Abstract
Diminished cardiac reactions to acute psychological stress are associated with adverse behavioral and health-relevant outcomes. It has been proposed that diminished cardiac reactivity may be a marker for deficits in motivational functioning both at the biological and behavioral levels. Social participation reflects the frequency with which individuals participate in social events and has motivational components. As such, it is a distinct construct from other constructs such as social integration, which measures the number of social roles an individual has. Additionally, lower levels of social participation and diminished reactivity have been associated with similar adverse health outcomes. Therefore, it is possible that diminished cardiac reactivity is associated with lower levels of social participation. We aimed to examine whether diminished cardiovascular reactivity in response to an acute lab stressor was associated with reported social participation. The analyses were conducted using publicly available data from the Pittsburgh Cold Study 3 (PCS3). The PCS3 was a prospective viral-challenge study, which included participants completing an in-lab social evaluative stressor (N=202, Age= M= 29.71, SD= 10.66) and measuring cardiovascular responses at baseline and in response to the stressor. Separate regression analyses for each cardiovascular variable (SBP, DBP, MAP, and HR) demonstrated that lower cardiovascular reactivity was associated with less social participation. These associations were still evident following adjustment for respective baseline cardiovascular levels, age, sex, race, depressive symptomology, body mass index, socioeconomic status, smoking status, and levels of social integration (R2 changes: ≥ .017; ps ≤ .02) The findings provide initial evidence that blunted cardiac reactivity may be a precursor to low levels of social participation.
Extreme cardiovascular responses to acute psychological stress are associated with adverse health outcomes [1,2]. Individuals who show exaggerated cardiovascular reactions to acute psychological stress are more likely to develop hypertension [3], atherosclerosis [4], and increased left ventricular mass [5]. They are also at an increased risk for cardiovascular disease mortality [6]. However, recent evidence suggests that diminished cardiovascular reactions to acute psychological stress are also associated with adverse health outcomes, mainly behavioral outcomes [2,7,8]. For example, diminished levels of cardiovascular reactivity are associated with lower levels of behavioral perseverance [9–10], and higher levels of depressive symptomology [11–15]. It has been proposed that diminished reactivity is a marker of a dysfunction of neural systems that support motivated and goal directed behavior and that it may be associated with outcomes that reflect, whether explicitly or inadvertently, variations in psychological effort and motivation [2,7.15].
Another behavioral outcome, which involves motivation, is social participation, or the frequency with which an individual engages in social activities with others [16].The act of social participation requires motivated effort, which is distinct from social integration. Social integration reflects the number of social roles an individual has, yet is also associated with a host of health-relevant outcomes [17]. An individual may have a high number of social roles, however, these roles do not necessarily require motivation and may or may not be desired by the individual. Given that diminished cardiovascular activity is associated with reduced motivated behavior, lower levels of perseverance, and higher levels of depression, it is possible that individuals with diminished cardiovascular responses to stress are less likely to engage in social participation.
Interestingly, many of the observed relationships between social participation and health are the same as the those observed between cardiovascular reactivity and health [1,2,18]. For example, higher levels of social participation are related to better self-rated health [19–20], less cognitive decline [21–22], and lower depressive symptoms [23–24]. Similarly, diminished cardiac reactivity to acute psychological stress is associated with worse self-rated health [25–26], greater cognitive decline [27,28], and higher depressive symptoms [29,30, 31]. Therefore, it is possible that in addition to being a marker of adverse health and behavioral outcomes, diminished reactivity may also be a characteristic of individuals who are less likely to engage in social participation. Extending from this, it is plausible that diminished cardiac reactivity to acute stress may precede lower levels of social participation, however, this relationship has yet to be examined. If diminished cardiac reactivity is associated with low levels of social participation, it could be another behavioral pathway through which diminished reactivity affects mental and physical health.
The aim of this study was to examine the association between cardiovascular responses to acute psychological stress and social participation. Based on the motivational component required of social participation and the similar relationships between health and social participation and health and cardiovascular reactivity, it was hypothesized that low cardiovascular reactivity would be associated with lower social participation.
