | ||||||||||||
Occasional smokers | 0.19 | 0.09–0.29 | 0.07 | –0.04–0.18 | Occasional smokers | –0.14 | –0.24 to –0.04 | –0.12 | –0.23 to –0.01 | ||||
Regular smokers | 0.48 | 0.33–0.62 | 0.24 | 0.06–0.41 | Regular smokers | 0.17 | 0.03–0.32 | 0.16 | –0.02–0.34 | ||||
Sex | –0.10 | –0.16 to –0.03 | Sex | 0.49 | 0.42–0.56 | ||||||||
Age | –0.09 | –0.14 to –0.04 | Age | –0.01 | –0.06–0.04 | ||||||||
Ethnicity | 0.11 | –0.03–0.26 | Ethnicity | 0.00 | –0.15–0.15 | ||||||||
Cannabis use | 0.11 | –0.04–0.25 | Cannabis use | 0.01 | –0.14–0.16 | ||||||||
Sleep disturbance | 0.32 | 0.28–0.35 | Sleep disturbance | 0.14 | 0.11–0.18 | ||||||||
Significant life events | 0.12 | 0.08–0.15 | Significant life events | –0.11 | –0.15 to –0.07 | ||||||||
Socio-economic status | –0.03 | –0.07–0.00 | Socio-economic status | 0.02 | –0.02–0.06 | ||||||||
Prenatal maternal smoking | –0.03 | –0.14–0.07 | Prenatal maternal smoking | 0.01 | –0.11–0.12 | ||||||||
Cognitive disorganization | 0.025 | 0.226 | Negative symptomsa | .011 | .052 | ||||||||
Tobacco use | Tobacco use | ||||||||||||
Occasional smokers | 0.20 | 0.10–0.30 | 0.09 | –0.02–0.19 | Occasional smokers | –0.06 | –0.16–0.04 | –0.06 | –0.18–0.05 | ||||
Regular smokers | 0.68 | 0.54–0.83 | 0.34 | 0.17–0.50 | Regular smokers | 0.45 | 0.30–0.59 | 0.23 | 0.04–0.41 | ||||
Sex | –0.30 | –0.37 to –0.24 | Sex | 0.19 | 0.12–0.26 | ||||||||
Age | –0.05 | –10 to –0.00 | Age | –0.10 | –0.15 to –0.04 | ||||||||
Ethnicity | 0.02 | –0.11–0.16 | Ethnicity | 0.06 | –0.09–0.21 | ||||||||
Cannabis use | 0.06 | –0.08–0.20 | Cannabis use | 0.05 | –0.10–0.21 | ||||||||
Sleep disturbance | 0.40 | 0.36–0.43 | Sleep disturbance | 0.14 | 0.10–0.18 | ||||||||
Significant life events | 0.06 | 0.02–0.09 | Significant life events | –0.02 | –0.06–0.02 | ||||||||
Socio-economic status | –0.08 | –0.11 to –0.04 | Socio-economic status | –0.13 | –0.17 to –0.09 | ||||||||
Prenatal maternal smoking | 0.03 | –0.08–0.13 | Prenatal maternal smoking | 0.13 | 0.01–0.24 |
Note: Significance at p < .05 is shown in boldface type. Adjusted R2 reported for adjusted models. Reference groups: for tobacco use, non-smokers; sex, female; for ethnicity, white; for cannabis use, no; for prenatal maternal smoking, no. Unadjusted model results: paranoia (F2,3600 = 30.33, p < .001), hallucinations (F2,3604 = 25.61, p < .001), cognitive disorganization (F2,3603 = 46.86, p < .001), grandiosity (F2,3602 = 4.52, p = .011), anhedonia (F2,3601 = 6.86, p = .001) and parent-rated negative symptoms (F2,3580 = 19.49, p < .001). Adjusted model results: paranoia (F10,2985 = 57.88, p < .001), hallucinations (F10,2989 = 51.86, p < .001), cognitive disorganization (F10,2989 = 88.65, p < .001), grandiosity (F10,2989 = 15.56, p < .001), anhedonia (F10,2986 = 28.02, p < .001), and parent-rated negative symptoms (F10,2970 = 17.30, p < .001).
Parent-rated.
Adjusted models accounted for 4.6% to 22.6% of variance in PEs, the highest being for cognitive disorganization (22.6%), paranoia (16%), and hallucinations (14.5%). Standardized coefficients indicated that paranoia increased by 0.13 and 0.17 SD in occasional and regular smokers, respectively, compared to nonsmokers. Models for hallucinations, cognitive disorganization, and parent-rated negative symptoms indicated an increase of 0.24, 0.34, and 0.23 SD, respectively, in regular smokers compared to nonsmokers; occasional smoking did not significantly predict these PE. Regular tobacco use did not significantly predict grandiosity in adjusted models. Anhedonia scores decreased on average by 0.12 SD in occasional smokers compared to nonsmokers. Compared to unadjusted models, standardized coefficients for tobacco use generally decreased in adjusted models. Sensitivity analyses using generalized estimating equation models to include both twins did not affect our conclusions (see Table S4, available online).
Genetic and Environmental Influences on Psychotic Experiences and Tobacco Use
Phenotypic correlations (Table 3) were sufficiently large (>0.15) to run bivariate models between tobacco use and paranoia, hallucinations, and cognitive disorganization.
Table 3.
Tobacco | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Phenotypic correlations | r | CI | ||
Paranoia | 0.19 | 0.15–0.24 | ||
Hallucinations | 0.15 | 0.11–0.20 | ||
Cognitive disorganization | 0.21 | 0.16–0.25 | ||
Grandiosity and delusions | 0.01 | –0.03–0.05 | ||
Negative symptomsa | 0.10 | 0.06–0.15 | ||
Anhedonia | 0.05 | 0.01–0.09 | ||
MZ | DZ | |||
Twin correlations | r | CI | R | CI |
Paranoia | 0.53 | 0.49–0.56 | 0.30 | 0.25–0.35 |
Hallucinations | 0.43 | 0.38–0.47 | 0.29 | 0.24–0.34 |
Cognitive disorganization | 0.46 | 0.41–0.50 | 0.22 | 0.17–0.28 |
Tobacco | 0.82 | 0.76–0.87 | 0.68 | 0.60–0.75 |
Cross-twin cross-trait correlations (psychotic experiences and tobacco) | ||||
Paranoia | 0.15 | 0.10–0.20 | 0.12 | 0.06–0.17 |
Hallucinations | 0.11 | 0.06–0.16 | 0.08 | 0.03–0.14 |
Cognitive disorganization | 0.18 | 0.13–0.22 | 0.11 | 0.05–0.16 |
Note: DZ = dizygotic twins; MZ = monozygotic twins.
