Skip to main content
F1000Research logoLink to F1000Research
. 2019 Feb 12;8:67. Originally published 2019 Jan 17. [Version 2] doi: 10.12688/f1000research.17157.2

A physiological examination of perceived incorporation during trance

Helané Wahbeh 1,2,a, Cedric Cannard 1, Jennifer Okonsky 1, Arnaud Delorme 1
PMCID: PMC6384530  PMID: 30815253

Version Changes

Revised. Amendments from Version 1

We would like to thank the reviewers for their thoughtful feedback to the manuscript. The revised manuscript has included all of their suggestions including:

  1. Original grant protocol in Extended Data 1

  2. Information about stories read for voice analysis

  3. More information about the use of depth of trance and incorporation values

  4. Addition of descriptive statistics for ECG and respiration within the text of paper (Table 4)

  5. Exploratory post-hoc paired t-test analysis of galvanic skin response

  6. Exploratory post-hoc analyses of EEG data by location with additional data table in Extended Data

  7. Use of "channeler" throughout the text to refer to the agent channeling

  8. Various suggestions for future studies. Additionally, minor editing changes were made to improve readability.

Abstract

Background: Numerous world cultures believe channeling provides genuine information, and channeling rituals in various forms are regularly conducted in both religious and non-religious contexts. Little is known about the physiological correlates of the subjective experience of channeling.

Methods: We conducted a prospective within-subject design study with 13 healthy adult trance channels. Participants alternated between 5-minute blocks of channeling and no-channeling three times while electroencephalography (EEG), electrocardiography (ECG), galvanic skin response (GSR), and respiration were collected on two separate days. Voice recordings of the same story read in channeling and no-channeling states were also analyzed.  

Results: The pre-laboratory survey data about demographics, perception of the source, purpose and utility of channeled information reflected previous reports. Most participants were aware of their experience (rather than in a full trance) and had varying levels of perceived incorporation (i.e. control of their body). Voice analysis showed an increase in voice arousal and power (dB/Hz) differences in the 125 Hz bins between 0 and 625 Hz, and 3625 and 3875 Hz when reading during the channeling state versus control. Despite subjective perceptions of distinctly different states, no substantive differences were seen in EEG frequency power, ECG measures, GSR and respiration.

Conclusions: Voice parameters were different between channeling and no-channeling states using rigorous controlled methods, but other physiology measure collected were not. Considering the subjective and phenomenological differences observed, future studies should include other measures such as EEG connectivity analyses, fMRI and biomarkers.

Keywords: trance channeling, mediumship, anomalous information reception, electroencephalography, electrocardiography, galvanic skin response, voice analysis, spirit possession

Introduction

Channeling has been defined as: “The communication of information to or through a physically embodied human being, from a source that is said to exist on some other level or dimension of reality than the physical as we know it, and that is not from the normal mind (or self) of the channel” 1. Numerous world cultures believe channeling provides genuine information, and channeling rituals in various forms are regularly conducted in both religious and non-religious contexts 24. Research suggests that channeling-related phenomena continue to be prevalent in contemporary cultures 5. In a recent survey of 899 people in the United States, 19.6% of respondents endorsed that they “Had a non-physical source from a different level or dimension of reality use your body as an instrument for communication?” 6. Trance channeling can be understood as a form of channeling in which an individual willingly enters degrees of trance-like states of consciousness whereby the channel connects with sources of information that appear to exist outside of their ego-awareness. Trance channels use their body as a “vehicle” for the purported disincarnate “being” to incorporate into and to communicate directly via speaking, writing, or movement. Religious groups such as the Spiritists in Brazil 7 and Spiritualists in the United Kingdom 8 engage in full-trance channeling as part of their traditions and provide training programs on how to channel.

There is a paucity of scientific information on channeling, what it is, and how it works. This may be due in part to a number of challenges to studying channeling, such as variability in channeling type, information source, and content. One of the most comprehensive works on the topic is by Jon Klimo who describes some of these variable components 1. For example, he describes many types of subjective channeling experiences:

  • mental - intuitive, telepathy, clairaudience, clairvoyance, clairsentience

  • automatism - a variant of conscious, but which includes kinesthetic expressions of automatic writing, Ouija board movement, or pendulum movement

  • full-trance – purported disincarnate being incorporates into channeler’s body to communicate

  • sleep and dream – channeling occurs during sleep and channeler recalls information

While the purported source and content of channeled information are variable, common sources and overarching themes have been noted 1. Purported sources include the “higher self,” deceased human beings, gods and/or God, a universal mind, collective unconscious, group beings, Jesus Christ, angels, devas/elementals, plants or animals, extraterrestrials, or earthbound spirits. Deceased humans are the largest reported category of source information. Common content themes are “ageless wisdom,” guidance and personal messages, descriptions of life in non-physical realms, the past and/or future, artistic/creative or scientific/technological material, health and healing, and information from or about deceased humans 9. A recent qualitative study of channeled material collected during a focus group of trance channels revealed five common content themes similar to the themes mentioned in Klimo’s book: 1) mechanisms of channeling; 2) the need to awaken humanity and methods by which to do so; 3) the nature of reality; 4) descriptions of multi-dimensional beings and worlds; and 5) suggestions for advancing channeling research 10.

Well-known trance channelers primarily channel one being (e.g. Esther Hicks and Abraham, Jane Roberts and Seth). However, not all trance channels report channeling only one purported source. A recent preliminary study observed multiple types of channeling in five trance channels with various purported beings communicating through the same channeler. The most common was the traditional subjective experience where one person channeled one “being.” There were also several instances where multiple channels (up to 4) channeled different purported “beings” simultaneously. In these cases, more than one channeler expressed that they were going to begin channeling and would then do so simultaneously. The purported incorporated “beings” would then have a conversation with each other and with the group. There were two attempts at materialization of a higher dimensional “being” and two instances where multiple channels channeled the same “being” sequentially 10.

Research on channeling has not yet revealed whether channeling is a unique state of consciousness (pathological or otherwise). Three types of dissociative disorders expressing these phenomena have been defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5 th edition (DSM-V) 11: Dissociative amnesia, depersonalization disorder and dissociative identity disorder (DID). DID is defined as a personality disorder, when two or more distinct identities or personalities are present, each with its own pattern of perceiving, relating to and thinking about the environment and self 11. Pathological dissociation is often associated with historic physical, emotional and sexual abuse 1214. Dissociative states are also prevalent in a number of other psychiatric disorders, such as Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) 12, 15, Attention deficit disorder 12, schizophrenia, and anxiety disorders 16, and are more prevalent in nonclinical populations at younger ages 15. Mixed results associate dissociative possession experiences with childhood or adult trauma; some preliminary studies show that the prevalence of possession and paranormal experiences is related to childhood trauma 17, whereas others do not 18. However, dissociative states exist on a continuum in the general population 1921, from non-pathological expressions such as highway hypnosis and day-dreaming, to pathological states of derealization (surrealness), and depersonalization (absence of identity) 14. Although trance states are currently considered a symptom of dissociation 16, 20, 22, trance mediums do not show higher rates of pathological dissociation than the general population 23, 24. Most researchers would define mediums as people who communicate with discarnate or deceased personalities on a regular basis 25 while trance mediums do so while under trance. Roxburgh and Roe surveyed 233 mediums and spiritualists in the United Kingdom and found no significant difference between mediums and non-medium spiritualists on the Dissociative Experience Scale. In fact, mediums scored significantly higher on psychological well-being and lower on psychological distress in comparison to non-mediums 8. Although, Negro, Palladino-Negro and Louza surveyed 110 participants of a Kardecist Center in Brazil and reported that mediumship was associated with dissociation, but not with high-level pathological dissociation 7. In general, most mediums and channels do not have dissociative symptoms in their daily lives above clinical cut-off scores 8, 10, 23, 24, 26.

One study evaluated the veracity of channeled information through automatic writing 27. A few studies have evaluated the veracity, information source and physiology of mental channels rather than trance channels 2831. Mental channels report that they communicate with deceased human beings or other discarnate entities mentally rather than the purported being using their body to communicate directly. Decreased frontal electroencephalography (EEG) power was associated with accurately discerning between photographs of deceased or living humans where visual processing as indexed by the early visual activity in the parieto-occipital right cortex differed between correct versus incorrect responses 32. While we may never know if the mental channeler is actually contacting deceased humans or tapping into some telepathic reservoir of knowledge 33, we can investigate the cognitive and physiological processes by which people access information that is not available to them through conventional means.

Can research reveal how channeling could work even if one hypothesized that channels were in fact relaying accurate information from purported discarnate “beings”? Very few studies have evaluated trance channels as opposed to mental channels. As far as we know, the first recorded trance channeler study with EEG was by Hughes et al. in 1990. They found large, statistically significant increases in the percentage of time that beta, alpha, and theta brainwave activity were observed during channeling sessions in 10 trance channels 34. The famous channeler JZ Knight was evaluated before, during, and after channeling. The researchers found large increases from baseline in electromyography, skin temperature, heart rate, blood volume pulse, and electrodermal activity; and increased variability in sympathetic activation 1. The details of this study’s methods and analysis are missing so it is uncertain how reliable the findings are. A recent systematic review evaluated trance channels during sessions and found an increase in noradrenaline levels, muscle tone, heart rate, and in spectral power in alpha, beta, and theta EEG frequency bands 26. Another study examined EEG before, during, and after channeling in 10 experienced full-trance channels and 10 non-channeler controls, all recruited from the same Spiritist cultural community 25. The study found that channels compared to the controls had greater beta EEG power in all phases of the experiment, greater theta power on one electrode out of 22 while communicating, and greater alpha power on one electrode during the post-communication phase. This result was not corrected for multiple comparisons and no within-group differences were noted. Another study of possession trances in Bali, Indonesia found that theta, alpha 1 and 2, and beta signals were significantly increased compared to control periods 35. Thus, while reported changes noted in EEG measures may be spurious, they are useful as preliminary efforts to study physiological differences between channeled trance-state and non-channeled states. There have been limited studies on other physiological measures. Bastos and colleagues conducted another study evaluating blood pressure, heart rate variability (HRV), and other neuroendocrine markers. All these measures except for the neuroendocrine markers increased immediately post-channeling 36. Studies of dissociative symptoms and physiology contribute to our understanding of channeling in general and to the unique nature of the channeling state specifically, yet more research is needed.

Anecdotally, many channels have reported unusual sensory or energetic sensations during channeling sessions. Thus, another approach to studying channeling is to incorporate objective measures that may be sensitive to subtle environmental effects associated with shifts in consciousness. In this regard, random number generators (RNG) have been used to study intentional and attentional-related effects in many laboratory and real-world experiments 3739. Our recent study found a statistically different RNG measure during channeling sessions compared to the control periods 10.