Methods
The data in this manuscript were from the Pittsburgh Cold Study 3 (PCS3). The PCS3 was a prospective viral challenge study with data collected between 2007–2011. The sample consisted of 213 healthy volunteers ages 18–55 (Mean=30.1; SD=10.9) from the Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania metropolitan area. Data were collected by members of the Laboratory for the Study of Stress, Immunity, and Disease at Carnegie Mellon University. This lab is directed by Sheldon Cohen, Ph.D. Data were obtained from the Common Cold Project website (www.commoncoldproject.com; grant number NCCIH AT006694).
Participants provided informed consent and received $1000 for their participation of the entire protocol. The study was approved by the Carnegie Mellon University and University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Boards. The exclusion criteria for the larger project were: regular medication regimen, previous nasal surgery, psychiatric hospitalization within the last 5 years history of chronic illness or any psychiatric disorder treated within one year of study enrollment, abnormal clinical profile (discovered via urinalysis, complete blood count or analysis of blood chemistry), human immunodeficiency virus seropositivity, current pregnancy or lactating, use of steroids or immunosuppressants within three months of trial, participation in another study involving psychological questionnaires and or investigational products within the last 30 days or plans to participate in such research while enrolled in the current study, cold or flu-like illness within 30 days prior to the infection with virus as part of this project, living with someone who has chronic obstructive pulmonary disease or an immunodeficiency, previous hospitalization for a flu like illness and allergies to eggs or egg products. In the current study, we focused on physiological responses to a stress task during a session was completed before virus inoculation and its relation to self-reported social participation. Social participation was measured in a questionnaire administered 6 weeks prior to the quarantine period.
For the stress task, participants attended an individual session that started between 3:00 pm and 9:00 pm at the University of Pittsburgh. Participants were asked to abstain from alcohol for 48 hours, from exercise and nonprescription medications for 24 h, from eating and drinking (except water) for 2 h, and from smoking for 1 h prior to the session. Upon arriving at the lab, participants were interviewed to ensure they had followed these instructions.
At the beginning of the stress protocol, participants were asked to sit quietly for 20 minutes (Habituation phase). Immediately following the Habituation phase, participants performed a modified version of the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) [32]. They were told they had to deliver a 5-minute videotaped speech defending themselves against an alleged transgression (shoplifting or traffic violation) and a 5-minute mental arithmetic task. The protocol included an Anticipatory phase in which the participants had 5 minutes to prepare their speech. Immediately following the Anticipatory phase, the participants performed their speech and completed the arithmetic task (Stress phase). Finally, the participants were asked to rest quietly for 50 minutes (Recovery phase).
Participants were fitted with automated blood pressure (BP) cuff for measurement of systolic and diastolic blood pressure (SBP and DBP), mean arterial pressure (MAP) and heart rate (HR). During the 20-minute habituation period, HR and BP were measured 4 times (every 120 seconds) during the last 6 minutes. During the 15-minute Stress phase, HR and BP were measured every 2 minutes, yielding 9 readings (start of Stress phase, 2 minutes, 4 minutes, 6 minutes, 8 minutes, 10 minutes, 12 minutes, 14 minutes and 16 minutes). While the stress task itself took approximately 15 minutes, there was some time allotted for transitions between tasks. As such, the final HR and BP readings were taken at 16 minutes after the beginning of the Stress phase. Three of these readings were taken during the speech preparation phase, 3 were taken during the speech delivery phase, and 3 were taken during the mental arithmetic phase. Finally, during the recovery phase, HR and BP were measured every 120 seconds for the first 15 minutes (8 readings) and then every 5 minutes (7 readings).
Measures
Cardiovascular Reactivity.
The study included two stress task sessions. However, only data from the first session are reported since the aim was not to examine habituation in physiological responses to a repeated task. Cardiovascular stress reactivity was defined as the difference between the mean averages during stress and baseline for each of the four cardiovascular variables.
Social Participation Measure.
