Abstract
An emerging focus on the geometry of representational structures is advancing a variety of areas in social perception, including social categorization, emotion perception, and trait impressions. Here, we review recent studies adopting a representational geometry approach, and argue that important advances in social perception can be gained by triangulating on the structure of representations via three levels of analysis: neuroimaging, behavioral measures, and computational modeling. Among other uses, this approach permits broad and comprehensive tests of how bottom-up facial features and visual processes as well as top-down social cognitive factors and conceptual processes shape perceptions of social categories, emotion, and personality traits. Although such work is only in its infancy, a focus on corroborating representational geometry across modalities is allowing researchers to use multiple levels of analysis to constrain theoretical models in social perception. This approach holds promise to further our understanding of the multiply determined nature of social perception and its neural basis.
When we encounter others, we perceive their social categories, emotional state, and even personality traits. Across social perception lies underlying representations – of social categories, emotions, traits – that in turn drive perceptual judgments and behavior. Here, we propose that important advances in social perception can be gained by triangulating on the structure of representations derived from multiple levels of analysis. Here we focus on three such levels of analysis: neuroimaging, behavioral measures, and computational modeling. We will show how an emerging focus on the geometry of representational structures is advancing a variety of areas in social perception, including social categorization, emotion perception, and trait impressions.
Social perception as movement through multidimensional space
Computational models of social perception [1-3] assume that any given representation (e.g., the social category “male”) is reflected by a unique pattern distributed over a population of nodes. It is the distributed pattern, dynamically re-instantiated in every new instance, that serves as the unique ‘code’ for a social category, stereotype, or trait. Such models are consistent with multi-cell recordings, which have shown that the communal activity of a population of neurons – a specific pattern of firing rates – provides the ‘code’ for various kinds of sensory and abstract cognitive information [i.e., a 'population code'; 4].
We can conceive of representations in social perception (e.g., social categories, emotions, traits) as points in a multidimensional space. The space can be measured using a variety of different modalities, such that the dimensions consist of neurons, fMRI voxels, or nodes in a computational model, such as a neural-network model. The specific representational structure can be estimated through patterns of activation from these different modalities, as well as through behavioral data. Although these multidimensional spaces from different modalities may be radically different in an absolute sense, it is valuable to estimate the extent to which a shared representational geometry (i.e., the pairwise distances among representations) is preserved. This analytic approach – representational similarity analysis [RSA; 5,6] – can inform our understanding not only of how representational spaces underlying social perception manifest at the neural level, but also help address fundamental questions about how social perception is shaped by both relevant bottom-up visual and top-down social cognitive processes.
In certain neural-network models and in the actual brain, neural-representational patterns operate as ‘attractors’, such that a neural system is attracted to complete those patterns when presented with a stimulus, allowing the system to descend from a high-energy state where the neuronal pattern is rapidly fluctuating to a lower-energy steady state in which the representational pattern comes to stabilize, i.e., an attractor state [7]. If the neuronal system’s state were imagined as a ball, the process of descending into an attractor state is analogous to a ball’s compulsion to roll down a hill, reflecting the increasing completion of the neural-representational pattern. Such attractor dynamics have long been observed in local populations of neurons in actual cortex, and serve as an intrinsic pattern-completion process allowing neuronal patterns to serve as stored representations [7].
Computational models.
Various forms of computational models may be used to corroborate representational geometries in social perception, including neural-network models. Imagine a system of 100 neurons, with two unique patterns of activation for the emotion categories Angry and Happy; each pattern is therefore a point in a 100-dimensional space. The system, once presented with a person’s face, will move through 100-dimensional space toward the Angry and Happy attractor state. Thus, at any moment, the neuronal population’s proximity to the Angry and Happy attractor state in 100-dimensional space can be said to reflect to what extent that category representation is activated. Just as a ball’s energy is higher at the top of a hill relative to resting on the ground, each point in neural state space has an associated energy level, which determines the trajectory of where the system will go. The system will gravitate toward energy minima, which are the stored representational patterns, i.e., attractor states [7]. If we were to project this 100-dimensional neural state space onto a more intuitive two-dimensional space, visualizing the energy levels at various points in the space, we can reveal these Angry and Happy category representations in the system (Fig. 1).