These limited preliminary studies have begun to examine the veracity of channeled information, the source of the channeled information, and channeling physiology, but more research is needed on these and other aspects of channeling. Because these experiences have been so foundational to massive movements in human society, such as religions and spiritual traditions, involved in major discoveries and insights, and are relied on by many people to guide everyday decisions (such as listening to God, guardian angels, spirit guides), the mechanisms underlying the subjective experience of channeling are worth investigating. Regardless of the source, altered states of consciousness may provide a pathway for receiving information from beyond the traditional five senses that we could access more reliably if we better understood the neurophysiological pathways mediating them.

The overall goal of this project was to assess neurophysiological correlates associated with the process of channeling. With a prospective within-participant design, the study’s objective was to evaluate neurophysiological measures in trance channels before, during and after channeling sessions to characterize correlates involved in the process of going from a baseline state to a channeling state and back again as pre-defined in the original grant protocol that funded this study (see Extended Data for grant proposal). We hypothesized that differences in EEG frequency measures would be observed between the channeling and no-channeling state. We also hypothesized that differences in the autonomic nervous system measures of heart rate and variability measures, respiration, and electrodermal activity would be observed between the channeling and no-channeling state in the direction of sympathetic activation during the channeling state, also distinguishing the two states as distinct.

Method

Study overview

We conducted a prospective within-subject design study with 13 adult trance channels at the Institute of Noetic Sciences laboratory. The study began January, 2018 and ended July, 2018. All participants completed a screening and data collection survey (Extended data 1 40), submitted a video of themselves channeling, and came to the laboratory for two separate sessions of alternating channeling and no-channeling states while connected to electroencephalography (EEG), electrocardiography (ECG), temperature, and electrodermal electrodes. The study was approved through the Institute of Noetic Sciences Institutional Review Board (IRB Protocol Number WAHH_2014_01). The grant protocol submitted to the BIAL Foundation No 72/16 represents the pre-registration of the measures and analyses included in this manuscript.

Participants

13 trance channels were recruited for this study with the following inclusion and exclusion criteria (assessed by questionnaires described in Procedures below). Inclusion: Adults aged 18 years and older; self-defined full trance channels who have a consensual working relationship with disincarnate beings; able to initiate connection at will; and able to remain still while channeling. Trance channeling was defined as “The channeler goes into a trance state at will (the depth of the trance may vary) and the disincarnate entity/spirit uses the channeler’s body with permission to communicate directly through the channeler's voice, body movements, etc. (rather than the channeler receiving information mentally or otherwise and then relaying what is being received).”

Exclusion: Significant acute medical illness that would decrease likelihood of study completion; psychotic symptoms; Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences-Positive Scale (CAPE-P 15) - Item 3, 4, 12, or 7 >0 41; Dissociative Experiences Scale-T - Score ≥20 42; mediums/channels who mentally relay information communicated by spirit. Because of the sensitivity of the physiological recording equipment, only participants who could remain still while channeling were recruited. Trance channels were used for this study because it was hypothesized that physiology would be affected more than in mental channeling. Exclusion criteria were chosen to ensure the participants were healthy, well-adjusted adults.

Procedures

Participants completed an online survey (Extended data 1 40) administered with the SurveyMonkey platform and developed for this project between January 2018 and May 13, 2018. A consent was embedded into the survey. The survey collected demographic data (race, gender, socioeconomic status 43, 44), inclusion/exclusion criteria information, frequency and characteristics of channeling experiences, paranormal beliefs, and traits relevant to channeling experiences. These questionnaires were chosen to evaluate qualities relevant to channeling experiences. At the end of the survey, volunteers had an opportunity to fill in their contact information if they were interested in participating in the study’s next steps.

The following measures were administered as part of the inclusion/exclusion criteria assessment:

  • Dissociation Experiences Scale Taxon (DES-T) indexes pathological dissociation and has been shown to differentiate between psychiatric presentations that contain dissociative symptoms and those that do not 45. The DES-T is an eight-item subscale of the full-scale DES 46, Cronbach ɑ of 0.75 and is significantly correlated to the larger DES scale ( r = 0.79) 47. Respondents selected a percentage number (e.g., 0% to 100%) indicating the frequency that they experienced the dissociative symptom. Each item was then scored on a scale from 1 to 100 and the overall score was the mean of the eight items. The DES-T distinguishes pathological dissociation more accurately than does the full-scale DES, with a cutoff score of 20 capturing nearly 90% of cases of pathological dissociation.

  • The Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences-Positive Scale (CAPE-P15) 48 is a self-screening questionnaire to address subclinical positive psychotic symptoms in community contexts. It is valid, reliable, has the same three‐factor structure as the lifetime version consisting of persecutory ideation, bizarre experiences and perceptual abnormalities, and is highly predictive of generalized distress ( r = .52) 41.

The following measures were administered to evaluate against population and clinical norms. That is, do the trance channels we recruited for our experiment differ significantly from the general or clinical populations in anxiety, depression, personality, empathy, sensitivity, absorption levels, and paranormal beliefs and experiences? These measures were also collected in order to evaluate them as predictors or moderators of any physiological changes we may have observed between channeling and no-channeling states.

  • Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4) 49 is a 4-item inventory rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale that is a very brief and accurate evaluation of depression and anxiety with established internal reliability, construct validity, and factorial validity 49.

  • Big Five Inventory-10 (BFI-10) 50 is a ten-item scale with personality categories of extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness. Each within each category are averaged to derive category scores (Cronbach α range from 0.74 - 0.89).

  • Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire Absorption Scale 51, 52 is one of the 11 component scales of the larger Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. It has 34 true/false self-report items that assess an individual’s openness to experience, emotional and cognitive alterations across a variety of situations. Summed scores on the instrument are calculated by identifying true responses as 1 and false responses as 0, creating a possible range of 0 to 34, with higher scores indicating stronger trait absorption. Tellegen reported high levels of internal reliability ( r = 0.88) and high levels of test– retest reliability ( r = 0.85) 53.

  • Empathy Quotient 54 is an 8-item questionnaire that was developed to evaluate empathy in the respondent. Each item is rated on a four-point scale (Strongly disagree = 0; Slight disagree = 0; Slightly agree = 1; Strongly agree = 2) with items 5-8 being reverse-scored. The summed value is then multiplied by 2.75 for comparison to original 22-item instrument 54, 55.

  • Highly Sensitive Person Scale is a 6-item self-report questionnaire that evaluates people sensitive to environment and emotions 56. The unidimensional scale has alphas of 0.65–0.85 across numerous samples and high test–retest reliability. Sample items include “Are you easily overwhelmed by things like bright lights, strong smells, coarse fabrics or sirens close by?” and “Do other people's moods affect you?” Respondents reply on a 7-item Likert scale with 1 anchored at Not at All and 7 anchored at Extremely. Items are averaged for a total score.

  • Paranormal Belief and Experience Scale was developed by the Institute of Noetic Sciences because of confounds in many paranormal scales of belief and experience. The instrument was developed to explicitly separate paranormal belief from perceived paranormal experience. Like all explicit questionnaires, bias exists and would be best administered with implicit measures as well. The scale contained ten statements about the belief in intuition, out of body experiences, extraterrestrials, precognition, near death experience, mediumship, clairvoyance, psychokinesis, telepathy, automatism which the participant rated on a slider from Disagree Strongly (0) to Agree Strongly (100). For each of the ten items, participants also answered whether they had ever personally experienced the phenomenon on a slider scale from Never (0) to Always (100). The sliders allow for gradation of belief and experience rather than binary or Likert responses as are often included in these types of questionnaires. Six of the 10 items were taken from the Australian Sheep-Goat Scale, three exact (#’s 9, 10, 11), and three modified (#’s 4, 5, 14) 57. The Cronbach α’s of the belief and experiences components are 0.90 and 0.93 respectively.

Once participants completed the survey, the team reviewed the data and identified participants who met the inclusion and exclusion criteria and expressed interest in the laboratory phase of the channeling study. Potential eligible participants were asked via email to share a five-minute video of their channeling. A member of the research team further screened the potential participants during a telephone screen to ensure they identified as a trance channeler as we defined it, and that they could remain still during their channeling session. The study team chose qualified trance channels after a review of the survey data, telephone screening information, and video. Eligible participants received study instructions and summary via email. A second consent for the in-lab portion of the study was sent via email through DocuSign (DocuSign, Inc. San Francisco, CA).

Participants returned the completed consent and study staff made their travel arrangements. Travel, meals and lodging were paid for by the study. In addition, participants received a $100 gift card at the end of their visit to compensate for their time.

Laboratory procedures

Participants were greeted on the day of the study and oriented to the IONS laboratory. Before being connected to equipment, the participants were asked if they needed to use the restroom, given a printed summary of the laboratory paradigm and informed that they could stop the experiment at any time. They were then seated in a 10 ft. cubed double steel-walled electromagnetically shielded room. Electrophysiological-monitoring devices were then attached to the participants.

Once all the equipment was set up and high signal quality was obtained from all sensors, the laboratory paradigm proceeded as presented in Figure 1. Paradigm A was used for Day 1 and a counter-balanced Paradigm B was used for Day 2. A counterbalanced design was used to ensure that any changes in physiology were not created by the regular alternating of the states or independent biological oscillatory components. Day 1 and Day 2 recordings were always at the same time of day for each participant. Each session consisted of two stories (one channeling, one no-channeling), three 5-minute channeling periods (Channel), three 5-minute mind-wandering periods (No Channeling), and two breaks. Sessions were always scheduled between 10 am and 5pm. First, participants read a story (Extended data 1 40) presented to them on a laminated paper sheet by the research assistant, and their voice was recorded using an Apple iPod (Cupertino, CA). They read “The North Wind” on the first day and the “Rainbow” on the second day. Stories were read for the voice analysis to ensure standardized content during the recordings. These methods are standard practice for voice analyses studies but as far as we know have not been used in a channeling study. During the consenting procedure, we clearly told the participants that this would be part of the protocol and all participants assured us that they would be able to read a story in a channeling and non-channeling state. Participants were then instructed to keep their eyes closed and to remain as still as possible during the physiological recordings. The same story was then read again while channeling. The participants were then asked if the purported discarnate being had a message they wanted to share, which was also audio recorded and then transcribed (qualitative analysis of these transcripts are in progress and will be reported elsewhere, transcripts available as Extended data 1 40).

Figure 1. Laboratory paradigm.

Figure 1.

Channeling and no-channeling states were counterbalanced within sessions and across days. Black dots represent research assistant trigger of marker for transition. Gray dots and dash lines represent participant trigger of marker to confirm completed transition into that state or beginning of immersion into the new state (e.g. after a break). The period between the Black and Gray dots represent transitions into and out of the channeling state. Chanl = Channeling; No Chanl = No Channeling. Note that the 5-minute timing began only after the participant triggered they had achieved the state.