Using a 6-point frequency scale (1= did not do this at all in the past year, 2= 1–5 times this past year, 3= once a month or every two months, 4= once every two or three weeks, 5= once a week, 6= more than once a week), respondents indicated how often they engaged in 16 social activities during the past 12 months (e.g., visited friends, participated in a competitive sport, went to a restaurant/bar/coffee shop with others). Responses were summed to reflect total social participation. The measure had good reliability in this study (α=0.77).
Social Integration.
Using data from the social network index, a measure of social integration was calculated. This is a measure of the number of different types of high contact social roles in which individuals participate. High contact roles are defined as those for which the respondent reports engaging at least once every two weeks. For example, for a participant to be assigned the role of “parent”, they must indicate that they talk to at least one of their children at least once every two weeks. Social integration is computed as the sum of all high contact roles [17,33]. Social integration was used as a covariate to examine if any relationships observed were due to active participation (with a motivational component) or simply from being involved in a number of social roles.
Covariates
Depressive Symptoms.
The Trait Affect Questionnaire was administered twice during the PCS3 [33,34]. It consisted of 25 items and produced 5 subscales of negative affect, including depression. The depression subscale consisted of three items including sadness, unhappiness, and depression. It was administered once 7–8 weeks before the quarantine period and again immediately preceding the quarantine period. As a measure of depressive symptoms, we utilized the score from the measurement immediately preceding the quarantine period. The measure had good reliability in this study (α=0.87).
Body Mass Index.
Body mass index was calculated using participants’ height and weight recorded at the reactivity session.
Socioeconomic Status.
As a measure of current socioeconomic status, participants reported their current annual income. Reported income was categorized across 13 categories (1= less than $5,000, 2= $5,000-$9,999, 3= $10,000-$14,999, 4= $15,000-$19,999, 5=$20,000-$29,999, 6=$30,000-$39,999, 7= $40,000-$49,999, 8=$50,000-$59,999, 9=$60,000-$74,99, 10=$75,000-$99,999, 11= $100,000-$124,99, 12=$125,000-$149,999, 13= $150,000 or more).
Smoking Status.
Participants reported whether they were a current smoker (0= no, 1= yes).
Statistical analyses
Analyses were conducted using SPSS version 25 (IBM Corp, USA). Repeated-measures ANOVAs, using baseline and stress task values, were undertaken to confirm that the TSST perturbed the cardiovascular system. The associations between cardiovascular reactivity and social participation were analyzed using multiple linear regression with social participation as the dependent variable first in models that adjusted only for respective baseline cardiovascular levels. Then analyses were run in models that additionally adjusted for age, sex, race (dummy coded as 1 = White, 0 = Other), depressive symptoms, body mass index, socioeconomic status, and smoking status. These covariates were selected based on previously documented associations between these variables and cardiovascular reactivity [2,35–37]. Lastly, to ensure that the social participation results were independent of social integration, a measure of number of high contact social roles, a third model additionally adjusting for social integration was run for each of the cardiovascular reactivity variables.
Results
Descriptive statistics are listed in Table 1 and bivariate correlations between main variables of interest are listed in Table 2. In this sample, social participation was associated social integration (r=.48).
Table 1.
Measure | n | M (SD)/% | Range |
---|---|---|---|
Age | 202 | 29.7 (10.7) | 18–55 |
Race | 202 | 64.8% White | |
Socioeconomic Status | 202 | 3.9 (2.6) | 1–13 |
Social Participation | 202 | 52.5 (11.9) | 25–86 |
Social Integration | 202 | 5.2 (1.9) | 2–10 |
Depressive Symptoms | 202 | .39 (.72) | 0–3 |
Body Mass Index | 202 | 27.4 (6.5) | 16.9–50.7 |
Current Smokers | 202 | 33.8% | |
Social Support | 202 | 29.2 (5.7) | 6–36 |
Table 2.