Behavior.
To measure these attractor dynamics during social perception, researchers have recently leveraged response-directed hand movement using computer mouse-tracking [8]. By examining how the hand settles into a response over time, and may be partially pulled toward other potential responses, mouse-tracking has charted out the real-time dynamics through which social categories, emotions, stereotypes, attitudes, and traits activate and resolve over hundreds of milliseconds [8,9]. At any given moment of time in a mouse-tracking task, the hand’s proximity to a given response (e.g., a social category, emotion, or trait) on the two-dimensional space of the screen – like the proximity of the system to a given attractor in high-dimensional neural state space – may index the extent to which that social category, emotion, or trait representation is currently activated [8]. Thus, the distance the hand travels toward an unselected response option (before arriving at the selected option) can be taken as a proxy – using the physical distance in two-dimensional space – of the distance between that representation and a perceiver’s current state in higher-dimensional neural state space (Fig. 1). Numerous other behavioral measures can be used, such as ratings or response times.
Neuroimaging.
Finally, computational models and behavioral measures can be combined with neuroimaging. FMRI studies have increasingly focused on multi-voxel patterns of activity within a functional region to understand representational structure [5,11]. Although a far coarser measure than measuring activity of actual neurons, individual voxels may contain different distributions of neurons selective for certain types of information, thereby allowing voxel patterns to serve as potential proxies of underlying neuronal patterns [11,12]. Recent multi-voxel pattern analyses have demonstrated reliable decoding of various kinds of representations in social perception [13]. For instance, studies have shown the fusiform gyrus (FG), a visual face-processing region [14], is involved in representing faces’ gender [15], race [16,17], and emotion [18]. Consider a brain region with 100 voxels, with two unique multi-voxel patterns associated with the race categories White and Black; each category representation is therefore a point in a 100-dimensional multi-voxel space. Just like distances between representations in the multidimensional space of a computational model, or physical distance within the two-dimensional space of a computer screen with mouse-tracking (or other behavioral measures), distances between representations in the multidimensional space of neural response patterns can be used to understand the structure of representations in social perception (Fig. 1). Depending on the theoretical question at hand, these neural response patterns can be localized to a specific region or distributed over a larger cortical area or even the entire brain. Moreover, using computational models of the function of different anatomical regions (e.g., the HMAX model of ventral-temporal cortex; [19]), the structure of simulated representations in a putative region can be corroborated with the structure of fMRI response patterns in that actual region.
Linking these levels of analysis – computational modeling, behavioral data, and neuroimaging – by comparing their relative representational geometries has recently been used to address theoretically important questions in social perception.
Social categorization
A glimpse of a face conveys a number of social categories, such as gender or race. A central challenge of social categorization is to take the inherent diversity in facial features out in the social world to form a coherent categorization. Current neural-network models predict that when facial features occasionally conflict (e.g., a man’s face with feminine features), as with natural variation in the population, multiple partially-active social categories will be activated (i.e., both male and female) that simultaneously compete over time [13] – a process for which representational geometry has recently been useful in understanding [20] (Fig. 2).
This category competition driving perception may also be affected by top-down factors. For instance, being exposed to a face has long been known to spontaneously activate relevant gender or racial stereotypes [21,22], but recent work has suggested a more bidirectional relationship. Like other forms of perception, top-down expectations may facilitate certain perceptual interpretations [23,24], which in social perception may include stereotype-based expectations [2,13]. Over the years, stereotype effects have been documented on perceptual judgments [25-27], but it has remained less clear how such biases might manifest earlier in the perceptual process.