In order to clearly distinguish physiology for the channeling and no-channeling states, a series of markers were inserted into the physiology file by the research assistant and the participant (see black and gray dots in Figure 1). The research assistant would verbally alert the participant to enter the channeling state. After the participant transitioned into the channeling incorporation state, they would press a button on a keyboard which would trigger a marker to note the channeling state had begun. We are calling this an incorporation state since the channeler is not speaking but is purportedly incorporating a discarnate being in their body. The research assistant then asked the participant to exit the channeling state. The participant would press the button on the keyboard to trigger a marker noting that they had transitioned out of the channeling state.

Channeling condition – The instructions for the channeling state were for them to go into their channeling state as they normally would except to remain as still and quiet as possible once they achieved that state.

Control condition – The instructions for the no-channeling control condition were for the participant to actively think about one of three following things for each no-channeling period respectively: 1. Think about all the places you have traveled or want to travel in your life; 2. Think about walking through the grocery store for one of your usual shopping visits (i.e. imagine walking through aisles, picking out items, placing them in your cart, checking out); 3. Think about all the steps required to plant a garden and all the things you would plant. This mind-wandering task was chosen to give the participants an active non-channeling task to focus on.

Breaks – During the breaks, the research assistant would enter the electromagnetically shielded box and ask the participant two questions ( Figure 2): 1. How would you rate the level of consciousness during the last session? The participant would then mark on a 10 cm line anchored by 0 for “Fully conscious” to 10 for “Fully Unconscious” (knowing that the level of trance during channeling could vary by session and by channeler); and 2) How would you rate the level of perceived incorporation? The participant would again mark on a 10 cm line anchored by “No incorporation, my body is not being directly used at all” to “Full incorporation, my whole body is being directly used.” The research assistant then measured the value of the mark and recorded it into the study database.

Figure 2. Levels of consciousness and incorporation rated by the channeler after each channeling session, during each break.

Figure 2.

Temperature and humidity collection: Ambient temperature and humidity of the room were measured with an EIVOTOR Temperature Hygrometer and recorded at the beginning of the experiment, during the breaks and at the end of the experiment.

Physiological Data Collection: All physiological data were recorded at 1024 Hz with an ActiveTwo Biosemi system (Biosemi, Amsterdam, Netherlands) that integrated EEG, ECG, and galvanic skin response (GSR).

  • EEG data were recorded through 64 active electrode channels placed in an electrode cap at standardized electrode positions (20 international 10–20 system locations and 12 intermediate ‘‘10–10’’ locations). SignaGel (Parker Laboratories, Inc.) was used to increase impedance for each electrode and was injected at the surface of the scalp manually. Active electrode offsets (rather than impedance as used with standard electrodes) were kept below manufacturer guidelines (i.e. -/+ 20 mV).

  • ECG data were collected with electrodes placed bilaterally just inferior to the clavicles.

  • Galvanic skin response (GSR) data were collected with one electrode on the palmar thenar eminence and the other on the dorsal aspect between the first and second digits. The GSR used two passive Nihon Kohden electrodes to induce an oscillator signal synchronized with the sample-rate. Because the BioSemi GSR uses "Lock-in detection", the stimulus-current can be as low as 1uA. The low-current and synchronized oscillator ensure that the biopotential measurements (ECG, EEG) are not corrupted by the GSR oscillator signal.

  • Body temperature was collected by an electrode placed on the right index finger of the right hand (palmar side).

Data analysis

For all physiological measures, data was segmented as follows and averages over these time periods were calculated: Day 1 – Chanl.1_1, Chanl.1_2, Chanl.1_3, NoChanl.1_1, NoChanl.1_2, NoChanl.1_3; Day 2 - Chanl.2_1, Chanl.2_2, Chanl.2_3, NoChanl.2_1, NoChanl.2_2, NoChanl.2_3.

EEG - The data was downsampled to 512 Hz with the Biosemi Decimator for analysis. All processing were conducted with the EEGLAB toolbox v. 14 58 in Matlab 2014b (The MathWorks, Inc, Natick, MA, USA). After removing auxiliary channels (processed and analyzed separately) and the offset, high-pass and low-pass filters were applied (i.e. 1 and 50 Hz, respectively). The toolbox Cleanline v. 1.03 was used to filter line-noise artifacts at 16 and 48 Hz. Bad channels and portions of data were removed by visual inspection of the continuous data by experimenter CC. Extended Infomax Independent Component Analysis (ICA) was then used to identify ocular and muscle artifacts (e.g. eye blinks, lateral eye movements). We used the IClabel v. 0.3EEGLAB plugin to classify components, and selected those components which maximum probability lied in the eye or muscle artifactual component category. The continuous data was then segmented into 1-second epochs that were 1-second away from each other (to decrease correlation between neighboring epochs). Traditional frequency analysis was performed using hamming tapered FFT with 1-Hz resolution.

ECG - ECG data was extracted from the Biosemi BDF files using EEGLAB in Matlab and processed as previously described 59. Briefly, ECG data was then imported into Kubios HRV Premium v 3.1.0 (University of Kuopio, Kuopio, Finland) to generate RR intervals, ECG-derived respiration, heart rate, very low, low, and high frequency domain measures (VLF: 0-0.04; LF: 0.04-0.15 Hz; HF: 0.15-0.4 Hz), frequency peak, absolute and normalized amplitude, and LF/HF ratio and nonlinear measures. HRV analysis parameters included a 100 s window width, 50 % window overlap; autoregressive spectrum model order = 16 with no factorization, and interpolation rate = 4 Hz. No participants were on medications that affected ECG (e.g., beta blockers and calcium channel blockers) 60, 61.

Respiration – ECG-derived respiration was calculated with Kubios HRV Premium (University of Kuopio, Kuopio, Finland) 62.

GSR and body temperature – The body temperature data was down-sampled at 32 Hz and the GSR data was down-sampled to 10 Hz. After removing the offset, artifacts were rejected manually (electrode disconnecting briefly due to a movement), and the data was segmented into different conditions for each subject from the markers embedded in the data. The standard deviation of each epoch was then computed in Matlab for statistical analysis 6365.

Voice analysis - Recordings were reviewed to ensure that only the participant reading the story was included in the analysis (i.e. not any conversation with the research staff). The Voice parameters were calculated with the Beyond Verbal emotion analytics application performing interface v. 5 (Beyond Verbal Communication, LTD), resulting in three measures (temper, valence, and arousal). Temper reflects a speaker’s temperament or emotional state, ranging from gloomy or depressive at the low end, to embracive and friendly in the mid-range, and confrontational or aggressive at the high end of the scale. Valence is an output that measures the speaker’s level of negativity at the lower end of the scale to positive attitude at the higher end. Arousal is an output that measures a speaker’s degree of energy, ranging from tranquil, bored, or sleepy at the lower end of the scale to excited and highly energetic at the higher end. A spectral power analysis of the entire recording period was then calculated. First an autocorrelation linear predictive coding was conducted with the following settings (Predictive Order = 16; Window Length = 0.025s; Time Step 0.005s). Then the power values were output at each 125 Hz spectral bin in dB/Hz using Praat voice analysis software (Praat 6.0.43, Phonetic Sciences, University of Amsterdam Netherlands). This method is comparable to the long-time average spectrum voice analysis that had been done to discriminate between male and female voices 66.

Statistical analysis

Demographics and survey data are described with means and standard deviations for continuous variables and percent endorsed for categorical variables. For temperature and humidity, values for day 1 and 2 were evaluated for statistical differences and collapsed. Values for before and after laboratory activities were evaluated with a Student’s paired t-test.

For the level of consciousness and incorporation, sessions were evaluated for statistical differences. If the same, then measure was collapsed across sessions by calculating the average. Then, the values for each Day were tested for statistical difference with a Student’s paired t-test. A test-retest parameter for Day 1 and Day 2 using Pearson’s correlation was calculated (i.e. do the measures remain the same on different days?). Day 1 and Day 2 values were then collapsed if statistically the same. All statistical analyses except the EEG were conducted in Stata 12.0 (StataCorp LLC, College Station, Texas).

For the EEG analyses, statistical tests were computed using the LIMO v. 2.0 plugin of EEGLAB, with sessions (1, 2, 3), day (day 1 and day 2), and condition (channeling vs control) as independent variables. We used general linear model weighted least square optimization between 1 and 40 Hz frequency band and used threshold-free cluster enhancement correction for multiple comparisons. For all other physiology measures, each participant’s average values were calculated for each session (1, 2, 3) on both days (Day 1, 2) such that there were the following segment data for all measures: Day 1 – Chanl.1_1, Chanl.1_2, Chanl.1_3, NoChanl.1_1, NoChanl.1_2, NoChanl.1_3; Day 2 - Chanl.2_1, Chanl.2_2, Chanl.2_3, NoChanl.2_1, NoChanl.2_2, and NoChanl.2_3. A repeated measures analysis of variance was conducted with each measure as the dependent variable, Condition (Channeling, No Channeling) as a factor, and Day (1, 2) and Session (1, 2, 3) as repeated variables. False discovery rate was used to adjust for multiple comparisons 67. Scripts used for analysis are available as Extended data 2 68.

Results

Participants

155 persons consented to the study and 91 identified as trance channels as defined within the survey. 58 were excluded, 57 (98%) of which was due to not giving direct or indirect permission. However, this exclusion overlapped with additional exclusion criteria (CAPE, 36%; Dissociative scale, 24%; Could not sit still, 15%; Not able to initiate, 12%; and 5% did not provide contact info). 33 were invited via email to submit a video, 12 did not reply to the invitation and 21 were reviewed. Of the 21 videos the research team reviewed, six were excluded; three due to inability to comply with study design, two were not in a trance state; and one due to a scheduling conflict. Two declined to participate; one of the reasons provided was her guides did not feel comfortable participating. 13 were invited and completed the study ( Figure 3).

Figure 3. Recruitment diagram.

Figure 3.

Survey data results

The thirteen trance channels met all the inclusion/exclusion criteria and completed the study. They were mostly Caucasian, older women with a college education ( Table 1). Dissociation, psychotic, anxiety and depression symptoms did not meet clinical cutoffs for any pathology. No participants were currently diagnosed with a psychiatric illness and only one was on a psychoactive medication. The following symptoms were endorsed for previous lifetime mental health history by the number of participants in the parentheses: major depression (1), posttraumatic stress disorder (2), anxiety disorder including phobia and panic attacks (3).

Table 1. Demographic variables for participants.

Mean ± standard deviation; %, percentage.

