Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 Feb 4;8:e42392. doi: 10.7554/eLife.42392

Retinal direction selectivity in the absence of asymmetric starburst amacrine cell responses

Laura Hanson 1,, Santhosh Sethuramanujam 1,, Geoff deRosenroll 1, Varsha Jain 1, Gautam B Awatramani 1,
Editors: Fred Rieke2, Ronald L Calabrese3
PMCID: PMC6377229  PMID: 30714905

Abstract

In the mammalian retina, direction-selectivity is thought to originate in the dendrites of GABAergic/cholinergic starburst amacrine cells, where it is first observed. However, here we demonstrate that direction selectivity in downstream ganglion cells remains remarkably unaffected when starburst dendrites are rendered non-directional, using a novel strategy combining a conditional GABAA α2 receptor knockout mouse with optogenetics. We show that temporal asymmetries between excitation/inhibition, arising from the differential connectivity patterns of starburst cholinergic and GABAergic synapses to ganglion cells, form the basis for a parallel mechanism generating direction selectivity. We further demonstrate that these distinct mechanisms work in a coordinated way to refine direction selectivity as the stimulus crosses the ganglion cell’s receptive field. Thus, precise spatiotemporal patterns of inhibition and excitation that determine directional responses in ganglion cells are shaped by two ‘core’ mechanisms, both arising from distinct specializations of the starburst network.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

The direction-selective (DS) circuit in the retina is arguably one of the most well-defined circuits in the mammalian brain (reviewed by Mauss et al., 2017; Vaney et al., 2012). In this circuit, direction is encoded by output DS ganglion cells (DSGCs), whose response properties are shaped by DS inputs arising from GABAergic/cholinergic starburst amacrine cells (starbursts) and non-DS inputs from glutamatergic bipolar cells (Figure 1A). However, despite comprehensive connectomic analysis of the circuit (Briggman et al., 2011; Ding et al., 2016) and many decades of physiological investigations, the fundamental mechanisms underlying direction selectivity, both at the level of starbursts and DSGCs remain to be fully elucidated.

Figure 1. Parallel ‘core’ mechanisms generating direction selectivity in DSGCs.

Figure 1.

(A) A cross section of the functional circuitry underlying direction selectivity in ON-OFF DSGCs. DSGCs receive glutamatergic inputs from ON and OFF bipolar cells, and inputs from mirror symmetric populations of ON and OFF GABAergic/cholinergic starburst amacrine cells. In this study, the stimuli used emphasized the ON responses. Preferred-side starbursts (p-starburst) provide mainly cholinergic excitation, while null-side starbursts (n-starburst) provide cholinergic excitation and a dominant GABAergic inhibition (Lee et al., 2010; Yonehara et al., 2011). Thus, GABAergic/cholinergic signals mediated by starbursts are differentially transmitted to the DSGC. (B) Schematic depicting the asymmetric anatomical connectivity between starbursts and DSGCs (top view). Bona fide ‘wrap-around’ synaptic connections (circled in red) are made largely by null-side starbursts, enabling DS dendrites of starbursts to mediate a ‘null’ inhibition (Briggman et al., 2011). This contrasts with the symmetrical functional cholinergic connectivity depicted in (A). Paracrine or ‘volume’ transmission could make cholinergic signals agnostic to the specific synaptic connectivity (Briggman et al., 2011; Brombas et al., 2017). (C) In conventional models, direction selectivity in DSGCs is largely shaped by asymmetric postsynaptic GABAergic inhibition. Asymmetric inhibition is contingent on the DS release of GABA from dendrites of starbursts; and on the asymmetric wiring (B). By contrast, excitation is non-directional, mainly mediated by glutamate released from bipolar cells (Park et al., 2014; Yonehara et al., 2013). In this model, the cholinergic receptive field is co-extensive with the glutamatergic receptive field. ACh is usually thought to play a non-directional role (Ariel and Daw, 1982; Amthor et al., 1996; Chiao and Masland, 2002Park et al., 2014), although some studies note amplitude differences between preferred and null-evoked cholinergic currents (as shown here) suggesting that ACh may directly contribute to direction selectivity (Lee et al., 2010; Pei et al., 2015). (D) In the revised model proposed here, cholinergic excitation is directional by virtue of its timing with GABAergic inhibition rather than by its response amplitude (C). The differences in the functional connectivity of GABA and ACh (A) predict that for preferred-direction motion, excitation would lead inhibition; and for null-direction motion, E/I would be activated together. In this study, the central hypothesis is that E/I offsets contribute to a parallel DS mechanism, which does not necessarily rely on the modulation of the peak amplitude of GABA or ACh inputs (in contrast to the model shown in C).

The first points in the visual system where direction selectivity is observed are the radiating dendrites of starbursts (Euler et al., 2002), which release GABA and acetylcholine (ACh) (O'Malley and Masland, 1989). To explain direction selectivity, initial studies emphasized mechanisms that were based on intrinsic properties of starburst dendrites (Gavrikov et al., 2006; Hausselt et al., 2007; Tukker et al., 2004). In contrast, more recent studies have proposed two distinct network mechanisms. The first relies on inhibition within the dense array of starburst cells, where dendrites of neighboring starbursts with opposite directional preferences mutually inhibit each other. This ensures that dendrites pointing in a particular direction are maximally activated as the moving stimulus sequentially activates the starbursts (Ding et al., 2016; Lee and Zhou, 2006; Münch and Werblin, 2006). The second network mechanism relies on the specific wiring of temporally distinct bipolar cells along the proximal-distal axis of the starburst dendrite. This arrangement results in an optimal summation of inputs when the stimulus moves centrifugally (from soma-to dendrite) along the starbursts dendrites (Fransen and Borghuis, 2017; Kim et al., 2014). When tested experimentally, however, neither of these mechanisms appears to be critically required for generating DS, at least in the context of simple spot/grating stimuli (Chen et al., 2016; Sethuramanujam et al., 2016).

Starbursts play an obligatory role in generating direction selectivity in downstream DSGCs (Vlasits et al., 2014; Yoshida et al., 2001). They provide the neural substrate for a powerful GABAergic ‘null’ inhibition that is required for the generation of DS responses in ganglion cells (Figure 1C). Null inhibition relies on both the DS properties of starburst dendrites and also a specific ‘anti-parallel’ wiring scheme (Figure 1B). Only starbursts with somas displaced to the null-side of the DSGC’s receptive field (i.e. the side from which null-stimuli enter the receptive field) make synaptic connections with DSGCs (Briggman et al., 2011; Brombas et al., 2017; Chen et al., 2016; Fried et al., 2002; Lee et al., 2010; Yonehara et al., 2011). By contrast, excitatory inputs to DSGCs arising from bipolar cells are non-DS (Park et al., 2014; Yonehara et al., 2013). Together, these findings have given rise to a well-accepted model in which direction selectivity in DSGCs is largely inherited from starburst dendrites, contingent on the DS release of GABA (Figure 1C) (reviewed by Mauss et al., 2017; Vaney et al., 2012).

However, in addition to GABA, starbursts also co-release ACh (O'Malley and Masland, 1989), which mediates fast excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) in DSGCs via the activation of nicotinic ACh (nACh) receptors (Brombas et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2010; Pei et al., 2015; Sethuramanujam et al., 2016; Taylor and Smith, 2012). Unlike GABA, however, cholinergic signals appear to be more symmetrical at the level of DSGCs, giving rise to the notion it modulates the amplitude of DSGC responses (Ariel and Daw, 1982; Amthor et al., 1996Chiao and Masland, 2002Park et al., 2014). Recent voltage-clamp recordings on the other hand, reveal amplitude differences between preferred and null-evoked cholinergic currents (Figure 1C), suggesting that ACh may directly contribute to direction selectivity (Lee et al., 2010Pei et al., 2015). However, the mechanisms underlying DS cholinergic excitation remain unknown.

Elegant starburst/DSGC paired recordings demonstrate that cholinergic inputs arise from starbursts surrounding DSGCs from all directions, in sharp contrast to GABA inputs (Figure 1A) (Brombas et al., 2017; Chen et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2010; Yonehara et al., 2011). Given the asymmetric anatomical wiring of the starbursts and DSGCs (Figure 1B), it is presumed that cholinergic excitation is mediated by paracrine mechanisms that are agnostic to the specific connectivity (Briggman et al., 2011; Brombas et al., 2017). Building on these findings, in this study we hypothesized that the differential functional connectivity of spatially offset GABA and ACh signals produces excitation-inhibition (E/I) timing differences that generate direction selectivity, as originally envisioned (Koch et al., 1982; Taylor et al., 2000; Torre and Poggio, 1978). This could serve as a parallel ‘hard-wired’ DS mechanisms that does not necessarily require DS starburst output (Figure 1D).

It is important to note that temporal E/I asymmetries have often been noted in the literature, but their impact has been difficult to assess, as they are always associated with changes in E/I amplitude ratio (Figure 1D) (Fried et al., 2005; Kostadinov and Sanes, 2015; Pei et al., 2015; Sethuramanujam et al., 2016; Taylor and Vaney, 2002). Further, blocking cholinergic inputs, which is expected to decrease E/I timing differences arising from the above-mentioned specific wiring of starburst ACh/GABA synapses (Figure 1D), does not appear to affect the ability of DSGCs to encode direction (Ariel and Daw, 1982; Kittila and Massey, 1997). It has also been shown that robust direction selectivity is observed in instances where E/I temporal offsets are not apparent (Taylor and Vaney, 2002). Additionally, modeling studies suggest that E/I temporal offsets play a negligible role in this computation in the context of the E/I amplitude modulation (Schachter et al., 2010). Thus, the contribution of E/I temporal offsets to direction coding by DSGCs are generally neglected.

Here, we describe two major advancements that better our understanding of the DS circuitry in the mouse retina. First, using a novel combination of optogenetics/mouse KO technology/pharmacology, we render starburst output non-DS for the first time. This provides valuable insights into the biophysical mechanisms underlying direction selectivity in starburst dendrites. Second, we found that blocking starburst DS had a surprisingly weak effect on direction selectivity in ganglion cells. This demonstrates the existence of a second ‘core’ DS mechanism, which we demonstrate relies on E/I temporal offsets. Interestingly, while changes in E/I ratios or timing differences are each sufficient to drive robust DS responses in ganglion cells, these mechanisms contribute to distinct phases of the DSGC response, ensuring that direction is computed rapidly and with high fidelity.

Results

Rendering starburst dendrites non-directional

We first sought to develop ways to abolish direction selectivity in starburst dendrites to test its contribution to direction selectivity in the postsynaptic DSGCs. Previous attempts to perturb DS in starburst dendrites have been met with little success. For example, selectively eliminating mutual inhibition between anti-parallel starburst dendrites using the GABAA α2 receptor KO mouse line (Gabra2 KO) leaves starburst DS largely intact (Chen et al., 2016). Consistent with these findings, we found the DS output of the starbursts measured as inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs) in DSGCs, was similar in Gabra2 KO mice and wild type mice. This was quantified using a direction selectivity index (DSI; calculated as the normalized vector sum of responses measured across eight directions; Wt = 0.33 ± 0.019, n = 6; KO = 0.28 ± 0.022; n = 6; data are presented as mean ± SEM; Figure 2A,B,E,F).