MAP residual | DBP residual | SBP residual | HR residual | |
---|---|---|---|---|
1. age |
−0.07 | −0.10 | −0.08 | −0.30** |
2. sex |
−0.20** | −.24** | −.19** | −.01 |
3. race |
.28** | .25** | .36** | .21** |
4. depressive symptoms |
.04 | .05 | −.02 | .02 |
5. social participation |
.28** | .26** | .30** | .26** |
6. social integration |
−.07 | .04 | .11 | .07 |
Note: MAP: Mean Arterial Pressure, DBP: Diastolic Blood Pressure, SBP: Systolic Blood Pressure, HR: Heart Rate. Sex: male= 0 , female = 1. Race: 1= White/Caucasian, 0= all others.
Cardiovascular Stress Reactivity
Repeated measures ANOVA (baseline, task) indicated that the TSST significantly perturbed SBP, F(1, 205) = 418.40, p < .001, peta2 = .671; DBP, F(1, 205) = 567.84, p < .001, peta2 = .735; MAP, F(1, 205) = 680.47, p < .001, peta2 = .768; and HR, F(1, 205) = 356.70, p < .001, peta2 = .635. The summary statistics are presented in Table 3.
Table 3.
SBP mmHg | DBP mmHg | MAP mmHg | HR bpm | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | 114.8 (12.6) | 67.5 (83) | 84.8 (8.8) | 69.2 (10.5) |
Stress* | 130.4(14.5) | 77.9 (8.1) | 98.3 (9.4) | 80.0 (12.6) |
Stress differed from Baseline for all four variables, p < .001 in each case.
Cardiovascular stress reactivity and Social Participation
Cardiovascular stress reactivity for all variables of interest (SBP, DBP, MAP, and HR) were positively associated with social participation; the lower the cardiovascular reactivity the lower the social participation. Effect sizes and other statistics of these associated are presented in Table 4. Table 4 also shows that, firstly, adjustment for respective baseline cardiovascular activity and then, additionally, for age, sex, race, depressive symptoms, body mass index, socioeconomic status, and smoking status did not abolish the associations between cardiovascular reactivity and social participation for any of the variables. Although the effect sizes became smaller, in all cases the associations remained statistically significant. Similarly, when additionally adjusting for social integration, the associations between cardiovascular reactivity and social participation remained statistically significant.
Table 4.
β | t | p | R2 change | |
---|---|---|---|---|
SBP reactivity | ||||
Model 1 | .31 | 4.45 | <.001 | .089 |
Model 2 | .24 | 3.36 | .001 | .046 |
Model 3 | .17 | 2.69 | .008 | .023 |
DBP reactivity | ||||
Model 1 | .28 | 3.85 | <.001 | .066 |
Model 2 | .22 | 2.89 | .004 | .034 |
Model 3 | .19 | 2.82 | .005 | .026 |
MAP reactivity | ||||
Model 1 | .30 | 4.25 | <.001 | .081 |
Model 2 | .24 | 3.33 | .001 | .045 |
Model 3 | .19 | 3.06 | .003 | .030 |
HR reactivity | ||||
Model 1 | .26 | 3.89 | <.001 | .068 |
Model 2 | .18 | 2.52 | .01 | .026 |
Model 3 | .14 | 2.28 | .023 | .017 |
Notes: Model 1 includes respective baseline cardiovascular activity; Model 2 additionally includes age, sex, race, depressive symptoms, body mass index, socioeconomic status, and smoking status; Model 3 additionally includes social integration.
Conclusion
Our analyses indicate that diminished cardiovascular reactivity in response to an acute laboratory stressor is associated with lower levels of self-reported social participation. The associations remained significant when controlling for important variables related to both acute psychological stress and social participation (e.g., age, gender, socioeconomic status) and when controlling for how many social contacts an individual has. These findings are in line with a growing body of research which indicates that diminished cardiovascular reactions to stress are associated with impairments in behaviors that require motivation [2,9,10].