Factor Level Channels (n-13)
Age, mean (SD) 57 ± 13
Gender Female 85%
Race White/Caucasian
Hispanic
85%
15%
Mean years of education (SD) 18 ± 4
Income $0-75,000
$75,000 and up
Decline to answer
53.9%
38.5%
7.7%
Relationship Status (In Relationship) 54%
Childhood Spirituality Practicing Religious
Spiritual but not religious
Minimally practicing religious
Not religious or spiritual
31%
15%
31%
23%
Current Spirituality Spiritual but not religious 100%
Dissociative Symptoms 2.7 ± 4.7
Psychotic Symptoms 0.17 ± 0.2
Anxiety and Depression 0.5 ± 1.1
Personality Extraversion
Agreeableness
Conscientiousness
Neuroticism
Openness
3.0 ± 1.3
4.1± 0.7
3.1 ± 0.8
2.2 ± 1.1
4.0 ± 0.9
Absorption 20.5 ± 8.1
Empathy 29.4 ± 6.3
Sensitivity 5.9 ± 0.9
Paranormal Belief 93.7 ± 6.0
Paranormal Experience 76.7 ± 9.3

The age channeling started was varied (mean age 39 ± 21, range 4 - 65). How the experiences started also varied (spontaneously - 46.2%, received training - 30.8%, trained myself - 23.8%). Most participants did not endorse other family members having similar skills (No or unknown - 62.5%). Participants perception of the average level of consciousness was 4.7 ± 3.8 (range 0-10; 0 = Fully Conscious) and average level of incorporation was 7.1 ± 2.7 (range 0-10; 0 = No Incorporation). The impact of channeling on the participant’s life was very positive (96.1 ± 7.1; 0 - very negative to 100 - very positive). All participants gave permission for the channeling, could tell when it began, and have told others about the experience. Most initiated the channeling session. The perceived source of information was from their higher self or group beings (a group entity or group mind is described as a coherent bundle of still-individual or once-individual beings who communicate as from a single integrated source.) Guidance and personal messages was the most common purpose of the channeling and it was most often utilized by recording it. See Table 2 for percentages endorsed for each category.

Table 2. Perceptual characteristics reported by participants from survey.

%, percent of participants who endorsed item.

%
Permission (Yes/No) Yes 100
Can tell when begins (Yes/No) Yes 100
Initiation, control and spontaneity Sometimes I initiate it, sometimes spontaneous.
I initiate it
Other
7.7
69.2
23.1
Perceived Source Your Higher Self
Group Beings
Other Ascended Masters
Extraterrestrials
Deceased Human Beings
The Universal Mind
Angels
Earthbound Spirits
Gods and/or God
Jesus Christ
Devas
Elementals
Plants or Animals
Collective Unconscious
69.2
69.2
46.2
46.2
46.2
30.8
30.8
23.1
15.4
15.4
15.4
15.4
15.4
7.7
Purpose of experience (content of
message)
Guidance and personal messages
Ageless wisdom
Descriptions of life in non-physical realms
Healing
The future and the past
Health
Information from or about deceased humans
Subject matter for artistic/creative expression
Scientific/technological material
92.3
84.6
76.9
69.2
61.5
61.5
53.8
46.2
46.2
Told others about experience 100.0
Utilization of experience I record it via audio or video equipment.
I speak it aloud to another.
I write it down.
I help heal others through energy work or other
means.
I create art, such as painting or music.
I use tools to communicate the information (e.g.
pendulum, Tarot cards).
84.6
76.9
53.8
53.8

7.7
7.7

There was one open text field in the survey asking if the participant could tell when the channeling was starting and how they could tell. All 13 channels entered text into this field. The following themes were reported with the number of participants who endorsed this concept after each theme (see Extended data for full text and coding): physical sensation (9); “being” initiates contact (5); channeler initiates contact (3); mental information (2); emotional change (2); and energetic sensation (2).

Laboratory paradigm results

Temperature and humidity in the electromagnetically shielded room. Temperature and humidity were similar on Day 1 and 2 so they were combined. The temperature in the electromagnetically shielded room changed from the beginning to the end of the experiment (Before - 70.6 ± 1.4; After - 72.2 ± 1.2; t = -8.4 , p < 0.00005). Humidity in the electromagnetically shielded room did not change (Before - 44.6 ± 9.8; After - 45.3 ± 10.1; t = -1.6 , p = 0.14).

Level of channeler’s consciousness and incorporation during channeling. The perceived level of consciousness and incorporation are listed in Table 3. Participants were in a deeper trance for each subsequent session on both days ( F(2,48) = 4.37; p= 0.018). The depth of trance was not significantly different on the two days but marginally so ( F(1,24) = 4.04; p= 0.056). There was no interaction of Day and Session. The Pearson’s correlation session by session between Day 1 and Day 2 (i.e. test-retest reliability) was high ( r = 0.87). The perceived level of incorporation was similar across sessions and days (non-significant Day, Session, Day x Session). The Pearson’s correlation session by session between Day 1 and Day 2 (i.e. test-retest reliability) was high ( r = 0.79). 7/13 participants perceived channeling the same purported being for all the sessions and six had different purported beings for the sessions.

Table 3. Perceived level of consciousness and incorporation.

Mean ± standard deviation. Consciousness scale was anchored by 0 for “Fully Conscious” to 10 for “Fully Unconscious”. Incorporation scale was anchored by 0 for “No incorporation, my body is not being directly used at all” to 10 for “Full incorporation, my whole body is being directly used.”

Day Session Consciousness Incorporation
1 1 4.5 ± 3.0 5.1 ± 2.9
2 5.0 ± 3.2 5.4 ± 3.2
3 6.1 ± 3.4 6.3 ± 3.0
2 1 5.2 ± 3.3 5.5 ± 3.5
2 5.7 ± 3.6 5.9 ± 3.1
3 6.7 ± 3.6 6.4 ± 3.4

EEG. There were no significant differences between channeling and no-channeling condition in the traditional frequency bands (theta 3–7 Hz; alpha 8–12 Hz; beta 13–20 Hz and low gamma 21–40 Hz) across the 64 channels. We observed what might be a trend in the uncorrected statistical maps at 13 Hz, but it did not resist correction for multiple comparisons ( Figure 4; raw data files available from figshare, Dataset 1 69).

Figure 4. EEG difference of channeling and no-channeling sessions.

Figure 4.

The x-axis on this figure represents the EEG frequency in hertz. The y-axis represents the various channels numbered 1-64. The t-value represents the statistical difference between channeling and no-channeling sessions. The topoplot takes one frequency (namely 13Hz) and plots scalp topography the differences between channeling and no-channeling conditions.

In response to a reviewers comments, we conducted exploratory analyses collapsing the 64 channels into nine sections as follows: frontal (FP1, Fpz, FP2, AF3, Afz, AF4, F3, Fz, F2), front right (AF8, F4, F6, F8), front left (AF7, F3, F5, F7), central (FC1, FCz, FC2, C1, Cz, C2, CP1, CPz, CP2), central right (FC4, FC6, FT8, C4, C6, T8, CP4, CP6, TP8), central left (FC3, FC5, FC7, C3, C5, T7, CP3, CP5, TP7), and parietal (P1, Pz, P2, PO3, POz, PO4, O1, Oz, O2), parietal right (P4, P6, P8, PO8) and parietal left (P3, P5, P7, PO7). This data was averaged across days and sessions by condition (channeling/no-channeling). Repeated measures ANOVA’s were conducted of EEG power to evaluate differences between conditions (channeling, no-channeling) at the nine locations for each frequency band. There was no statistical difference in condition by location interactions ( Table 4).

Table 4. EEG parameter values and statistical tests by electrode location.

Mean ± standard deviation; F, F-statistic; p, probability.

No-Channeling Channeling
Theta Frontal 1.01 ± 0.39 0.99 ± 0.40 F (8,176) = 0.14 , p = 0.99
Front Right 0.95 ± 0.38 0.95 ± 0.39
Front Left 0.94 ± 0.37 0.91 ± 0.48
Central 0.86 ± 0.41 0.77 ± 0.33
Central Right 0.97 ± 0.58 0.86 ± 0.46
Central Left 0.89 ± 0.49 0.84 ± 0.56
Parietal 1.47 ± 0.68 1.33 ± 0.61
Parietal Right 1.76 ± 1.18 1.63 ± 1.05
Parietal Left 1.6 ± 0.94 1.58 ± 1.09
Alpha Frontal 5.53 ± 4.25 4.52 ± 3.33 F (8,176) = 0.15 , p = 0.99
Front Right 4.82 ± 3.86 4.06 ± 3.2
Front Left 4.98 ± 4.19 3.97 ± 3.13
Central 4.19 ± 3.68 3.46 ± 2.61
Central Right 4.68 ± 4.88 3.77 ± 3.39
Central Left 4.25 ± 4.05 3.58 ± 3.25
Parietal 15.66 ± 15.27 13 ± 11.43
Parietal Right 15.94 ± 16.04 13.43 ± 11.85
Parietal Left 13.21 ± 10.21 12.42 ± 9.73
Beta Frontal 0.58 ± 0.49 0.50 ± 0.40 F (8,176) = 0.03 , p = 1.0
Front Right 0.60 ± 0.53 0.54 ± 0.43
Front Left 0.56 ± 0.41 0.51 ± 0.38
Central 0.64 ± 0.68 0.53 ± 0.53
Central Right 0.77 ± 0.77 0.68 ± 0.61
Central Left 0.74 ± 0.65 0.65 ± 0.53
Parietal 1.09 ± 0.88 1.00 ± 0.71
Parietal Right 1.34 ± 1.30 1.24 ± 1.08
Parietal Left 1.32 ± 1.14 1.23 ± 0.93
Gamma Frontal 0.14 ± 0.11 0.23 ± 0.34 F (8,176) = 0.60 , p = 0.76
Front Right 0.18 ± 0.14 0.24 ± 0.25
Front Left 0.17 ± 0.12 0.27 ± 0.36
Central 0.12 ± 0.12 0.11 ± 0.09
Central Right 0.17 ± 0.14 0.19 ± 0.16
Central Left 0.16 ± 0.14 0.18 ± 0.13
Parietal 0.20 ± 0.15 0.20 ± 0.12
Parietal Right 0.22 ± 0.20 0.25 ± 0.2
Parietal Left 0.21 ± 0.15 0.25 ± 0.14

ECG. The average data length was 302.2 ± 18.2 seconds. The ECG data was exceptionally clean. Only 47 of the 156 segments had any artifact heartbeats. The average percentage of data with artifact heartbeats was only 2.9% ± 2.4. ECG measures were the statistically the same in the channeling and no-channeling conditions (all models p’s greater than 0.05; Heart rate beats per minute – Channel: 76.2 ± 11.2, No-Channel: 76.7 ± 11.0 ( F (2,48) = 1.86, p = 0.17); SDNN ms – Channel: 46.2 ± 50.6, No-Channel: 45.7 ± 49.6 ( F (2,48) = 1.82, p = 0.28); Very low frequency FFT/ms 2 – Channel: 48.3 ± 43.9, No-Channel: 52.1 ± 53.9 ( F (2,48) = 2.48, p = 0.10); Low frequency - FFT/ms 2 Channel: 698.1 ± 733, No-Channel: 705.2 ± 719.2 ( F (2,48) = 2.07, p = 0.14); High frequency - FFT/ms 2 Channel: 396.8 ± 5752.8, No-channel: 2470.4 ± 6112.4 ( F (2,48) = .72, p = 0.49)). Respiration collected as breaths per minute was also the same in the channeling and no-channeling condition (all models p’s greater than 0.05; Channel: 13.8 ± 2.9; No-channel: 13.9 ± 2.6, ( F (2,48) = 1.95, p = 0.15)).

