Abstract
The present study extends prior research on the link between neighborhood disadvantage and chronic illness by testing an integrated model in which neighborhood characteristics exert effects on health conditions through accelerated cardiometabolic aging. Hypotheses were tested using a sample of 408 African Americans from the Family and Community Health Study. Using four waves of data spanning young adulthood (ages 18 ─ 29), we first found durable effects of neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated cardiometabolic aging and chronic illness. Then, we used marginal structural modeling to adjust for potential neighborhood selection effects. As expected, accelerated cardiometabolic aging was the biopsychosocial mechanism that mediated much of the association between neighborhood disadvantage and chronic illness. This finding provides additional support for the view that neighborhood disadvantage can influence morbidity and mortality by creating social contexts that becomes biologically embedded. Importantly, perceived neighborhood collective efficacy served to buffer the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and biological aging, identifying neighborhood-level resilience factor. Overall, our results indicate that neighborhood context serves as a fundamental cause of weathering and accelerated biological aging. Residing in a disadvantaged neighborhood increases biological wear and tear that ultimately leads to onset of chronic illness, but access to perceived collective efficacy buffers the impact of these neighborhood effects. From an intervention standpoint, identifying such an integrated model may help inform future health-promoting interventions.
Keywords: neighborhood disadvantage, neighborhood collective efficacy, speed of cardiometabolic aging, chronic illness, selection bias
Over the past two decades, there has been increasing interest in how social conditions influence health and disease by becoming biologically embedded (Beach et al., 2016, 2017; Brody et al., 2014; Chen & Miller, 2013; Link & Phelan, 1995; Phelan, Link, & Tehranifar 2010; Rutter, 2016; Simons et al., 2017, 2018). It has become increasingly clear that social circumstances such as lower socioeconomic status and exposure to adversity are associated with a higher prevalence of chronic illnesses in adult, including diabetes, hypertension, cardiovascular disease, and the dysregulation of the immune system (Belsky et al., 2015; Evans et al., 2007; Lei, Beach, & Simons, 2018; Miller, Chen, & Parker 2011; Simons et al., 2017).
Going beyond individual experiences of adversity and individual socioeconomic status, theory and evidence have suggested that the neighborhoods in which people reside are also fundamental contexts for understanding the biological embedding of social circumstances and accounting for effects on physical health and well-being (Aneshensel, 2009; Bosma et al., 2001; Causadias, 2013; Diez Roux, 2003; Finegood et al., 2017; Hill, Ross, & Angel 2005; Lei et al., 2016, 2017a; Ross & Mirowsky, 2001). This is due to the fact that individuals residing in socio-economically disadvantaged neighborhoods are exposed to a constellation of social stressors associated with increased presence of social disorder and subculture (Anderson, 1999; Burt, Lei, & Simons, 2017; Hill et al., 2005; Latkin & Curry, 2003; Ross & Mirowsky, 2009). These conditions foster concerns about safe and feelings of powerlessness and social isolation (Ross, 2011). Given that youth typically leave home at age 18 (Arnett, 2004), the period of emerging adulthood represents a developmental period in which neighborhood context is of increased importance for trajectories of behavior and health. The present study is concerned with increasing our understanding of how the duration and timing of exposure to neighborhood disadvantage combines with various social and biological processes to forecast the onset of chronic illness and more stringently testing models of biological embedding.
Neighborhood Influences and Health
Sociological and social-epidemiological studies of illness have been informed, in large measure, by the stress process model proposed by Leonard Pearlin (1989, 1999). The model asserts that health inequalities are caused by differential exposure to stressful environments. Prolonged exposure to such conditions is viewed as fostering a physiological stress response that increases the chances of chronic illness by damaging tissue and immune cells. Given that stress exposure often occurs in multilevel contexts, ranging from individual to neighborhood and broader contexts (Cicchetti & Garmezy, 1993; Cicchetti & Toth, 2009), in recent years, researchers (e.g., Aneshensel, Harig & Wight, 2016) have drawn upon the stress process model to develop arguments regarding the role of neighborhood context in explaining health inequalities. Neighborhood context is defined as a distinct geographical area (e.g., residential census tracts) in which people live, interact with each other, and share norms and values (Sampson, Morenoff, & Gannon-Rowley, 2002). To measure neighborhood context, neighborhood researchers (Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997; Ross & Mirowsky, 2001) have focused largely on socioeconomic measures of neighborhood context, often called “neighborhood disadvantage.”
Social disorganization theory (Shaw & McKay, 1969), posits that neighborhoods characterized by socio-economic disadvantage are often marked by visible cues indicating a breakdown of social order and control (Browning, Cagney, & Iveniuk, 2012; Hill et al., 2005; Ross & Mirowsky, 2001, 2009). These cues can be physical (e.g., graffiti, scattered trash, vandalism, run-down and abandoned buildings) or social (e.g., public drinking, people hanging out on the streets, trouble with neighbors). Given that residents in socio-economic disadvantaged neighborhoods find such conditions to be threatening and demoralizing, the neighborhood stress process model (Aneshensel et al., 2016) proposes that neighborhoods are a critical context for health. The distress of living under disadvantaged circumstances is seen as promoting psychological distress and physiological stress that can lead to chronic illness. Support for this idea has been provided by studies reporting an association between neighborhood adversity such as poverty or neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage and various health outcomes (e.g., chronic illness, blood pressure, obesity, and cardiovascular diseases) even controlling for individual socioeconomic measures and health-related variables (e.g., Aneshensel & Sucoff, 1996; Blair, Ross, Gariepy, & Schmitz, 2014; Boardman et al., 2005; Brody et al., 2014; Browning et al., 2012; Finegood et al., 2017; Hill et al., 2005; Latkin & Curry, 2003; Lei et al., 2015; Ross & Mirowsky, 2001).
Although previous studies suggest that neighborhood disadvantage is associated with health outcomes, studies to date have been limited by theoretical and methodological issues that remain to be addressed. First, the timing of neighborhood context effects is rarely examined. Recent studies on obesity suggest this may be problematic (Do & Zheng, 2017; Kravitz-Wirtz, 2016). Using a longitudinal approach to examine timing-specific exposure to neighborhood disadvantage on body mass index, these studies found support for a temporal model in which durable exposure to neighborhood disadvantage is more important for obesity than exposure during any particular stage of life. These findings highlight the likelihood that neighborhood disadvantage during young adulthood can add to disadvantage experienced in childhood, influencing obesity incrementally. However, it is still unclear how the temporal effects of neighborhood disadvantage become linked with biological age and chronic illness in adulthood. This issue is of considerable interest and importance for developmental psychopathology.
Second, from a methodological perspective, most studies of neighborhood effects have utilized self-report measures of both neighborhood context and general chronic health status. Thus the associations reported in past research are subject to criticism for potentially reflecting shared method variance (measures from the same source), leading the associations to be inflated (Podsakoff et al. 2003). We avoid this problem by using Census data to measure neighborhood disadvantage (Lei et al., 2017a; Sampson et al., 1997) and the Framingham cardiovascular risk score to assess accelerated cardiometabolic aging (D’Agostino et al., 2008). Accordingly, both the primary predictor and outcome variable are not based on self-report, and there is little method overlap between them in the current investigation.
In addition, prior research can be criticized as being driven by non-random assignment of individuals into neighborhoods, again potentially inflating associations and mispecifying the causal significance of neighborhoods (e.g., Humphrey & Root, 2017; Sampson et al., 2002). For example, families with low socioeconomic status are more likely to live in disadvantage neighborhoods. This may produce spurious neighborhood effects that are actually better attributed to socioeconomic status. However, simply incorporating control variables into a regression model has been criticized for over-controlling potential neighborhood effects (Sampson et al., 2002) and being inadequate to control for confounding variables that vary over time and are affected by previous neighborhood effects (Robins, 2000). In particular, traditional regression models ignore the fact that reciprocal cause-and-effect relationships exist between exposures and covariates. To adjust for non-random assignment to neighborhood without over controlling potential confounds, we use marginal structural modeling (MSM), strengthening our tests of causal hypotheses.
Neighborhood Disadvantage on Chronic Illness through Biological Aging
Prior research also has provided little guidance regarding the biopsychosocial mechanisms whereby neighborhood factors may become biologically embedded and thereby influence health (Causadias, Telzer, & Lee et al., 2017; Johnson & Acabchuk, 2018). It is generally assumed that adverse neighborhood conditions, such as concentrated socio-economic disadvantage, foster increased physiological stress that, over time, leads to disease (Pearlin, 1989, 1999; Thoits, 2010). While this general model is useful as a first approximation, identification of specific potential biopsychosocial mechanisms allows for stronger tests of theory and ultimately the design of stronger preventive interventions.
In the past decade, scholars have proposed different methods to estimate biological age. Recently, the most popular measures of biological aging are the epigenetic measures developed by Horvath (2013) and Hannum and colleagues (2013). Given that methylation at specific CpG sites provides an epigenetic ”signature” to track the state of cellular aging, they identified CpG sites scattered throughout the human genome where methylation levels were significantly associated with chronological age. Many of the CpG sites included in the Hannum’s and Horvath’s measures are within or near genes with known functions in aging-related conditions, including DNA damage, tissue degradation, cardiovascular disease, and chronic diseases. Indeed, the correlation between a person’s chronological age and their predicted age based on either of these two indices is roughly.90 (Beach et al., 2015; Horvath, 2013). Recent studies have indicated that accelerated epigenetic aging is a robust predictor of mortality (Marioni et al., 2015), and some chronic illnesses (Horvath et al., 2014; Perna et al., 2016; Wallace et al., 2017; Zheng et al., 2016). And, importantly, there is evidence that accelerated epigenetic aging is affected by social environment conditions (Brody et al., 2016a; Simons et al., 2016; Zannas et al., 2015).
Although DNA methylomic age provides one window onto accelerated biological aging, use of these assays is limited by relatively high costs. Further, these studies show weak and inconsistent relationships with health risk factors such as elevated blood pressure, cholesterol, glucose, or body mass index, and they do not predict onset of chronic illnesses such as myocardial infarction, stroke, or diabetes (Murabito et al., 2017). To overcome these limitations, we use a different approach to assessing the speed of biological aging. Specifically, rather than use methylomic measures of aging, we estimate biological age using the Framingham algorithm. Using data from the Framingham Study, D’Agostino et al. (2008) developed a measure of cardiometabolic age based upon seven metabolic syndrome and cardiovascular risk factors, including systolic blood pressure, diabetes, body mass index, treatment for hypertension, age, gender, and smoking. Studies have established that cardiometabolic aging is a biological process associated with the whole organism that ultimately leads to the onset of chronic illness (Groenewegen et al., 2015; Hirsch et al., 2018; Wallace et al., 2017). This measure has been shown to have high validity and reliability (D’Agostino et al., 2013) and has been commonly used by physicians to monitor their patient’s health condition (Davies et al., 2013; Schaefer et al., 2017).