Diminished cardiovascular reactivity is believed to be a marker of a dysfunction of the neural systems, which support motivated and goal directed behavior [2,38]. Blunted cardiovascular reactors exhibit reduced activation during acute psychological stress in areas involved in autonomic regulation [38,39]. Areas of the brain implicated in autonomic processing also comprise a network vital for motivated behavior [40]. It has been proposed that individuals with diminished reactivity to stress have a wider biological disengagement which prevents full engagement in activities that require motivated behavior [41]. Therefore, if blunted reactivity is a marker of a wider biological disengagement, it is possible that individuals with diminished cardiovascular reactivity are less able to engage in social participation because of differences in the function of neural systems which are critical to the promotion of social engagement and participation.
Given that social participation requires motivation, lower levels of cardiovascular reactivity could give rise to lower levels of social participation resulting in poorer health outcomes. This is supported by the overlap in negative health outcomes associated with both low cardiovascular reactivity and social support (e.g., depression, cognitive decline, poorer self-rated health). Additionally, social participation has potential health benefits. Social participation is viewed as social sharing of individual resources through active participation in social activities and is believed to exert positive effects on health by providing a sense of meaning and increasing access to social support [42]. Future work is needed to understand whether cardiovascular reactivity associates with future social participation and whether levels of social participation may affect downstream health outcomes by shaping psychosocial factors and resources.
There are important limitations to this research. First, since the research is cross-sectional, causality cannot be inferred [43]. For example, it is possible that social experience may shape cardiovascular reactivity or more specifically that lack of social engagement could lead to diminished reactivity. The current origins of diminished reactivity are unknown [2]. Longitudinal or experimental research examining cardiovascular reactivity and social participation is needed to help establish directionality. While analyses adjusted for depressive symptomology, the scale used was not a clinically validated scale. Future research should aim to replicate and extend these findings using a clinically validated depression scale. Further, while cardiovascular reactivity was measured in real-time in a lab setting, the measure of social participation reflects how often participants engaged in a variety of social activities during the last 12 months and is self-report. As noted previously, this data cannot speak to whether cardiovascular reactivity would predict future levels of social participation. However, previous research does indicate that levels of social participation and cardiovascular reactivity are relatively stable over time [44,45]. Future research should explore whether individuals who exhibit diminished cardiovascular reactivity are unsatisfied with their level of social participation and whether they are motivated to become more socially engaged.
Highlights:
Blunted Cardiovascular stress reactivity associates with lower social participation
Relationship was independent of depressive symptoms and social integration
Cardiovascular stress reactivity could be a precursor to social participation
Acknowledgments
The data used for this article were collected by the Laboratory for the Study of Stress, Immunity, and Disease at Carnegie Mellon University under the directorship of Sheldon Cohen, PhD; and were accessed via the Common Cold Project (CCP) website (www.commoncoldproject.com). CCP data were made available to the public through a grant from the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (NCCIH AT006694). The study was supported by grants from the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases (R01 AI066367). Secondary funding was provided by the National Institutes of Health through a grant awarded to the University of Pittsburgh Clinical and Translational Science Institute (UL1 RR024153 and UL1 TR000005).