GSR and body temperature. There was no significant difference in the standard deviation of the body temperature between channeling and no channeling conditions (Body Temperature: channeling 3.2 ± 2.5; no-channeling 3.5 ± 4.4; all model p’s greater than 0.05). The standard deviation of GSR values for channeling and no-channeling were channeling 244.4 ± 134.3; no-channeling 335.8 ± 202.5. Because the collapsed mean values for GSR appeared numerically different, an exploratory post-hoc paired Student’s t-test was conducted of the collapsed average values for channeling and no-channeling and was significant ( t = -2.39, p = 0.04). The GSR repeated measures model including day 1,2 and session 1,2,3 were not significant.

Voice. The story reading during the channeling state was significantly slower than during the no-channeling state (Story 1: no-channel - 38.2 s ± 2.5; channel - 56.0 s ± 18.6; Story 2: no-channel - 36.8 s ± 4.1; channel - 67.3 s ± 32.6; F(1,51) = 15.44, p = 0.0006). Valence was significantly lower during channeling compared to no-channeling ( Table 5). There was no significant difference in arousal or temper.

Table 5. Voice parameter values and statistical tests.

Mean ± standard deviation; F, F-statistic; p, probability.

Channeling No-Channeling Statistics
    Day 1                      Day 2     Day 1                      Day 2
Arousal 18.8 ± 18.4          19.1 ± 14.0 28.1 ± 14.6          29.6 ± 16.8 F(1,51) = 3.74, p = 0.07
Temper 22.8 ± 17.7          28.3 ± 19.6 30.8 ± 11.0          34.0 ± 18.6 F(1,51) = 1.67, p = 0.21
Valence 24.7 ± 17.3          28.5 ± 19.2 36.8 ± 15.3          37.3 ± 16.4 F(1,51) = 4.55, p = 0.04

There were significant power (dB/Hz) differences between the story read during channeling compared to no-channeling in the 125 Hz bins between 0 and 625 Hz, and 3625 and 3875 Hz ( Figure 5); see data available for values at each frequency bin; p-values for these bins varied from .03- 0.0007 and remained significant with False Discovery Rate correction).

Figure 5. Spectral power analysis of voice recordings of story read during channeling and no-channeling states.

Figure 5.

* = significantly different between channeling and no-channeling readings with False Discovery Rate correction.

Discussion

Participants

Recruitment for the study was feasible. From the 155 persons that consented to the survey originally, thirteen met all the inclusion/exclusion criteria and completed the study (21% of those who identified as trance channels and 39% of those who were invited to submit a video). Participant demographic characteristics were similar to other studies on anomalous information reception, namely Caucasian, educated older women 6, 23, 24. The participants also did not have any psychiatric pathologies evident through self-report of diagnoses or medications, which is similar to other studies 7, 8, 10, 23, 24. All dissociative and psychotic symptoms scores were less than commonly used clinical cutoffs for pathological dissociation 45 and psychosis 70. Participants personality values were similar to other general healthy adults 71, 72. Absorption was higher than a large college-student samples which had an average of about 20 (compared to our 30) 73, but lower than Brazilian spiritists 74. Empathy score means were higher than other population norms 54, 55. Sensitivity scores reflected the channels being highly sensitive people (as opposed to non-sensitives) 56, 75. Paranormal beliefs and experiences were much higher than observed in 350 general population respondents (beliefs - 59.3 ± 21.7; experience - 43.7 ± 25.3) as one would expect considering their channeling experiences 76.

Survey results: channeling characteristics

The mean age at which participants had their first channeling experience was older than other types of anomalous information reception experiences 6 and other self-reported trance channels where such data were reported 10, 25, 36 although the standard deviation and range of the starting age was large. Participants reported a medium level of trance (4.7 out of 10) during channeling which was not as deep one might imagine because of ethnographic reports of deep trances during possession rituals 77. As far as we know, no other trance channeling study has formally evaluated level of incorporation. Incorporation level was moderately high. The impact of channeling on the participants lives was very positive as has been noted in other channeling studies 8, 10, 26 and other anomalous information reception studies 6, 23, 24, 78. The survey data reflected similar responses about channels characteristics, perception of source, purpose and utilization to other trance channeler studies 1, 10 and reflected consensual experiences with varied purported sources and content.

Laboratory paradigm results

Temperature was approximately 1.2 degree higher after the experiment than when it began. This is likely due to the fact that people were present in the electromagnetically shielded room and resulted in a slight increase in ambient temperature. There were no humidity changes.

Most channels were aware of their experience (rather than in a full trance) and reported varying levels of perceived incorporation. Trance depth increased over the sessions each day, but was not different across days and test-retest reliability of consciousness level was high. It is reasonable that as the channelers became more comfortable with the setting on each day, their trance state would deepen. Future studies should collect depth of trance and perceived level of use of the channeler’s body for the no-channeling conditions as well so that the variables could be included as covariates in the model comparing channeling to no-channeling conditions on physiological measures. There were no differences in level of incorporation. While micro-variations may have occurred, our values reflect consistent levels of consciousness and incorporation during our laboratory paradigm for these particular participants on a statistical level. Like other trance channeler studies, participants reported a variety of purported sources being incorporated 10.

Our hypothesis that channeling and no-channeling states would be reflected with distinct EEG measures was not demonstrated. Bastos et al. also did not find any EEG differences in beta, theta and alpha frequency bands before, during, and after mediumistic communication in ten Spiritist mediums in Brazil 25. They did find EEG differences when comparing trance channels to controls that were collected simultaneously in a group setting. Increased beta was found in four electrodes pre-communication, two electrodes during communication, and one electrode post-communication, increased theta in one electrode during communication, and increased alpha power in one electrode post-communication for trance channels compared to gender and age matched controls. Bastos et al. compared the two groups by time-point (i.e. rather than using a repeated-measures model) and did not adjust for multiple comparisons. Also, the authors state that only Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, F7, and F8 were used in the final analysis although they collected data from 22 electrodes because these areas are “widely accepted as being involved with spiritual experiences” 25. Hughes et al. found a greater percentage of time that theta, alpha and beta were observed in the trance state compared to pre-trance, and greater beta and alpha comparing the trance state to post-trance for ten trance channels 34. The randomization test for matched pairs was used to analyzed the data rather than a repeated measures model and no multiple comparison corrections were made. The EEG was recorded during "talking baseline" before and after the trance period which “consisted of talking, listening and answering questions in what might be described as an "intellectual" or "philosophical conversation" mode of discourse” we assume with eyes open. This is a more naturalistic setting for the trance channels especially considering many of our participants stated that it felt strange and was unique that they would incorporate their “beings” and not say anything. We chose to have the participant stay silent with their eyes closed to ensure a clean EEG signal. Our study also had the unique design of all recordings being collected in an electromagnetically shielded environment which may have reduced additional outside “noise” to the EEG signal. Bastos et al. dealt with this issue in a different way 25. Participants were asked to stay seated with eyes closed and avoid blinking during EEG collection. They did communicate verbally during the channeling period but then only EEG epochs free of muscular artifacts were used in the analysis 25. Two other studies have evaluated possession trances in a naturalistic setting in Bali, Indonesia 35, 79 and found differences in the EEG in possession trance ceremonies compared to control conditions and/or control participants. These studies are unique in that they record the EEG during a sacred ceremony while the participant is moving. Oohashi et al. accurately summarize three main issues with in situ recordings of this type: 1) the often sacred nature of the process precludes invasive physiology recording equipment; 2) finding reliable portable devices (although this issue has improved greatly with the invention of increasingly more mobile EEG equipment); and 3) contamination of the data by artifacts. There are only so many post-recording processes that can be done to preserve high quality EEG when the participant is moving and/or receiving or processing different stimuli than are present during the control conditions (e.g. removal of artifacts and Independent Component Analysis (ICA) 58, 80). However, even with gross artifact removal, filtering of the EEG, and ICA, the naturalistic setting does not control for the other types of activities that could cause EEG differences during the communication such as eyes open versus eyes closed, and auditory or visual stimuli and processing. Collecting and analyzing EEG signals in trance channels is certainly a challenge and each paradigm has it benefits and limitations in adding to our understanding of how channeling may work. We can continue to work towards improving our methods in order to gain more information about the channeling phenomenon and our future studies can consider the following EEG design elements and caveats depending on the specific research question: within- versus between-participant analyses including baseline correction for differences between groups, and repeated measures statistical models, naturalistic recordings versus laboratory recordings ensuring attention to eyes open versus eyes closed states and avoidance of cross-comparison; and speaking versus being in stillness yet incorporated.

Our study did not find any changes in heart rate or heart rate variability measures during the channeling state compared to a control condition. Two studies have found increased heart rate from before to after a trance channeling 36, 81 and one found in heart rate variability compared to controls 36, suggesting greater arousal during these states. The difference in results may be due to our laboratory design where the channelers did not speak as they normally would during a channeling communication. Similarly, respiration and galvanic skin response were not statistically different across conditions. Our exploratory paired t-test of the GSR collapsed average values did show significance and likely, our repeated measures model did not have enough statistical power with only thirteen participants to detect differences. Despite our lack of significant differences with our a prior repeated measures models, future studies should still consider including measures evaluating autonomic nervous system function.

As far as we know, this is the first study to objectively evaluate voice measures in trance channeling. Voice valence was significantly lower when read during channeling compared to no-channeling, arousal trended towards being significantly lower and there was no difference in temper. Valence measures the speaker’s level of negativity at the lower end of the scale to positive attitude at the higher end. Arousal is an output that measures a speaker’s degree of energy ranging from tranquil, bored or sleepy to excited and highly energetic. To the subjective listener, the channeled readings were softer in volume and slower in pace, which likely resulted in lower valence and arousal measures. We also observed power differences at specific frequencies using long-time average spectrum voice analysis, one method that has been used to discriminate gender and individuals 66. There is the possibility that participants impersonated a different voice during the channeled reading periods, however, the participants exhibited no signs of multiple personality disorder, dissociative identity disorder, or psychotic symptoms reflecting the pathology or motivation to do so. There is also a possibility that there was role-playing, albeit unconsciously, during the channeling states such as is observed in ventriloquists and puppeteers. Future studies could consider having the channeler channel as they would normally rather than using a scripted story. Small segments of the transcripts of this recording could then be read by the same channeler in a no-channeling state. This would allow for naturalistic communication during channeling and control for the content spoken during the non-channeling state for any voice analysis being conducted.