The gap between cardiometabolic and chronological age can be considered a measure of person’s speed of aging. It can be used as a “biological clock” to assess the extent to which an individual is experiencing accelerated or decelerated cardiometabolic aging (Beach et al., 2015; Simons et al., 2016). Thus, accelerated cardiometabolic aging or premature aging is defined as people who are biologically older than their chronological age.
As outlined by Arline Geronimus (2013), chronic exposure to prolonged stressful environments can result in physiological wear and tear that may lead to onset of chronic illness. She used the term “biological weathering” to refer to accelerated biological aging that results from the physiological wear and tear that happens in response to stress. Thus, a greater burden of illness among African Americans results from increased “biological weathering,” a form of accelerated biological aging that is attributable to discrimination and marginalization in a society. This suggests that speed of biological aging is a weathering process fostered by adverse environments and that this biopsychosocial process may explain the relationship between chronic stress and health status. Based on this idea, residing in a disadvantaged neighborhood can exert a weathering effect in the form of accelerated cardiometabolic age. These metabolic and cardiovascular changes, in turn, ultimately lead to chronic illness.
Resilience Mechanisms in Neighborhoods
According to the classical neighborhood assumption, people from the same neighborhood are more similar to each other than to those from different neighborhoods (Foster & Brooks-Gunn, 2013). However, while individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhoods are more likely to become ill than those residing in more advantaged areas, many, if not most, will remain healthy (Fone et al., 2007; Lei et al., 2015; Miller et al., 2011), a phenomenon labeled “neighborhood resilience” (Brody, Yu, Beach, 2016b; Masten & Coatsworth, 1998; Norris et al., 2008). This raises the question: Why and how do some people living in disadvantaged neighborhoods develop chronic illnesses whereas others do not? Understanding neighborhood characteristics that may moderate the relationship between neighborhood stressors, such as neighborhood disadvantage, and physical health is fundamental to the advancement of neighborhood research.
Sampson and colleagues (1997) proposed that a set of neighborhood processes, which they termed neighborhood collective efficacy, would promote neighborhood cohesion and protect residents from the adverse effects of disadvantaged neighborhood contexts. The concept of neighborhood collective efficacy is defined as “the linkage of mutual trust and the willingness to intervene for the common good [of the neighborhood]” (Sampson et al., 1997, p. 917). In neighborhoods with high collective efficacy, residents trust each other and there is cohesion among neighbors who are willing to intervene to help each other to reach collective goals. The literature on collective efficacy has focused on positive adaptation in response to neighborhood stressors and has demonstrated that collective efficacy can promote resilience (Browning, Gardner, Maimon, & Brooks-Gunn, 2014; Diez-Roux & Mair, 2010; Foster & Brooks-Gunn, 2013; Liu et al., 2016; Ross & Jang, 2000; Simons et al., 2005; Wickes et al., 2013). For example, among African Americans from disadvantaged neighborhoods, Liu and colleagues (2016) found that exposure to neighborhood stressors and racial discrimination was associated with comorbid problems. But, the association was less pronounced for those who perceived higher levels of neighborhood collective efficacy.
As this example suggests, perceived collective efficacy has the potential to promote resilience to neighborhood stressors, indicating that perceived neighborhood collective efficacy may help to buffer against other facets of the neighborhood that may be stressful and so enhances an individual’s well-being despite ongoing stressors. Although social scientists have long been interested in the extent to which social support affects health (Phelan et al., 2010; Turner & Marino, 1994) and have proposed the stress-buffering hypothesis which argues that social support moderates the relationship between stress and health (Cohen, 1988; Lin & Ensel, 1989), to our knowledge, no prior research has examined perceived neighborhood collective efficacy buffers in modifying the impact of neighborhood-level disadvantage on biological mechanisms linked to health.
Why Focus on African Americans?
People living in disadvantaged environments have greater prevalence, earlier onset and more complications for several age-related diseases (Aneshensel, 2009; Bosma et al., 2001). This greater burden of illness results from increased “biological weathering” (Geronimus, 2013), a form of accelerated biological aging that is attributable to chronic stress and marginalization in a race-conscious society. This is particularly true for African Americans who are more likely to reside in extremely disadvantaged neighborhoods (Peterson & Krivo, 2010). In More than Just Race, William Julius Wilson (2009, p. 43) states that “one of the effects of living in a racially segregated, poor neighborhood, is the exposure to cultural framing, habits, styles of behavior, and particular skills that emerged from patterns of racial exclusion; these attributes and practices may not be conducive to facilitating social mobility.” Thus, African Americans experience health inequality is a consequence of the cumulative impact of life in a society where they suffer social, economic, and racial exclusion. Supporting this contention, several studies have found that this disproportionate burden is due, in large measure, to disparities in stress-related weathering and wear and tear of African Americans’ bodies, processes that often begin years or decades prior to the onset of disease (Genovese et al., 2010; Konen et al., 1999; Simons et al., 2018; Thomas et al., 2005).
Because differences in acculturation status and racial-ethnic backgrounds may condition response to, and interpretation of, neighborhood contexts, assessments of neighborhood disadvantage may not be fully comparable across ethnic groups even when using similar items. In addition, studies that search for biomarkers using multiple distinct ethnic groups are at increased risk for spurious findings due to background variation in biomarkers across ethnic groups (Hamer & Sirota, 2000). Consequently, a focus on African Americans seems appropriate for examination of weathering model that may have implications for health effects and possible points of preventive intervention for African Americans.
The Present Study
Summarizing, Figure 1 illustrates the theoretical model tested in the present study using multilevel data from a sample of approximately 400 African Americans. According to the neighborhood stress process model (Aneshensel et al., 2016) and the weathering hypothesis (Geronimus, 2013), the first set of hypotheses addresses the relationships among neighborhood disadvantage, speed of cardiometabolic aging, and chronic illness after controlling for covariates. Further, the temporal effects of neighborhood disadvantage have often been ignored in previous research (e.g., for an exception, see White et al., 2017). To address this issue, we examine timing-specific (ages 18, 21, 24, and 29) exposure to neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated aging and chronic illness. Then, we test durable effects of neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated aging and chronic illness using data from ages 18 to 29. We hypothesize that:
Figure 1.
Theoretical model linking neighborhood disadvantage to cardiometabolic aging and ultimately chronic illness response. The model indicates that the theoretical link is mediated and moderated by perceived neighborhood collective efficacy.
Hypothesis 1a:
At each wave, neighborhood disadvantage will be associated with a small-to-medium effect on accelerated cardiometabolic aging and chronic illness (Pathways a and b), even after controlling for potential confounders (e.g., gender, income, education, romantic relationship, health insurance, healthy diet, exercise, and sleep quality).
Hypothesis 1b:
Durable exposure to neighborhood disadvantage will be more important for accelerated aging and chronic illness than exposure during any particular stage of life.
As discussed, past research has reported that stressful circumstances (e.g., neighborhood disadvantage, racial discrimination, stressful life events, and harsh parenting) have a small-medium sized association with biological weathering and accelerated aging (Brody et al., 2016a; Geronimus, 2013; Lei et al., 2017a, 2017b; Miller et al., 2015; Moffitt et al., 2017; Simons et al., 2016). In addition, studies have shown that accelerated aging is a robust predictor of all chronic age-related diseases such as heart disease, liver disease, type II diabetes, hypertension, and Alzheimer’s disease (Marioni et al., 2015; Murabito et al., 2017). Building on these findings, the present study tests the hypothesis that it is speed of cardiometabolic aging that links adverse neighborhood conditions to adult illness.
Hypothesis 2:
The effect of neighborhood disadvantage on chronic illness will be mediated by accelerated cardiometabolic aging (Pathways a and c) even after potentially confounding variables (e.g., sociodemographic and health-related variables) and neighborhood selection bias are controlled.
Finally, given the modest association that has been shown to exist between neighborhood disadvantage and health outcomes, researchers (Aneshensel, 2009; Brody et al., 2014; Fone et al., 2007) have turned their attention to protective and resilience factors that might buffer adversity and so account for why some people become ill in the face of adversity whereas others do not. The present study investigates the extent to which perceived neighborhood collective efficacy buffers the deleterious effects of neighborhood disadvantage on cardiometabolic aging and chronic illness.
Hypothesis 3:
Drawing upon the stress-buffering hypothesis (Phelan, Link, & Tehranifar, 2010), the associations between neighborhood disadvantage and accelerated cardiometabolic aging will be stronger for individuals who perceived lower collective efficacy from their neighborhood than those who did not (Pathway d).
Hypothesis 4:
Based on hypotheses 2 and 3, we hypothesize that the indirect effect of neighborhood disadvantage on chronic illness through accelerated cardiometabolic aging will vary as a function of the degree to which individuals perceive collective efficacy from their neighborhood.
Research Design
Sample
To examine the effects of neighborhood concentrated disadvantage during emerging and young adulthood, we tested hypotheses using data from the four waves of data (ages 18 to 29) from the Family and Community Health Study (FACHS), an ongoing research project designed to increase understanding of the contextual risk and protective factors associated with the health and well-being in adulthood for African Americans (see Beach et al., 2017; Simons et al., 2018). The sample strategy was intentionally designed to generate families representing a range of socioeconomic status and neighborhood settings. At the first wave (1997−1998), the FACHS sample consists of 889 African American fifth-grade children. The mean ages were 10.56 years (SD = .631; range 9–13). The sample had an average family per capita income of $6,956. Thirty six percent of the families were below the poverty line, and fifty one percent of the respondents identified as single parents. The second, third, fourth, fifth, and sixth waves of data were collected in 1999–2000, 2001–2002, 2004–2005, 2007–2008, and 2011–2012 to capture information when the target children were ages 12 to 13, 14 to 15, 17 to 18, 20 to 21, and 23 to 24, respectively.
In 2014–2015, a Wave 7 of data collection was completed that included blood draws. The mean age was 29 years. Given the logistics of scheduling home visits by phlebotomists, only members of the sample residing in Georgia, Iowa, or a contiguous state were identified as eligible. After also excluding persons who were deceased, incarcerated, or otherwise unreachable, we were left with a pool of 545 individuals, 470 (86%) of whom agreed to be interviewed and to provide blood. In the current study, analyses are based on the 408 respondents (150 men and 258 women), nested within 201 Census tracts at Wave 7, who agreed to provide blood samples at age 29, and for whom data on all study measures from wave 7. To further assess attrition bias, we used Heckman’s (1979) two-step procedure to estimate sample selection bias. The results showed that the inverse Mills ratio was not significant, and including this ratio parameter in our models did not change the findings. There were no significant differences between those remaining in the panel and those dropping out with regard to a variety of measures such as neighborhood characteristics, education, income, and health-related variables.