Parent-rated.
Univariate MZ twin correlations (Table 3) were higher than DZ correlations for tobacco use, paranoia, hallucinations, and cognitive disorganization, which implied genetic influences (A). Common environmental influences (C) were indicated for hallucinations and tobacco use, and somewhat for paranoia, because DZ correlations were greater than half the MZ correlations, but not for cognitive disorganization. Unique environmental influences (E) were indicated for all measures because MZ correlations were <1.
A full ACE liability-threshold model was indicated for tobacco use (see Table S5, available online) with A = 0.32 (CI = 0.17−0.49), C = 0.51 (0.36−0.64), and E = 0.17 (0.13−0.29). A third of the variance in tobacco use was explained by genetic influences and half due to common environment. Univariate models for SPEQ subscales have been published previously from the TEDS sample21 showing genetic (A = 0.27−0.54) and nonshared environmental (E = 0.12−0.50) influences explain most variation in PE.
Cross-twin cross-trait (CTCT) correlations (Table 3) were higher in MZ than in DZ pairs, indicating A on the covariance between PE and tobacco use. The DZ CTCT correlations were greater than half those of MZ correlations for paranoia and hallucinations (but not for cognitive disorganization), implicating C influences on covariation. Some E influences on the covariation between tobacco use with paranoia, hallucinations, and cognitive disorganization were suggested, as MZ CTCT correlations were lower than phenotypic correlations.
Fit statistics for bivariate models (Table 4) indicated that, compared to saturated models, ACE models did not fit significantly worse. Partial AE models, in which C parameters were dropped for PE and for covariance paths between tobacco and PE (C was retained for tobacco because univariate results indicated that C influences tobacco use), were compared to the full ACE models. Partial AE models did not fit significantly worse than ACE models for tobacco use with both paranoia and cognitive disorganization. Partial AE models dropping genetic correlations had significantly worse fits compared to ACE models.
Table 4.
Model | Base | Model Fit |
Bivariate Statistics From Most Parsimonious Models (95% CI) |
||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
EP | −2LL | df | AIC | Δ−2LL | Δ df | p | Biva2 | Bivc2 | Bive2 | rA | rC | rE | |||
Paranoia | Saturated | – | 13 | 18583.63 | 9990 | –1396.37 | – | – | – | ||||||
ACE | Sat | 12 | 18585.27 | 9993 | –1400.73 | 1.64 | 3 | .650 | |||||||
AE (retained C for tobacco)a | ACE | 10 | 18588.47 | 9995 | –1401.53 | 3.20 | 2 | .202 | 0.16 (0.11–0.21) | – | 0.03 (–0.01–0.06) | 0.37 (0.25–0.53) | – | 0.11 (–0.01–0.21) | |
ACE dropped rA | ACE | 11 | 18587.60 | 9994 | –1400.40 | 2.33 | 1 | .127 | |||||||
ACE dropped rC | ACE | 11 | 18587.21 | 9994 | –1400.79 | 1.94 | 1 | .164 | |||||||
ACE dropped rA and rC | ACE | 10 | 18626.77 | 9995 | –1363.23 | 41.50 | 2 | <.001 | |||||||
AE dropped rA | ACE | 9 | 18628.58 | 9996 | –1363.42 | 43.31 | 3 | <.001 | |||||||
Hallucinations | Saturated | – | 13 | 18696.66 | 9960 | –1223.34 | – | – | – | ||||||
ACEa | Sat | 12 | 18698.42 | 9963 | –1227.58 | 1.76 | 3 | .624 | 0.07 (–0.05–0.19) | 0.05 (–0.05–0.15) | 0.04 (0.01–0.07) | 0.25 (–0.17–0.67) | 0.16 (NA–0.51) | 0.12 (0.01–0.23) | |
AE (retained C for tobacco) | ACE | 10 | 18706.94 | 9965 | –1223.06 | 8.52 | 2 | .014 | |||||||
ACE dropped rAb | ACE | 11 | 18699.63 | 9964 | –1228.37 | 1.21 | 1 | .272 | – | 0.10 (0.06–0.14) | 0.05 (0.02–0.08) | – | 0.34 (0.19–0.63) | 0.16 (0.06–0.25) | |
ACE dropped rCb | ACE | 11 | 18699.38 | 9964 | –1228.62 | 0.96 | 1 | .327 | 0.12 (0.07–0.17) | – | 0.03 (–0.01–0.06) | 0.40 (0.23–0.67) | – | 0.10 (–0.01–0.20) | |
ACE dropped rA and rC | ACE | 10 | 18720.89 | 9965 | –1209.11 | 22.47 | 2 | <.001 | |||||||
Cognitive disorganization | Sat | – | 13 | 18734.01 | 9990 | –1245.99 | – | – | – | ||||||
ACE | Sat | 12 | 18735.12 | 9993 | –1250.88 | 1.11 | 3 | .775 | |||||||
AE (retained C for tobacco)a | ACE | 10 | 18735.36 | 9995 | –1254.64 | 0.24 | 2 | .886 | 0.17 (0.13–0.22) | – | 0.03 (–0.01–0.06) | 0.45 (0.31–0.64) | – | 0.09 (–0.02–0.19) | |
ACE dropped rA | ACE | 11 | 18741.80 | 9994 | –1246.20 | 6.68 | 1 | .010 | |||||||
ACE dropped rC | ACE | 11 | 18735.36 | 9994 | –1252.64 | 0.24 | 1 | .623 | |||||||
ACE dropped rA and rC | ACE | 10 | 18784.06 | 9995 | –1205.94 | 48.94 | 2 | <.001 | |||||||
AE dropped rA | ACE | 9 | 18784.06 | 9996 | –1207.94 | 48.94 | 3 | <.001 |
Note: Δdf = difference in degrees of freedom; Δ-2LL = log-likelihood-ratio χ2 test comparing each model to the base; –2LL = minus 2 log-likelihood; A = additive genetic influences; AIC = Akaike’s Information Criterion; Base = comparison model; Biva2=bivariate heritability; Bivc2 = bivariate common environments; Bive2 = bivariate unique environment; C = common environmental influences; df = degrees of freedom; E = unique environmental influences; EP = estimated parameters; rA = genetic correlation; rC = common environmental correlation; rE = unique environmental correlation.