Limitations

There are number of limitations to this study that should be considered when interpreting its results. The study was designed as a within-participant controlled paradigm with multiple state shifts and test-retest data collections for multiple reasons. The repeated measures on Day 1 and Day 2 were done to evaluate test-retest reliability of the measures across days. The repeated sessions were done to evaluate repeatability within the same day and to increase data-points collected. However, the alternating between channeling and no-channeling states may have created a carryover effect, although we gave ample time for the transition states, and the mind-wandering task was perceived as subjectively different than the channeling. Because the trials were alternating often and lasted only five minutes, it is conceivable that the participants were not completely switching between states and that they were still partially in a trance state during some control trials. Or even if they were, it is possible that physiological systems take longer to go back to baseline (e.g. HRV), therefore decreasing the difference between the two conditions. Perhaps five minutes of data in each condition was not long enough to generate changes and longer segments need to be used. We purposefully kept the segment times at five minutes to reduce participant burden for the laboratory visit. Future studies could use longer segment periods but fewer conditions switches. Also, the channels baseline state may not be different from the channeling state just like many expert meditators baseline states are altered from their extensive meditation practice. Novice meditators often show more dramatic changes in neurophysiology when meditating versus when they are not meditating because these states are markedly different for novices. However, expert meditators may have a baseline non-meditating state that is not as different than their meditating state (i.e. their meditating and non-meditating states are more similar to each other compared to a novice’s) 8284. Perhaps future studies could include participants who have just begun channeling and those who have years of experience to explore differences in their physiology.

It is possible that the small sample size did not allow the analyses to detect changes between the two conditions. While the repeated measures design attempted to mitigate the smaller participant number, future studies could include more participants.

In order to increase the EEG data quality, all frequencies above 40 Hz were filtered out. We can only conclude that there were no differences in the channeling versus no-channeling conditions in the frequency bands that we measure. Future secondary analyses of this data set will include higher frequency bands with sophisticated ICA methods to remove electromyographic sources from the signal that are associated with higher frequencies.

Furthermore, some participants reported different purported beings incorporating during each session which could trigger different neurophysiological signatures. It is possible that no homogenous states were present across all channeling trials, and could consequently appear as noise. However, one would anticipate that this would result in increased variation between the channeling sessions which was not observed. Secondary analyses will include additional within subject analysis for participants who reported different beings incorporating during the sessions. Future studies may also consider instructing participants to channel the same purported “being” for each trial.

Additionally, the channels reported that they normally speak when channeling as the purpose is to deliver a message, and that holding the incorporation of the purported being without speaking was unusual for them. This was done to ensure a clean EEG signal of high quality but perhaps future technological advances will allow for clean EEG collection while the channeler is speaking which may more clearly delineate EEG frequency differences or less optimally select out non-speaking segments as others have done 25. As is common with all studies that attempt to replicate real-world experiences in the laboratory, there is the consideration that the laboratory environment was unique to the channels. Thus, they may not have been as comfortable as they normally are when they channel, which could have interfered with their performance although the participants did not comment on this.

Previous qualitative reports of trance channeled content suggest a concept that may provide context why no physiological differences were seen across conditions 10. One concept is that trance channels are channeling an aspect of themselves or higher self, that is, that the incorporated “beings” are actually multi-dimensional aspects of the channeler themselves. These multi-dimensional aspects then need to match their energetic vibrational frequency of the channeler to be able to use their body for communication. No measures are available at this time to confirm or deny this theory but perhaps measures will be developed to assess this in the future. It is also possible that differences do exist between channeling and no-channeling states but that the measures we used were not able to adequately capture them. For example, we did not do an EEG coherence or synchrony evaluation that may reflect changes in connectivity. Secondary analyses for these measures are planned.

Future directions

Further analysis will examine the specific purported beings channeled for each segment compared to its control, transition periods between channeling and no-channeling states, EEG connectivity, magnetometer data, and qualitative evaluation of the channeled content. A composite measure of autonomic nervous system values and EEG values may also be explored. Based on our results on voice valence and power spectrum, future analyses could include pitch and intensity in order to study this effect in more detail.

This study has moved channeling research forward by evaluating physiological measure differences between the channeling and no-channeling states using rigorous controlled methods. Despite subjective perceptions of distinctly different states, no substantive differences were seen in EEG frequency power, ECG measures, and respiration. However, the voice measure of valence and spectral power in specific frequencies were lower in the channeling state while reading a story.

Data availability

Underlying data

Underlying data including voice analysis data, survey data, lab temperature and humidity, raw physiology data files, and transcripts of open-ended questions are available from Figshare

Figshare: Dataset 1. Wahbeh_Data for Trance Channel Study. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.7355027.v2 69

Extended data

The online survey, stories read by the participants, transcripts, grant proposal and scripts for process the data are available as Extended data from figshare.

Figshare: Extended data 1. Wahbeh_Additional Data for Trance Channel Study. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.7423454.v2 40

Figshare: Extended data 2. Wahbeh_Scripts from Trance Channel study. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.7469972.v1 68

All underlying and extended data are available under a CC0 1.0 Universal Public Domain Dedication.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the Institute of Noetic Sciences, the participants of the study, the Ray Benton Foundation, the Federico and Elvia Faggin Foundation and FUNDAÇÃO BIAL for their support of this and other projects in this research program.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by FUNDAÇÃO BIAL (grant number No. 72/16). This work was also supported by the Ray Benton Foundation and the Federico and Elvia Faggin Foundation.

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

[version 2; referees: 2 approved]