Figure 2. Direction selectivity in starburst dendrites relies on both excitatory and inhibitory network mechanisms.

Figure 2.

(A–D) Starburst output monitored as IPSCs in voltage-clamped DSGCs (VHOLD ~0 mV) in a variety of mouse lines (as indicated). For simplicity, only ON IPSCs are shown for stimulus motion in the DSGC’s preferred (P) or null-direction (N) under conditions in which (A) all synaptic inputs are intact; (B) mutual inhibition between starbursts is selectively disrupted using the conditional Gabra2 KO; (C) bipolar cell inputs are blocked and ChR2-expressing starbursts are directly stimulated; (D), both mutual inhibition and bipolar cell inputs are blocked. Vertical scale bar = 100 pA (A–C) or 200 pA (D). Responses are averaged over three trials. (E) The peak amplitudes of the IPSCs evoked during preferred- and null-direction motion are plotted against each other for the conditions noted in (A–D). A reference line with slope = 1 (dashed line) is shown to facilitate comparisons. n = 6 for wild type, Gabra2 KO and ChR2; n = 7 for Gabra2 KO/ChR2. (F) The average direction-selectivity index (DSI) computed from the normalized vector sum of the peak amplitude of IPSCs evoked by stimuli moving in eight directions, for the conditions in (A–D) (See Materials and methods for DSI calculation; DSI = 0 indicates non-directional responses; DSI = 1 indicates strongly tuned responses). Pooled data are represented as mean ± SEM (Solid circles), while single cell responses are denoted by open circles. Statistical significance was estimated by unpaired t-tests, where *, ** or n.s. indicate p=0.0026, 0.0001 and 0.1068, respectively.

Figure 2—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 2A-F.
elife-42392-fig2-data1.xlsx (376.7KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.004

Arguing against excitatory network mechanisms (Fransen and Borghuis, 2017; Kim et al., 2014), robust DS output was observed in the absence of bipolar cell input, when the starbursts were directly stimulated using optogenetics (Figure 2C,E & F; Sethuramanujam et al., 2016). In these experiments, photoreceptor/bipolar cell driven responses were blocked using conventional pharmacology (50 µM DL-AP4, 10 µM UBP310 and 20 µM CNQX) and the stimulus intensity was increased 1000-fold (R*/s) to directly activate starburst-expressing channelrhodopsin2 (ChR2) in relative isolation. While there was considerable variability in the amplitude of IPSCs, which may have arisen from variable expression of ChR2 and/or run down of optogenetic responses over the course of the recording session (which could last up to 8 hr), within a given cell the responses and their direction selectivity remained reliable between trials. We thus included weaker responding cells in this analysis.

Remarkably, combining these two approaches to block both inhibitory and excitatory network DS mechanisms, led to the near-complete block of the asymmetry in responses mediated by starburst cells, i.e. the amplitudes of inhibitory inputs evoked by optogenetic stimulation of starbursts in the Gabra2 KO background were nearly equal for preferred and null-direction motion (DSI = 0.07 ± 0.02, n = 7; Figure 2D–F). The ability to block direction selectivity in starbursts, while leaving its output relatively intact, provides for the first time direct evidence for the mechanisms generating it. The requirement to block both the excitatory (Fransen and Borghuis, 2017; Kim et al., 2014) and inhibitory network mechanisms (Ding et al., 2016; Lee and Zhou, 2006; Münch and Werblin, 2006) suggest that they work in parallel to shape DS responses in starburst dendrites.

Retinal direction selectivity in the absence of asymmetric starburst amacrine cell responses

Abolishing the directional properties of starbursts is expected to suppress the output of DSGCs, as under these conditions DSGCs would receive strong inhibition in all directions. Contrary to this notion, we found that DSGCs in Gabra2 KO mice continued to exhibit robust spiking responses when starburst output was rendered non-DS (Figure 3A). Strikingly, these spiking responses were robustly tuned for direction similar to control conditions (Figure 3B–D). While the direction encoded was the same under conditions in which starburst output was DS or non-DS (Figure 3A), we did observe a significant reduction in the duration of the DSGC’s spiking response (Figure 3A). Since starbursts are critically required for DS computation (Vlasits et al., 2014; Yoshida et al., 2001), it follows that they must utilize an alternate mechanism to confer direction selectivity upon DSGCs, in the absence of amplitude modulation of inhibitory inputs. In addition, the finding that the direction tuning properties of the DSGC remains unchanged when starburst output is rendered non-DS (Figure 3B,D), indicates that this second DS mechanism is well-aligned to the classical mechanism relying on the direction selectivity of starburst dendrites.

Figure 3. Retinal direction selectivity in the absence of asymmetric starburst amacrine cell responses.

Figure 3.

(A) Spiking responses from a DSGC in the Gabra2 KO/ChR2 mouse line measured under conditions when photoreceptor (PR) output is intact (top), when photoreceptor synapses are blocked pharmacologically (50 µM DL-AP4, 10 µM UBP310 and 20 µM CNQX (middle)), and subsequently, when stimulus intensity is increased to directly activate starbursts using ChR2 (bottom). This enables a direct comparison of direction selectivity in a DSGC under conditions in which starburst output is DS (top) or non-DS (bottom). Stimuli were moved in eight directions indicated by the arrows at a velocity of 1200 μm/s. The smooth traces on top of each spike train indicates the average spike rate estimated by low-pass filtering the spike train via convolution with a Gaussian kernel (σ = 25 ms). (B) Polar plots of the peak spike rates for the responses in the two conditions shown in (A). The arrow indicates the DSGC’s preferred direction, scaled to the DSI. (C–D) A comparison of the preferred direction (C) and DSI (D) of the responses evoked during ChR2 stimulation, and during intact photoreceptor stimulation in the Gabra2 KO/ChR2 mouse line (n = 7).

Figure 3—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 3A-D.
elife-42392-fig3-data1.xlsx (759.3KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.006

We hypothesized that asymmetries in the timing of excitation and inhibition arising from the differential functional starburst-DSGC connectivity gives rise to a parallel DS mechanism (Figure 1D). When we examined the onset latencies for GABA and ACh in the Gabra2 KO, we found E/I temporal offsets were exquisitely tuned for direction (Figure 4A–D). The magnitude of the offsets provided a good indication of the DSGC’s preferred-direction when compared to its spiking responses measured prior to the voltage-clamp experiments (Figure 4B–D). These results provided strong evidence that temporal asymmetries in E/I onsets alone can drive direction selectivity in ganglion cells. Moreover, since temporal asymmetries are observed under conditions in which photoreceptor/glutamate receptors are blocked, it indicates that they arise from the starburst network itself, through a differential functional connectivity of cholinergic/GABAergic synapses to DSGCs (Figure 1B,D).

Figure 4. Directionally tuned E/I temporal offsets maintain direction selectivity in DSGCs in the absence of asymmetric starburst responses.

Figure 4.

(A) Optogenetically evoked spikes, EPSCs and IPSCs recorded in succession from the same DSGC in the Gabra2 KO/ChR2 mouse line (in the absence of photoreceptor input). The onset of excitation and inhibition (indicated by ●), was estimated by back extrapolating the lines of best fit of the initial response (grey lines; see Materials and methods). The shaded box indicates the E/I temporal offset window during which a large fraction of the spikes appear to be generated. (B) Polar plots of the peak firing rates (top) and E/I temporal offsets (bottom) for optogenetic responses (depicted in A) to eight stimulus directions. C–D, A comparison of the DSI (C) and direction coded (D) estimated from the peak firing rates or E/I temporal offsets (as shown in B) across 7 cells (open circles). Pooled data (closed circles) are represented as mean ± SEM. (E–H) Similar to A–D but for responses measured under physiological conditions i.e. responses driven by photoreceptors in the wild-type retina (n = 11). The DSI of the E/I temporal offsets was higher than the DSI of spiking (p<0.001). Pooled data are represented as mean ± SEM.

Figure 4—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 4A-D.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.008
Figure 4—source data 2. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 4E-H.
elife-42392-fig4-data2.xlsx (488.7KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.009

Differential transmission of ACh/GABA under physiological conditions

Temporal asymmetries were also observed under natural conditions, i.e. when responses were driven by photoreceptors (Figure 4E–H), consistent with previous studies (Fried et al., 2002; Kostadinov and Sanes, 2015; Pei et al., 2015; Sethuramanujam et al., 2016; Taylor and Vaney, 2002). Similar to the optogenetic experiments, the magnitude of the E/I temporal offsets measured under physiological conditions (i.e. photoreceptor mediated) was greatest for preferred-direction motion and progressively decreased as the stimulus direction approached the DSGC’s null-direction (Figure 4F). In these experiments, superior coding DSGCs were excluded from the analysis, since in these ganglion cells gap junctions are known to drive early excitatory inputs (Trenholm et al., 2013). The E/I temporal offset tuning was significantly sharper when compared to the tuning of the spiking responses, as indicated by the DSI (Figure 4G). In a later section, we use modeling to show that this difference arises because the DS spiking responses are shaped by two distinct mechanisms with different tuning properties (Figure 7). By contrast, the predicted direction based on offsets were almost perfectly aligned with the direction encoded based on the spiking responses (Figure 4H).

Consistent with the notion that E/I temporal offsets arise from a differential transmission of ACh/GABA (Figure 1D), blocking nicotinic ACh (nACh) receptors using a specific antagonist (100 µM hexamethonium) increased the onset latency for excitation evoked by motion in the preferred-direction, and largely abolished the E/I temporal offsets in this direction (Figure 5A). For null-direction motion, blocking these receptors resulted in a negative offset, as the GABAergic inhibition arising from null-side starbursts was no longer balanced by cholinergic inputs but continued to arrive before glutamate inputs (Figure 5A). Further blocking GABAergic inhibition did not alter the delay induced by blocking nACh receptors, indicating that the E/I temporal offset estimates were not confounded by voltage-clamp errors associated with large inhibitory conductances (Figure 5—figure supplement 1; Poleg-Polsky and Diamond, 2011). Plotting the E/I temporal offsets for all stimulus directions reveals that hexamethonium uniformly reduces the E/I temporal offsets by ~25 ms (average delay = 26 ± 2 ms; n = 8) regardless of direction (Figure 5B,C), consistent with the idea that starbursts make cholinergic connections from all around the DSGC (Figure 1D). Given the stimulus moved at 1000 μm/s, this corresponds to a spatial offset of ~25 μm (see Discussion). It is also worth noting that, both ACh and GABA signals arising from starburst dendrites are spatially offset relative to glutamate inputs (Figure 1B,D), unlike the co-extensive glutamatergic/cholinergic receptive fields observed in rabbit retina (Brombas et al., 2017; Fried et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2010). Therefore, we conclude that the differential transmission of ACh/GABA produces E/I that are temporally synchronized in the null- but not preferred-direction (Figure 1D and Figure 5A), providing the substrate for a timing based mechanism for generating direction selectivity in ganglion cells.

Figure 5. Cholinergic excitation shapes temporal E/I temporal offsets under physiological conditions.