Most parsimonious model.
ACE models with dropped rA or rC indistinguishable in terms of fit and reported full ACE model results.
For tobacco use and hallucinations, ACE models dropping either rA or rC, but not both, were not significantly worse in terms of fit compared to the full ACE model. The AIC values for the ACE models that dropped either rA or rC were equally low and could not be distinguished in terms of fit. These results indicated overlapping familial (genetic and/or shared environmental) influences between tobacco use and hallucinations. Thus, the full ACE model is reported.
Parameters from the most parsimonious models (Table 4; see Figure S1, available online) suggested significant genetic overlap between tobacco use with paranoia (rA =.37) and with cognitive disorganization (rA = 0.45), no shared environmental overlap, and modest overlapping unique environmental influences (rE = 0.09−0.12). As a proportion of the phenotypic correlations (Table 3), genetic influences accounted for 84% of the covariance between tobacco use and paranoia and 81% between tobacco use and cognitive disorganization. The ACE model for hallucinations and tobacco indicated familial influences (A or C) accounted for 80% of the phenotypic correlation. See Table S6, available online, for bivariate statistics for full ACE models. Sensitivity analyses employing a dichotomous definition of tobacco use corroborated these findings (see Table S7, Table S8; Figure S2, available online).
Discussion
This study investigated two separate questions about the relationship between PE and tobacco use. First, we established that associations between tobacco use and specific types of PE were present during adolescence and remained for most PE after controlling for several covariates. Second, this was the first twin study to investigate the degree to which genetic and environmental influences explain the association between tobacco use and PE. Tobacco use was associated with paranoia and cognitive disorganization largely due to overlapping genetic influences, and with hallucinations due to familial influences that may include genes and shared environment.
Our results suggest that some of the same genetic factors that influence tobacco use in adolescents also influence PE. These results may also indicate gene−environment correlations whereby the genetic factors that influence PE create environments that make tobacco use more likely, and vice versa. Although the genetic correlation could indicate a causal association between PE and tobacco use in either direction, this was beyond the scope of this paper.
Our finding that tobacco use is associated with PE in adolescents is in line with previous findings. Gage et al.5 found an association between tobacco at age 16 years and PE at age 18 after controlling for cannabis use, familial depression, maternal education, IQ, bullying, and childhood psychosocial issues. McGrath et al.6 reported that those who commenced smoking before the age of 15 years were more likely to experience hallucinations at age 21. We found similar evidence while accounting for confounders not previously considered, such as sleep disturbance and SLE.
Tobacco use predicted PE to a lesser extent after including covariates, and adjusted models explained more variation in PE than models for tobacco use only. Therefore, some of the association between tobacco use and PE was attributable to the covariates tested, most notably to sleep disturbance. Considering these results and the known etiological association between PE and sleep disturbance,24 future studies could explore the relationship between PE, tobacco use, and sleep disturbance. A previous TEDS study concluded that PE and cannabis use co-occurred due to shared environmental factors.21 In our analyses, we controlled for confounding by several other (environmental) factors, which may explain why cannabis use did not predict PE here.
We found regular smoking to be associated with paranoia, hallucinations, cognitive disorganization, and parent-rated negative symptoms, the latter two being less commonly studied compared to positive PE. The relationship between tobacco use and anhedonia diverged from this trend. Occasional smoking appears to be associated with lower anhedonia, but regular smoking, albeit not significantly, with higher anhedonia. Perhaps adolescents who experimented with tobacco, but not those who habitually smoked, were more likely to engage in pleasure-seeking behavior. Negative symptoms were parent-rated, whereas anhedonia was self-rated, which may partly account for different effect sizes between these PE types.
Another finding was that 50% of variation in adolescent tobacco use was due to common environmental influences and a third due to additive genetics. Twin studies have previously investigated the heritability of adolescent smoking.30 Estimates of heritability and environmental influences can change over time and are context and population specific.39 Anti-smoking regulations, legislation, and social attitudes toward smoking have changed rapidly over recent years. Between the early 2000s and the current study, the United Kingdom has banned smoking in public places, increased the legal age for purchasing tobacco products, introduced bold health warnings on tobacco products, restricted advertising, increased prices, and made nicotine replacement therapies more accessible.40
A consideration is our operationalization of the tobacco use variable. We chose a three-level rather than a binary definition to improve power and to distinguish between the effects of regular and occasional smoking, in line with previous studies.2, 5 Our twin models assume linearity among not smoking, occasional smoking, and regular smoking. The prior regression analyses showed a linear association between tobacco use with paranoia, hallucinations, and cognitive disorganization, which supports this assumption. We note that anhedonia showed a nonlinear pattern, which could be explored in future research.
Our study was not designed to assess nicotine dependency, as never-smokers, for whom susceptibility to dependency is unknown, were included in our tobacco use measure. It is known that there may be some etiological differences between smoking initiation and nicotine dependency.41 We also cannot rule out attrition bias, because participating families were more likely to report higher socio-economic status than nonparticipating families.
The association between PE and tobacco use is significant and modest, and findings should be viewed in this context. We could not distinguish between genetic and common environmental influences shared between tobacco use and hallucinations, likely because the phenotypic correlation was lower than for our other bivariate models. Low phenotypic correlations also meant that we could not perform bivariate models between tobacco with grandiosity, anhedonia, and parent-rated negative symptoms.