References

  • 1. Klimo J: Channeling: Investigations on receiving information from paranormal sources. Berkeley, CA: North Atlantic Books;1998. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Crook JH: The indigenous psychiatry of Ladakh, Part I: Practice theory approaches to trance possession in the himalayas. Anthropol Med. 1997;4(3):289–307. 10.1080/13648470.1997.9964539 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Hageman JH, Peres JFP, Moreira-Almeida A, et al. : The Neurobiology of Trance and Mediumship in Brazil. In: Krippner SC, Friedman HL, eds. Mysterious Minds: The Neurobiology of Psychics, Mediums, and Other Extraordinary People USA: Praeger;2009. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Kua EH, Sim LP, Chee KT: A Cross-Cultural Study of the Possession-Trance in Singapore. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 1986;20(3):361–4. 10.3109/00048678609158883 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Haraldsson E, Houtkooper JM: Psychic experiences in the multinational human values study: Who reports them. J Am Soc Psych Res. 1991;85(2):145–165. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Wahbeh H, Radin D, Mossbridge J, et al. : Exceptional experiences reported by scientists and engineers. Explore (NY). 2018;14(5):329–341. 10.1016/j.explore.2018.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Negro PJ, Jr, Palladino-Negro P, Louzã MR: Do religious mediumship dissociative experiences conform to the sociocognitive theory of dissociation? J Trauma Dissociation. 2002;3(1):51–73. 10.1300/J229v03n01_05 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Roxburgh EC, Roe CA: A survey of dissociation, boundary-thinness, and psychological wellbeing in spiritualist mental mediumship. J Parapsychol. 2011;75(2):279 Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Klimo J: Channeling research: A progress report with recommendations. Paper presented at: International Conference on Paranormal Research; Fort Collins, CO.1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Wahbeh H, Carpenter L, Radin D: A mixed methods phenomenological and exploratory study of channeling. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research. 2018;82(3):129–148. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 11. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. (5th ed.). Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Publishing Inc.2013. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Coons PM: Confirmation of childhood abuse in child and adolescent cases of multiple personality disorder and dissociative disorder not otherwise specified. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1994;182(8):461–464. 10.1097/00005053-199408000-00007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Ogawa JR, Sroufe LA, Weinfield NS, et al. : Development and the fragmented self: longitudinal study of dissociative symptomatology in a nonclinical sample. Dev Psychopathol. 1997;9(4):855–879. 10.1017/S0954579497001478 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Stolovy T, Lev-Wiesel R, Witztum E: Dissociation: adjustment or distress? Dissociative phenomena, absorption and quality of life among Israeli women who practice channeling compared to women with similar traumatic history. J Relig Health. 2015;54(3):1040–1051. 10.1007/s10943-014-9885-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Ross CA, Joshi S, Currie R: Dissociative experiences in the general population. Am J Psychiatry. 1990;147(11):1547–1552. 10.1176/ajp.147.11.1547 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Seligman R: Distress, dissociation, and embodied experience: Reconsidering the pathways to mediumship and mental health. Ethos. 2005;33(1):71–99. 10.1525/eth.2005.33.1.071 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Sar V, Alioğlu F, Akyuz G: Experiences of possession and paranormal phenomena among women in the general population: are they related to traumatic stress and dissociation? J Trauma Dissociation. 2014;15(3):303–318. 10.1080/15299732.2013.849321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Schaffler Y, Cardeña E, Reijman S, et al. : Traumatic Experience and Somatoform Dissociation Among Spirit Possession Practitioners in the Dominican Republic. Cult Med Psychiatry. 2016;40(1):74–99. 10.1007/s11013-015-9472-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Kihlstrom JF: Dissociative disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1:227–253. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143925 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Seligman R, Kirmayer LJ: Dissociative experience and cultural neuroscience: narrative, metaphor and mechanism. Cult Med Psychiatry. 2008;32(1):31–64. 10.1007/s11013-007-9077-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Spitzer C, Barnow S, Freyberger HJ, et al. : Recent developments in the theory of dissociation. World Psychiatry. 2006;5(2):82–86. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Castillo RJ: Trance, functional psychosis, and culture. Psychiatry. 2003;66(1):9–21. 10.1521/psyc.66.1.9.20285 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Wahbeh H, Radin D: People reporting experiences of mediumship have higher dissociation symptom scores than non-mediums, but below thresholds for pathological dissociation [version 3; referees: 2 approved, 1 not approved]. F1000Res. 2017;6:1416. 10.12688/f1000research.12019.3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Wahbeh H, McDermott K, Sagher A: Dissociative Symptoms and Anomalous Information Reception. Act Nerv Super (Praha). 2018;60(3–4):75–85. 10.1007/s41470-018-0023-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Bastos MA, Bastos PR, Osório IH, et al. : Frontal electroencephalographic (EEG) activity and mediumship: a comparative study between spiritist mediums and controls. Arch Clin Psychiat. 2016;43(2). 10.1590/0101-60830000000076 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Bastos MA, Bastos PR, Goncalves LM, et al. : Mediumship: review of quantitatives studies published in the 21 st century. Arch Clin Psychiat. 2015;42(5):129–138. 10.1590/0101-60830000000063 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Rocha AC, Paraná D, Freire ES, et al. : Investigating the fit and accuracy of alleged mediumistic writing: a case study of Chico Xavier's letters. Explore (NY). 2014;10(5):300–8. 10.1016/j.explore.2014.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Beischel J, Boccuzzi M, Biuso M, et al. : Anomalous information reception by research mediums under blinded conditions II : replication and extension. Explore (NY). 2015;11(2):136–142. 10.1016/j.explore.2015.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Rock AJ, Beischel J, Boccuzzi M, et al. : Discarnate readings by claimant mediums: Assessing phenomenology and accuracy under beyond double-blind conditions. J Parapsychol. 2014;78(2):183–194. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Beischel J, Schwartz GE: Anomalous information reception by research mediums demonstrated using a novel triple-blind protocol. Explore (NY). 2007;3(1):23–27. 10.1016/j.explore.2006.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Delorme A, Beischel J, Michel L, et al. : Electrocortical activity associated with subjective communication with the deceased. Front Psychol. 2013;4:834. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00834 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Delorme A, Pierce A, Michel L, et al. : Intuitive Assessment of Mortality Based on Facial Characteristics: Behavioral, Electrocortical, and Machine Learning Analyses. Explore (NY). 2018;14(4):262–267. 10.1016/j.explore.2017.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Beischel J, Rock AJ: Addressing the survival versus psi debate through process-focused mediumship research. J Parapsychol. 2009;73:71 Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Hughes DJ, Melville NT: Changes in brainwave activity during trance channeling: a pilot study. J Transpersonal Psy. 1990;22(2):175–189. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Kawai N, Honda M, Nishina E, et al. : Electroencephalogram characteristics during possession trances in healthy individuals. Neuroreport. 2017;28(15):949–955. 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000857 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Bastos MAV, Jr, Bastos PRHO, Osório IHS, et al. : Physiologic correlates of culture-bound dissociation: A comparative study of Brazilian spiritist mediums and controls. Transcult Psychiatry. 2018;55(2):286–313. 10.1177/1363461518759202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Cardeña E, Palmer J, Marcusson-Clavertz D: Parapsychology: A handbook for the 21st century. McFarland;2015. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Nelson RD: Implicit Physical Psi: The Global Consciousness Project. In: Cardena E, Palmer J, Marcusson-Clavertz D, eds. Parapsychology: A Handbook for the 21st Centry Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company, In., Publishers;2015;1:282–292. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Varvoglis M, Bancel PA: Micro-psychokinesis. In: Cardena E, Palmer J, Marcusson-Clavertz D, eds. Parapsychology: A Handbook for the 21st Century Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company, Inc., Publishers;2015;1:266–281. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Wahbeh H, cannard C, Delorme A, et al. : Wahbeh_Additional Data for Trance Channel Study.2019. 10.6084/m9.figshare.7423454.v2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Capra C, Kavanagh DJ, Hides L, et al. : Current CAPE-15: a measure of recent psychotic-like experiences and associated distress. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2015;11(5):411–417. 10.1111/eip.12245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Chu JA: Rebuilding shattered lives: treating complex PTSD and dissociative disorders. 2nd ed. Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley & Sons;2011. 10.1002/9781118093146 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Barratt W: The Barratt Simplified Measure of Social Status (BSMSS) measuring SES.2006. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Cirino PT, Chin CE, Sevcik RA, et al. : Measuring socioeconomic status: reliability and preliminary validity for different approaches. Assessment. 2002;9(2):145–155. 10.1177/10791102009002005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Waller N, Putnam FW, Carlson EB: Types of dissociation and dissociative types: A taxometric analysis of dissociative experiences. Psychol Methods. 1996;1(3):300–321. 10.1037/1082-989X.1.3.300 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Waller NG, Ross CA: The prevalence and biometric structure of pathological dissociation in the general population: taxometric and behavior genetic findings. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106(4):499–510. 10.1037/0021-843X.106.4.499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Zingrone NL, Alvarado CS: The Dissociative Experiences Scale-II: Descriptive Statistics, Factor Analysis, and Frequency of Experiences. Imagin Cogn Pers. 2001;21(2):145–157. 10.2190/K48D-XAW3-B2KC-UBB7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Capra C, Kavanagh DJ, Hides L, et al. : Brief screening for psychosis-like experiences. Schizophr Res. 2013;149(1–3):104–107. 10.1016/j.schres.2013.05.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB, et al. : An ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: the PHQ-4. Psychosomatics. 2009;50(6):613–621. 10.1176/appi.psy.50.6.613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Rammstedt B, John OP: Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. J Res Pers. 2007;41(1):203–212. 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Tellegen A: A brief manual for the differential personality questionnaire.Minneapolis: Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota;1982. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Tellegen A, Atkinson G: Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences ("absorption"), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. J Abnorm Psychol. 1974;83(3):268–277. 10.1037/h0036681 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Kihlstrom JF, Register PA, Hoyt IP, et al. : Dispositional correlates of hypnosis: a phenomenological approach. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1989;37(3):249–263. 10.1080/00207148908414476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Loewen PJ, Lyle G, Nachshen JS: An eight-item form of the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and an application to charitable giving.2009. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Wakabayashi A, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, et al. : Development of short forms of the Empathy Quotient (EQ-Short) and the Systemizing Quotient (SQ-Short). Pers Individ Dif. 2006;41(5):929–940. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.03.017 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Aron EN, Aron A: Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion and emotionality. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;73(2):345–368. 10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Lange R, Thalbourne MA: Rasch scaling paranormal belief and experience: structure and semantics of Thalbourne's Australian Sheep-Goat Scale. Psychol Rep. 2002;91(3 Pt 2):1065–1073. 10.2466/pr0.2002.91.3f.1065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Delorme A, Makeig S: EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;134(1):9–21. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Wahbeh H, Oken BS: Peak high-frequency HRV and peak alpha frequency higher in PTSD. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2013;38(1):57–69. 10.1007/s10484-012-9208-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Goodnick PJ, Jerry J, Parra F: Psychotropic drugs and the ECG: focus on the QTc interval. Expert Opin Pharmacother. 2002;3(5):479–498. 10.1517/14656566.3.5.479 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Olgin J, Zipes D: Specific arrhythmias: diagnosis and treatment.In: Libby P, Bonow R, Mann D, Zipes D, eds. Braunwald's Heart Disease: A Textbook of Cardiovascular Medicine.St. Louis, Mo: WB Saunders;2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Schmidt M, Schumann A, Müller J, et al. : ECG derived respiration: comparison of time-domain approaches and application to altered breathing patterns of patients with schizophrenia. Physiol Meas. 2017;38(4):601–615. 10.1088/1361-6579/aa5feb [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Soleymani M, Chanel G, Kierkels JJ, et al. : Affective characterization of movie scenes based on content analysis and physiological changes. Int J Semant Comput. 2009;3(02):235–254. 10.1142/S1793351X09000744 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Koelstra S, Yazdani A, Soleymani M, et al. : Single trial classification of EEG and peripheral physiological signals for recognition of emotions induced by music videos.Paper presented at: International Conference on Brain Informatics 2010. 10.1007/978-3-642-15314-3_9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Khalili Z, Moradi MH: Emotion recognition system using brain and peripheral signals: using correlation dimension to improve the results of EEG.Paper presented at: Neural Networks, IJCNN 2009. International Joint Conference on2009. 10.1109/IJCNN.2009.5178854 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Mendoza E, Valencia N, Muñoz J, et al. : Differences in voice quality between men and women: use of the long-term average spectrum (LTAS). J Voice. 1996;10(1):59–66. 10.1016/S0892-1997(96)80019-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y: Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Roy Stat Soc B Met. 1995;57(1):289–300. 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Wahbeh H, Delorme A, cannard C, et al. : Wahbeh_Scripts from Trance Channel study.2018. 10.6084/m9.figshare.7469972.v1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Wahbeh H, Delorme A, okonsky J, et al. : Wahbeh_Data for Trance Channel Study.2018. 10.6084/m9.figshare.7355027.v2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Bukenaite A, Stochl J, Mossaheb N, et al. : Usefulness of the CAPE-P15 for detecting people at ultra-high risk for psychosis: Psychometric properties and cut-off values. Schizophr Res. 2017;189:69–74. 10.1016/j.schres.2017.02.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Rammstedt B: The 10-item Big Five Inventory: Norm values and investigation of sociodemographic effects based on a German population representative sample. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2007;23(3):193 10.1027/1015-5759.23.3.193 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Srivastava S, John OP, Gosling SD, et al. : Development of personality in early and middle adulthood: set like plaster or persistent change? J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;84(5):1041–53. 10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Glisky ML, Tataryn DJ, Tobias BA, et al. : Absorption, openness to experience, and hypnotizability. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;60(2):263–72. 10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Hageman JH, Krippner S, Wickramasekera II I: Across cultural boundaries: psychophysiological responses, absorption, and dissociation comparison between Brazilian spiritists and advanced meditators. Neuroquantology. 2011;9(1) 10.14704/nq.2011.9.1.386 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Aron EN, Aron A, Jagiellowicz J: Sensory processing sensitivity: a review in the light of the evolution of biological responsivity. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2012;16(3):262–282. 10.1177/1088868311434213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Wahbeh H, Yount G, Delorme A, et al. : The paranormal belief and experience scale: validation and reliability. In preparation2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Seligman R: From affliction to affirmation: narrative transformation and the therapeutics of Candomblé mediumship. Transcult Psychiatry. 2005;42(2):272–294. 10.1177/1363461505052668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Kennedy J, Kanthamani H, Palmer J: Psychic and spiritual experiences, health, well-being, and meaning in life. J Parapsychol. 1994;58(4):353–383. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Oohashi T, Kawai N, Honda M, et al. : Electroencephalographic measurement of possession trance in the field. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;113(3):435–445. 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00002-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Brown GD, Yamada S, Sejnowski TJ: Independent component analysis at the neural cocktail party. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24(1):54–63. 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01683-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Kawai N, Honda M, Nakamura S, et al. : Catecholamines and opioid peptides increase in plasma in humans during possession trances. Neuroreport. 2001;12(16):3419–3423. 10.1097/00001756-200111160-00009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Cahn BR, Polich J: Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(2):180–211. 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Fox KC, Nijeboer S, Dixon ML, et al. : Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;43:48–73. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Fox KC, Dixon ML, Nijeboer S, et al. : Functional neuroanatomy of meditation: A review and meta-analysis of 78 functional neuroimaging investigations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;65:208–228. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
F1000Res. 2019 Feb 20. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.19896.r44383

Reviewer response for version 2

Marco Aurélio Vinhosa Bastos Jr 1

The revision has taken into account reviewer comments and is improved. I confirm to have checked all new statistics included those presented in Table 4. I consider that the paper can now be indexed.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2019 Feb 14. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.19896.r44384

Reviewer response for version 2

Patrizio Tressoldi 1

The authors replied to all my comments in a satisfactory way. I confirm to have checked all new statistics included those presented in Table 4.

I noted a couples of typos:

  • On Pg. 12: "In response to a reviewers comments", should be "In response to a reviewer's comments”.