Procedures
The protocol and all study procedures were approved by the University of Georgia Institutional Review Board. To enhance rapport and cultural understanding, African American university students and community members served as field researchers to collect data from the families in their homes. Prior to data collection, the researchers received one month of training in the administration of the self-report instruments. The questions were administered in the respondent’s home and took on average about 2 hours to complete. Some of the instruments administered in waves 5─7 included questions regarding illegal or potentially embarrassing sexual activities. Hence, in an effort to further enhance anonymity, we used audio-enhanced, computer-assisted, self-administered interviews (ACASI). Using this procedure, the respondent sat in front of a computer and responded to questions as they are both presented visually on the screen and auditorily via earphones. In addition to the interview data, participants were also asked to provide a blood sample at Wave 7. After blood was drawn into serum separator tubes by certified phlebotomists, it was frozen and shipped via courier to a laboratory at the University of Iowa to allow assessment of Hemoglobin A1c (HbA1C), a marker of elevated blood sugar, as well as other blood-based indices.
Measures
Chronic illness.
Self-reported chronic illness was measured at wave 7. Respondents were asked, “Have you even been diagnosed from any of the following health problems?” The list of health problems consisted of eight illnesses: coronary heart disease, hypertension, diabetes, peptic ulcer, kidney disease, liver disease, and thyroid disease. For each illness, “no” was coded as 0 and “yes” was coded as 1. Items were summed to form an index of chronic illness that ranged from 0 to 8. The mean score for this variable was 0.267 (SD = 0.564), with roughly 22% of the sample reporting that they had at least one diagnosed chronic disease.
Speed of cardiometabolic aging.
Cardiometabolic age at wave 7 was calculated following the gender-specific Framingham algorithm proposed by D’Agostino and colleagues (publicly available online tool: https://www.framinghamheartstudy.org/fhs-risk-functions/cardiovascular-disease-10-year-risk/). To estimate cardiometabolic aging, the Framingham algorithm uses systolic blood pressure, body mass index, and blood glucose level (HbA1C), plus it adjusts for an individual’s chronological age and gender, and whether they currently smoke (1 = yes, 24.26%) or take antihypertensive medication (1 = yes, 8.09%). Resting systolic blood pressure was monitored with Dinamap Pro 100 (Critikon; Tampa, FL) while the participants sat reading quietly. Three readings were taken every 2 min, and the average of the last two readings was used as the resting index. Mean systolic blood pressure was 122.476 (SD = 16.291). An individual’s body mass index score is calculated by weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters. Mean body mass index was 31.564 (SD = 8.522). Hemoglobin A1c (HbA1c) level is an indicator of average blood glucose concentrations over the preceding 2 to 3 months. It was determined by the University of Iowa Clinical Pathology Laboratories using a protocol previously described (Philibert et al., 2011). In the current study, about 4% had HbA1c above 6. Finally, we formulated a measure of accelerated cardiometabolic aging using the unstandardized residual scores from the regression of cardiometabolic age on chronological age (Lei et al., 2017a). These residuals had a mean of zero and represented both positive and negative deviations from chronological age (in years), with positive scores indicating accelerated aging.
Neighborhood disadvantage.
Neighborhood disadvantage was assessed with the U.S. Census Bureau’s American Community Survey (ACS) 5-Year Estimates (2008–2012), which was mapped onto study participants’ residential addresses in 2004 (wave 4), 2007 (wave 5), 2011 (wave 6), and 2014 (wave 7) using a Federal Information Processing Standard (FIPS) census tract code. Following previous studies (Sampson et al., 1997), the scale include six items: median household income (revers coded), percent unemployed, percent of residents below the poverty threshold, percent who are single-mother families, percent receiving public assistance, and percentage of residents less than age 18. The six items were standardized and summed. A higher score represented a more disadvantaged neighborhood. At age 29, 37.7% never moved, 52.5% moved one or two times, and 9.8% of respondents moved three or more times, and the average number of moves was 1.034 (SD = 1.037) in the past 10 years. The correlations among neighborhood disadvantage across waves were over 0.40. The mean of the proportion of households in participants’ tract level neighborhoods whose income fell below the federal poverty level was 0.21 at W4, 0.20 at W5, 0.20 at W6, and 0.19 at W7. That is, despite the relatively high rate of relocation, the percentage of participants residing in economically disadvantaged neighborhoods remained relatively constant across waves.
Racial composition.
An item from the U.S. Census Bureau’s ACS 5-Year Estimates (2008–2012), assesses the percentage of residents in the respondent’s census tract who are non-Hispanic black at waves 4 to 7 ().
Perceived collective efficacy.
At the time of the blood draw, participants completed a short form (5 items) of the collective efficacy scale (Sampson et al., 1997) that asked them to indicate how much they believe (1 = not at all true, 3 = very true) with various statements about their neighborhood, e.g., many of the adults in the neighborhood do get along with each other, adults in the neighborhood would call the police if they saw someone breaking the law, and adults in the neighborhood would tell them to behave if individuals got loud or disorderly. A higher score indicates greater perceived collective efficacy from neighborhoods. Coefficient alpha for the scale was above 0.70.
Covariates.
To account for measures that could provide plausible rival explanations, all analyses statistically controlled for gender and a comprehensive set of time-varying covariates at waves 4 to 7. Annual income at each wave was assessed by asking participants to report their income in the past year. Education was measured in years of education completed at each wave. At waves 4 to 7, respondents reported their relationship status with a romantic partner (1 = married or cohabiting; 0 = other). Employment status was measured by a question that asked respondents if they have a part-time or full-time job (1 = yes). Living with parents at each wave was assessed using the item: “In the past 12 months, did you live with your parents?” The item assessed the victimization of violence in the past 12 months. In addition, we included health-related time-varying covariates at waves 4 to 7. First, the item asks respondents about their health insurance status (1 = having health insurance) during the previous year. Second, healthy diet across waves was assessed using two items that asked about frequency of fruit and vegetable consumption during the previous 7 days. Responses ranged from 1 (none) to 6 (more than once every day) and were averaged to form the healthy diet variable. Third, exercise at waves 4 to 7 was measured with two items (e.g., On how many of the past 7 days did you exercise or participate in physical activity for at least 30 min that made you breathe hard such as running or riding a bicycle hard?) The response categories ranged from 1 (0 days) to 5 (all 7 days). Scores on the two items were averaged to form the exercise measure. Finally, sleep quality was measured using the subjective item (1= very bad, 4, very good): “During the past month, how would you rate your sleep quality overall?”
Analytic Strategy
All analyses were run using STATA 15 (StataCorp, 2017). Because missing data might influence our results, we used the last observation carried forward approach for imputing missing values at waves 4 to 6. In order to test the duration effects, we will first run a set of traditional multilevel models to examine the impact of timing-specific exposure to neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated cardiometabolic aging and chronic illness. This method is comparable to previous human development studies.
Given that respondents are not randomly assigned to residential locations, we used marginal structural models with inverse probability of treatment weighting (IPTW) to adjust for a potential neighborhood selection bias. This approach allows us to identify and control individual characteristics that could potentially confound the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and outcomes. Then, using inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting of the marginal structural model, we adjust for potential neighborhood selection bias (e.g., Humphrey & Root, 2017; Wodtke, Elwert, & Harding, 2016). This method involves transforming neighborhood from a continuous to a categorical variable. This allows for calculation of propensity scores using a multinomial logistic regression with time-varying and invariant covariates.
Recently, Lei and colleagues (2017a) extended previous studies by using a continuous measure of neighborhood disadvantage to estimate inverse-probability-of-treatment weights through conditional densities (Robins, 2000) with clustered standard errors. Let Ni denote neighborhood disadvantage and Ci be a set of time-varying and invariant covariates. The inverse-probability-of-treatment weights are given by
For individual i, the inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting calculates conditional densities of exposure to disadvantaged neighborhoods at time t, conditional on the set of neighborhood disadvantage and time-varying and invariant covariates at t ─ 1. However, the weight (wi) would have infinite variance. Hernán, Brumback, & Robins (2000) recommend using stabilized inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting to reduce bias:
where stabilized weights (swi) has the same denominator as in the inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting. But, the numerator of the stabilized weights (swi) comprises the conditional densities of exposure to a disadvantaged neighborhood given neighborhood disadvantage at t ─ 1. This weighting method creates a distribution reflecting sample characteristics except that there is no association between neighborhood measures and covariates (e.g., individual socioeconomic measures). The new distribution retains the effect of exposure on outcomes. Thus, marginal structural modeling not only provides more accurate estimates of time-varying measures than traditional regression models, but also makes causal inference possible with observational data if the assumptions of exchangeability (i.e., no unobserved confounding), positivity (i.e., positive probability of exposure at every level of observed confounders), consistency (i.e., well-defined exposure), and correct model specification are met. Accordingly, the result is a more stringent test of causal hypotheses. Finally, pooled regression models of panel data are weighted by the stabilized inverse-probability-of-treatment weights to adjust for a potential neighborhood selection bias. Thus, individuals who are underrepresented in exposure assignment are given proportionately higher weights, while those who are highly represented in exposure assignment are given proportionally lower weights.
To test the second set of hypotheses, pooled regression models with inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting were used with cardiometabolic aging as the outcome. We began by testing the main effect of neighborhood disadvantage, controlling for sociodemographic and health-related variables. Next, we estimated a model that included a measure of chronic illness to test the mediating hypothesis. Then, Pooled Poisson models with inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting were used for chronic illness as the outcome. Poisson models were used at this stage because this measure is a count variable. We first tested the main effect of neighborhood disadvantage. Finally, we included a measure of cardiometabolic aging to test the mediating hypothesis.
Turning to the final set of hypotheses, the studies included two models with inverse-probability-of-treatment weights to test for the direct effect and the buffering effect of perceived collective efficacy. In these two models, the first model included a measure of perceived neighborhood collective efficacy to test the main effect. Model 2 included the interaction term (neighborhood disadvantage × collective efficacy) necessary to test the moderating hypothesis. When the interaction effects were significant, post hoc analyses of interaction terms were conducted using the average marginal effects. Finally, the moderated-mediation model (Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes, 2007) was used to examine speed of cardiometabolic aging as a possible mediator of the two-way interaction effect of neighborhood disadvantage and collective efficacy on chronic illness. The logic of the moderated-mediation model is similar to traditional mediated models except that it focuses on testing the mediated effect of the interaction term on the outcome rather than examining the effect of the independent variables.
Results
Initial findings
At the time of the blood draw, about 22 percent of participants reported that they had at least one or more symptoms using self-reported questionnaires. Turning to each of biomarkers, about 49 percent of samples had a body mass index greater than 30, and about 4 percent of those had an HbA1c greater than 6. Approximately 23 percent of participants (143 individuals) had hypertension (systolic blood pressure ≥ 130 mm Hg and diastolic blood pressure ≥ 80 mm Hg, American Heart Association, 2017). The mean chronological age of the respondents was 29.158 years (SD = 0.744), and the mean cardiometabolic age, calculated as the algorithm identified by D’Agostino et al. (2008), was 33.016 (SD = 6.574). Using the r-squared values to indicate the magnitude of significant relationships (Cohen, 1982), as expected, cardiometabolic age had a small-medium association with chronological age (). Mean cardiometabolic age derived from the Framingham algorithm was 3.858 years higher than the actual chronological age of the sample. Sixty-eight percent of respondents had a cardiometabolic age greater than their chronological age, indicating a tendency in the sample toward accelerated aging. The correlation matrix is presented in Table 1. It shows a medium significant correlation between chronic illness and accelerated cardiometabolic aging (), and both of these variables display significant associations with each of cardiovascular risk biomarkers.