In one set of neuroimaging studies [10], the structure (i.e., all pairwise similarities) of gender, race, and emotion categories was assessed across several domains: multi-voxel response patterns to faces (via fMRI), subjective perception of faces (via mouse-tracking), and stereotype knowledge (via a stereotype content task). The results revealed a shared representational geometry across modalities (stereotype knowledge, subjective perceptions, and multi-voxel response patterns in the face-processing right FG). The similarity (i.e., distance) between any given pair of social categories (e.g., Black and anger) in stereotype knowledge predicted a corresponding similarity in how faces belonging to those categories were perceived (via mouse-tracking) as well as in faces’ multi-voxel response patterns. For example, the more similarly the categories ‘Black’ and ‘anger’ were believed to be in terms of their stereotype knowledge predicted a greater tendency to perceive Black faces and angry faces more similarly (e.g., a partial attraction to ‘angry’ even for a non-angry Black face). This greater perceptual similarity was in turn reflected by an increased similarity in the multi-voxel representations of Black faces and angry faces in the FG (Fig. 3a). That these stereotypical biases on perception manifest in a visual face-processing region’s representational structure suggests that social cognitive processes can impact visual representation.
Emotion perception
As in social categorization, the notion that cognitive processes may exert top-down impacts on perception has become increasingly important in understanding emotion perception, particularly the role of emotion-concept knowledge [28,29]. Early “basic emotion” approaches emphasized six universal facial expressions of emotion, associated with specific action units that may be read directly from a face. Constructionist approaches and recent neural-network models suggest that not only do facial features related to, fear, for example, drive categorizations, but so does conceptual knowledge about what fear means. Recent work has used representational geometry to provide a comprehensive test of how inter-individual variability in conceptual knowledge may shape facial emotion perception.
In one set of studies [30], the structure (i.e., pairwise similarities) of the six basic emotion categories was assessed at the level of conceptual knowledge (via a rating task), facial emotion perception (via mouse-tracking) and faces’ intrinsic physical properties (via measuring facial action units). A corresponding geometry was observed between conceptual knowledge and facial emotion perception, controlling for faces’ inherent physical similarity. When individuals believed two emotions (e.g., anger and disgust) to be conceptually more similar, faces belonging to those categories were perceived with a corresponding similarity (i.e., mouse trajectories were more attracted to both emotion responses in parallel, although each face only depicted one emotion) (Fig. 3b). Such findings suggest that subtle individual differences in the conceptual understanding of what different emotions mean are reflected in how those emotions are perceived from a face. A neuroimaging study [31] replicated this general finding but also showed that such a conceptually shaped representational structure was reflected in the structure of FG multi-voxel patterns, thereby suggesting its impact on visual representation of faces. Thus, believing anger and disgust are more conceptually related is associated with an increased bias to perceive angry and disgusted faces more similarly, as well as increased similarity in their multi-voxel response patterns in the FG.
Other work has used representational geometry to test for shared neural representations across faces and voices [32] or competing models of inferring the emotional experience of another person [33]. For instance, in response to emotional episodes, the similarity structure of multi-voxel patterns in the TPJ and dmPFC, regions implicated in theory of mind, were compared to the similarity structure of several candidate theoretical models. Among these was an appraisal model, relating episodes by their similarity across 38 appraisal dimensions (i.e., abstract features of the causal contexts that elicit emotions). The geometry of neural patterns in theory-of-mind regions corresponded best with the geometry of this 38-dimensional appraisal model, suggesting that these regions contain a high-dimensional model capturing the appraisal of others’ emotional experiences that cannot be reduced merely to a two-dimensional circumplex model (valence and arousal) or six-dimensional basic-emotion model (also see Fig. 4).
Trait impressions
Perceiving personality traits from a face – warmth, intelligence – may seem ambiguous and arbitrary relative to perceiving aspects like social categories or emotions. But much research has demonstrated that arrangements of facial features reliably relate to specific trait perceptions [34,35], a process requiring minimal visual exposure [36,37]. Popular models of face-based trait impressions focus exclusively on the role of bottom-up facial features [34,38]. However, an emerging literature has noted the considerable influence of top-down cognitive processes in face impressions [6,35,39,40], for which representational geometry has recently been valuable.