Adolescent tobacco use is modifiable risk factor. Understanding the nature of the association between PE, a possible early manifestation of psychiatric disorder in some individuals, and tobacco use is of great interest. We have contributed to this field by showing that significant associations exist between specific PE domains and tobacco use in mid-adolescence that are not fully accounted for by confounding factors. We have provided novel insights into the etiology of the covariation between some PE and tobacco use that may inform further molecular genetic studies and developmental models.
Acknowledgments
The authors gratefully acknowledge the ongoing contribution of the participants in the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) and their families.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the UK Medical Research Council (G1100559 to Prof. Ronald). The Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) is supported by a program grant to Prof. Robert Plomin from the UK Medical Research Council (MR/ M021475/1 and previously G0901245). Mr. Barkhuizen is funded by the Camara-Rijvers David Studentship. Funding sources had no involvement in the current study’s design, data collection, analysis, the interpretation of the results, the writing of this manuscript, nor in the decision to submit the article for publication.
Disclosure: Prof. Freeman is a co-founder of Oxford VR, a University of Oxford spin-out company. Dr. Taylor, Prof. Ronald, and Mr. Barkhuizen report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Supplemental Material
Table S1.
Participating |
Nonparticipating |
t Test | d | p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | Mean (SD) | n | Mean (SD) | ||||
Socio-economic status at first contact | 3695 | 0.25 (0.98) | 3722 | –0.09 (0.97) | –15.09 | .35 | <.001 |
Natural mother’s age at birth of twins | 3943 | 31.47 (4.57) | 4290 | 30.60 (4.96) | –8.27 | .18 | <.001 |
Natural father’s age at birth of twins | 3651 | 33.84 (5.67) | 3757 | 33.30 (5.95) | –3.99 | .09 | <.001 |
n | % | n | % | χ2 | φc | p | |
Ethnicity of twins | 15.18 | .04 | <.001 | ||||
White | 3726 | 93.45% | 3996 | 91.15% | |||
Other | 261 | 6.55% | 388 | 8.85% | |||
Proportion with cohabiting parents at first contact | 3738 | 93.54% | 3895 | 88.58% | 64.29 | .09 | <.001 |
Has higher qualification | |||||||
Mothers | 1172 | 29.72% | 863 | 19.98% | 104.72 | .11 | <.001 |
Fathers | 1473 | 40.06% | 1073 | 28.33% | 113.78 | .12 | <.001 |
Mother’s employment status at first contact | 54.48 | .08 | <.001 | ||||
Unemployed | 397 | 10.00% | 662 | 15.20% | |||
Employed | 1888 | 47.56% | 1866 | 42.84% | |||
Staying home to look after children | 1685 | 42.44% | 1828 | 41.96% | |||
Father’s employment status at first contact | 23.13 | .06 | <.001 | ||||
Unemployed | 154 | 4.13% | 251 | 6.53% | |||
Employed | 3499 | 93.86% | 3503 | 91.08% | |||
Staying home to look after children | 75 | 2.01% | 92 | 2.39% |
Note: Higher qualification defined as those who have post-school qualifications (usually after the age of 18) including higher diploma/certificate, undergraduate degree, or postgraduate degrees; d = Cohen’s d where small effect d = 0.2, medium d = 0.5, and large d = 0.8; φc = Phi, an estimation of effect size appropriate for χ2 tests with small effect φc = 0.1, medium φc = 0.3, and large φc = 0.5.
Table S2.
Smoking frequency | Nonsmokers |
Occasional Smokers (<50 Cigarettes) |
Regular Smokers (≥50 Cigarettes) |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | % | n | % | n | % | |
Never tried smoking | 2,789 | 93.43 | – | – | – | – |
Tried once or twice | 161 | 5.39 | 294 | 67.43 | 0 | 0.00 |
Used to smoke but not now | 23 | 0.77 | 53 | 12.16 | 20 | 10.58 |
<1 cigarette/wk | 0 | 0.00 | 57 | 13.07 | 26 | 13.76 |
1–6 cigarettes/wk | 0 | 0.00 | 6 | 1.38 | 10 | 5.29 |
>6 cigarettes/wk but not daily | 0 | 0.00 | 1 | 0.23 | 7 | 3.70 |
≥1 cigarette/d | 0 | 0.00 | 3 | 0.69 | 112 | 59.26 |
No information on smoking frequency | 12 | 0.40 | 22 | 5.05 | 14 | 7.41 |
Total | 2,985 | 100.00 | 436 | 100.00 | 189 | 100.00 |
Table S3.
Paranoia | Hallucinations | Cognitive Disorganization | Grandiosity | Anhedonia | Negative Symptoms | Tobacco Use | Age | Ethnicity | Cannabis | Sleep Disturbance | Stressful Life Events | SES | Prenatal Maternal Smoking | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Paranoia | 1 | |||||||||||||
Hallucinations | .46§ | 1 | ||||||||||||
Cognitive disorganization | .42§ | .44§ | 1 | |||||||||||
Grandiosity | .10§ | .18§ | .04∗ | 1 | ||||||||||
Anhedonia | .08§ | .03 | .01 | –.19§ | 1 | |||||||||
Negative symptoms | .14§ | .11§ | .25§ | –.02 | .12§ | 1 | ||||||||
Tobacco use | .18§ | .16§ | .20§ | .06∗ | 0 | .08§ | 1 | |||||||
Age | –.02 | –.01 | .01 | .02 | –.02 | –.05§ | .19§ | 1 | ||||||
Ethnicity | –.01 | –.09∗ | –.06∗ | –.20§ | .02 | –.01 | .11∗ | –.06∗ | 1 | |||||
Cannabis | .19§ | .17§ | .17§ | .06∗ | .04 | .10§ | .83§ | .17§ | .01 | 1 | ||||
Sleep disturbance | .37§ | .35§ | .43§ | –.01 | .11§ | .13§ | .17§ | .06§ | –.05 | .17§ | 1 | |||
Stressful life events | .16§ | .17§ | .13§ | .16§ | –.09§ | .01 | .32§ | 19§ | –.13§ | .39§ | .14§ | 1 | ||
SES | .07§ | –.04∗ | –.08§ | –.02 | .03 | –.13§ | –.04 | –.07§ | .05 | 0.05 | .02 | -.05§ | 1 | |
Prenatal maternal smoking | .05 | .07§ | .11§ | 0 | –.01 | .17§ | .31§ | .04 | .08 | .26§ | .07∗ | .10§ | -39§ | 1 |
Note: Pearson correlation reported between two continuous variables. Polyserial correlations conducted between ordinal-continuous variables. Tetrachoric/polychoric correlations reported between two ordinal variables. SES = socio-economic status.
p < .05;
p >.005.