(A) The EPSCs and IPSCs elicited in a DSGC for preferred and null stimuli, in control (black), and in the presence of a nicotinic acetylcholine receptor antagonist (hexamethonium, HEX; red traces). A normalized version of the EPSC measured in HEX (light red trace) is illustrated to emphasize the delay in excitation that occurs when cholinergic receptors are blocked. (B) The directional tuning of the E/I temporal offsets in control and HEX averaged across 8 cells. The offsets were aligned to the preferred direction (P) of the spike responses, which were measured in the same DSGC before performing the whole cell recording. HEX significantly reduced the E/I temporal offsets across all directions (p<0.01). Data are represented as mean ± SEM. (C) The directional tuning of the timing delay of the EPSC in HEX relative to control (n = 8). Data are represented as mean ± SEM. The increased onset latency observed in HEX continued to be observed when inhibition was blocked, indicating that they cannot be accounted for by large voltage–clamp errors associated with inhibitory conductances (Figure 5—figure supplement 1).

Figure 5—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 5.
elife-42392-fig5-data1.xlsx (932.5KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.016

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. The EPSC timing delay in HEX is independent of inhibition.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A – B) The EPSCs elicited in a DSGC to preferred (A) and null stimuli (B), in control, HEX (to block nicotinic acetylcholine receptors), and HEX + SR-95531 (to block GABAA receptors). Note that the responses have been normalized to show that the timing of the EPSCs is altered upon addition of HEX, but not upon further addition of SR. (stimulus velocity = 600 μm/s) (C–D) The onset of the preferred (C) and null EPSCs (D) in control (n = 10), HEX (n = 10), and HEX +SR (n = 7) shown relative to HEX. The EPSCs in HEX were significantly delayed to control (*p<0.001). But further addition of SR did not alter the timing. Pooled data are represented as mean ± SEM.

E/I temporal offset mechanisms contribute to direction selectivity over a range of velocities

Next, we examined how E/I temporal offsets could contribute to direction selectivity across a range of stimulus velocities. In the Gabra2 KO-ChR2, where DS in DSGCs relies solely on temporal E/I offsets, the number of optogenetically evoked spikes were found to decrease as a function of velocity, as expected (Figure 6A). However, despite the drop in the spike responses at higher stimulus velocities, the direction selectivity was not altered (Figure 6A). As it is not possible to isolate the effects of temporal offsets under physiological conditions, instead, we measured the velocity dependence of the E/I temporal offsets under physiological conditions (Figure 6B), and used these values to drive DS responses in a model DSGC, generated in the NEURON environment (Figure 6D; see Materials and methods for details).

Figure 6. Velocity tuning of E/I temporal offsets in DSGCs.

Figure 6.

(A) A plot of the average number of ChR2-evoked spikes for preferred direction motion as a function of stimulus velocity (left panel; n = 6), measured from DSGCs in the Gabra2 KO/ChR2 mouse line. The average DSI and preferred direction remains stable across velocity (right panel; n = 6). Data are represented as mean ± SEM. (B–C) The E/I temporal offsets measured from DSGCs in the wild-type retina, for both preferred and null directions as a function of velocity (B); n = 8). The offsets during preferred direction motion were significantly larger than those for null direction motion for all velocities (p<0.001). The temporal offsets correspond to a relatively fixed spatial offset (calculated by velocity x E/I temporal offsets) across a range of velocities tested (inset). The effects of blocking cholinergic transmission (hexamethonium) on E/I temporal offsets is shown in C). Data are represented as mean ± SEM. (D) Spike output of a computer simulated model DSGC in which the E/I ratio was fixed (1/2) but the E/I temporal offsets were varied according to measurements in B). E/I temporal offsets alone could generate robust DS across a range of velocities, except of the lowest velocity tested (see text for details).

Figure 6—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 6B-C.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.018

The amplitude of E/I temporal offsets measured in the DSGC’s preferred direction reduced with velocity, but were still measurable at the highest velocities tested (2400 μm/s; Figure 6B). The values of the E/I temporal offsets measured at different speeds corresponded to a ~ 30 μm spatial offset between excitatory and inhibitory receptive fields (Figure 6B inset). Addition of nACh receptor antagonists (hexamethonium) abolished the E/I temporal offsets, confirming that the spatial offsets are mediated by starburst inputs at all velocities (Figure 6C). Conversely, for null-direction motion, E/I were co-activated through most of the velocity range, except for the lower velocities where inhibition tended to lead excitation (Figure 6B). For null-direction motion, the nACh receptor antagonists revealed negative offsets, as previously noted (Figure 6C).

Next, to examine the impact of E/I temporal offsets at different velocities we generated a model DSGC with excitatory and inhibitory inputs having timing offsets that varied with direction as measured experimentally, but with their amplitude ratio constant (E:I set at 1:2 for all directions; Figure 4F). As with our optogenetic study, for preferred motion, the number of spikes decreased as a function of velocity (Figure 6D), but remained robustly directional through most of velocity range. Only at the lowest speed, where the edges moving in the null direction spent considerable time on the excitatory receptive field after inhibition ceased, the model generated a few null-direction spikes, resulting in a decreased DSI (Figure 6D). In real DSGC this does not appear to be an issue, indicating that GABAergic inhibition must be slower and more powerful than that implemented in our model. On the other hand, the direction encoded remained largely unaltered with velocity. Thus, the simplified model captures many aspects of the DSGC response properties, and supports the idea that the E/I temporal offset mechanism contributes to direction selectivity in ganglion cells over a wide range of velocities.

Complementary roles for E/I temporal offset and amplitude based DS mechanisms

Finally, we explored the possible computational benefits offered by the distinct DS mechanisms relying on temporal and amplitude E/I asymmetries. When the spiking responses were averaged over the entire trial, the direction selectivity generated with or without DS inhibition was similar, and thus the DS mechanisms appear redundant (Figure 3). However, when the DS tuning was examined on a finer time-scale (Figure 7), it showed distinct characteristics in the early and peak phases of the response, raising the possibility that the two mechanisms operate on distinct time-scales. While the direction encoded remained constant throughout the response, the tuning curve broadened as the response approached its peak, resulting in a marked decrease in the DSI (Figure 7A–D). Moreover, the initial estimate of direction was more variable compared to that made during the peak of the response (standard deviation of early and peak responses were 16 ± 20; and 8 ± 20, respectively; Figure 7B,E,F). This difference in variability may reflect the ability of distal vs. proximal dendritic sites to effectively transmit directional information to the soma via dendritic spikes (Sivyer and Williams, 2013). It is also important to note that the broadening of the tuning curve did not arise simply from a thresholding effect (i.e. an iceberg effect), as the last spikes at the tail end of the response were not sharply tuned (data not shown).

Figure 7. Two ‘core’ DS mechanisms are engaged during different phases of the DSGC response.

(A) Spike rates during stimulus motion in the preferred or null directions in control conditions (black), after cholinergic excitation is blocked by HEX (red), and in the added presence of a GABA receptor blocker (5 μM SR-95531; green) to isolate the glutamate receptive field. Preferred and null responses could be accurately aligned, temporally, based on the glutamatergic receptive field measurement, which was important in estimating the temporal evolution of the DS tuning shown in (B–G). The effects of HEX on the response latency were reversible, and not observed when responses were weakened by blocking NMDA receptors (see Figure 7—figure supplement 1). PS and NS indicates the preferred- and null-sides of the DSGC. (B) Polar plots of a DSGC’s early (left) and peak (right) spike responses (the 50 ms shaded regions in A) over 29 trials. The mean response is shown in black, while the individual trials are shown in grey. Note early responses are more sharply tuned but provide a less reliable indication of the direction (i.e. the direction encoded is more variable; orange lines) compared to the peak response. (C–D) A comparison of the average DSI (C) and preferred direction (D) calculated from the early and peak spike responses for individual cell (n = 8). The DSI of the early responses was significantly higher than the peak responses (p<0.01), while the direction encoded in both phases were near identical. Pooled data are represented as mean ± SEM. (E–F) A comparison of the trial-to-trial variability of the DSI (E) and preferred direction (F) of the early and peak responses (n = 8). Trial-to-trial variability was estimated by the standard deviation of DSI/preferred direction across trials within each cell (minimum 10 trials). The variability was higher for early responses for both DSI (p<0.05) and preferred direction (p<0.005). Pooled data are represented as mean ± SEM. (G) Tuning properties of a computer simulated model DSGC in which both E/I temporal offsets and amplitude asymmetries (top), only E/I temporal offsets (middle) or only E/I amplitude asymmetry (bottom) were implemented. E/I temporal offsets alone resulted in a sharper tuning similar to the early responses in B), but the tuning became broader when E/I ratios were intact, similar to the peak responses in B) (see Figure 7—figure supplement 2 for synaptic currents and spiking responses measured in the model). Responses were averaged over 20 trials.

Figure 7—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 7.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.013

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. The effects of blocking nACh receptors on the latency of the DSGC response is reversible, and contrasts with the effects of NMDA receptor antagonists.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A) The average ON spiking responses, convolved with a Gaussian kernel (σ = 25 ms), measured in control (black), in the presence of a nicotinic ACh receptor antagonist (50 µM hexamethonium; HEX, red) and in wash conditions (grey). HEX increased the spiking latency by 51 ± 7 ms. Traces depict the mean (solid line)± SEM (shaded region). (B) The average spiking responses measured in control (black), in the presence of a NMDA receptor antagonist (50 µM D-AP5, blue) and in the added presence of HEX (red). AP5 did not alter the spike latency (2 ± 4 ms), while further addition of HEX increased the latency (45 ± 11 ms).
Figure 7—figure supplement 2. Input/output measured in a model DSGC in which responses are driven by directional changes in inhibition and/or E/I temporal offsets.

Figure 7—figure supplement 2.

(A) Single representative spike trains in a model DSGC are overlaid over the underlying EPSCs and IPSCs (averaged over 20 trials) for preferred (top), orthogonal (middle) and null direction motion (bottom) under conditions in which both E/I ratios (directional inhibition) and E/I temporal offsets were present. Note that the spiking responses in the null direction were delayed compared to the preferred direction spiking, resulting in changes in direction selectivity over the time-course of the response. (B) As in (A) but only E/I temporal offsets were used to drive DS. The strength of inhibition is non-DS. (C) As in (A) but only directionally tuned inhibitory charge was used to drive DS. All E/I timing offsets were removed. This simulation illustrates the workings of conventional models of DS. The spiking onset was delayed compared to (A and B), but DS did not evolve over time.

Given that early spikes appear to be driven largely by cholinergic excitation (Figure 7A), but not by glutamate inputs (Figure 7—figure supplement 1), we envisioned that E/I temporal offsets are likely to be more important in determining direction selectivity during the initial phase of the response, while the later phase would likely be dominated by E/I amplitude differences. It is important to note that both DS mechanisms rely on transmitter release from starbursts and thus operate on roughly the same spatial scales. Next, we tested these ideas in our model DS circuit, where we could easily control the timing and amplitude of E/I, independently (see Materials and methods for details).