One set of studies [41] tested whether individual differences in conceptual trait associations shape how those traits are perceived from a face. The structure (i.e., all pairwise similarities) of traits was assessed at the level of conceptual knowledge (e.g., “if someone is agreeable, are they also open-minded?”) and face impressions. A reverse correlation technique was used to estimate each perceiver’s visual prototypes for different traits. Indeed, to the extent a perceiver believed any pair of traits to be more related (e.g., openness and agreeableness), they perceived those traits in faces with a corresponding similarity (e.g., greater resemblance in the visual prototypes for the two traits; Fig. 3c). This showed that face impressions arise not just from bottom-up features but also the conceptual understanding of what those traits mean. Although top-down factors have long been recognized as affecting personality inferences in general [42], current models of face-based trait impressions focus on a relatively fixed and universal two-dimensional structure derived from bottom-up facial features alone. These findings suggest a more dynamic multidimensional structure that varies across perceivers and contexts, not necessarily tied to any evolutionary or functional value [6]. It also empirically connects more recent face-based trait impressions research to more classic work on the conceptual shaping of personality inferences.
Other recent research went so far as to quantify the role of conceptual trait knowledge in social perception generally – across domains of face impressions, familiar person knowledge, and group stereotypes [43]. Analyses demonstrated a strong correspondence in the geometry of trait representations across these social perception models, suggesting that a perceiver’s conceptual trait associations may provide a domain-general model for inferring about not only faces but also familiar others and social groups. Finally, representational geometry has additionally been used to understand inferences about others transient mental states [44,45], finding that a three-dimensional model of rationality, social impact, and valence best predict the structure of behavioral data and neural patterns associated with mental states.
Conclusion
Across various domains of social perception, an emerging focus on representational geometry is exploring how both bottom-up facial features and a variety of top-down social cognitive factors together shape perceptions of other people. While the general correlational approach with representational geometry has permitted broad tests of how an entire system of representations in social perception may be determined by novel factors (e.g., stereotypes, emotion-concept knowledge), future research could establish more causal links through manipulating these factors. Although this work is in its early stages, linking across computational modeling, neuroimaging, and behavioral data allows researchers to use multiple levels of analysis to constrain theoretical models. It provides promise for furthering our understanding of the multiply determined nature of social perception and its neural basis.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported in part by research grants NSF BCS-1654731 and NIH R01-MH112640 to J.B.F.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest
Declarations of interest: none
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Smith ER, DeCoster J: Knowledge acquisition, accessibility, and use in person perception and stereotyping: Simulation with a recurrent connectionist network. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1998, 74:21–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Freeman JB, Ambady N: A dynamic interactive theory of person construal. Psychological Review 2011, 118:247–279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kunda Z, Thagard P: Forming impressions from stereotypes, traits, and behaviors: A parallel-constraint-satisfaction theory. Psychological Review 1996, 103:284–308. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Averbeck BB, Latham PE, Pouget A: Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nature reviews neuroscience 2006, 7:358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Diedrichsen J, Kriegeskorte N: Representational models: A common framework for understanding encoding, pattern-component, and representational-similarity analysis. PLoS computational biology 2017, 13:e1005508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Stolier RM, Hehman E, Freeman JB: A dynamic structure of social trait space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Zylberberg J, Strowbridge BW: Mechanisms of persistent activity in cortical circuits: possible neural substrates for working memory. Annual review of neuroscience 2017, 40:603–627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Freeman JB: Doing psychological science by hand. Current Directions in Psychological Science 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Freeman JB, Dale R, Farmer TA: Hand in motion reveals mind in motion. Frontiers in Psychology 2011, 2:59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Stolier RM, Freeman JB: Neural pattern similarity reveals the inherent intersection of social categories. Nature Neuroscience 2016, 19:795–797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Haynes J-D: A primer on pattern-based approaches to fMRI: principles, pitfalls, and perspectives. Neuron 2015, 87:257–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Chaimow D, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, Shmuel A: Modeling and analysis of mechanisms underlying fMRI-based decoding of information conveyed in cortical columns. Neuroimage 2011, 56:627–642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Freeman JB, Johnson KL: More than meets the eye: Split-second social perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Grill-Spector K, Weiner KS, Kay K, Gomez J: The functional neuroanatomy of human face perception. Annual review of vision science 2017, 3:167–196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Freeman JB, Rule NO, Adams RB, Ambady N: The neural basis of categorical face perception: Graded representations of face gender in fusiform and orbitofrontal cortices. Cerebral Cortex 2010, 20:1314–1322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Contreras JM, Banaji MR, Mitchell JP: Multivoxel patterns in fusiform face area differentiate faces by sex and race. PloS one 2013, 8:e69684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ratner KG, Kaul C, Van Bavel JJ: Is race erased? Decoding race from patterns of neural activity when skin color is not diagnostic of group boundaries. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 2013, 8:750–755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Wegrzyn M, Riehle M, Labudda K, Woermann F, Baumgartner F, Pollmann S, Bien CG, Kissler J: Investigating the brain basis of facial expression perception using multivoxel pattern analysis. Cortex 2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Serre T, Oliva A, Poggio T: A feedforward architecture accounts for rapid categorization. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences 2007, 104:6424–6429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Stolier RM, Freeman JB: A neural mechanism of social categorization. Journal of Neuroscience 2017, 37:5711–5721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Macrae CN, Bodenhausen GV: Social cognition: Thinking categorically about others. Annual Review of Psychology 2000, 51:93–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mason MF, Cloutier J, Macrae CN: On construing others: Category and stereotype activation from facial cues. Social Cognition 2006, 24:540–562. [Google Scholar]
- 23.O’Callaghan C, Kveraga K, Shine JM, Adams RB Jr, Bar M: Predictions penetrate perception: Converging insights from brain, behaviour and disorder. Consciousness and cognition 2017, 47:63–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hanks TD, Summerfield C: Perceptual decision making in rodents, monkeys, and humans. Neuron 2017, 93:15–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Johnson KL, Freeman JB, Pauker K: Race is gendered: How Covarying Phenotypes and Stereotypes Bias Sex Categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2012:doi: 10.1037/a0025335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bijlstra G, Holland RW, Dotsch R, Hugenberg K, Wigboldus DH: Stereotype associations and emotion recognition. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 2014:0146167213520458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Freeman JB, Penner AM, Saperstein A, Scheutz M, Ambady N: Looking the part: Social status cues shape race perception. PLoS ONE 2011, 6:e25107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Barrett LF: How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Barrett LF: The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 2017, 12:1–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brooks JA, Freeman JB: Conceptual knowledge predicts the representational structure of facial emotion perception. Nature Human Behaviour in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Brooks JA, Chikazoe J, Sadato N, Freeman JB: The neural representation of emotion perception reflects cultural and individual variability in conceptual knowledge. in prep. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kuhn LK, Wydell T, Lavan N, McGettigan C, Garrido L: Similar representations of emotions across faces and voices. Emotion 2017, 17:912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Skerry AE, Saxe R: A common neural code for perceived and inferred emotion. Journal of Neuroscience 2014, 34:15997–16008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Oosterhof NN, Todorov A: The functional basis of face evaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2008, 105:11087–11092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hehman E, Sutherland CA, Flake JK, Slepian ML: The unique contributions of perceiver and target characteristics in person perception. Journal of personality and social psychology 2017, 113:513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Willis J, Todorov A: First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science 2006, 17:592–598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Freeman JB, Stolier RM, Ingbretsen ZA, Hehman EA: Amygdala responsivity to high-level social information from unseen faces. The Journal of Neuroscience 2014, 34:10573–10581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Vernon RJ, Sutherland CA, Young AW, Hartley T: Modeling first impressions from highly variable facial images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2014, 111:E3353–E3361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sutherland CA, Young AW, Mootz CA, Oldmeadow JA: Face gender and stereotypicality influence facial trait evaluation: Counter- stereotypical female faces are negatively evaluated. British Journal of Psychology 2015, 106:186–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sutherland CA, Oldmeadow JA, Young AW: Integrating social and facial models of person perception: Converging and diverging dimensions. Cognition 2016, 157:257–267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Stolier RM, Hehman E, Keller M, Walker M, Freeman JB: The conceptual structure of face impressions. PNAS invited revision. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Schneider DJ: Implicit personality theory: A review. Psychological bulletin 1973, 79:294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Stolier RM, Hehman E, Freeman JB: A common trait space for social cognition. under review. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Tamir DI, Thornton MA, Contreras JM, Mitchell JP: Neural evidence that three dimensions organize mental state representation: Rationality, social impact, and valence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2016, 113:194–199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Thornton MA, Mitchell JP: Theories of person perception predict patterns of neural activity during mentalizing. Cerebral cortex 2017:1–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]