  • On Pg. 13: "ECG measures were the statistically the same" should be "ECG measures were statistically non-significant.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2019 Jan 28. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.18757.r43134

Reviewer response for version 1

Marco Aurélio Vinhosa Bastos Jr 1

The article is a detailed account of an experiment investigating physiological correlates of channeling, a type of mediumistic experience. Specifically, the investigators selected only 'trance channels' as participants, i.e. when discarnate entities allegedly speak directly through the mouth of the 'channel' (medium), rather than those individuals with more mental and intuitive mediumistic experiences. In addition to several psychometric measurements, the method included evaluation of many physiological parameters (EEG, ECG, respiration, 'electrodermal activity', voice analysis), during two days of experiments in the Laboratory, adopting a 'within-subject' design – comparison of the findings in the 'channeling' and 'no-channeling' conditions. Basically, no differences were noted in subjects' physiological parameters between the channeling and no-channeling conditions, except for the voice analysis, which showed lower valence and lower arousal in the channeling condition. The experiment is interesting and seems to have been well executed (e.g. appropriate counterbalanced design of tasks to avoid ‘carry over effects’, clear exposition of recruitment strategy and response-rates, sound statistical analysis), and the paper is well written.

However, I have some specific comments, as follows:

Major points:

  • In order to avoid artifacts during EEG recording of the experiment, only mediums that assured being able to stand still during the channeling were selected. Additionally, being able to control when to start and when to terminate the channeling experience and having a ‘consensual working relationship with discarnate beings’ were also inclusion criteria. The authors discuss their results, in view of the few studies on the topic available in the literature. The findings (basically ‘no-differences’) are somewhat incongruent to most of the previous studies (pointed out in the papers' References), that have suggested greater arousal in subjects during the mediumistic experience, both in 'within-subject' and in between-group design (comparing with non-medium subjects from the same cultural contexts) experiments. The authors consider that the explanation for this incongruence to previous studies may be the more rigorous statistical methods they adopted, although recognizing that it may also stem from the un-naturalistic design of the study. In the present study, the channels needed to enter into trance but the communicating entities could not speak, except for the periods when the investigators asked them to read standard texts, so that the medium` voice could be recorded for analysis. This point is approached mainly in the Limitations section (pages 15, last paragraph), but some words about it should be included when discussing ECG data as well.

  • The finding of differences in voice analysis, which is interesting and deserves to be confirmed in other studies, could maybe be tested with a more naturalistic (and perhaps more appropriate) approach, allowing the entity's spontaneous speech through the medium, and then performing transcription of the speech. This could then be read by the same medium at another moment, in the 'no-channeling' condition, comparing voice parameters. It can be anticipated that results of voice analysis might be very different if spontaneous mediumistic speech is allowed.

  • Differently from what the authors state in the Discussion section (page 14, first paragraph), Hughes et al. (1990) have not evaluated theta, alpha and beta POWER pre-, during and post-trance in channels. Instead, they have evaluated “the percentage of time that theta (or alpha, or beta) brainwave activity was registered by the EEG equipment” 1. This may have a different physiological meaning.  

  • I agree with Professor Tressoldi, it is important to include the results of ECG data in the article (mean ± standard deviation, statistics). The same for respiration data. 

Minor points:

  • I wonder whether Figure 4 could be reframed so that it becomes clearer for the non-specialist reader.

  • Last paragraph of the Introduction section: ‘distinguishing these two states as distinct’. This sentence seems overly redundant.

  • Page 6, third paragraph: ‘confounds in many paranormal belief scales of belief’. This is probably a spelling error.

  • Page 14, penultimate line of first paragraph: ‘considering may of our participants stated that it felt strange’. It should read ‘many of our participants stated’.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard, however I have significant reservations, as outlined above.

References

  • 1. Hughes DJ, Melville NT: Changes in brainwave activity during trance channeling: a pilot study. The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology.1990;22(2) : [Google Scholar]
F1000Res. 2019 Feb 4.
Helané Wahbeh 1

Thank you for your thoughtful comments about our manuscript. We have incorporated your suggestions into the manuscript as outlined in italics under each point.

  • In order to avoid artifacts during EEG recording of the experiment, only mediums that assured being able to stand still during the channeling were selected. Additionally, being able to control when to start and when to terminate the channeling experience and having a ‘consensual working relationship with discarnate beings’ were also inclusion criteria. The authors discuss their results, in view of the few studies on the topic available in the literature. The findings (basically ‘no-differences’) are somewhat incongruent to most of the previous studies (pointed out in the papers' References), that have suggested greater arousal in subjects during the mediumistic experience, both in 'within-subject' and in between-group design (comparing with non-medium subjects from the same cultural contexts) experiments. The authors consider that the explanation for this incongruence to previous studies may be the more rigorous statistical methods they adopted, although recognizing that it may also stem from the un-naturalistic design of the study. In the present study, the channels needed to enter into trance but the communicating entities could not speak, except for the periods when the investigators asked them to read standard texts, so that the medium` voice could be recorded for analysis. This point is approached mainly in the Limitations section (pages 15, last paragraph), but some words about it should be included when discussing ECG data as well.

- We have added some language about this point in the ECG discussion section.

  • The finding of differences in voice analysis, which is interesting and deserves to be confirmed in other studies, could maybe be tested with a more naturalistic (and perhaps more appropriate) approach, allowing the entity's spontaneous speech through the medium, and then performing transcription of the speech. This could then be read by the same medium at another moment, in the 'no-channeling' condition, comparing voice parameters. It can be anticipated that results of voice analysis might be very different if spontaneous mediumistic speech is allowed.

- Thank you, we have added text about this into our discussion section.

  • Differently from what the authors state in the Discussion section (page 14, first paragraph), Hughes et al. (1990) have not evaluated theta, alpha and beta POWER pre-, during and post-trance in channels. Instead, they have evaluated “the percentage of time that theta (or alpha, or beta) brainwave activity was registered by the EEG equipment” 1. This may have a different physiological meaning.  

- We have corrected this.

  • I agree with Professor Tressoldi, it is important to include the results of ECG data in the article (mean ± standard deviation, statistics). The same for respiration data. 

- ECG and respiration descriptive statistics have been included.

Minor points:

  • I wonder whether Figure 4 could be reframed so that it becomes clearer for the non-specialist reader.

- We have added a legend for Figure 4 that helps clarify what it means.

  • Last paragraph of the Introduction section: ‘distinguishing these two states as distinct’. This sentence seems overly redundant.

- This phrase has been removed.

  • Page 6, third paragraph: ‘confounds in many paranormal belief scales of belief’. This is probably a spelling error.

- Extra belief has been removed

  • Page 14, penultimate line of first paragraph: ‘considering may of our participants stated that it felt strange’. It should read ‘many of our participants stated’.

- This has been fixed.

F1000Res. 2019 Jan 24. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.18757.r43131

Reviewer response for version 1

Patrizio Tressoldi 1

As the authors said "This study has moved channeling research forwards by evaluating physiological measure differences between the channeling and no-channeling states using rigorous controlled methods".

A further strength of this study is the test of the reliability of their physiological measures.

Main comments:

  • Both in the introduction and the discussion of their study, it is important to make explicit the exploratory approach for the choice of the variables and their parameters, unless pre-defined in the grant protocol of the funding institution. In this case, the authors should make this document available.

  • The procedure to read stories both in non-channeling and channeling condition is unusual and should comment a little bit further in particular considering how to convince the purported entity to read a story chosen by the experimenters.

  • The data presented in Table 3, shows a clear trend from session 1 to 3 that, in my view, is important to take in consideration.

  • As for all other variables, add the descriptive statistics of the ECG and respiration data.

  • The difference in the GSR variable must be considered given the non trivial difference and not ignored simply because the p value exceed the conventional cutoff of .05. In fact, as acknowledged by the authors, the statistical power with only 13 participants is not sufficient to detect medium to small effect size differences.

  • For the EEG data, I wonder whether it could be more appropriate to cluster the 64 channels in order to investigate specific regions of interest, enhancing the statistical power of the statistics.

Minor comments:

  • Channel or channeler? I wonder whether to use channeler instead of channel could be more precise in conveying the difference between the agent and his/her action, e.g. in the sentence "Well-known trance channels primarily channel one being"

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2019 Feb 4.
Helané Wahbeh 1

Thank you for your thoughtful comments about our manuscript. We have incorporated your suggestions into the manuscript as outlined in italics under each point.

Main comments:

  • Both in the introduction and the discussion of their study, it is important to make explicit the exploratory approach for the choice of the variables and their parameters, unless pre-defined in the grant protocol of the funding institution. In this case, the authors should make this document available.

- The analysis of the study were pre-defined in the grant protocol. The original grant protocol has been added to the extended data stores. In the introduction, we have added a sentence regarding this and it is also already included in the Methods in the Study Overview section.

  • The procedure to read stories both in non-channeling and channeling condition is unusual and should comment a little bit further in particular considering how to convince the purported entity to read a story chosen by the experimenters.

- We have added more information about this in the Methods section.

  • The data presented in Table 3, shows a clear trend from session 1 to 3 that, in my view, is important to take in consideration.

Yes we agree. We have added a few sentences about this in the discussion session. We had only asked about these two scales during the channeling condition. Because of this we can’t look at these values as a covariate to see if any of the physiology are different between channeling and no-channeling were influenced by these factors. I did a brief exploratory analysis looking at only the channeling alpha EEG collapsing all channels and there the unconsciousness and incorporation variables were not significant by themselves or as an interaction with session number. It would be important to consider this for future studies and if possible, incorporate it in way so that they could be included in the statistical model.

  • As for all other variables, add the descriptive statistics of the ECG and respiration data.

- Descriptive statistics for ECG and respiration data have been added to the text.

  • The difference in the GSR variable must be considered given the non-trivial difference and not ignored simply because the p value exceed the conventional cutoff of .05. In fact, as acknowledged by the authors, the statistical power with only 13 participants is not sufficient to detect medium to small effect size differences.

- We have added an exploratory paired t-test comparing the collapsed average values of GSR for channeling and no-channeling. The results were significant. We also added a few sentences about this in the discussion section.

  • For the EEG data, I wonder whether it could be more appropriate to cluster the 64 channels in order to investigate specific regions of interest, enhancing the statistical power of the statistics.

- We have taken this advice and clustered the 64 channels by location and conducted exploratory analyses. The descriptive statistics and analyses results are now included in the paper.

Minor comments:

  • Channel or channeler? I wonder whether to use channeler instead of channel could be more precise in conveying the difference between the agent and his/her action, e.g. in the sentence "Well-known trance channels primarily channel one being"

-Channeler has been included to refer to the agent throughout the manuscript.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Availability Statement

    Underlying data

    Underlying data including voice analysis data, survey data, lab temperature and humidity, raw physiology data files, and transcripts of open-ended questions are available from Figshare

    Figshare: Dataset 1. Wahbeh_Data for Trance Channel Study. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.7355027.v2 69

    Extended data

    The online survey, stories read by the participants, transcripts, grant proposal and scripts for process the data are available as Extended data from figshare.

    Figshare: Extended data 1. Wahbeh_Additional Data for Trance Channel Study. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.7423454.v2 40

    Figshare: Extended data 2. Wahbeh_Scripts from Trance Channel study. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.7469972.v1 68

    All underlying and extended data are available under a CC0 1.0 Universal Public Domain Dedication.


    Articles from F1000Research are provided here courtesy of F1000 Research Ltd

    RESOURCES