Indeed, the characteristic properties of the DSGC’s direction tuning were reproduced in a model DSGC driven with both temporal and amplitude asymmetries in E/I that arise from the specific arrangement of GABA, ACh and glutamate inputs (Figure 7G; Figure 7—figure supplement 2A illustrates the synaptic inputs/spiking response of the model DSGC). When starbursts were rendered non-DS in the model DSGC (Figure 7G; Figure 7—figure supplement 2B), temporal asymmetries alone were sufficient to generate DS responses with sharp tuning (Figure 7G), as observed in the ChR2-Gabra2 KO mouse. When E/I temporal offsets were removed, modulating the peak amplitude of inhibition generated robust DS responses, but with wider tuning (Figure 7G; Figure 7—figure supplement 2C). However, in this model lacking offsets, the early responses were lost (Figure 7G). Thus, the DS mechanisms based on temporal and amplitude asymmetries appear to be complementary, each conferring distinct advantages: the former enables DSGCs to respond in a direction-selective manner sooner than they would have done otherwise, while the latter enables DSGCs to encode direction with higher fidelity albeit on a slower time scale.

Discussion

Parallel ‘core’ mechanisms generating direction selectivity in the retina

In the mammalian retina, direction selectivity is observed at the level of starburst dendrites as well as in downstream DSGCs. Our experiments using various pharmacological, optogenetic and cell-specific KO strategies to analyse synaptic inputs to DSGCs, suggest that multiple circuit mechanisms serve to generate direction-selectivity at both these points in the DS circuit.

At the level of starburst dendrites, three distinct mechanisms have been proposed to underlie direction selectivity. These include excitatory and inhibitory network mechanisms (Ding et al., 2016; Fransen and Borghuis, 2017; Kim et al., 2014; Lee and Zhou, 2006; Münch and Werblin, 2006), and cell intrinsic mechanisms (Euler et al., 2002; Hausselt et al., 2007; Tukker et al., 2004). However, pinpointing the ‘core’ mechanism has been complicated because blocking these mechanisms individually, leaves starburst DS output relatively intact. Our result, using ChR2 to stimulate starburst output in a mouse line where inhibitory network mechanisms have been inactivated (Chen et al., 2016), demonstrate for the first time a condition where starburst release of GABA is non-directional. These results highlight the apparently redundant roles for excitatory and inhibitory network mechanisms and help amalgamate divergent views (Ding et al., 2016; Fransen and Borghuis, 2017; Kim et al., 2014; Lee and Zhou, 2006; Münch and Werblin, 2006).

While intrinsic DS mechanisms in starburst dendrites may be important for DS computations, the current results do not appear to reveal them in an obvious manner. This could be for several reasons. First, it is possible that the artificial activation of starbursts using optogenetics does not recruit dendritic voltage-gated channels that may underlie direction selectivity (reviewed in Taylor and Smith, 2012). Second, the removal of GABAergic inhibition between starbursts may hinder or occlude intrinsic DS mechanisms. Third, it is also possible that the limited set of stimuli (moving spots) which we utilized were not optimally designed to recruit cell intrinsic mechanisms. Thus, although DS is suppressed under conditions in which inhibitory and excitatory network mechanisms are blocked, we do not exclude the possibility that intrinsic mechanisms could serve as a third ‘core’ DS mechanism. A challenge for future experiments is to determine if and how each of these mechanisms work to maintain starburst DS output under different stimulus conditions.

At the level of DSGCs too, multiple DS mechanisms appear to be in play. Here, two fundamental mechanisms appear to originate from unique specializations of the starburst network. The first mechanism, which relies on amplitude asymmetries arising from the DS starburst dendrites (Figure 1C) and asymmetric starburst-DSGC wiring (Figure 1B), is well established (reviewed by Mauss et al., 2017; Vaney et al., 2012). The second mechanism relies on temporal asymmetries in excitation and inhibition (Figure 1D). While E/I temporal offsets have often been noted in the literature (Fried et al., 2005; Kostadinov and Sanes, 2015; Pei et al., 2015Sethuramanujam et al., 2016; Sivyer et al., 2010; Taylor and Vaney, 2002), our results demonstrate for the first time that they are tuned precisely to the DSGC’s preferred direction, and are alone sufficient to generate direction selective responses in ganglion cells.

Recent studies using voltage-clamp techniques have also shown the amplitude of the cholinergic signal to vary according to direction (Lee et al., 2010; Pei et al., 2015; Park et al., 2014; Sethuramanujam et al., 2016). However, these results need to be interpreted with caution. Dynamic changes in the inhibitory conductance could lead to serious voltage-clamp errors and estimates of the relative peak amplitude of cholinergic input in the preferred and null direction (Poleg-Polsky and Diamond, 2011; Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Thus, the development of alternate methods, such as functional imaging, is required to fully resolve this issue. Importantly, the E/I offset mechanisms generating DS described here do not require amplitude changes in cholinergic inputs.

It is also worth noting that cholinergic, GABAergic and glutamatergic synapses are made relatively uniformly throughout the DSGC receptive field and are thought to constitute repeating dendritic DS subunits (Barlow and Levick, 1965; Taylor et al., 2000). Thus, although the temporal asymmetries are most apparent at response onset in our recordings made from the DSGC soma, they are likely present throughout the dendritic tree. Local E/I amplitude and timing differences are expected to shape dendritic responses, which may be further refined by dendritic non-linearities (Oesch et al., 2005; Sivyer and Williams, 2013; Trenholm et al., 2014). This is one reason why the spikes generated by E/I temporal offset mechanisms alone often outlast the E/I temporal offsets measured macroscopically (Figure 7G). The precise degree to which timing and amplitude differences in local inputs influence the DSGC’s response directly depends on the extent to which dendrites operate independently (Schachter et al., 2010).

The network of starburst amacrine cells produces temporal E/I asymmetries in DSGCs

Before the discovery of DS in starburst dendrites, classic models suggested that DS responses in ganglion cells were based on E/I timing differences (Koch et al., 1982; Taylor et al., 2000; Torre and Poggio, 1978). Previous studies have speculated that an intermediary cell in the inner and/or outer retina may control E/I timing differences at DSGCs (Rivlin-Etzion et al., 2012; Taylor and Vaney, 2002; Vlasits et al., 2014). In contrast, another study found that perturbing starburst cell morphology led to parallel changes in E/I amplitude ratio and E/I temporal offsets suggesting that these processes may be inextricably linked (Kostadinov and Sanes, 2015).

Our combined results from optogenetic, pharmacological and modelling experiments indicate that the temporal asymmetries in E/I are a natural consequence of the asymmetric/symmetric GABAergic/cholinergic connectivity of starburst cells to DSGCs (Figure 1; Chen et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2010; Yonehara et al., 2011). In this scheme, null-side starbursts extend the inhibitory receptive fields of DSGCs during null motion, producing a more powerful inhibition (Fried et al., 2002). Conversely, cholinergic excitation provided by preferred-side starbursts enable DSGCs to respond sooner to the moving edge than they would otherwise. We found that the degree to which cholinergic inputs advance the response is proportional to stimulus velocity, corresponding to a fixed spatial offset ~30 µm. In mouse retina, starbursts receive excitatory and inhibitory inputs on proximal dendrites but release transmitter from varicosities located on their distal dendrites (Ding et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2014; Vlasits et al., 2016). Mapping the receptive fields of individual starburst varicosities using Ca2+ imaging shows them to be displaced by ~25 µm towards the soma (Poleg-Polsky et al., 2018). Thus, the extent to which cholinergic inputs expand the receptive field of the DSGC is well predicted by the input/output properties of starburst dendrites. The dimensions of the cholinergic receptive fields also indicate that despite being generated by paracrine mechanisms (which loosens the constraints set by the asymmetrical starburst-DSGC ‘hard-wiring’; Figure 1B) (Briggman et al., 2011; Brombas et al., 2017), the distance over which ACh spreads appears relatively limited.

In contrast to our findings, in rabbit retina, cholinergic excitation is co-extensive with the glutamatergic receptive field and does not appear to extend the DSGC’s receptive field beyond the dendritic field (Brombas et al., 2017; Fried et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2010; Yang and Masland, 1992). One reason for this could be that static stimuli employed in previous studies to measure the receptive field properties of DSGCs do not optimally stimulate starbursts, and therefore fail to reveal cholinergic excitation (Fried et al., 2005; Yang and Masland, 1992). Alternatively, in rabbit retina, the release of ACh may require a stronger stimulation of the starbursts (Lee et al., 2010). This causes a delay in the release of ACh, making it co-extensive with glutamate input (Lee et al., 2010). Indeed, under conditions where inhibition is blocked and starburst dendrites are more strongly stimulated, cholinergic inputs do provide lateral excitation (Fried et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2010; Vaney et al., 2012). Together, these findings have given rise to the notion that the role of ACh is modulatory (Ariel and Daw, 1982; Amthor et al., 1996; Chiao and Masland, 2002; Lee et al., 2010; but see Pei et al., 2015), in contrast to what we observed in mouse retina.

The demonstration that the parallel circuit mechanisms driving direction selectivity in ganglion cells are individually dispensable necessitates a re-evaluation of the conclusions drawn from a multitude of studies carried out over the last several decades that considered a single ‘core’ DS mechanism. For example, numerous studies over the last forty years have found that blocking cholinergic receptors does not affect direction selectivity in DSGCs and have taken this to mean that ACh does not play an integral role in DS (reviewed by Mauss et al., 2017; Vaney et al., 2012), but rather provides an additional source of non-directional excitation to boost DSGC responses (Brombas et al., 2017; Chen et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2010), that is especially important under limiting conditions (Sethuramanujam et al., 2016; Sethuramanujam et al., 2017). In sharp contrast, our results here indicate that in fact ACh signals (unlike glutamate signals) are highly directional, not so much in their amplitudes as previously envisioned (Chen et al., 2016; Grzywacz et al., 1998; Grzywacz et al., 1997; Lee et al., 2010; Pei et al., 2015), but rather by virtue of their relative timing with inhibition. Cholinergic excitation and GABAergic inhibition are relatively imbalanced (or out of phase) during preferred-direction motion, but extremely well-balanced (or in phase) during null-direction motion, which is important for the computation (Figure 1D).

Spatiotemporal dynamics of ACh, GABA and glutamate inputs

Traditional models of DS raised questions regarding the role of cholinergic excitation in driving direction selectivity. In the revised model (Figure 1D), the role of canonical bipolar cell glutamate pathways becomes questionable. Previously, we have shown that under low contrast conditions, glutamate appears to be mediated by NMDA receptors (Sethuramanujam et al., 2016; Sethuramanujam et al., 2017) and play an important role modulating cholinergic excitation. Under high contrast conditions, as we have used here, glutamate signaling is mediated by both AMPA and NMDA receptors. Glutamate input is relatively delayed and therefore makes the DSGC’s preferred response more sustained. This contributes to changes of the DS tuning over time.

Moreover, since AMPA inputs are non-directional they are expected to broaden the DS tuning curve, by changing the E/I ratio across direction (Poleg-Polsky and Diamond, 2016). Indeed, a measurable broadening of the real DSGC tuning curves is noted at higher contrasts, although these effects are relatively small (Sethuramanujam et al., 2017). Additionally, given the difference in receptive fields between bipolar and starburst inputs to DSGCs, the E/I ratio is predicted to change over the duration of directional light responses (Figure 1D). Our model indicates that this effect is most pronounced during slow stimulus velocities, as temporal offsets caused by spatial differences are exacerbated as more time is spent between activations of each input to the DSGC, leading to a deterioration of DS (Figure 6F). However, in real DSGCs, DS is stable across velocities (Brombas et al., 2017), indicating that there must be additional mechanisms that compensate for these effects (e.g. inhibition from non-DS sources).