Table 1.
Correlations between chronic illness, cardiometabolic aging, and biomarkers.
Variable or statistic | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Accelerated cardiometabolic aging (Age 29) | — | |||||
2. chronic illness (Age 29) | .396** | — | ||||
3. Body mass index (Age 29) | .391** | .184** | — | |||
4. HbA1c > 6 (Age 29) | .411** | .336** | .180** | — | ||
5. Systolic blood pressure (Age 29) | .714** | .219** | .190** | .104* | — | |
6. Diastolic blood pressure (Age 29) | .520** | .313** | .199** | .138** | .659** | — |
Mean | .000 | .267 | 31.564 | .042 | 122.476 | 79.393 |
SD | 6.482 | .565 | 8.522 | .200 | 16.291 | 10.750 |
Range | 38.77 | 3.00 | 45.03 | 1.00 | 106.50 | 82.00 |
Skewness | .961 | 2.508 | 1.007 | 4.604 | .847 | .558 |
Kurtosis | 1.138 | 7.301 | .886 | 19.294 | 1.375 | 1.547 |
Note: Accelerated cardiometabolic aging was defined as residual from a linear regression model that regressed cardiometabolic age on chronological age. Hemoglobin A1c (HbA1c) was coded as dichotomous (1 = above 6).
p ≤ .05
p ≤ .01 (two-tailed tests); N = 408
H1a: Neighborhood disadvantage will be related to both accelerated cardiometabolic aging and chronic illness even after controlling for potential confounders
Multilevel modeling began with an unconditional model to estimate how much variability in accelerated cardiometabolic aging exists at neighborhood level. This model has no predictors at the respondent and neighborhood levels. Our results indicate that the neighborhood random effect is significant []. About 3% (intraclass correlation coefficient = 0.028) of the total variance in accelerated aging occurs across neighborhoods, suggesting that there is sufficient variation across neighborhoods in biological aging. Table 2 shows the results of using multilevel modeling to examine the impact of time-specific exposure to neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated cardiometabolic aging. Model 1 shows that neighborhood concentrated disadvantage at age 18 (wave 4) was associated with accelerated cardiometabolic aging (b = 0.707) after taking into account the various controls. It suggests that a standard deviation increase in neighborhood disadvantage is association with 0.707 years increase in cardiometabolic age. We next ran three models, entering neighborhood disadvantage at ages 21, 24, and 29, respectively, as predictors. As shown in Models 2 to 4, there was evidence of a significant effect of neighborhood disadvantage at each wave on accelerated cardiometabolic aging after taking into account all of the various controls.
Table 2.
Multilevel regression models examining effects of neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated cardiometabolic aging
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
b (SE) | b (SE) | b (SE) | b (SE) | |
Concentrated disadvantage (Age 18) | 0.679* (0.346) |
|||
Concentrated disadvantage (Age 21) | 0.811* (0.357) |
|||
Concentrated disadvantage (Age 24) | 0.773* (0.367) |
|||
Concentrated disadvantage (Age 29) | 1.044** (0.374) |
|||
Constant | 14.338** (3.786) |
9.279** (3.476) |
8.835** (3.148) |
10.328** (3.160) |
Random effect | ||||
4.17e-06 | 5.06e-12 | 0.823 | 1.064 | |
38.921 | 38.875 | 37.337 | 36.694 | |
Deviance | 2651.755 | 2651.282 | 2643.419 | 2638.962 |
Note: Unstandardized (b) shown with robust standard errors in parentheses; neighborhood concentrated disadvantage are standardized by z-transformation (mean = 0 and SD = 1); racial composition, males, annual income, education, married or cohabiting, employment status, living with parents, victimization, health insurance, healthy diet, exercise, and sleep quality are controlled at the same age as concentrated disadvantage.
N(persons) = 408.
p ≤ .05,
p ≤ .01 (two-tailed tests).
Turning to the models for chronic illness as the dependent variable, Table 3 shows that the results replicate those shown in Table 2. Models 1 to 4 shows that neighborhood disadvantage at each wave was associated with chronic illness. Consistent with study hypothesis, neighborhood disadvantage, measured by census ACS data, was significantly associated with both accelerated cardiometabolic aging and chronic illness, and this association held after controlling for a variety of covariates.
Table 3.
Multilevel Poisson regression models examining effects of neighborhood disadvantage on chronic illness
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
b (SE) | b (SE) | b (SE) | b (SE) | |
Concentrated disadvantage (Age 18) | 0.231* (0.112) |
|||
Concentrated disadvantage (Age 21) | 0.231* (0.115) |
|||
Concentrated disadvantage (Age 24) | 0.253* (0.099) |
|||
Concentrated disadvantage (Age 29) | 0.361** (0.112) |
|||
Constant | −1.398 (1.199) |
−2.226* (1.099) |
−1.729 (0.980) |
−2.224** (0.969) |
Random effect | ||||
0.160 | 0.144 | 3.02e-32 | 6.69e-33 | |
Deviance | 515.036 | 514.136 | 502.143 | 484.278 |
Note: Unstandardized (b) shown with robust standard errors in parentheses; neighborhood concentrated disadvantage are standardized by z-transformation (mean = 0 and SD = 1); racial composition, males, annual income, education, married or cohabiting, employment status, living with parents, victimization, health insurance, healthy diet, exercise, and sleep quality are controlled at the same age as concentrated disadvantage.
N(persons) = 408.
p ≤ .05
p ≤ .01 (two-tailed tests)
H1b: Durable exposure to neighborhood disadvantage will be more important for accelerated cardiometabolic aging and chronic illness than exposure during any particular stage of life.
Next, we ran a path model to identify whether durable effects of neighborhood disadvantage exist. As shown in Figure 2, the association between neighborhood disadvantage at age 18 and neighborhood disadvantage at age 21 was significant (β = 0.453, p < .001), the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage at age 21 and neighborhood disadvantage at age 24 was also significant (β = 0.373, p < .001), and the association between neighborhood disadvantage at age 24 and neighborhood disadvantage at age 29 was statistically significant (β = 0.158, p = .005). Finally, exposure to neighborhood disadvantage at age 29 was associated with accelerated cardiometabolic aging (β = 0.130, p = .028). Using an indirect effect test, the results revealed that the mediating effect of early neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated cardiometabolic aging through neighborhood disadvantage at ages 21, 24, and 29 was significant (indirect effect = .005, 95% confidence interval [0.001, 0.015]), suggesting that it is the durable effect of neighborhood disadvantage that accelerates cardiometabolic aging. Further, Figure 3 (indirect effect = .004, 95% confidence interval [0.001, 0.009]) indicates a pattern virtually identical to those depicted in Figure 2, indicating durable effects of neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated cardiometabolic aging and chronic illness among African-Americans.
Figure 2.
Path model of the relationship between early and later neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated cardiometabolic aging. *p ≤ .05, **p ≤ .01 (two-tailed tests).
Figure 3.
Path model of the relationship between early and later neighborhood disadvantage on chronic illness. *p ≤ .05, **p ≤ .01 (two-tailed tests).
H2: The effect of neighborhood disadvantage on chronic illness will be mediated by accelerated cardiometabolic aging even after confounding confounders and neighborhood selection bias are controlled.
Due to non-random assignment of individuals into neighborhoods, we used marginal structural modeling to control for selection bias through inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting. The inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting was calculated using a regression model with cluster adjusted standard errors to estimate each respondent’s probability of receiving the treatment of exposure to neighborhood concentrated disadvantage. That is, neighborhood disadvantage was regressed on gender and time-varying sociodemographic covariates from ages 18 to 29. The descriptive statistics for all time-varying covariates in the inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting are shown in Table 4. To avoid an extreme variation of weights, we first checked the variance of inverse-probability-of-treatment weights. Unstabilized inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting () were associated with substantial variability (SD = 11279.82), whereas this variability was eliminated by using stabilized inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting (SD = 0.06). Given that extreme weights can bias the standard error estimates (Hernán, Brumback, & Robins, 2000), all models were weighted by stabilized inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting to reduce neighborhood selection bias.
Table 4.
Descriptive statistics for time-varying and time-invariant covariates in the inverse probability treatment weighting
Wave 1 (Age 18, N = 408) | Mean | SD | Range | Skewness | Kurtosis |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Time-varying covariates | |||||
Annual income | 6981.88 | 10098.89 | 0 ─ 84,000 | 2.467 | 14.192 |
Education (years) | 11.86 | 1.08 | 8─15 | .310 | 3.481 |
Married/cohabiting | 0.04 | 0.21 | 0─1 | 2.488 | 7.191 |
Employed | 0.51 | 0.50 | 0─1 | −.049 | 1.002 |
Living with parents | 0.75 | 0.44 | 0─1 | −1.125 | 2.265 |
Victimization | 0.02 | 0.14 | 0─1 | 6.930 | 49.020 |
Wave 2 (Age 21, N = 408) | Mean | SD | Range | Skewness | Kurtosis |
Time-varying covariates | |||||
Annual income | 11101.41 | 12104.20 | 0 ─ 96,000 | 1.640 | 9.186 |
Education (years) | 12.50 | 1.33 | 8─16 | .390 | 3.546 |
Married/cohabiting | 0.11 | 0.31 | 0─1 | 4.440 | 20.713 |
Employed | 0.67 | 0.47 | 0─1 | −.719 | 1.517 |
Living with parents | 0.53 | 0.50 | 0─1 | −.128 | 1.016 |
Victimization | 0.05 | 0.22 | 0─1 | 4.060 | 17.482 |
Wave 3 (Age 24, N = 408) | Mean | SD | Range | Skewness | Kurtosis |
Time-varying covariates | |||||
Annual income | 12765.29 | 12827.55 | 0 ─ 84,000 | 1.478 | 7.227 |
Education (years) | 12.78 | 1.50 | 9─17 | .397 | 3.169 |
Married/cohabiting | 0.10 | 0.30 | 0─1 | 2.658 | 8.063 |
Employed | 0.71 | 0.45 | 0─1 | −.930 | 1.865 |
Living with parents | 0.48 | 0.50 | 0─1 | .098 | 1.010 |
Victimization | 0.02 | 0.15 | 0─1 | 6.508 | 43.356 |
Wave 4 (Age 29, N = 408) | Mean | SD | Range | Skewness | Kurtosis |
Time-varying covariates | |||||
Annual income | 21450.35 | 16432.24 | 0 ─ 83,136 | .642 | 3.551 |
Education (years) | 13.25 | 1.65 | 9─17 | .294 | 2.636 |
Married/cohabiting | 0.26 | 0.44 | 0─1 | 1.067 | 2.138 |
Employed | 0.81 | 0.39 | 0─1 | −1.591 | 3.531 |
Living with parents | 0.29 | 0.45 | 0─1 | .930 | 1.865 |
Victimization | 0.02 | 0.15 | 0─1 | 6.150 | 38.825 |
Time-invariant covariate | |||||
Males | 0.37 | 0.48 | 0─1 | .549 | 1.301 |
Table 5 presents pooled regression models with stabilized inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting. This model allows us to include time-varying sociodemographic and health-related control variables and control for potential neighborhood selection effects. Model 1A reveals that a measure of time-varying neighborhood disadvantage had a significant effect on accelerated cardiometabolic aging (b = 0.749, p = .002). The result indicates that a standard deviation increase in neighborhood disadvantage is associated with 0.749 years increase in cardiometabolic age. To further interpret this finding, we graphed the effect in Figure 4 for levels of neighborhood disadvantage that range from −2 to +2 SD from the mean. As can be seen, the regression line crosses the line of deviation of cardiometabolic age from chronological age at zero, suggesting that individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhoods show significantly accelerated aging whereas those living in advantaged neighborhoods demonstrate significantly decelerated aging. Further, Model 1B shows that this association was maintained (b = 0.723, p = .002) after controlling for time-varying socioeconomic status and various health-related behaviors. Next, Model 1C presents the result of entering chronic illness as a mediator. The impact of neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated cardiometabolic aging remained significant, indicating chronic illness is not a mediator of this relationship.