Table S4.
Unadjusted Model |
Adjusted Model |
Unadjusted Model |
Adjusted Model |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | CI | β | CI | β | CI | β | CI | ||
Paranoia | Grandiosity | ||||||||
Tobacco use (ref: nonsmokers) | Tobacco use (ref: nonsmokers) | ||||||||
Occasional smokers | 0.32 | 0.23–.41 | 0.15 | 0.05–0.24 | Occasional smokers | –0.06 | –0.15–0.03 | –0.07 | –0.17–0.03 |
Regular smokers | 0.48 | 0.33–0.64 | 0.17 | 0–.35 | Regular smokers | 0.14 | –0.01–0.29 | 0.04 | –0.16–0.24 |
Sex | 0.00 | –0.06–.07 | Sex | 0.27 | 0.20–0.33 | ||||
Age | –0.10 | –0.15 to –0.05 | Age | –0.01 | –0.06–0.04 | ||||
Ethnicity | –0.14 | –28–0 | Ethnicity | 0.26 | 0.11–0.41 | ||||
Cannabis use | 0.10 | –0.04–0.24 | Cannabis use | 0.06 | –0.09–0.21 | ||||
Sleep disturbance | 0.35 | 0.31–0.38 | Sleep disturbance | –0.04 | –0.07–0 | ||||
Significant life events | 0.09 | 0.06–0.13 | Significant life events | 0.15 | 0.12–0.19 | ||||
Socio-economic status | 0.07 | 0.03–0.10 | Socio-economic status | 0.01 | –0.03–0.05 | ||||
Prenatal maternal smoking | 0.01 | –0.09–0.11 | Prenatal maternal smoking | –0.03 | –0.14–0.08 | ||||
Hallucinations | Anhedonia | ||||||||
Tobacco use (ref: nonsmokers) | Tobacco use (ref: nonsmokers) | ||||||||
Occasional smokers | 0.22 | 0.13–0.32 | 0.09 | –0.01–0.19 | Occasional smokers | 0 | –0.10–0.09 | –0.01 | –0.12–0.09 |
Regular smokers | 0.42 | 0.26–0.59 | 0.21 | 0.02–0.40 | Regular smokers | 0.20 | 0.02–0.38 | 0.18 | –0.03–0.39 |
Sex | –0.03 | –0.09–0.04 | Sex | 0.51 | 0.44–0.58 | ||||
Age | –0.09 | –0.14 to –0.04 | Age | –0.05 | –0.10–0 | ||||
Ethnicity | 0.07 | –0.07–0.21 | Ethnicity | –0.02 | –0.18–0.14 | ||||
Cannabis use | 0.00 | –0.16–0.16 | Cannabis use | 0.07 | –0.08–0.22 | ||||
Sleep disturbance | 0.33 | 0.30–0.36 | Sleep disturbance | 0.12 | 0.08–0.16 | ||||
Significant life events | 0.11 | 0.07–0.14 | Significant life events | –0.11 | –0.14 to –0.07 | ||||
Socio-economic status | –0.03 | –0.07–0 | Socio-economic status | 0.00 | –0.03–0.04 | ||||
Prenatal maternal smoking | 0.00 | –0.10–0.11 | Prenatal maternal smoking | 0.04 | –0.07–0.16 | ||||
Cognitive disorganization | Negative symptomsa | ||||||||
Tobacco use (ref: nonsmokers) | Tobacco use (ref: nonsmokers) | ||||||||
Occasional smokers | 0.30 | 0.20–0.39 | 0.15 | 0.05–0.24 | Occasional smokers | –0.04 | –0.14–0.05 | –0.07 | –0.18–0.03 |
Regular smokers | 0.59 | 0.43–0.74 | 0.24 | 0.08–0.40 | Regular smokers | 0.52 | 0.35–0.70 | 0.28 | 0.07–0.49 |
Sex | –0.27 | –0.33 to –0.21 | Sex | 0.17 | 0.09–0.24 | ||||
Age | –0.05 | –0.10 to –0.01 | Age | –0.12 | –0.18 to –0.06 | ||||
Ethnicity | –0.01 | –0.16–.13 | Ethnicity | 0.05 | –0.12–0.22 | ||||
Cannabis use | 0.05 | –0.08–0.19 | Cannabis use | 0.10 | –0.06–0.25 | ||||
Sleep disturbance | 0.39 | 0.36–0.42 | Sleep disturbance | 0.14 | 0.10–0.17 | ||||
Significant life events | 0.05 | 0.02–0.08 | Significant life events | –0.03 | –0.06–0.01 | ||||
Socio-economic status | –0.06 | –0.10 to –0.03 | Socio-economic status | –0.11 | –0.15 to –0.07 | ||||
Prenatal maternal smoking | 0.04 | –0.05–0.14 | Prenatal maternal smoking | 0.15 | 0.02–0.27 |
Note: Both twins (using exclusion criteria as described in the Methods for bivariate twin analyses) included in Generalized Estimating Equation (GEE) models. GEE models were conducted in the drgee package for R. Reference group for sex: “female,” for ethnicity: “white,” for cannabis use: “no,” and for maternal smoking during pregnancy: “no.”
Parent-rated.
Table S5.