Conclusions

Here we have shown how E/I amplitude and timing differences are orchestrated by parallel circuit mechanisms. In the case of pinpointing the mechanisms generating direction selectivity in starbursts (Ding et al., 2016; Fransen and Borghuis, 2017; Kim et al., 2014; Lee and Zhou, 2006; Münch and Werblin, 2006), or understanding whether timing (Koch et al., 1982; Taylor et al., 2000; Torre and Poggio, 1978) or amplitude ratios generate DS in ganglion cells, the realization of multiple DS mechanisms helps settle divergent views. In contrast, the suggestion of a central role for starburst ACh in driving E/I temporal offsets, necessitates re-enquiry into previous results which indicated a modulatory role.

Given the multiple DS mechanisms in the retina, it is interesting to speculate that DS neurons in higher visual areas are also created using a variety of circuit mechanisms. This may help explain conflicting results from recent studies regarding the origin of DS in cortex. For example, thalamocortical inputs to the visual cortex exhibit strong DS properties (Sun et al., 2016), apparently with retinal origin (Cruz-Martín et al., 2014). Yet, abolishing retinal direction selectivity has modest effects on cortical DS (Hillier et al., 2017), indicating that direction selectivity must also arise de novo through alternate parallel mechanisms (Lien and Scanziani, 2018; Saul et al., 2005; Wilson et al., 2018). Strikingly, in primate visual cortex, distinct mechanisms appear to shape early and late response (Pack and Born, 2001; Thiele et al., 2004) similar to what we have observed here for DSGCs, suggestive of a common organizational principle. It is also interesting to note that direction selectivity in other sensory areas including the auditory and somatosensory systems (Kuo and Wu, 2012; Wilent and Contreras, 2005) may also rely on temporal and amplitude E/I asymmetries (Li et al., 2015; Ye et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2003). While large-scale ultrastructural circuit mapping is bound to reveal circuit elements involved in the computation, it is only through the marriage of anatomical and physiological approaches can the circuit mechanisms be fully realized, as we have demonstrated here for the generation of direction selectivity at multiple levels in the mouse retina.

Materials and methods

Animals

Experiments were performed using adult (either sex) Trhr-EGFP (RRID: MMRRC_030036-UCD) or ChAT-IRES-Cre (RRID: MGI_5475195) crossed with Ai32 (RRID: MGI_5013789) with or without Gabra2tm2.2Uru (RRID: MGI_5140553). All procedures were performed in accordance with the Canadian Council on Animal Care and approved by the University of Victoria’s Animal Care Committee.

Physiological recordings

Mice were dark-adapted for approximately 30–60 min before being briefly anesthetized and decapitated. The retina was extracted and dissected in Ringer’s solution under infrared light. The isolated retina was then mounted on a 0.22 mm membrane filter (Millipore) with a pre-cut window to allow light to reach the retina, enabling the preparation to be viewed with infrared light using a Spot RT3 CCD camera (Diagnostic Instruments) attached to an upright Olympus BX51 WI fluorescent microscope outfitted with a 40 × water immersion lens (Olympus Canada). The isolated retina was then perfused with warmed Ringer’s solution (35–37°C) containing 110 mM NaCl, 2.5 mM KCl, 1 mM CaCl2, 1. 6 mM MgCl2, 10 mM dextrose and 22 mM NaHCO3 that was bubbled with carbogen (95% O2:5% CO2).

DSGCs were identified by their genetic labeling or by their characteristic DS responses. Light stimuli, produced using a digital light projector (Hitachi Cpx1, refresh rate 75 Hz), were focused onto the outer segments of the photoreceptors using the sub-stage condenser. The background luminance, measured with a calibrated spectrophotometer (Ocean Optics), was set to ~10 photoisomerisations/s (R*/sec). Visual stimuli created in the Matlab environment (Psychtoolbox) were of positive contrasts, ranging between 15% and 1,000% (Weber contrast). Stimulus intensity was increased by five log units using neutral density filters to stimulate starburst-ChR2. Light-evoked activity was measured for 200 µm spot moving in eight directions at 1–1.6 mm/s.

Spike recordings were made with the loose cell-attached patch-clamp technique using 5–10 MΩ electrodes filled with Ringer’s solution. Voltage-clamp whole-cell recordings were made using 4–7 MΩ electrodes containing 112. 5 mM CH3CsO3S, 7. 75 mM CsCl, 1 mM MgSO4, 10 mM EGTA, 10 mM HEPES, 5 mM QX-314 (Tocris) and 100 µM spermine (Abcam Biochemicals). The pH was adjusted to 7.4 with CsOH. The reversal potential for chloride was calculated to be –56 mV. The recordings were not corrected for junction potential. Recordings were made with a MultiClamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices). Signals were digitized at 10 kHz (PCI-6036E acquisition board, National 9 Instruments) and acquired using custom software written in LabVIEW developed by Dr. David Balya (Friedrich Miescher Institute, Switzerland; https://github.com/GBAlab/Presentinator). Unless otherwise noted, all reagents were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich Canada. D-AP5, and UBP310 were purchased from ABCAM Biochemicals. DL-AP4, SR-95531 and CNQX were purchased from Tocris.

Computational modeling

Here we modified a detailed multi-compartmental model that was built previously by Poleg-Polsky and Diamond, 2016 in the NEURON simulation environment (Hines and Carnevale, 1997) (https://github.com/geoffder/Spatial-Offset-DSGC-NEURON-Model). This model was based on a morphological reconstruction of a real DSGC (Poleg-Polsky and Diamond, 2016), with membrane capacitance and axial resistance set to 1 µF/cm2 and 100 Ωcm respectively. Membrane channels and noise were modelled using a stochastic Hodgkin and Huxley distributed mechanism (NEURON MOD file) as previously described. Non-voltage-gated leak conductances were set to reverse at −60 mV. Active membrane conductances were placed at the soma, primary dendrites and dendrites with the following densities (mS/cm2) for soma, primary and terminal dendrites respectively: sodium (150/150/30), potassium rectifier (70/70/35), delayed rectifier (3/0.8/0.4). Sodium and potassium conductances were blocked for voltage-clamp recordings. This enabled the model DSGC to process inputs actively i.e. via dendritic spikes (Sivyer and Williams, 2013).

In our model DSGC, synapses were placed on terminal dendrites throughout the dendritic arbor. Synapses housed glutamatergic (AMPA receptor), cholinergic (nicotinic receptor) and GABAergic (GABAA receptor) synaptic inputs, whose kinetic properties were based on experimentally measured miniature events. Synaptic inputs were activated by a simulated bar moving over the model DSGC in 8 directions at 1 mm/s. Cholinergic inputs were symmetrical across the simulated directions, with their probabilities of release (Pr) set at 0.5. Cholinergic inputs were also spatially offset by ~50 µm (corresponding to an E/I temporal offset of 50 ms for preferred stimuli moving at 1 mm/s), and for simplicity modeled based on fast modes of transmission, (similar to AMPA) rather paracrine transmission (see Discussion). AMPA receptor-mediated inputs were modelled to occur without any offset relative to their position on the DSGC’s dendritic arbor. Thus they were simply activated on average (Pr ~0.5) when the simulated light stimulus passed over their synapse location. By contrast, GABAergic inhibitory inputs were made highly asymmetric, their Pr scaling down from ~0.5 in the null to ~0.012 in the preferred direction to simulate control conditions; or Pr was set constant at 0.5 to simulate conditions in which the starburst dendrites were non-DS. Their spatial offset ranged from ~50 µm in the null to ~0 µm in the preferred direction. The precise values across direction were estimated using sigmoidal fits to the tuning curves of the IPSCs (Sethuramanujam et al., 2017) and E/I temporal offsets measured experimentally (Figure 4):

Pr=pPr+(nPrpPr)(1.00.98/(1.0+e(angle91.0)/25.0))
spatialoffset=pOff(pOffnOff)(1.00.98/(1.0+e(angle74.69)/24.36))

Where, pPr, nPr refer to the probabilities of release in preferred and null directions, respectively. pOFF, nOFF refer to the spatial offsets in preferred and null directions, respectively; and angle refers to the direction of stimulated bar stimulus motion (0° is preferred, and 180° is null). Spatial offsets were then divided by the stimulus velocity to arrive at the final temporal offset. Importantly, the sequential activation of synapses by a moving bar stimulus, produced macroscopic inhibitory and excitatory currents which were well-matched to those measured experimentally using whole-cell patch-clamp techniques (Figure 7—figure supplement 2; Figure 7).

Quantification and statistical analysis

DSI was calculated as (Taylor and Vaney, 2002):

DSI=viri

where vi are vectors pointing in the direction of the stimulus and having length ri, equal to the number of spikes recorded during that stimulus. DSI ranged from 0 to 1, with 0 indicating a perfectly symmetrical response, and 1 indicating a response in only one of eight directions. The angle was calculated from the direction of the resultant of Σvi.

The E/I temporal offset window was calculated with the following protocol. First, the 20–80% rise time of the synaptic currents (EPSCs or IPSCs) was fit by a straight line (Figure 4). The response onset latency was measured as the point at which the extrapolated linear fit crossed the x axis (time axis). The E/I temporal offsets were calculated as difference in the excitatory and inhibitory onsets. Positive offsets indicate that excitation leads inhibition and vice versa. For the purposes of estimating DSI, negative offsets were set to 0.

In order to determine the early and peak spike responses (Figure 7), the spike trains in control conditions were aligned to the edge of the glutamate receptive field, measured either by the spike activity or EPSCs in glutamate isolation i.e. in cholinergic and GABA receptor antagonists (Figure 7A). After alignment, the number of spikes occurring before the stimulus entered the glutamate receptive field (~50 ms in the preferred direction) was considered as the early phase responses. These spikes were completely blocked by cholinergic receptor antagonists (Figure 7A, Figure 7—figure supplement 1). The peak phase was estimated as a ~ 50 ms region close to the peak firing rate in the preferred direction (Figure 7A).

Population data have been expressed as mean ± SEM and are indicated in the figure legend along with the number of samples. Student’s t test was used to compare values under different conditions and the differences were considered significant when p0.05, unless otherwise noted in the figure legend.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Gautam B Awatramani, Email: gautam@uvic.ca.

Fred Rieke, University of Washington, United States.

Ronald L Calabrese, Emory University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grant:

  • Canadian Institutes of Health Research 159444 to Gautam B Awatramani.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Figures 2, 3, 4A-D, 6A.

Data curation, Writing—review and editing, All experiments in Figures 4E-F, 5,6B-C & 7A-F.

Data curation, Writing—review and editing, All modeling in Figures 6 and 7.

Methodology, Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All procedures were performed in accordance with the Canadian Council on Animal Care and approved by the Animal Care Committee (protocol 2016-015) of the University of Victoria.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.019

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.