Table 5.
Pooled regression models using four waves (ages 18 to 29) of data examining effects of neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated cardiometabolic aging and chronic illness
Speed of aginga |
Chronic illnessb |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1A | Model 1B | Model 1C | Model 2A | Model 2B | Model 2C | |
b (SE) | b (SE) | b (SE) | b (SE) | b (SE) | b (SE) | |
Concentrated disadvantage | 0.749* (0.242) |
0.723** (0.227) |
0.456* (0.201) |
0.214** (0.079) |
0.203** (0.071) |
0.098 (0.065) |
Racial composition | 0.541 (0.937) |
0.688 (0.843) |
−0.107 (0.345) |
−0.274 (0.308) |
||
Chronic illness | 4.591** (0.669) |
|||||
Speed of aging | 0.093** (0.010) |
|||||
Males | 1.127 (0.625) |
1.315* (0.579) |
−0.163 (0.240) |
−0.069 (0.230) |
||
Annual income | −0.471* (0.213) |
−0.362 (0.198) |
−0.101 (0.111) |
−0.051 (0.112) |
||
Education | −0.238* (0.104) |
−0.326* (0.101) |
0.063* (0.028) |
0.086** (0.028) |
||
Married/cohabiting | 1.262 (0.945) |
1.030 (0.854) |
0.195 (0.260) |
0.121 (0.233) |
||
Employment status | 0.259 (0.516) |
0.269 (0.466) |
0.012 (0.182) |
−0.045 (0.175) |
||
Living with parents | 0.550 (0.489) |
0.294 (0.449) |
0.212 (0.146) |
0.059 (0.140) |
||
Victimization | 0.674 (1.096) |
0.596 (0.982) |
0.063 (0.300) |
0.012 (0.269) |
||
Health insurance | 0.689 (0.665) |
0.805 (0.603) |
−0.097 (0.221) |
−0.102 (0.195) |
||
Healthy diet | 0.024 (0.111) |
0.039 (0.103) |
−0.015 (0.028) |
−0.017 (0.028) |
||
Exercise | 0.150 (0.110) |
0.128 (0.098) |
0.019 (0.033) |
0.001 (0.029) |
||
Sleep quality | 0.312 (0.364) |
0.651 (0.335) |
−0.255** (0.097) |
−0.253** (0.079) |
||
Constant | −0.011 (0.317) |
−0.489 (0.483) |
−1.705** (0.462) |
−1.345** (.104) |
−1.361** (0.158) |
−1.598** (0.176) |
Note: Unstandardized (b) shown with robust standard errors in parentheses; models adjust for selection effects through application of inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting; income and neighborhood disadvantage are standardized by z-transformation (mean = 0 and SD = 1); all time-varying covariates are lagged to the previous wave (T-1). N(persons) = 408.
p ≤ .05,
p ≤ .01 (two-tailed tests).
Pooled regression model for a continuous outcome
Pooled Poisson regression model for a count outcome
Figure 4.
Scatter plot representing the association between neighborhood disadvantage and accelerated cardiometabolic aging using Framingham algorithm. The solid line displays the predicted regression line, and the dashed lines are the 95% confidence interval for the fitted line. Predicted scores represent residual biological age after controlling for chronological age. N = 408.
Given that the measure of chronic illness is a count variable, pooled Poisson models with stabilized inverse-probability-of-treatment weights provide a good fit of the data. Model 2A of Table 5 shows that the main effect of neighborhood disadvantage was significant (b = 0.214, IRR = 1.239, p =.007), indicating that a standard deviation increase in neighborhood disadvantage increases the expected number of symptoms by 23.9%. Model 2B added the potentially time-varying confounding variables. As hypothesized, long-term exposure to neighborhood disadvantage was related to chronic illness (b = 0.203, IRR = 1.225, p =.004). The results are consistent with numerous studies (Aneshensel, 2009; Hill et al. 2005; Lei et al., 2015; Ross & Mirowsky, 2001, 2009) reporting that residing in a disadvantaged neighborhood has a deleterious effect on physical health even after controlling for selection bias, socioeconomic status, and health-related behaviors.
Model 2C introduced the measure of accelerated cardiometabolic aging using the residual scores from the regression of cardiometabolic age on chronological age. The difference in deviance between Model 2B and Model 2C was significant (Δχ2 = 229.368, df = 1, p < .001), suggesting that cardiometabolic aging improves the model fit. As predicted, accelerated cardiometabolic aging was positively and significantly related to chronic illness (b = 0.093, IRR = 1.098, p < .001), indicating that each 1-year increase in cardiometabolic age was associated with an estimated 9.8% increase in the incidence of chronic illness. Moreover, consistent with the mediation argument, the relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and chronic illness was no longer significant when the measure of accelerated aging is included in the model. This pattern of results suggests that accelerated cardiometabolic aging is a mediator of the effect of neighborhood concentrated disadvantage on chronic illness.
H3: The associations between neighborhood disadvantage and accelerated cardiometabolic aging will be stronger for individuals who perceived lower neighborhood collective efficacy than those who did not.
Model 1 of Table 6 introduces the variable of perceived neighborhood collective efficacy. Although the effect of neighborhood disadvantage was significant, there was no significant association between perceived collective efficacy and accelerated cardiometabolic aging. The last model in Table 6 incorporates the interaction of neighborhood disadvantage with neighborhood collective efficacy to test stress-buffering effects. As predicted, the interaction effect of neighborhood disadvantage and their collective efficacy was significant (b = −0.611, p = .011), we graphed the effect in Figure 5. In the figure, high perceived collective efficacy is defined as 1 SD above the sample mean, and low perceived collective is defined as 1 SD below the sample mean. Consistent with the stress-buffering hypothesis, the figure demonstrates that individuals living in advantaged neighborhoods showed significantly decelerated cardiometabolic aging. In addition, perceived neighborhood collective efficacy had no effect on accelerated cardiometabolic aging of those living in advantaged neighborhoods. However, individuals living in disadvantaged neighborhoods with low perceived collective efficacy in their neighborhood showed significantly accelerated biological aging. Conversely, individuals with high collective efficacy were protected, supporting the buffering hypothesis.
Table 6.
Pooled regression models using four waves (ages 18 to 29) of data examining the relationships among neighborhood disadvantage, perceived collective efficacy, and accelerated cardiometabolic aging
Model 1 |
Model 2 |
|
---|---|---|
b (SE) | b (SE) | |
Concentrated disadvantage (CS) | 0.718** (0.225) |
0.713** (0.222) |
Perceived collective efficacy | −0.245 (0.307) |
−0.230 (0.299) |
CS × Perceived collective efficacy | −0.611* (0.238) |
|
Racial composition | 0.554 (0.933) |
0.571 (0.923) |
Males | 1.152 (0.643) |
1.153 (0.625) |
Annual income | −0.468* (0.216) |
−0.504* (0.220) |
Education | −0.248* (0.104) |
−0.249* (0.104) |
Married/cohabiting | 1.210 (0.944) |
1.296 (0.943) |
Employment status | 0.242 (0.517) |
0.235 (0.512) |
Living with parents | 0.522 (0.485) |
0.582 (0.483) |
Victimization | 0.549 (1.096) |
0.545 (1.111) |
Health insurance | 0.679 (0.664) |
0.663 (0.661) |
Healthy diet | 0.026 (0.111) |
0.021 (0.111) |
Exercise | 0.154 (0.110) |
0.139 (0.109) |
Sleep quality | 0.359 (0.358) |
0.404 (0.351) |
Constant | −0.509 (0.484) |
−0.561 (0.480) |
Note: Unstandardized (b) shown with robust standard errors in parentheses; models adjust for selection effects through application of inverse-probability-of-treatment weighting; income and neighborhood disadvantage are standardized by z-transformation (mean = 0 and SD = 1); all time-varying covariates are lagged to the previous wave (T-1). N(persons) = 408.
p≤ .05
p≤ .01 (two-tailed tests)
Figure 5.
The effect of neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated cardiometabolic aging by level of perceived collective efficacy. Vertical bars indicate the 95% confidence interval. N = 408.
H4: The indirect effect of neighborhood disadvantage on chronic illness through accelerated cardiometabolic aging will vary as a function of the degree to which individuals perceive neighborhood collective efficacy
Given that perceived neighborhood collective efficacy buffers the effect of neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated cardiometabolic aging, a conditional indirect effect model (Preacher et al., 2007) was used to test the extent to which the interaction of neighborhood disadvantage and collective efficacy on chronic illness was mediated by accelerated cardiometabolic aging. As expected, the indirect effect of this interaction term on report of diagnosed chronic illness through accelerated cardiometabolic aging was significant [indirect effect = −0.356, 95% CI (−0.595, −0.117)] and accounted for about 88% of the total variance, suggesting it is important in explaining neighborhood level effects on chronic illness. This pattern of findings supports the hypothesis that the indirect effect of neighborhood disadvantage through speed of aging on chronic illness is conditional on levels of perceived neighborhood collective efficacy.
Power Analysis
To ensure adequate power to detect a significant interaction effect given the current sample size, power analyses were conducted based on the results showed in Tables 2 and 3. According to the G*Power program (Faul et al., 2007) for a regression model, for an effect of this size, the current sample (n = 408) provided more than 90% power. This suggests that our sample was an adequate size to test our theoretical models.
Discussion
Social stress is related to illness, with much of the supporting research guided by the stress process model (Pearlin, 1989, 1999). This model posits that prolonged exposure to stressful conditions chronically activates a physiological stress response that increases the chances of tissue damage, impaired immune functioning, and illness. Studies examining the link between stress and illness have typically focused on personal stressors. Recently, however, researchers have begun to investigate the extent to which adverse neighborhood conditions might also increase the risk for disease (Aneshensel et al., 2016). A popular hypothesis is that it is the ambient threat posed by socioeconomically disadvantaged neighborhoods that is highly stressful and leads to illness beyond the contribution of individual socioeconomic status and individual experiences and health behaviors. Consonant with this idea, several studies have reported an association between neighborhood context and various health outcomes (Brody et al., 2014; Browning et al., 2012; Chen et al., 2015; Hill et al., 2005; Lei et al., 2017a; Ross & Mirwosky, 2009).