Footnotes
Declarations of interest: none
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Chida Y, & Steptoe A, Greater cardiovascular responses to a laboratory mental stress are associated with poor subsequent cardiovascular risk status: a meta-analysis of prospective evidence, Hypertens. 55 (2010) 1026–1032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Carroll D, Ginty AT, Whittaker AC, Lovallo WR, & De Rooij SR, S.R, The behavioural, cognitive, and neural corollaries of blunted cardiovascular and cortisol reactions to acute psychological stress, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 77 (2017) 74–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Carroll D, Phillips AC, Der G, Hunt K, & Benzeval M, Blood pressure reactions to acute mental stress and future blood pressure status: data from the 12-year follow-up of the West of Scotland study, Psychosom. Med 73 (2011) 737–742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Barnett PA, Spence JD, Manuck SB, Jennings JR, Psychological stress and the progression of carotid artery disease, J. Hypertens 15 (1997) 49–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kapuku GK, Treiber FA, Davis HC, Harshfield GA, Cook BB, & Mensah GA, Hemodynamic function at rest, during acute stress, and in the field: predictors of cardiac structure and function 2 years later in youth, Hypertens. 34 (1999) 1026–1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Carroll D, Ginty AT, Der G, Hunt M, Benzeval M, & Phillips AC, Increased blood pressure reactions to acute mental stress are associated with 16-year cardiovascular disease mortality, Psychophysiology. 49 (2012) 1444–1448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Carroll D, Lovallo W, & Phillips A, Are Large Physiological Reactions to Acute Psychological Stress Always Bad for Health? Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 3 (5) (2009) 725–743. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Phillips AC, Ginty AT, & Hughes BM, The other side of the coin: Blunted cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity are associated with negative health outcomes, Int. J. Psychophysiology 90(1) (2013) 1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.M. Al’absi M, Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenocortical responses to psychological stress and risk for smoking relapse, Int. J. Psychophysiology 59 (2006) 218–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ginty AT, Brindle RC, & Carroll D, Cardiac stress reactions and perseverance: Diminished reactivity is associated with study non-completion, Biol. Psychol 109 (2015) 200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Carroll D, Phillips AC, Hunt K, & Der G , Symptoms of depression and cardiovascular reactions to acute psychological stress: Evidence from a population study, Biol. Psychol 75(1) (2007) 68–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Brindle RC, Ginty AT, & Conklin SM, Is the association between depression and blunted cardiovascular stress reactions mediated by perceptions of stress? Int. J. Psychophysiology 90(1) (2013) 66–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Franzen J, & Brinkmann K, Anhedonic symptoms of depression are linked to reduced motivation to obtain a reward, Motiv. Emot 40(2), (2016) 300–308. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Schwerdtfeger A, & Rosenkaimer A, Depressive symptoms and attenuated physiological reactivity to laboratory stressors. Biol. Psychol 87(3) (2011) 430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Salomon K, Clift A, Karlsdóttir M, Rottenberg J, & Kaplan RM, Major Depressive Disorder Is Associated With Attenuated Cardiovascular Reactivity and Impaired Recovery Among Those Free of Cardiovascular Disease, Health Psychol. 28(2) (2009) 157–165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Chen JH, Lauderdale D, & Waite L, Social participation and older adults’ sleep, Soc. Sci. Med 149, (2016) 164–173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, Rabin BS, & Gwaltney JM, Social ties and susceptibility to the common cold, JAMA 277, (1997) 1940–1944. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Dahan-Oliel N, Gelinas I & Mazer B, Social participation in the elderly: what does the literature tell us? Crit. Rev. Phys. Rehabil. Med 20, (2008) 159–176. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Li Y, Xu L, Chi I, & Guo P, Participation in productive activities and health outcomes among older adults in urban china, Gerontologist 54 (2014) 784–796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lee HY, Jang SN, Lee S, Cho SI, & Park EO, The relationship between social participation and self-rated health by sex and age: a cross-sectional survey, Int. J. Nurs. Stud 45 (2008) 1042–1054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Haslam C, Cruwys T, & Haslam SA, “The we’s have it”: Evidence for the distinctive benefits of group engagement in enhancing cognitive health in aging, Soc. Sci. Med 120(C) (2014) 57–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bourassa K, Memel M, Woolverton C, & Sbarra D, Social participation predicts cognitive functioning in aging adults over time: Comparisons with physical health, depression, and physical activity, Aging Ment. Health, 21(2) (2017) 