References

  1. Amthor FR, Grzywacz NM, Merwine DK. Extra-receptive-field motion facilitation in on-off directionally selective ganglion cells of the rabbit retina. Visual Neuroscience. 1996;13:303–309. doi: 10.1017/S0952523800007549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ariel M, Daw NW. Pharmacological analysis of directionally sensitive rabbit retinal ganglion cells. The Journal of Physiology. 1982;324:161–185. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1982.sp014105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barlow HB, Levick WR. The mechanism of directionally selective units in rabbit's retina. The Journal of Physiology. 1965;178:477–504. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1965.sp007638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Briggman KL, Helmstaedter M, Denk W. Wiring specificity in the direction-selectivity circuit of the retina. Nature. 2011;471:183–188. doi: 10.1038/nature09818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brombas A, Kalita-de Croft S, Cooper-Williams EJ, Williams SR. Dendro-dendritic cholinergic excitation controls dendritic spike initiation in retinal ganglion cells. Nature Communications. 2017;8:15683. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chen Q, Pei Z, Koren D, Wei W. Stimulus-dependent recruitment of lateral inhibition underlies retinal direction selectivity. eLife. 2016;5:e21053. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chiao CC, Masland RH. Starburst cells nondirectionally facilitate the responses of direction-selective retinal ganglion cells. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:10509–10513. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10509.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cruz-Martín A, El-Danaf RN, Osakada F, Sriram B, Dhande OS, Nguyen PL, Callaway EM, Ghosh A, Huberman AD. A dedicated circuit links direction-selective retinal ganglion cells to the primary visual cortex. Nature. 2014;507:358–361. doi: 10.1038/nature12989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Ding H, Smith RG, Poleg-Polsky A, Diamond JS, Briggman KL. Species-specific wiring for direction selectivity in the mammalian retina. Nature. 2016;535:105–110. doi: 10.1038/nature18609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Euler T, Detwiler PB, Denk W. Directionally selective calcium signals in dendrites of starburst amacrine cells. Nature. 2002;418:845–852. doi: 10.1038/nature00931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fransen JW, Borghuis BG. Temporally diverse excitation generates Direction-Selective responses in ON- and OFF-Type retinal starburst amacrine cells. Cell Reports. 2017;18:1356–1365. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.01.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Fried SI, Münch TA, Werblin FS. Mechanisms and circuitry underlying directional selectivity in the retina. Nature. 2002;420:411–414. doi: 10.1038/nature01179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fried SI, Münch TA, Werblin FS. Directional selectivity is formed at multiple levels by laterally offset inhibition in the rabbit retina. Neuron. 2005;46:117–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gavrikov KE, Nilson JE, Dmitriev AV, Zucker CL, Mangel SC. Dendritic compartmentalization of chloride cotransporters underlies directional responses of starburst amacrine cells in retina. PNAS. 2006;103:18793–18798. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604551103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Grzywacz NM, Tootle JS, Amthor FR. Is the input to a GABAergic or cholinergic synapse the sole asymmetry in rabbit's retinal directional selectivity? Visual Neuroscience. 1997;14:39–54. doi: 10.1017/S0952523800008749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Grzywacz NM, Merwine DK, Amthor FR. Complementary roles of two excitatory pathways in retinal directional selectivity. Visual Neuroscience. 1998;15:1119–1127. doi: 10.1017/S0952523898156109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hausselt SE, Euler T, Detwiler PB, Denk W. A dendrite-autonomous mechanism for direction selectivity in retinal starburst amacrine cells. PLOS Biology. 2007;5:e185. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hillier D, Fiscella M, Drinnenberg A, Trenholm S, Rompani SB, Raics Z, Katona G, Juettner J, Hierlemann A, Rozsa B, Roska B. Causal evidence for retina-dependent and -independent visual motion computations in mouse cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:960–968. doi: 10.1038/nn.4566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hines ML, Carnevale NT. The NEURON simulation environment. Neural Computation. 1997;9:1179–1209. doi: 10.1162/neco.1997.9.6.1179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kim JS, Greene MJ, Zlateski A, Lee K, Richardson M, Turaga SC, Purcaro M, Balkam M, Robinson A, Behabadi BF, Campos M, Denk W, Seung HS, EyeWirers Space-time wiring specificity supports direction selectivity in the retina. Nature. 2014;509:331–336. doi: 10.1038/nature13240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kittila CA, Massey SC. Pharmacology of directionally selective ganglion cells in the rabbit retina. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1997;77:675–689. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.2.675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Koch C, Poggio T, Torres V. Retinal ganglion cells: a functional interpretation of dendritic morphology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1982;298:227–263. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1982.0084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kostadinov D, Sanes JR. Protocadherin-dependent dendritic self-avoidance regulates neural connectivity and circuit function. eLife. 2015;4:e08964. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kuo RI, Wu GK. The generation of direction selectivity in the auditory system. Neuron. 2012;73:1016–1027. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lee S, Kim K, Zhou ZJ. Role of ACh-GABA cotransmission in detecting image motion and motion direction. Neuron. 2010;68:1159–1172. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lee S, Zhou ZJ. The synaptic mechanism of direction selectivity in distal processes of starburst amacrine cells. Neuron. 2006;51:787–799. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Li YT, Liu BH, Chou XL, Zhang LI, Tao HW. Strengthening of direction selectivity by broadly tuned and spatiotemporally slightly offset inhibition in mouse visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25:2466–2477. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lien AD, Scanziani M. Cortical direction selectivity emerges at convergence of thalamic synapses. Nature. 2018;558:80–86. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0148-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mauss AS, Vlasits A, Borst A, Feller M. Visual circuits for direction selectivity. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2017;40:211–230. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Münch TA, Werblin FS. Symmetric interactions within a homogeneous starburst cell network can lead to robust asymmetries in dendrites of starburst amacrine cells. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2006;96:471–477. doi: 10.1152/jn.00628.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. O'Malley DM, Masland RH. Co-release of acetylcholine and gamma-aminobutyric acid by a retinal neuron. PNAS. 1989;86:3414–3418. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.9.3414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Oesch N, Euler T, Taylor WR. Direction-selective dendritic action potentials in rabbit retina. Neuron. 2005;47:739–750. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Pack CC, Born RT. Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain. Nature. 2001;409:1040–1042. doi: 10.1038/35059085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Park SJ, Kim IJ, Looger LL, Demb JB, Borghuis BG. Excitatory synaptic inputs to mouse on-off direction-selective retinal ganglion cells lack direction tuning. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:3976–3981. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5017-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Pei Z, Chen Q, Koren D, Giammarinaro B, Acaron Ledesma H, Wei W. Conditional Knock-Out of vesicular GABA transporter gene from starburst amacrine cells reveals the contributions of multiple synaptic mechanisms underlying direction selectivity in the retina. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:13219–13232. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0933-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Poleg-Polsky A, Ding H, Diamond JS. Functional compartmentalization within starburst amacrine cell dendrites in the retina. Cell Reports. 2018;22:2898–2908. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.02.064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Poleg-Polsky A, Diamond JS. Imperfect space clamp permits electrotonic interactions between inhibitory and excitatory synaptic conductances, distorting voltage clamp recordings. PLOS ONE. 2011;6:e19463. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Poleg-Polsky A, Diamond JS. NMDA receptors multiplicatively scale visual signals and enhance directional motion discrimination in retinal ganglion cells. Neuron. 2016;89:1277–1290. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Rivlin-Etzion M, Wei W, Feller MB. Visual stimulation reverses the directional preference of direction-selective retinal ganglion cells. Neuron. 2012;76:518–525. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Saul AB, Carras PL, Humphrey AL. Temporal properties of inputs to direction-selective neurons in monkey V1. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;94:282–294. doi: 10.1152/jn.00868.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Schachter MJ, Oesch N, Smith RG, Taylor WR. Dendritic spikes amplify the synaptic signal to enhance detection of motion in a simulation of the direction-selective ganglion cell. PLOS Computational Biology. 2010;6:e1000899. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Sethuramanujam S, McLaughlin AJ, deRosenroll G, Hoggarth A, Schwab DJ, Awatramani GB. A central role for mixed acetylcholine/GABA transmission in direction coding in the retina. Neuron. 2016;90:1243–1256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Sethuramanujam S, Yao X, deRosenroll G, Briggman KL, Field GD, Awatramani GB. "Silent" NMDA Synapses Enhance Motion Sensitivity in a Mature Retinal Circuit. Neuron. 2017;96:1099–1111. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.09.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sivyer B, van Wyk M, Vaney DI, Taylor WR. Synaptic inputs and timing underlying the velocity tuning of direction-selective ganglion cells in rabbit retina. The Journal of Physiology. 2010;588:3243–3253. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.192716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sivyer B, Williams SR. Direction selectivity is computed by active dendritic integration in retinal ganglion cells. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1848–1856. doi: 10.1038/nn.3565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sun W, Tan Z, Mensh BD, Ji N. Thalamus provides layer 4 of primary visual cortex with orientation- and direction-tuned inputs. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:308–315. doi: 10.1038/nn.4196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Taylor WR, He S, Levick WR, Vaney DI. Dendritic computation of direction selectivity by retinal ganglion cells. Science. 2000;289:2347–2350. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5488.2347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Taylor WR, Smith RG. The role of starburst amacrine cells in visual signal processing. Visual Neuroscience. 2012;29:73–81. doi: 10.1017/S0952523811000393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Taylor WR, Vaney DI. Diverse synaptic mechanisms generate direction selectivity in the rabbit retina. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:7712–7720. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-17-07712.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Thiele A, Distler C, Korbmacher H, Hoffmann KP. Contribution of inhibitory mechanisms to direction selectivity and response normalization in macaque middle temporal area. PNAS. 2004;101:9810–9815. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0307754101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Torre V, Poggio TA. A synaptic mechanism possibly underlying directional selectity to motion. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 1978;202:400–416. [Google Scholar]
  52. Trenholm S, Schwab DJ, Balasubramanian V, Awatramani GB. Lag normalization in an electrically coupled neural network. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:154–156. doi: 10.1038/nn.3308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Trenholm S, McLaughlin AJ, Schwab DJ, Turner MH, Smith RG, Rieke F, Awatramani GB. Nonlinear dendritic integration of electrical and chemical synaptic inputs drives fine-scale correlations. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1759–1766. doi: 10.1038/nn.3851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Tukker JJ, Taylor WR, Smith RG. Direction selectivity in a model of the starburst amacrine cell. Visual Neuroscience. 2004;21:611–625. doi: 10.1017/S0952523804214109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Vaney DI, Sivyer B, Taylor WR. Direction selectivity in the retina: symmetry and asymmetry in structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13:194–208. doi: 10.1038/nrn3165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Vlasits AL, Bos R, Morrie RD, Fortuny C, Flannery JG, Feller MB, Rivlin-Etzion M. Visual stimulation switches the polarity of excitatory input to starburst amacrine cells. Neuron. 2014;83:1172–1184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Vlasits AL, Morrie RD, Tran-Van-Minh A, Bleckert A, Gainer CF, DiGregorio DA, Feller MB. A role for synaptic input distribution in a dendritic computation of motion direction in the retina. Neuron. 2016;89:1317–1330. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Wilent WB, Contreras D. Dynamics of excitation and inhibition underlying stimulus selectivity in rat somatosensory cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1364–1370. doi: 10.1038/nn1545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Wilson DE, Scholl B, Fitzpatrick D. Differential tuning of excitation and inhibition shapes direction selectivity in ferret visual cortex. Nature. 2018;560:97–101. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0354-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Yang G, Masland RH. Direct visualization of the dendritic and receptive fields of directionally selective retinal ganglion cells. Science. 1992;258:1949–1952. doi: 10.1126/science.1470920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ye CQ, Poo MM, Dan Y, Zhang XH. Synaptic mechanisms of direction selectivity in primary auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:1861–1868. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3088-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Yonehara K, Balint K, Noda M, Nagel G, Bamberg E, Roska B. Spatially asymmetric reorganization of inhibition establishes a motion-sensitive circuit. Nature. 2011;469:407–410. doi: 10.1038/nature09711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Yonehara K, Farrow K, Ghanem A, Hillier D, Balint K, Teixeira M, Jüttner J, Noda M, Neve RL, Conzelmann KK, Roska B. The first stage of cardinal direction selectivity is localized to the dendrites of retinal ganglion cells. Neuron. 2013;79:1078–1085. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Yoshida K, Watanabe D, Ishikane H, Tachibana M, Pastan I, Nakanishi S. A key role of starburst amacrine cells in originating retinal directional selectivity and optokinetic eye movement. Neuron. 2001;30:771–780. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00316-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Zhang LI, Tan AY, Schreiner CE, Merzenich MM. Topography and synaptic shaping of direction selectivity in primary auditory cortex. Nature. 2003;424:201–205. doi: 10.1038/nature01796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Fred Rieke1
Reviewed by: Fred Rieke2

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Retinal direction selectivity in the absence of asymmetric starburst amacrine cell responses" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Fred Rieke as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Ronald Calabrese as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

The reviewers were all excited about the potential of this work to transform how we think about coding of direction in the retina. Several substantive points arose in the reviews, and these were reinforced in consultation among the reviewers. The most salient of those are briefly described below, with more detail in the individual reviews.