Using marginal structural modeling to account for neighborhood selection effects, the present study was an attempt to overcome methodological and theoretical limitations that have hampered previous studies of the link between neighborhood and health. Our goal was to utilize more objective measures of both neighborhood characteristics and illness, and to expand the basic neighborhood stress process model by incorporating a plausible biological mediator, cardiometabolic aging. Specifically, we extended prior research in several ways.
First, most studies employ self-reported measures from the same source. As a consequence, associations reported between neighborhood characteristics and illness may be inflated due to shared methods variance (Podsakoff et al., 2003). We avoided this problem in the present study by using multiple methods of assessment. We used the American Community Survey (ACS), collected by the US Census Bureau, to measure objective characteristics present within neighborhoods, self-report to assess perceived neighborhood collective efficacy, blood-derived biomarkers and biometics to assess cardiometabolic age, and report of diagnosed chronic illness to assess morbidity and health.
In addition to the problem of shared methods variance, many neighborhood studies limit their analysis to a single time point of neighborhood characteristics (e.g., for an exception, see White et al., 2017). As a result, we cannot examine possible developmental mechanisms of health and determine the age at which neighborhood effects began or exert their strongest influence. For instance, is current exposure to disadvantaged neighborhoods more important for accelerated aging and health? Is durable exposure to disadvantaged neighborhoods more consequential for accelerated aging and health than exposure during any specified period of time? Using four waves of data across the span of young adulthood (ages 18 ─ 29), the current results indicated that across measurement methods, neighborhood disadvantage at each wave was associated with accelerated cardiometabolic aging and chronic illness. Importantly, long-term exposure to neighborhood disadvantage has durable effects on accelerated biological aging and chronic illness. Thus, neighborhood disadvantage effects on individual outcomes tend to be relatively stable over time among African Americans. Just as Robert Sampson and colleagues (2008, p. 852) notes, “When we consider moves into and out of concentrated disadvantage among a representative sample of black children, not just the poor, durable inequality matters.” This durable-effects model of neighborhood disadvantage that takes account of a temporal process is particularly important for sociological models of health.
Second, the stress process model posits that heightened physiological stress serves as the mechanism that links adverse conditions to illness. Nevertheless, potential mediating physiological factors are rarely assessed. This is particularly true of neighborhood studies. According to the weathering hypothesis (Geronimus, 2013), the present study tested the hypothesis that accelerated cardiometabolic aging is an important physiological mechanism through wear and tear on the body that mediates much of the impact of neighborhood disadvantage on chronic illness, and may be a particularly important mechanism for young-adult African Americans. Consistent with previous studies (Lei et al., 2017a; Simons et al., 2016), we found a robust association between neighborhood disadvantage and accelerated cardiometabolic aging that was robust to controls for neighborhood selection bias, sociodemographic, and health-related variables. Further, we found that speed of cardiometabolic aging mediated much of the association between neighborhood disadvantage and illness. Indeed, such association was no longer significant once accelerated aging was entered into the model.
Finally, although we found significant relationships between neighborhood disadvantage and both accelerated aging and chronic illness, the magnitude of these associations, as expected, were modest. Amplifying the importance of neighborhood characteristics, we also found results supporting the stress-buffering hypothesis where high levels of collective efficacy moderated the effect of neighborhood disadvantage on health. Theory and research have emphasized the extent to which neighborhood collective efficacy can operate as a resilience and protective mechanism (e.g., Liu et al., 2016; Norris et al., 2008; Simons et al., 2005) that improve people’s ability to cope with neighborhood disadvantages and stressors. Consonant with this idea, our results indicated that the association between neighborhood disadvantage and speed of aging was ameliorated when individuals perceived high levels of collective efficacy from their neighborhood. And, findings from the moderated-mediation analysis revealed that a substantial portion of the explained variance in chronic illness was accounted for by the interaction of neighborhood disadvantage and perceived neighborhood collective efficacy. These results provide important evidence that the health consequences of neighborhood conditions may not be the same for all people. Perceived neighborhood collective efficacy may be an important protective factor for those residing in disadvantaged neighborhoods. In the presence of lower collective efficacy, individuals will tend to be less resilient to neighborhood stress effects, and so develop more problematic health outcomes. If so, interventions designed to enhance level of perceived neighborhood collective efficacy and cohesion may counter the stressors, which over time, lead to biological embedding and onset of chronic illness.
Unlike in some previous studies, however, our analyses did not reveal a main effect for neighborhood collective efficacy on accelerated aging and chronic illness. The explanations for this finding may be methodological or theoretical. First, the focus of the current study is on the lived experiences of African Americans. Yet, African Americans are unevenly distributed geographically, being concentrated in disadvantaged neighborhoods (Peterson & Krivo, 2010). Given that disadvantaged neighborhoods typically have lower collective efficacy (Sampson et al., 1997), the problem of restricted distributions may also have limited our ability to test the main effect hypothesis.
On the other hand, the absence of a main effect may be a consequence of the inherent nature of collective efficacy. Studies finding a main effect, such as Sampson and colleagues (1997), were focused on violence or criminal behaviors as outcomes. Research focused on health (e.g., Burdette, Wadden, & Whitaker, 2006; Franzini et al., 2005) often has not found a main effect of collective efficacy on health outcomes. This may be because collective efficacy, at least as it is defined and measured by Sampson and colleagues, emphasizes trust and the belief that one’s neighbors will intervene for the good of the community (e.g., call the police, pick up trash, reprimand children engaging in antisocial behavior). Thus collective efficacy is a cultural resource, a set of norms and commitments shared with other members of the neighborhood, which can be relied upon when the order and safety of the neighborhood are threatened. Collective efficacy is a type of social capital but it is a property of the neighborhood rather than the individual, and its value is most evident when social disorder poses a threat.
This is in contrast to social capital in the form of social support. Social support is an individual-level resource that involves access to emotional and instrumental assistance from the members of one’s social network. Social support implies social connections, social integration, and close ties to others. As such, it would be expected to directly affect an individual’s mental and physical health. Although social support may help an individual cope with various personal stressors encountered in life, it may be that support from a friend can do little to help a person deal with the anxiety and helplessness of living in a neighborhood characterized by crime and social disorder. Collective efficacy, in the other hand, is a community resource that directly addresses the potential dangers and threats of life in a disadvantaged neighborhood. All of this suggests that collective efficacy is unlikely to have a main effect on health; rather its importance is as a moderator of the effect of community disadvantage on health. Conversely, social support might be expected to have a main effect on health, but fail to moderate the effect of community disadvantage. There is a need for future research that focuses on the different ways in which these two types of social capital influence health.
Together, the current study presents an integrated model that combines environments and biology to understand the influence of neighborhood disadvantage on health during the transition to adulthood. Beyond this contribution, findings from the link between neighborhood and health also advance the literature on the field of culture and biology. Given that culture is embedded in multiple social contexts (Causadias, 2013), research has emphasized that exposure to neighborhood disadvantage is often associated with cultural framing, habits, norms, and values (Anderson, 1999; Wilson, 2009). For example, in Code of the Street, Elijah Anderson (1999) indicated that residents of disadvantaged neighborhoods tend to adopt the norms of the street code subculture to handle their everyday routines. Studies of African Americans have also highlighted cultural promotive and protective factors (e.g., cultural pride, spiritually, and cultural socialization) among African American neighborhoods and cultures and its protective role in the face of adversities and stressors (Burt, Lei, & Simons, 2017; Causadias, 2013; Constantine, Donnelly, & Myers, 2002; Gaylord-Harden, Burrow, & Cunningham, 2012).
In addition to individual behaviors, recent research has established ways to integrate cultural experiences and biological processes in development (Causadias, 2013). It may be that culture, as both social and individual sets of processes, has an effect on our biological systems. Their findings combined with those for the present study, suggest that durable exposure to disadvantaged neighborhoods gives rise to cultural (e.g., social norms and values) and social schemas (e.g., street code and cultural pride) that increase or decrease the probability of accelerated biological aging and chronic illness during adulthood. Accordingly, an important future direction will be to integrated culture into the biopsychosocial models and to show that cultural effects mediate the association between neighborhood disadvantage and aging/chronic illness.
Limitations
Although our study was able to extend past research in several respects, it also suffered from various limitations. Chief among them is the fact that our sample was limited to African Americans, suggesting the need to see whether similar processes can be identified in other vulnerable groups who may experience elevated levels of neighborhood disadvantage. In some respects, however, this shortcoming might be seen as a strength. Myriad studies have established that African Americans are at higher risk than other ethnic groups for chronic illness (Adler & Rehkopf, 2008), biological weathering due to social context (Geronimus, 2013; Simons et al., 2016), and exposure to disadvantaged neighborhoods (Peterson & Krivo, 2010). All of this argues for the relevance of research investigating the effect of neighborhood conditions on the health of African Americans. That said clearly there is a need to replicate our findings with samples that are more ethnically diverse.
Second, several studies (Clark, Anderson, Clark, & Williams, 1999; Pascoe & Richman, 2009; Simons et al., 2018) have indicated a relation between discrimination and health outcomes among African Americans through biopsychosocial mechanisms. Given that racial discrimination is a common experience for those living in disadvantaged neighborhoods (Massey, 2004), this may be the case for African Americans who live in these neighborhoods. But, this was not tested in the present work. Future research should incorporate discrimination into the link between neighborhood context and health outcomes.
A third limitation that deserves to be mentioned is our use of marginal structural modeling to adjust neighborhood selection bias. Although we included a series of potential confounders that have been previously reported to be associated with neighborhood disadvantage (Sampson et al., 2008) and biological aging (Lei et al., 2017a), our results may have been influenced by unobserved confounders that violated the assumption of marginal structural modeling. Accordingly, findings should be viewed as tentative pending replication with other samples and sets of control variables.
Another caveat is related to our specification of theoretical pathways. In line with the weathering hypothesis (Geronimus, 2013; Simons et al., 2016), we hypothesized that accelerated cardiometabolic aging as an index of wear and tear on the body would mediated the association between neighborhood disadvantage and chronic illness. It is also possible, however, that chronic illness is a mediator of neighborhood disadvantage on speed of aging during early adulthood. To investigate this idea, we tested a model using chronic illness as a mediator. Our results did not support this reversal of the mediating hypothesis. However, because measures of cardiometabolic age and chronic illness in our data set were measured in age 29, our ability to test a causal mediator may be limited (MacKinnon, Fairchild, & Fritz, 2007). Accordingly, the results obtained in the current study need to be replicated in a data set with multiple assessments of biological aging and chronic illness.