Model | Base | EP | Model Fit |
df | AIC | Δ–2LL | Δ df | p | Parameters for Most Parsimonious Model |
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
–2LL | A (CI) | C (CI) | E (CI) | ||||||||
Sat | – | 10 | 4755.55 | 4894 | –5032.45 | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Sub 1 | Sat | 6 | 4757.02 | 4898 | –5038.98 | 1.47 | 4 | .650 | – | – | – |
Sub 2 | Sat | 4 | 4758.86 | 4900 | –5041.14 | 3.31 | 6 | .202 | – | – | – |
ACEa | Sat | 5 | 4758.86 | 4901 | –5043.14 | 3.31 | 7 | .860 | 0.32 (0.17–0.49) | 0.51 (0.36–0.64) | 0.17 (0.13–0.21) |
AE | ACE | 4 | 4792.97 | 4902 | –5011.03 | 34.12 | 1 | <.001 | – | – | – |
CE | ACE | 4 | 4776.80 | 4902 | –5027.20 | 17.94 | 1 | <.001 | – | – | – |
E | ACE | 3 | 5266.30 | 4903 | –4539.70 | 507.44 | 2 | <.001 | – | – | – |
Note: Δdf = difference in degrees of freedom comparing each model to the base model; Δ–2LL=log-likelihood ratio χ2 test comparing the –2LL fit of each model to the –2LL fit of the base model; –2LL= minus 2 log-likelihood. A = additive genetic influences; AIC = Akaike’s Information Criterion (lower values reflect a better fit); Base = comparison model (full ACE compared to saturated models and sub models compared to full ACE); C = common environmental influences; E = unique environmental influences; df = degrees of freedom; EP = number of estimated parameters; Sub 1 = Submodel 1 with equal thresholds across twin order; Sub 2 = Submodel 1 with equal thresholds across twin order and zygosity.
Most parsimonious model.
Table S6.
Model | Bivariate Statistics From ACE and Models With Lowest AIC (95% CI) |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Biva2 | Bivc2 | Bive2 | rA | rC | rE | ||
Paranoia | ACE | 0.09 (–0.02–0.20) | 0.07 (–0.01–0.17) | 0.04 (0.01–0.07) | 0.23 (0.01–0.52) | 0.36 (–0.70–1.00) | 0.13 (0.02–0.07) |
AE (retained C for tobacco)a | 0.16 (0.11–0.21) | – | 0.03 (–0.01–0.06) | 0.37 (0.25–0.53) | – | 0.11 (–0.01–0.21) | |
Hallucinations | ACE | 0.07 (–0.05–0.19) | 0.05 (–0.05–0.15) | 0.04 (0–0.07) | 0.25 (–0.17–0.67) | 0.16 (–0.17–0.53) | 0.12 (0–0.23) |
ACE dropped rAb | – | 0.10 (0.06–0.14) | 0.05 (0.02–0.08) | – | 0.34 (0.19–0.63) | 0.16 (.06–0.25) | |
ACE dropped rCb | 0.12 (0.07–0.17) | – | 0.03 (–0.01–0.06) | 0.40 (0.23–0.67) | – | 0.10 (–0.01–0.20) | |
Cognitive disorganization | ACE | 0.15 (0.04–0.26) | 0.02 (–0.07–0.12) | 0.03 (–0.01–0.06) | 0.38 (0.10–0.69) | 0.99 (–1–NAc) | 0.10 (–0.02–0.21) |
AE (retained C for tobacco)a | 0.17 (0.13–0.22) | – | 0.03 (–0.01–0.06) | 0.45 (0.31–0.64) | – | 0.09 (–0.02–0.19) |
Note: A = additive genetic influences; Biva2 = bivariate heritability; Bivc2 = bivariate common environments; Bive2 = bivariate unique environment; C = common environmental influences; E = unique environmental influences; rA = genetic correlation; rC = common environmental correlation; rE = unique environmental correlation.
Most parsimonious model.
ACE models with dropped rA or rC indistinguishable in terms of fit and thus reported full ACE model results.
Upper confidence interval not estimated likely because there was no common environmental influences on cognitive disorganization with implications for the estimate of rC.
Table S7.
Tobacco | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Phenotypic correlations | r | CI | ||
Paranoia | 0.17 | 0.11–0.23 | ||
Hallucinations | 0.16 | 0.10–0.22 | ||
Cognitive disorganization | 0.22 | 0.16–0.28 | ||
Grandiosity and delusions | 0.08 | 0.03–0.14 | ||
Negative symptoms | 0.22 | 0.16–0.28 | ||
Anhedonia | 0.08 | 0.03–0.14 | ||
MZ | DZ | |||
Twin correlations | r | CI | r | CI |
Paranoia | 0.53 | 0.49–0.56 | 0.30 | 0.25–0.35 |
Hallucinations | 0.43 | 0.38–0.47 | 0.29 | 0.24–0.34 |
Cognitive disorganization | 0.46 | 0.41–0.50 | 0.22 | 0.17–0.28 |
Negative symptoms | 0.81 | 0.80–0.83 | 0.54 | 0.50–0.58 |
Tobacco | 0.92 | 0.86–0.97 | 0.76 | 0.60–0.82 |
Cross-twin cross-trait correlations (PE and tobacco) | ||||
Paranoia | 0.16 | 0.09–0.26 | 0.10 | 0.02–0.17 |
Hallucinations | 0.13 | 0.06–0.20 | 0.07 | −0.01–0.14 |
Cognitive disorganization | 0.20 | 0.13–0.27 | 0.10 | 0.02–0.18 |
Negative symptomsa | 0.24 | 0.17–0.30 | 0.18 | 0.10–0.25 |
Note: Nonsmokers were defined as those who had never smoked or who smoked less than 50 lifetime cigarettes. Smokers were those who smoked 50 or more lifetime cigarettes and who smoked in the past year. DZ = dizygotic twins; PE = psychotic experiences.
Parent-rated.
Table S8.
Model | Base | EP | Model fit |
Df | AIC | Δ–2LL | Δ df | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
–2LL | |||||||||
Paranoia | Saturated model | 10 | 15445.00 | 9993 | –4541.00 | ||||
ACE | Sat | 11 | 15445.00 | 9994 | –4543.00 | 0.01 | 1 | .999 | |
AE (retained C for tobacco)a | ACE | 9 | 15446.78 | 9996 | –4545.22 | 1.78 | 2 | .411 | |
ACE dropped rA | ACE | 10 | 15447.58 | 9995 | –4542.42 | 2.58 | 1 | .108 | |
ACE dropped rC | ACE | 10 | 15445.17 | 9995 | –4544.83 | 0.17 | 1 | .682 | |
ACE dropped rA and rC | ACE | 9 | 15466.39 | 9996 | –4525.61 | 21.39 | 2 | <.001 | |
AE dropped rA | ACE | 8 | 15468.29 | 9997 | –4525.71 | 23.29 | 3 | <.001 | |
Hallucinations | Saturated model | 10 | 15613.03 | 10001 | –4388.97 | ||||
ACE | Sat | 11 | 15613.03 | 10002 | –4390.97 | 0.01 | 1 | .999 | |
AE (retained C for tobacco) | ACE | 9 | 15621.10 | 10004 | –4386.90 | 8.07 | 2 | .018 | |
ACE dropped rA | ACE | 10 | 15614.98 | 10003 | –4391.02 | 1.95 | 1 | .162 | |
ACE dropped rCa | ACE | 10 | 15613.04 | 10003 | –4392.96 | 0.01 | 1 | .929 | |
ACE dropped rA and rC | ACE | 9 | 15625.36 | 10004 | –4382.64 | 12.34 | 2 | .002 | |
Cognitive disorganization | Saturated model | 10 | 15583.50 | 9993 | –4402.50 | ||||
ACE | Sat | 11 | 15583.62 | 9994 | –4404.38 | 1.12 | 1 | .733 | |
AE (retained C for tobacco) a | ACE | 9 | 15583.63 | 9996 | –4408.38 | 0.01 | 2 | .998 | |
ACE dropped rA | ACE | 10 | 15589.15 | 9995 | –4400.85 | 5.53 | 1 | .019 | |
ACE dropped rC | ACE | 10 | 15583.63 | 9995 | –4406.38 | 0.01 | 1 | .944 | |
ACE dropped rA and rC | ACE | 9 | 15615.04 | 9996 | –4376.96 | 31.42 | 2 | <.001 | |
AE dropped rA | ACE | 8 | 15615.04 | 9997 | –4378.96 | 31.42 | 3 | <.001 | |
Negative symptoms | Saturated model | 10 | 14113.87 | 10003 | –5892.13 | ||||
ACE a | Sat | 11 | 14113.87 | 10004 | –5894.13 | 0.01 | 1 | 0.999 | |
(parent-rated) | AE (retained C for tobacco) | ACE | 9 | 14144.63 | 10006 | –5867.37 | 30.76 | 2 | <.001 |
ACE dropped rA | ACE | 10 | 14117.12 | 10005 | –5892.88 | 3.25 | 1 | 0.072 | |
ACE dropped rC | ACE | 10 | 14117.19 | 10005 | –5892.81 | 3.32 | 1 | 0.069 | |
ACE dropped rA and rC | ACE | 9 | 14162.75 | 10006 | –5849.25 | 48.88 | 2 | <.001 |
Note: Saturated models constrained means, thresholds and phenotypic correlations across twin order. Nonsmokers defined as those who had never smoked or who smoked less than 50 lifetime cigarettes. Smokers were those who smoked 50 or more lifetime cigarettes and who smoked in the past year. –2LL = minus 2 log-likelihood; Δdf = difference in degrees of freedom compared to the base model; Δ–2LL=log-likelihood ratio chi-square test comparing the –2LL of each model to the –2LL fit of the base model; A = additive genetic influences; Base = comparison model; AIC = Akaike's Information Criterion (lower values reflect a more parsimonious and therefore preferred fit); C = common environmental influences; df = degrees of freedom; E = unique environmental influences; EP = estimated parameters; rA = genetic correlation; rC = common environmental correlation; rE = unique environmental correlation
Most parsimonious model.
References
- 1.Rossler W., Ajdacic-Gross V., Haker H., Rodgers S., Muller M., Hengartner M.P. Subclinical psychosis syndromes in the general population: results from a large-scale epidemiological survey among residents of the canton of Zurich, Switzerland. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2015;24:69–77. doi: 10.1017/S2045796013000681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Saha S., Scott J.G., Varghese D., Degenhardt L., Slade T., McGrath J.J. The association between delusional-like experiences, and tobacco, alcohol or cannabis use: a nationwide population-based survey. BMC Psychiatry. 2011;11:202. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-11-202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Mackie C.J., Castellanos-Ryan N., Conrod P.J. Developmental trajectories of psychotic-like experiences across adolescence: impact of victimization and substance use. Psychol Med. 2011;41:47–58. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710000449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bhavsar V., Jauhar S., Murray R.M. Tobacco smoking is associated with psychotic experiences in the general population of South London. Psychol Med. 2018;48:123–131. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gage S.H., Hickman M., Heron J. Associations of cannabis and cigarette use with psychotic experiences at age 18: findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Psychol Med. 2014;44:3435–3444. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.McGrath J.J., Alati R., Clavarino A. Age at first tobacco use and risk of subsequent psychosis-related outcomes: a birth cohort study. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2016;50:577–583. doi: 10.1177/0004867415587341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Koyanagi A., Stickley A., Haro J.M. Psychotic symptoms and smoking in 44 countries. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2016;133:497–505. doi: 10.1111/acps.12566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wiles N.J., Zammit S., Bebbington P., Singleton N., Meltzer H., Lewis G. Self-reported psychotic symptoms in the general population: results from the longitudinal study of the British National Psychiatric Morbidity Survey. Br J Psychiatry. 2006;188:519–526. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.012179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Jones H.J., Gage S.H., Heron J. Association of combined patterns of tobacco and cannabis use in adolescence with psychotic experiences. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75:240–246. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.4271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Dominguez M.D., Wichers M., Lieb R., Wittchen H.U., van Os J. Evidence that onset of clinical psychosis is an outcome of progressively more persistent subclinical psychotic experiences: an 8-year cohort study. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37:84–93. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kelleher I., Cannon M. Psychotic-like experiences in the general population: characterizing a high-risk group for psychosis. Psychol Med. 2011;41:1–6. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fisher H.L., Caspi A., Poulton R. Specificity of childhood psychotic symptoms for predicting schizophrenia by 38 years of age: a birth cohort study. Psychol Med. 2013;43:2077–2086. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712003091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kaymaz N., Drukker M., Lieb R. Do subthreshold psychotic experiences predict clinical outcomes in unselected non-help-seeking population-based samples? A systematic review and meta-analysis, enriched with new results. Psychol Med. 2012;42:2239–2253. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.van Os J., Linscott R.J., Myin-Germeys I., Delespaul P., Krabbendam L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med. 2009;39:179–195. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kelleher I., Keeley H., Corcoran P. Clinicopathological significance of psychotic experiences in non-psychotic young people: evidence from four population-based studies. Br J Psychiatry. 2012;201:26–32. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.101543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Office of National Statistics Statistics on smoking, England 2017. NHS Digital. http://digital.nhs.uk/pubs/smoking17 Published June 15, 2017. Available at:
- 17.Kasza K.A., Ambrose B.K., Conway K.P. Tobacco-product use by adults and youths in the United States in 2013 and 2014. N Engl J Med. 2017;376:342–353. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa1607538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.O’Malley P.M., Miech R.A., Bachman J.G., Schulenberg J.E. Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975-2015: overview, key findings on adolescent drug use. http://monitoringthefuture.org/pubs/monographs/mtf-overview2015.pdf Published June 2015. Available at:
- 19.van Gastel W.A., Wigman J.T., Monshouwer K. Cannabis use and subclinical positive psychotic experiences in early adolescence: findings from a Dutch survey. Addiction. 2012;107:381–387. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03626.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Griffith-Lendering M.F.H., Wigman J.T.W., Prince van Leeuwen A. Cannabis use and vulnerability for psychosis in early adolescence—a TRAILS study. Addiction. 2013;108:733–740. doi: 10.1111/add.12050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Shakoor S., Zavos H.M., McGuire P., Cardno A.G., Freeman D., Ronald A. Psychotic experiences are linked to cannabis use in adolescents in the community because of common underlying environmental risk factors. Psychiatry Res. 2015;227:144–151. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.03.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Shakoor S., Zavos H.M., Haworth C.M. Association between stressful life events and psychotic experiences in adolescence: evidence for gene-environment correlations. Br J Psychiatry. 2016;208:532–538. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.159079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cullen A.E., Fisher H.L., Roberts R.E., Pariante C.M., Laurens K.R. Daily stressors and negative life events in children at elevated risk of developing schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2014;204:354–360. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.127001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Taylor M.J., Gregory A.M., Freeman D., Ronald A. Do sleep disturbances and psychotic-like experiences in adolescence share genetic and environmental influences? J Abnorm Psychol. 2015;124:674–684. doi: 10.1037/abn0000057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Reeve S., Emsley R., Sheaves B., Freeman D. Disrupting sleep: the effects of sleep loss on psychotic experiences tested in an experimental study with mediation analysis. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44:662–671. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Zammit S., Thomas K., Thompson A. Maternal tobacco, cannabis and alcohol use during pregnancy and risk of adolescent psychotic symptoms in offspring. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195:294–300. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.062471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.McGue M., Elkins I., Iacono W.G. Genetic and environmental influences on adolescent substance use and abuse. Am J Med Genet. 2000;96:671–677. doi: 10.1002/1096-8628(20001009)96:5<671::aid-ajmg14>3.0.co;2-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Maes H.H., Woodard C.E., Murrelle L. Tobacco, alcohol and drug use in eight- to sixteen-year-old twins: the Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behavioral Development. J Stud Alcohol. 1999;60:293–305. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1999.60.293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Koopmans J.R., Slutske W.S., Heath A.C., Neale M.C., Boomsma D.I. The genetics of smoking initiation and quantity smoked in Dutch adolescent and young adult twins. Behav Genet. 1999;29:383–393. doi: 10.1023/a:1021618719735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hopfer C.J., Crowley T.J., Hewitt J.K. Review of twin and adoption studies of adolescent substance use. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42:710–719. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046848.56865.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bares C.B., Kendler K.S., Maes H.H. Developmental changes in genetic and shared environmental contributions to smoking initiation and subsequent smoking quantity in adolescence and young adulthood. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2015;18:497–506. doi: 10.1017/thg.2015.48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ronald A. Recent quantitative genetic research on psychotic experiences: new approaches to old questions. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2015;2:81–88. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Haworth C.M., Davis O.S., Plomin R. Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): a genetically sensitive investigation of cognitive and behavioral development from childhood to young adulthood. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013;16:117–125. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ronald A., Sieradzka D., Cardno A.G., Haworth C.M., McGuire P., Freeman D. Characterization of psychotic experiences in adolescence using the specific psychotic experiences questionnaire: findings from a study of 5000 16-year-old twins. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40:868–877. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Buysse D.J., Reynolds C.F., 3rd, Monk T.H., Berman S.R., Kupfer D.J. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Coddington R.D. The significance of life events as etiologic factors in the diseases of children. II. A study of a normal population. J Psychosom Res. 1972;16:205–213. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(72)90045-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.R Core Team. R . R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna, Austria: 2017. A language and environment for statistical computing. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Neale M.C., Hunter M.D., Pritikin J.N. OpenMx 2.0: extended structural equation and statistical modeling. Psychometrika. 2016;81:535–549. doi: 10.1007/s11336-014-9435-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Turner J.R., Cardon L., Hewitt J.K. Springer Science Business Media; New York: 2013. Behavior Genetic Approaches in Behavioral Medicine. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Action on Smoking and Health (ASH) Key dates in the history of anti-tobacco campaigning. http://ash.org.uk/informationand-resources/briefings/key-dates-in-the-history-of-anti-tobacco-campaigning Published February 2017. Available at:
- 41.Vink J.M., Willemsen G., Boomsma D.I. Heritability of smoking initiation and nicotine dependence. Behav Genet. 2005;35:397–406. doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-1327-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]