1) Relation between space and time. How a spatial offset gets converted to a difference in timing deserves more attention. In particular, whether (and if so how) such a mechanism can account for the speed invariance of directional tuning should be explored. This conversion is central to the proposed mechanism, and hence clearly establishing and describing it is critical to the paper.

2) Presentation. The presentation of the work needs to be improved in several ways. This includes some specific figures, and accessibility to a non-specialist. See the reviews below for specifics and suggestions.

Reviewer #1:

This paper provides new insights into the computations underlying directionally-selective responses of retinal ganglion cells. A compelling explanation for directional selectivity has emerged from work from several laboratories over the past 15 years. This picture is based on spatial asymmetries in excitatory and inhibitory inputs to DSGCs, and the recruitment of stronger inhibitory input for null direction compared to preferred direction motion. The current paper suggests that this picture is incomplete, and that a second mechanism also contributes to directional selectivity. The new proposed mechanism is based on differences in the timing of excitatory and inhibitory input in preferred and null directions. The paper is clearly written and the conclusions generally are well supported. There are several issues, however, that I think need to be strengthened:

1) Timing of excitatory and inhibitory input (Figure 3). The similarity of the relative timing of excitatory and inhibitory input in the absence of ACh signaling and the corresponding slowing of excitatory input are important parts of the paper. Yet the data for these points is not presented in a particularly convincing way. First, it would be helpful to see the excitatory inputs in the same cell before and after applying HEX. Second, the relative timing of excitatory and inhibitory inputs is difficult to appreciate given the differences in scale (particularly true in Figure 3G, but also to a lesser extent in Figure 3A and D). Can you add a normalized version of those traces to facilitate a temporal comparison?

2) Variability in magnitude of inhibitory input. The measured inhibitory inputs vary widely in amplitude (e.g. Figure 1E) – even when recorded for similar stimuli. This raises concerns about experiments in which comparisons are made across recording conditions or across mice. Do you have an explanation for the variability in inhibitory input? Some discussion of that would be helpful.

3) Ch2R activation. It is not clear from the main text how Ch2R activation was used to produce motion signals in the SACs. This is slightly clearer from reading the methods, but only filling in some retina-specific details about the orderly spatial arrangement of SACs. This should get described clearly in the Results with a non-retina reader in mind.

4) Details of modeling. The model of Figure 4 needs to be described in considerably more detail. One specific issue is that it is not clear how much of the model is constrained from experiment. A second issue is how spikes are generated in the model.

Reviewer #2:

The manuscript by Hanson et al. reports a major refinement of the well-studied model of the direction-selectivity (DS) circuit in the mammalian retina. The key insight is that the temporal asymmetry between excitation (both ACh and gluamate-driven) and inhibition alone can drive DS in RGCs, even without a contribution from the DS computation of the starburst dendrites, which causes null direction inhibition to be larger than that in the preferred direction.

Despite a huge amount of previous work on this circuit and hints of this phenomenon in previous work (which the authors cite), this is an important contribution. In general, the manuscript is well written, and the data are clear and compelling.

My major concern is that the authors need to do a better job connecting the spatial offset that underlies this phenomenon to its description as a temporal offset. The experiments presented in Figure 2B explore a large range of speeds and show that the temporal offset DS computation is robust. How is this the case? This deserves more exploration. If the E vs. I RFs are spatially offset, as suggested, and that is the whole story, the mechanism should only work over a reasonably small range of speeds. Do some speeds depend on the ACh to GABA offset and others depend on the glutamate to GABA offset? Since the ACh RF is larger, it should be tuned to higher speeds.

A nice experiment to clear this up would be a careful measurement of the RF of each of the 3 synaptic components (ACh, glutamate, and GABA) with small spots of light. The authors have the genetic and pharamacological tools to make these measurements. With these spatial RFs, one could construct a simple linear model of how they could give you a temporal offset and over what speeds the temporal offset would be effective in preventing null direction spikes.

Reviewer #3:

The paper by Hanson and colleagues investigates, and answers, two central questions concerning direction selectivity in the mammalian retina: a) what is the mechanism underlying direction selectivity in starburst amacrine cells?, and b) what is the mechanism underlying direction selectivity in the direction selective ganglion cells? In brief, knowledge about the first question allows the authors to make starburst amacrine cells non-directional and to investigate the source of direction selectivity in ganglion cells. Surprisingly, they find ganglion cells to remain directionally selective when fed with non-directional signals from amacrine cells. As demonstrated by further analysis, the relative timing of excitatory and inhibitory input from amacrine cells is sufficient for direction selectivity, even when the amplitudes of these signals are the same for preferred and null direction motion.

To me, this is an extremely important piece of work, which will have a strong impact on the field. My only concern is the way the data are presented: here I see room for improvement.

Quite in general, the two questions mentioned above need to be better separated and presented as such. For someone not in the field, the discovery about the origin of direction selectivity in amacrine cells is almost invisible. The authors should clearly mark their findings as such and discuss what kind of model for amacrine cell direction selectivity they favor and which can be firmly excluded. As an example, the way I understand the data, one can now exclude a pure electrotonic summation of postsynaptic excitation as a source for direction selectivity: without inhibitory amacrine-amacrine synapses, no direction selectivity remains in response to channelrhodopsin-stimulation. Along the same line, the authors need to discuss the source of the timing difference between the excitatory and the inhibitory inputs to ganglion cells and present a network model, at least a scheme, which explains their findings. Lastly, the role of cholinergic transmission is only explicitly mentioned at the end of the manuscript: I would put the mysterious role of acetylcholine in this circuit right in the beginning and get back to it at the end.

As another general comment, the manuscript in its current form is rather inaccessible to non-specialists: the circuit needs to be introduced and current hypotheses need to be explained. Only then can the reader fully appreciate the astonishing results. The current manuscript seems to be written as a short report, but there is no need for that: it should have full length with more figures.

Once the manuscript is structured and extended in the way outlined above, this should make a beautiful publication and a major contribution to our understanding of the mechanism underlying direction selectivity in the retina. Congratulations!

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for submitting your article "Retinal direction selectivity in the absence of asymmetric starburst amacrine cell responses" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been re-reviewed by Fred Rieke as the Reviewing Editor, and the evaluation has been overseen by Ronald Calabrese as the Senior Editor.

This is a revision of a paper describing a new mechanism for direction selectivity in the retina. The authors did a good job responding to the first set of reviews, and the paper has improved considerably. I think there are a few additional changes that could help improve the clarity of the paper for a general audience, as detailed below.

Figure 1: This figure sets up the paper nicely and helps clarify the issues to be tested. I think this could be further improved in two ways. First, it would be nice to include a simple anatomical diagram in the figure showing connections between starburst amacrines and DS ganglion cells. This could include the paracrine nature of ACh release and the asymmetric GABAergic synapses. Second, there are several places in the text in which referring back to the figure could be helpful to remind a reader of the basic model.

Subsection “Retinal direction selectivity in the absence of asymmetric starburst amacrine cell responses”, second paragraph: the transition here is abrupt. It would also help to refer back to Figure 1B here.

Subsection “Differential transmission of ACh/GABA under physiological conditions”, second paragraph: the different spatial offsets that are important could get laid out more clearly.

Subsection “Complementary roles for E/I offset and amplitude based DS mechanisms”, second paragraph: this could get elaborated a bit as it is not immediately clear how the broadening of tuning coincides with an increase in reliability of estimated direction.

eLife. 2019 Feb 4;8:e42392. doi: 10.7554/eLife.42392.022

Author response


The reviewers were all excited about the potential of this work to transform how we think about coding of direction in the retina. Several substantive points arose in the reviews, and these were reinforced in consultation among the reviewers. The most salient of those are briefly described below, with more detail in the individual reviews.

1) Relation between space and time. How a spatial offset gets converted to a difference in timing deserves more attention. In particular, whether (and if so how) such a mechanism can account for the speed invariance of directional tuning should be explored. This conversion is central to the proposed mechanism, and hence clearly establishing and describing it is critical to the paper.

2) Presentation. The presentation of the work needs to be improved in several ways. This includes some specific figures, and accessibility to a non-specialist. See the reviews below for specifics and suggestions.

We thank the reviewers for their constructive comments and enthusiasm. We fully agree with the consensus critiques and have taken substantive steps to address the two major concerns outlined above.

First, to clarify relationship between space and time we have conducted several new experiments:

– We examined E/I offsets at different velocities

– We tested the role of cholinergic excitation in mediating offsets at different velocities using pharmacology

– We constructed a computer model DSGC, which was driven with a fixed E/I ratio, but variable timing offsets (according to our experimental results mentioned above).

Results from these new experiments and analyses help define the role of E/I offsets over a range of velocities and clarify how spatial offsets get converted into timing differences.

Second, to improve the general presentation we have thoroughly re-worked the manuscript. We now include:

– A simple schematic of the DS circuit mechanisms has been presented upfront to make the proposed hypotheses clear and accessible to the non-specialist.

– As suggested, we have expanded the paper from a short to a long format (4 figures to 7 figures)

– We now have emphasized both the major advances i.e. DS mechanisms in starburst dendrites as well as DS mechanisms at the level of DSGCs

– As suggested by the reviewer, we now discuss the role of acetylcholine in the DS computation sooner in the Introduction

– Figures have been modified to better display the measured E/I offsets and the effects of cholinergic blockers (see responses to specific queries below).

– A broader discussion of parallel mechanisms generating direction selectivity in visual cortex and other areas of brain has been added.

Overall, we feel these changes have strengthened the conclusions of this study, and once again thank all the reviewers for the insightful feedback.