Finally, although it seems likely that shared method variance would result in inflated associations (Podsakoff et al., 2003), and so the effect of self-reported neighborhood characteristics on health would be inflated if both were self-reported, we did not directly test this hypothesis. Future studies should replicate and extend the current study by including self-reported measure of neighborhood characteristics to compare with census tract measures such as those used in the current study. In addition, our assessment of biological aging relied on the Framingham algorithm. While this aging score is commonly used and comprises biomarkers that can be obtained easily and inexpensively, future studies should replicate the current study with other measures of age acceleration including measures of accelerated epigenetic aging.
Conclusion
In conclusion, while our study suffered from certain constraints, it also extended prior research in a number of respects. In addition to proffering more convincing evidence of an association between neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage and illness, we found durable effects of neighborhood disadvantage on accelerated aging and chronic illness. This is particularly important in the study of health and aging. We also identified speed of cardiometabolic aging as an important mediating biopsychosocial mechanism that links stressful neighborhood conditions to chronic illness. Finally, our analyses indicated that collective efficacy from neighborhoods serves to buffer the association between neighborhood and health. Overall, our findings suggest neighborhoods as fundamental causes of health and illness. In addition, the importance of constructing integrated models that bring together social and biological variables. By specifying biopsychosocial mechanisms that link social conditions to illness, models of biological embedding of social circumstances become more comprehensive and precise. They also become more useful and compelling for medical and public health professionals concerned with designing social policies and preventive interventions to enhance health.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by Award Number R01 HL8045 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, R01 HD080749 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, R01 AG055393 from the National Institute on Aging, and P30 DA027827 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Contributor Information
Man-Kit Lei, Department of Sociology and Center for Family Research University of Georgia.
Steven R.H. Beach, Department of Psychology and Center for Family Research University of Georgia
Ronald L. Simons, Department of Sociology University of Georgia
References
- Adler NE, & Rehkopf DH (2008). US disparities in health: descriptions, causes, and mechanisms. Annual Review of Public Health, 29, 235–252. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090852 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson E (1999). Code of the streets: Decency, violence, and the moral life of the inner city. New York, NY: Norton. [Google Scholar]
- Aneshensel CS, & Sucoff CA (1996). The neighborhood context of adolescent mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 37, 293–310. doi: 10.2307/2137258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aneshensel CS (2009). Neighborhood as a social context of the stress process In Avison WR, Aneshensel CS, Schieman S, & Wheaton B (Eds.). Advances in the Conceptualization of the Stress Process: Essays in Honor of Leonard I. Pearlin (pp. 35–52). Springer, New York: NY. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-1021-9_3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Aneshensel CS, Harig F, & Wight RG (2016). Aging, neighborhoods, and the built environment In Gorge LK & Ferraro KF (Eds.) Handbook of Aging and the Social Sciences (8th ed., pp. 315–335). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-417235-7.00015-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Arnett JJ (2004). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens through the twenties. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Beach SRH, Dogan MV, Lei M-K, Cutrona CE, Gerrard M, Gibbons FX, … Philibert RA (2015). Methylomic aging as a window onto the influence of lifestyle: Tobacco and alcohol use alter the rate of biological aging. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 63, 2447–2654. doi: 10.1111/jgs.13830 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beach SRH, Lei M-K, Brody GH, Kim S, Barton AW, & Philibert RA (2016). Parenting, socioeconomic status risk, and later young adult health: Exploration of opposing indirect effects via DNA methylation. Child Development, 87, 111–121. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12486 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beach SR, Lei MK, Simons RL, Barr AB, Simons LG, Ehrlich K, … & Philibert RA (2017). When inflammation and depression go together: The longitudinal effects of parent–child relationships. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 1969–1986. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417001523 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Ruttle PL, Boyce WT, Armstrong JM, & Essex MJ (2015). Early trauma, elevated stress physiology, accelerated sexual maturation and poor health in female. Developmental Psychology, 51, 816–822. doi: 10.1037/dev0000017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair A, Ross NA, Gariepy G, & Schmitz N (2014). How do neighborhoods affect depression outcomes? A realist review and a call for the examination of causal pathways. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49, 873–887. doi: 10.1007/s00127-013-0810-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boardman JD, Onge JMS, Rogers RG, & Denney JT (2005). Race Differentials in Obesity: The Impact of Place. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 46, 229–243. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600302 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bosma H, Dike van de Mheen H, Borsboom GJ, & Mackenbach JP (2001). Neighborhood socioeconomic status and all-cause mortality. American Journal of Epidemiology, 153, 363–371. doi: 10.1093/aje/153.4.363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody GH, Lei MK, Chen E, & Miller GE (2014). Neighborhood poverty and allostatic load in African American youth. Pediatrics, 134, e1362–e1368. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-1395 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody GH, Miller GE, Yu T, Beach SR, & Chen E (2016a). Supportive family environments ameliorate the link between racial discrimination and epigenetic aging: A replication across two longitudinal cohorts. Psychological Science, 27, 530–541. doi: 10.1177/0956797615626703 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody GH, Yu T, & Beach SR (2016b). Resilience to adversity and the early origins of disease. Development and Psychopathology, 28, 1347–1365. doi: 10.1017/s0954579416000894 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Browning CR, Cagney KA, & Iveniuk J (2012). Neighborhood stressors and cardiovascular health: Crime and C-reactive protein in Dallas, USA. Social Science & Medicine, 75, 1271–1279. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.03.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Browning CR, Gardner M, Maimon D, & Brooks-Gunn J (2014). Collective efficacy and the contingent consequences of exposure to life-threatening violence. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1878–1890. doi: 10.1037/a0036767 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burdette HL, Wadden TA, & Whitaker RC (2006). Neighborhood safety, collective efficacy, and obesity in women with young children. Obesity, 14, 518–525. doi: 10.1038/oby.2006.67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burt CH, Lei M-K, & Simons RL (2017). Racial discrimination, racial socialization, and crime over time: A social schematic theory model. Criminology, 55, 938–979. doi: 10.1111/1745-9125.12164 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Causadias JM (2013). A roadmap for the integration of culture into developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 1375–1398. doi: 10.1017/s0954579413000679 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Causadias JM, Telzer EH, & Lee RM (2017). Culture and biology interplay: An introduction. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 23, 1–4. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen E, & Miller GE (2013). Socioeconomic status and health: mediating and moderating factors. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 723–749. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185634 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen E, Miller GE, Brody GH, & Lei M (2015). Neighborhood poverty, college attendance, and diverging profiles of substance use and allostatic load in rural African American youth. Clinical Psychological Science, 3, 675–685. doi: 10.1177/2167702614546639 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, & Garmezy N (1993). Prospects and promises in the study of resilience. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 497–502. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400006118 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, & Toth SL (2009). The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 16–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01979.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark R, Anderson NB, Clark VR, & Williams DR (1999). Racism as a stressor for African Americans: A biopsychosocial model. American Psychologist, 54, 805–816. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.10.805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S (1988). Psychosocial models of the role of social support in the etiology of physical disease. Health Psychology, 7, 269–297. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.7.3.269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantine MG, Donnelly PC, & Myers LJ (2002). Collective self-esteem and africultural coping styles in African American adolescents. Journal of Black Studies, 32, 698–710. doi: 10.1177/00234702032006004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- D’Agostino RB, Vasan RS, Pencina MJ, Wolf PA, Cobain M, Massaro JM, & Kannel WB (2008). General cardiovascular risk profile for use in primary care. Circulation, 117, 743–753. doi: 10.1161/circulationaha.107.699579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Agostino RB, Pencina MJ, Massaro JM, & Coady S (2013). Cardiovascular disease risk assessment: insights from Framingham. Global Heart, 8, 11–23. doi: 10.1016/j.gheart.2013.01.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davies TL, Gompels M, Johnston S, Bovill B, & May MT (2013). Mind the gap: difference between Framingham heart age and real age increases with age in HIV-positive individuals–a clinical cohort study. BMJ Open, 3, e003245. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-003245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diez Roux AV, & Mair C (2010). Neighborhoods and health. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1186, 125–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05333.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Do DP, & Zheng C (2017). A marginal structural modeling strategy investigating short and long-term exposure to neighborhood poverty on BMI among US black and white adults. Health & Place, 46, 201–209. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2017.05.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans GW, Kim P, Ting AH, Tesher HB, & Shannis D (2007). Cumulative risk, maternal responsiveness, and allostatic load among young adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 43, 341–351. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.341 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, & Buchner A (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finegood ED, Rarick JR, Blair C, & Family Life Project Investigators. (2017). Exploring longitudinal associations between neighborhood disadvantage and cortisol levels in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 1649–1662. doi: 10.1017/s0954579417001304 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fone D, Dunstan F, Lloyd K, Williams G, Watkins J, & Palmer S (2007). Does social cohesion modify the association between area income deprivation and mental health? A multilevel analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology, 36, 338–345. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster H, & Brooks-Gunn J (2013). Neighborhood influences on antisocial behavior during childhood and adolescence In Gibson CL & Krohn MD (Eds), Handbook of Life Course Criminology: Emerging Trends and Directions for Future Research (pp. 69–90). New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5113-6_5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Franzini L, Caughy M, Spears W, & Esquer MEF (2005). Neighborhood economic conditions, social processes, and self-rated health in low-income neighborhoods in Texas: A multilevel latent variables model. Social Science & Medicine, 61, 1135–1150. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.02.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaylord-Harden NK, Burrow AL, & Cunningham JA (2012). A cultural-asset framework for investigating successful adaptation to stress in African American youth. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 264–271. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00236.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Genovese G, Friedman DJ, Ross MD, Lecordier L, Uzureau P, Freedman BI, … & Bernhardy AJ (2010). Association of trypanolytic ApoL1 variants with kidney disease in African Americans. Science, 329, 841–845. doi: 10.1126/science.1193032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geronimus AT (2013). Deep integration: Letting the epigenome out of the bottle without losing sight of the structural origins of population health. American Journal of Public Health, 103, S56–S63. doi: 10.2105/ajph.2013.301380 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groenewegen KA, Den Ruijter HM, Pasterkamp G, Polak JF, Bots ML, & Peters SA (2016). Vascular age to determine cardiovascular disease risk: a systematic review of its concepts, definitions, and clinical applications. European Journal of Preventive Cardiology, 23, 264–274. doi: 10.1177/2047487314566999 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamer D, & Sirota L (2000). Beware the chopsticks gene. Molecular Psychiatry, 5, 11–13. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000662 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hannum G, Guinney J, Zhao L, Zhang L, Hughes G, Sadda S, … & Deconde R (2013). Genome-wide methylation profiles reveal quantitative views of human aging rates. Molecular Cell, 49, 359–367. doi: 10.1016/j.molcel.2012.10.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heckman JJ (1979). Sample selection bias as a specification error. Econometrica, 47, 153–161. doi: 10.2307/1912352 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hernán MÁ, Brumback B, & Robins JM (2000). Marginal structural models to estimate the causal effect of zidovudine on the survival of HIV-positive men. Epidemiology, 11, 561–570. doi: 10.1097/00001648-200009000-00012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill TD, Ross CE, & Angel RJ (2005). Neighborhood disorder, psychophysiological distress, and health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 46, 170–186. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirsch JR, Waits G, Li Y, & Soliman EZ (2018). Racial differences in heart age and impact on mortality. Journal of the National Medical Association, 110, 169–175. doi: 10.1016/j.jnma.2017.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horvath S (2013). DNA methylation age of human tissues and cell types. Genome Biology, 14, R115. doi: 10.1186/gb-2013-14-10-r115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horvath S, Erhart W, Brosch M, Ammerpohl O, von Schönfels W, Ahrens M, … & Deloukas P (2014). Obesity accelerates epigenetic aging of human liver. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 15538–15543. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1412759111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humphrey JL, & Root ED (2017). Spatio-temporal neighborhood impacts on internalizing and externalizing behaviors in US elementary school children: Effect modification by child and family socio-demographics. Social Science & Medicine, 180, 52–61. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson BT, & Acabchuk RL (2018). What are the keys to a longer, happier life? Answers from five decades of health psychology research. Social Science & Medicine, 196, 218–226. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konen CJ, Summerson JH, Bell RA, & Curtis LG (1999). Racial differences in symptoms and complications in adults with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Ethnicity and Health, 4, 39–49. doi: 10.1080/13557859998182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kravitz-Wirtz N (2016). Temporal effects of child and adolescent exposure to neighborhood disadvantage on black/white disparities in young adult obesity. Journal of Adolescent Health, 58, 551–557. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.01.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latkin CA, & Curry AD (2003). Stressful neighborhoods and depression: a prospective study of the impact of neighborhood disorder. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 44, 34–44. doi: 10.2307/1519814 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lei MK, Beach SR, Simons RL, & Philibert RA (2015). Neighborhood crime and depressive symptoms among African American women: Genetic moderation and epigenetic mediation of effects. Social Science & Medicine, 146, 120–128. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.10.035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lei MK, Beach SR, Simons RL, Barr AB, Cutrona CE, & Philibert RA (2016). Stress, relationship satisfaction, and health among African American women: Genetic moderation of effects. Journal of Family Psychology, 30, 221–232. doi: 10.1037/fam0000140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lei MK, Simons RL, Beach SR, & Philibert RA (2017a). Neighborhood disadvantage and biological aging: Using marginal structural models to assess the link between neighborhood census variables and epigenetic aging. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. Advance online publication, doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbx015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lei MK, Beach SR, Dogan MV, & Philibert RA (2017b). A pilot investigation of the impact of smoking cessation on biological age. The American Journal on Addictions, 26, 129–135. doi: 10.1111/ajad.12502 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lei MK, Beach SRH, & Simons RL (2018). Childhood trauma, pubertal timing, and cardiovascular risk in adulthood. Health Psychology. Advance online publication, doi: 10.1037/hea0000609 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin N, & Ensel WM (1989). Life stress and health: Stressors and resources. American Sociological Review, 54, 382–399. doi: 10.2307/2095612 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Link BG, & Phelan J (1995). Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 35, 80–94. doi: 10.2307/2626958 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, Mustanski B, Dick D, Bolland J, & Kertes DA (2016). Risk and protective factors for comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems among economically disadvantaged African American youth. Development and Psychopathology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416001188 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Fairchild AJ, & Fritz MS (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 593–614. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marioni RE, Shah S, McRae AF, Chen BH, Colicino E, Harris SE, … & Deary IJ (2015). DNA methylation age of blood predicts all-cause mortality in later life. Genome Biology, 16, 25. doi: 10.1186/s13059-015-0584-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Massey DS (2004). Segregation and stratification: a biosocial perspective. Du Bois Review: Social Science Research on Race, 1, 7–25. doi: [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS, & Coatsworth JD (1998). The development of competence in favorable and unfavorable environments. American Psychologist, 53, 205–220. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.53.2.205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller GE, Chen E, & Parker KJ (2011). Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 959–997. doi: 10.1037/a0024768 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller GE, Yu T, Chen E, & Brody GH (2015). Self-control forecasts better psychosocial outcomes but faster epigenetic aging in low-SES youth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 10325–10330. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1505063112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt TE, Belsky DW, Danese A, Poulton R, & Caspi A (2017). The longitudinal study of aging in human young adults: knowledge gaps and research agenda. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A, 72, 210–215. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw191 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murabito JM, Zhao Q, Larson MG, Rong J, Lin H, Benjamin EJ, … & Lunetta KL (2017). Measures of biologic age in a community sample predict mortality and age-related disease: The Framingham Offspring Study. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A. Advance online publication, doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norris FH, Stevens SP, Pfefferbaum B, Wyche KF, & Pfefferbaum RL (2008). Community resilience as a metaphor, theory, set of capacities, and strategy for disaster readiness. American Journal of Community Psychology, 41, 127–150. doi: 10.1007/s10464-007-9156-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pascoe EA, & Richman LS (2009). Perceived discrimination and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 531–554. doi: 10.1037/a0016059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearlin LI (1989). The sociological study of stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 30, 241–256. doi: 10.2307/2136956 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearlin LI (1999). The stress process revisited: Reflections on concepts and their interrelationships In Aneshensel CS & Phelan JC (Eds.), Handbook of the Sociology of Mental Health (pp. 395–415). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. doi: 10.1007/0-387-36223-1_19 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Perna L, Zhang Y, Mons U, Holleczek B, Saum KU, & Brenner H (2016). Epigenetic age acceleration predicts cancer, cardiovascular, and all-cause mortality in a German case cohort. Clinical Epigenetics, 8. doi: 10.1186/s13148-016-0228-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterson RD, & Krivo LJ (2010). Divergent social worlds: Neighborhood crime and the racial-spatial divide. Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
- Phelan JC, Link BG, & Tehranifar P (2010). Social conditions as fundamental causes of health inequalities: theory, evidence, and policy implications. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51, S28–S40. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383498 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Philibert RA, Beach SR, Gunter TD, Todorov AA, Brody GH, Vijayendran M, … & Cutrona C (2011). The relationship of deiodinase 1 genotype and thyroid function to lifetime history of major depression in three independent populations. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 156, 593–599. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee JY, & Podsakoff NP (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ, Rucker DD, & Hayes AF (2007). Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 185–227. doi: 10.1080/00273170701341316 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robins JM (2000). Marginal structural models versus structural nested models as tools for causal inference In Elizabeth Halloran M & Berry D (Eds.), Statistical models in epidemiology, the environment, and clinical trials (pp. 95–134). New York, NY: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4612-1284-3_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ross CE, & Jang SJ (2000). Neighborhood disorder, fear, and mistrust: The buffering role of social ties with neighbors. American Journal of Community Psychology, 28, 401–420. doi: 10.1023/a:1005137713332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross CE, & Mirowsky J (2001). Neighborhood disadvantage, disorder, and health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42, 258–276. doi: 10.2307/3090214 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross CE, & Mirowsky J (2009). Neighborhood disorder, subjective alienation, and distress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 50, 49–64. doi: 10.1177/002214650905000104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross CE (2011). Collective threat, trust, and the sense of personal control. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 52, 287–296. doi: 10.1177/0022146511404558 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M (2016). Why is the topic of the biological embedding of experiences important for translation?. Development and Psychopathology, 28, 1245–1258. doi: 10.1017/s0954579416000821 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, & Earls F (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science, 277, 918–924. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.918 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, & Gannon-Rowley T (2002). Assessing “neighborhood effects”: Social processes and new directions in research. Annual Review of Sociology, 28, 443–478. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.28.110601.141114 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Sharkey P, & Raudenbush SW (2008). Durable effects of concentrated disadvantage on verbal ability among African-American children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 845–852. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710189104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer JD, Caspi A, Belsky DW, Harrington H, Houts R, Israel S, … & Moffitt TE (2015). Early-life intelligence predicts midlife biological age. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 71, 968–977. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw CR & McKay HD (1969). Juvenile Delinquency and Urban Areas (revised ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Simons RL, Simons LG, Burt CH, Brody GH, & Cutrona C (2005). Collective efficacy, authoritative parenting and delinquency: A longitudinal test of a model integrating community‐and family‐level processes. Criminology, 43, 989–1029. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2005.00031.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Simons RL, Lei MK, Beach SR, Philibert RA, Cutrona CE, Gibbons FX, & Barr A (2016). Economic hardship and biological weathering: the epigenetics of aging in a US sample of black women. Social Science & Medicine, 150, 192–200. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons RL, Lei MK, Beach SR, Barr AB, Cutrona CE, Gibbons FX, & Philibert RA (2017). An index of the ratio of inflammatory to antiviral cell types mediates the effects of social adversity and age on chronic illness. Social Science & Medicine, 185, 158–165. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons RL, Lei MK, Beach SR, Barr AB, Simons LG, Gibbons FX, & Philibert RA (2018). Discrimination, segregation, and chronic inflammation: Testing the weathering explanation for the poor health of Black Americans. Developmental Psychology. Advance online publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thoits PA (2010). Stress and health: Major findings and policy implications. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51, S41–S53. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas AJ, Eberly LE, Davey Smith G, Neaton JD, & Stamler J (2005). Race/ethnicity, income, major risk factors, and cardiovascular disease mortality. American Journal of Public Health, 95, 1417–1423. doi: 10.2105/ajph.2004.048165 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner RJ, & Marino F (1994). Social support and social structure: A descriptive epidemiology. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 35, 193–212. doi: 10.2307/2137276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace RG, Twomey LC, Custaud MA, Turner J, Moyna N, Cummins PM, & Murphy RP (2017). The role of epigenetics in cardiovascular health and aging: a focus on physical activity and nutrition. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development. Advance online publication, doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2017.11.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White RMB, Zeiders KH, Knight GP, Roosa MW, & Tein J-Y (2014). Mexican Origin Youths’ Trajectories of Perceived Peer Discrimination from Middle Childhood to Adolescence: Variation by Neighborhood Ethnic Concentration. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43, 1700–1714. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0098-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wickes R, Hipp JR, Sargeant E, & Homel R (2013). Collective efficacy as a task specific process: Examining the relationship between social ties, neighborhood cohesion and the capacity to respond to violence, delinquency and civic problems. American Journal of Community Psychology, 52, 115–127. doi: 10.1007/s10464-013-9582-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson WJ (2009). More than just race: Being black and poor in the inner city. New York, NY: WW Norton & Company. [Google Scholar]
- Wodtke GT, Elwert F, & Harding DJ (2016). Neighborhood effect heterogeneity by family income and developmental period. American Journal of Sociology, 121, 1168–1222. doi: 10.1086/684137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zannas AS, Arloth J, Carrillo-Roa T, Iurato S, Röh S, Ressler KJ, … Mehta D (2015). Lifetime stress accelerates epigenetic aging in an urban, African American cohort: relevance of glucocorticoid signaling. Genome Biology, 16(1). doi: 10.1186/s13059-015-0828-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zheng Y, Joyce BT, Colicino E, Liu L, Zhang W, Dai Q, … & Jafari N (2016). Blood epigenetic age may predict cancer incidence and mortality. EBioMedicine, 5, 68–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2016.02.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]