133–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Croezen S, Avendano M, Burdorf A, & Van Lenthe F, Social Participation and Depression in Old Age: A Fixed-Effects Analysis in 10 European Countries, Am. J. Epidimiol 182(2), (2015) 168–176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Takagi D, Kondo K, & Kawachi I, I, Social participation and mental health: Moderating effects of gender, social role and rurality, BMC Pub. Health, 13(1), (2013) 701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Phillips AC, Blunted cardiovascular reactivity relates to depression, obesity, and self-reported health, Biol. Psychol 86(2) (2011) 106–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.De Rooij S & Roseboom T, Brief Reports: Further evidence for an association between self-reported health and cardiovascular as well as cortisol reactions to acute psychological stress, Psychophysiology. 47(6), (2010) 1172–1175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ginty AT, Phillips AC, Roseboom T, Carroll D, & Derooij S, Cardiovascular and cortisol reactions to acute psychological stress and cognitive ability in the Dutch Famine Birth Cohort Study: Reactivity and cognitive ability, Psychophysiology. 49(3) (2012) 391–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Yano Y, Ning H, Reis J, Lewis C, Launer L, Bryan R,– Liu K, Blood Pressure Reactivity to Psychological Stress in Young Adults and Cognition in Midlife: The Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults ( CARDIA ) Study, J. Am. Heart Assoc (2016) 5(1) 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Franzen J, & Brinkmann K, Blunted cardiovascular reactivity in dysphoria during reward and punishment anticipation, Int. J. Psychophysiology (2015) 95(3), 270–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Schiweck C, Piette D, Berckmans D, Claes S, & Vrieze E, Heart rate and high frequency heart rate variability during stress as a biomarker for clinical depression. A systematic review, Psychol. Med (2019) 49(2) 200–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Salomon K, Bylsma LM, White KE, Panaite V, & Rottenberg J, Is blunted cardiovascular reactivity in depression mood-state dependent? A comparison of major depressive disorder remitted depression and healthy controls, Int. J. Psychophysiology. (2013) 90(1), 50–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kirschbaum C, The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’—a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting, Neuropsychobiology, 28 (1993) 76–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Turner RB, Alper CM, & Skoner DP, Emotional style and susceptibility to the common cold, Psychosom, Med, 65 (2003) 652–657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Usala PD, & Hertzog C, Measurement of affective states in adults. Evaluation of an adjective rating scale instrument, Res. Aging (1989) 403–426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kelly JS, & Vickery S, Effect of Smoking status on cardiovascular reactivity in young adults, Psychophysiology. 44 (2007) S98–S98. [Google Scholar]
- 36.La Marca R, Loza N, Lekic C, Kuehne F, Rutishauser A, Farahmand F, Ehlert U, Body mass index is related to blunted cardiovascular reactivity in healthy young men, Psychoneuroendocrinol. 71 (2016) 35–35. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Chen E, Matthews KA, Socioeconomic status and cardiovascular reactivity: The development of a measure to test the mediating role of interpretations of social situations, Psychosom. Med 63 (2001) 110–110. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Ginty AT, Gianaros PJ, Derbyshire SW, Phillips AC, & Carroll D, Blunted cardiac stress reactivity relates to neural hypoactivation, Psychophysiology. 50 (2013) 219–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gianaros PJ, Sheu LK, Matthews KA, Jennings JR, Manuck SB, & Hariri AR, Individual differences in stressor-evoked blood pressure reactivity vary with activation, volume, and functional connectivity of the amygdala, J. Neurosci 28 (2008) 990–999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Gianaros PJ, Onyewuenyi IC, Sheu LK, Christie IC, & Critchley HD, Brain systems for baroreflex suppression during stress in humans, Hum. Brain Mapp 33 (2012) 1700–1716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ginty AT, Blunted responses to stress and reward: reflections of biological disengagement, Int. J. Psychophysiology 90 (2013) 90–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kawachi I, & Berkman LF, Social ties and mental health, J. Urban Health 78, (2001) 458–467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Christenfeld N, Sloan RP, Carroll D, & Greenland S, Risk factors, confounding, and the illusion of statistical control, Psychosom. Med 66 (2004) 868–875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hassellund SS, Flaa A, Sandvik L, Kjeldsen SE, & Rostrup M, Long-term stability of cardiovascular and catecholamine responses to stress tests: an 18-year follow-up study, Hypertens. 55 (2010) 131–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hyyppa MT, J. Maki J, Alanen E, Impivaara O, & Aromaa A, Long term stability of Social Participation, Soc. Indic. Res 88, (2007) 388–396. [Google Scholar]