Reviewer #1:

[…] The paper is clearly written and the conclusions generally are well supported. There are several issues, however, that I think need to be strengthened:

1) Timing of excitatory and inhibitory input (Figure 3). The similarity of the relative timing of excitatory and inhibitory input in the absence of ACh signaling and the corresponding slowing of excitatory input are important parts of the paper. Yet the data for these points is not presented in a particularly convincing way. First, it would be helpful to see the excitatory inputs in the same cell before and after applying HEX. Second, the relative timing of excitatory and inhibitory inputs is difficult to appreciate given the differences in scale (particularly true in Figure 3G, but also to a lesser extent in Figure 3A and D). Can you add a normalized version of those traces to facilitate a temporal comparison?

We have taken several steps to better characterize and more clearly present E/I timing differences, as described below:

– We have now described how onsets of excitation and inhibition were measured and marked the onsets on current traces, and shaded E/I windows (Figure 4)

– As suggested, we now show EPSCs/IPSCs recorded from the same DSGC, before and after application of HEX (now Figure 5)

– We also quantify the effect of HEX on E/I offsets across 8 stimulus directions, and demonstrate that excitation is consistently delayed by ~25ms (Figure 5)

– We performed new control experiments to ensure that delays observed in HEX do not arise from voltage-clamp errors associated with large inhibitory conductances (Figure 5—figure supplement 1).

– We also measured E/I offsets in the presence of HEX across a range of velocities (Figure 6B)

– To facilitate comparison of the EPSC timing differences in control and HEX, we have added the normalized versions of the EPSCs in Figure 5 and Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

2) Variability in magnitude of inhibitory input. The measured inhibitory inputs vary widely in amplitude (e.g. Figure 1E) – even when recorded for similar stimuli. This raises concerns about experiments in which comparisons are made across recording conditions or across mice. Do you have an explanation for the variability in inhibitory input? Some discussion of that would be helpful.

The variability in the IPSCs (now in Figure 2E) could arise from differences in ChR2 expression levels and/or run down of ChR2 responses over the course of the recording session (which could last up to 8 hours). However, within a single cell we found that the responses and the directional tuning was stable across trials and hence we chose to include the weaker responses in our dataset. These points have been included in the Results section.

3) Ch2R activation. It is not clear from the main text how Ch2R activation was used to produce motion signals in the SACs. This is slightly clearer from reading the methods, but only filling in some retina-specific details about the orderly spatial arrangement of SACs. This should get described clearly in the Results with a non-retina reader in mind.

In the revised manuscript, we better describe the DS circuit in the Introduction, highlighting the inhibitory connections between anti-parallel SAC dendrites.

We have also included details in the Results section regarding the methods used for ChR2 activation and how this technique was used to manipulate the starburst mechanisms of direction selectivity.

4) Details of modeling. The model of Figure 4 needs to be described in considerably more detail. One specific issue is that it is not clear how much of the model is constrained from experiment.

In the revised methods, the computational model has been presented in greater detail. In the model, the kinetics of the synaptic inputs were modeled based on experimentally measured miniature events, and as such produce macroscopic inhibitory and excitatory currents that are well-matched to those measured experimentally at the soma. These are illustrated in Figure 7—figure supplement 2. The code for the model has also been made publicly available (https://github.com/geoffder/Spatial-Offset-DSGC-NEURON-Model).

A second issue is how spikes are generated in the model.

In our model, spikes originate in the active dendritic arbours of the DSGC. Dendrites and the soma were endowed with voltage-gated sodium and potassium conductances. This enabled local synaptic inputs to generate dendritic spikes, which forward propagate and generate full-blown somatic action potentials with high fidelity, as observed experimentally (Sivyer et al., 2013). This information has been included in the Materials and methods section.

Reviewer #2:

[…] In general, the manuscript is well written, and the data are clear and compelling.

My major concern is that the authors need to do a better job connecting the spatial offset that underlies this phenomenon to its description as a temporal offset. The experiments presented in Figure 2B explore a large range of speeds and show that the temporal offset DS computation is robust. How is this the case? This deserves more exploration. If the E vs. I RFs are spatially offset, as suggested, and that is the whole story, the mechanism should only work over a reasonably small range of speeds. Do some speeds depend on the ACh to GABA offset and others depend on the glutamate to GABA offset? Since the ACh RF is larger, it should be tuned to higher speeds.

We took several steps to do a better job of relating spatial and temporal offsets:

– We examined E/I offsets at different velocities (new Figure 6B)

– We tested the role of cholinergic excitation in mediating offsets at different velocities using pharmacology (new Figure 6B)

– In the insets in Figure 6B, we plot the spatial offsets (estimated as velocity * temporal offset) and show it to be relatively constant across velocity

– We constructed a computer model DSGC, which was driven with a fixed E/I ratio but variable timing offsets (according to our experimental results mentioned above). As the reviewer suspects, the direction selectivity was stable for most velocities, except the slowest velocity tested (Figure 6). However, in real DSGCs, DSI was stable across velocity indicating additional mechanisms may exist to compensate for the expected deterioration of DS arising from spatial offsets. We have added these points to the Discussion section. (Also see the response to reviewer 1’s first question.)

– Also, while it is true that ACh RF is larger, it is dynamic. Only one half is activated for every direction, since the release of ACh is presumably directional.

A nice experiment to clear this up would be a careful measurement of the RF of each of the 3 synaptic components (ACh, glutamate, and GABA) with small spots of light. The authors have the genetic and pharamacological tools to make these measurements. With these spatial RFs, one could construct a simple linear model of how they could give you a temporal offset and over what speeds the temporal offset would be effective in preventing null direction spikes.

RF measurements using small spots might not reveal the spatial extent of cholinergic excitation accurately, as static stimuli do not optimally stimulate starburst dendrites. In fact, such RF measurements made in the rabbit DSGCs failed to reveal offset cholinergic inputs (Fried et al., 2005; Yang and Masland, 1994). Thus, we felt the simple mapping technique was not likely to be fruitful. We have included these points in the Discussion. On the other hand, our biophysical model using experimentally driven synaptic inputs (in response to moving stimuli) nicely recapitulates the DSGC’s response properties, including the temporal evolution of the DS tuning (Figure 7).

Reviewer #3:

[…] To me, this is an extremely important piece of work, which will have a strong impact on the field. My only concern is the way the data are presented: here I see room for improvement.

Quite in general, the two questions mentioned above need to be better separated and presented as such. For someone not in the field, the discovery about the origin of direction selectivity in amacrine cells is almost invisible. The authors should clearly mark their findings as such and discuss what kind of model for amacrine cell direction selectivity they favor and which can be firmly excluded. As an example, the way I understand the data, one can now exclude a pure electrotonic summation of postsynaptic excitation as a source for direction selectivity: without inhibitory amacrine-amacrine synapses, no direction selectivity remains in response to channelrhodopsin-stimulation.

In the revised manuscript we now make it clear in the Introduction that this study makes two major advancements; one relating to DS in amacrine cell dendrites, the other to DS in ganglion cells

The reviewer is correct in stating that dendritic filtering is unlikely to play an important role. However, we choose to interpret the data conservatively for the following reasons. First, it is not clear how ChR2 stimulates starburst release. It is possible that the artificial activation of starbursts using optogenetics does not recruit dendritic voltage-gated channels that may underlie direction selectivity. Second, the removal of GABAergic inhibition between starbursts may hinder or occlude intrinsic DS mechanisms. Third, it is also possible that the limited set of stimuli (moving spots) which we utilized were not optimally designed to recruit cell intrinsic mechanisms. These points are made in the Discussion section.

Along the same line, the authors need to discuss the source of the timing difference between the excitatory and the inhibitory inputs to ganglion cells and present a network model, at least a scheme, which explains their findings.

As mentioned previously, we now present schematics of the different possible configurations of the DS circuit and highlight how offset cholinergic excitation might generate E/I timing differences (Figure 1).

Lastly, the role of cholinergic transmission is only explicitly mentioned at the end of the manuscript: I would put the mysterious role of acetyl choline in this circuit right in the beginning and get back to it at the end.

As another general comment, the manuscript in its current form is rather inaccessible to non-specialists: the circuit needs to be introduced and current hypotheses need to be explained. Only then can the reader fully appreciate the astonishing results. The current manuscript seems to be written as a short report, but there is no need for that: it should have full length with more figures.

Thanks for these suggestions. We have now introduced the role of ACh earlier and reformatted the manuscript to a full-length form with 7 figures (instead of the original 4). We hope in the revised form it is more accessible to the non-specialist.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

This is a revision of a paper describing a new mechanism for direction selectivity in the retina. The authors did a good job responding to the first set of reviews, and the paper has improved considerably. I think there are a few additional changes that could help improve the clarity of the paper for a general audience, as detailed below.

Figure 1: This figure sets up the paper nicely and helps clarify the issues to be tested. I think this could be further improved in two ways. First, it would be nice to include a simple anatomical diagram in the figure showing connections between starburst amacrines and DS ganglion cells. This could include the paracrine nature of ACh release and the asymmetric GABAergic synapses. Second, there are several places in the text in which referring back to the figure could be helpful to remind a reader of the basic model.

We have now added two new diagrams to illustrate the DSGC circuitry (Figure 1A) and the starburst-DSGC connectivity (Figure 1B), to better understand why paracrine ACh has been postulated. We have referred to these diagrams wherever appropriate.

Subsection “Retinal direction selectivity in the absence of asymmetric starburst amacrine cell responses”, second paragraph: the transition here is abrupt. It would also help to refer back to Figure 1B here.

We have added the following statement to smooth the transition: “We hypothesized that asymmetries in the timing of E and I arising from the differential functional starburst-DSGC connectivity gives rise to a parallel DS mechanism (Figure 1D).”

Subsection “Differential transmission of ACh/GABA under physiological conditions”, second paragraph: the different spatial offsets that are important could get laid out more clearly.

We have re-worked this section and moved it to the Discussion. It now reads:

“We found that the degree which cholinergic inputs advance the response is proportional to stimulus velocity, corresponding to a fixed spatial offset ~ 30 µm. […] Thus, the extent to which cholinergic inputs expand the receptive field of the DSGC is well predicted by the input/output properties of starburst dendrites.”

Subsection “Complementary roles for E/I offset and amplitude based DS mechanisms”, second paragraph: this could get elaborated a bit as it is not immediately clear how the broadening of tuning coincides with an increase in reliability of estimated direction.

We now offer a possible explanation for the changes in variability: “While the direction encoded remained constant throughout the response, the tuning curve broadened as the response approached its peak, resulting in a marked decrease in the DSI (Figure 7A-D). […] It is also important to note that the broadening of the tuning curve did not arise simply from a thresholding effect (i.e. an iceberg effect), as the last spikes at the tail end of the response were not sharply tuned (data not shown).”

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 2—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 2A-F.
    elife-42392-fig2-data1.xlsx (376.7KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.004
    Figure 3—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 3A-D.
    elife-42392-fig3-data1.xlsx (759.3KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.006
    Figure 4—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 4A-D.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.008
    Figure 4—source data 2. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 4E-H.
    elife-42392-fig4-data2.xlsx (488.7KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.009
    Figure 5—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 5.
    elife-42392-fig5-data1.xlsx (932.5KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.016
    Figure 6—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 6B-C.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.018
    Figure 7—source data 1. Included is an exccel file containing the data presented in Figure 7.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.013
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.42392.019

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES