Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2019 Feb 26;9:2779. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-39174-6

Repeated neonatal isoflurane exposures in the mouse induce apoptotic degenerative changes in the brain and relatively mild long-term behavioral deficits

Susan E Maloney 1,4,6, Carla M Yuede 1,2, Catherine E Creeley 3, Sasha L Williams 1, Jacob N Huffman 1, George T Taylor 4, Kevin N Noguchi 1,6, David F Wozniak 1,5,6,
PMCID: PMC6391407  PMID: 30808927

Abstract

Epidemiological studies suggest exposures to anesthetic agents and/or sedative drugs (AASDs) in children under three years old, or pregnant women during the third trimester, may adversely affect brain development. Evidence suggests lengthy or repeated AASD exposures are associated with increased risk of neurobehavioral deficits. Animal models have been valuable in determining the type of acute damage in the developing brain induced by AASD exposures, as well as in elucidating long-term functional consequences. Few studies examining very early exposure to AASDs suggest this may be a critical period for inducing long-term functional consequences, but the impact of repeated exposures at these ages has not yet been assessed. To address this, we exposed mouse pups to a prototypical general anesthetic, isoflurane (ISO, 1.5% for 3 hr), at three early postnatal ages (P3, P5 and P7). We quantified the acute neuroapoptotic response to a single versus repeated exposure, and found age- and brain region-specific effects. We also found that repeated early exposures to ISO induced subtle, sex-specific disruptions to activity levels, motor coordination, anxiety-related behavior and social preference. Our findings provide evidence that repeated ISO exposures may induce behavioral disturbances that are subtle in nature following early repeated exposures to a single AASD.

Introduction

There is continuing concern that exposing the developing brain to anesthetic agents and/or sedative drugs (AASDs) may induce neuropathologic effects leading to compromised cognitive functions. This concern is evidenced by a recent warning from the U.S. Food and Drug Administration, which stated that repeated or lengthy exposure to AASDs in children under the age of three, or in pregnant women during the third trimester, may affect children’s brain development1,2. However, the warning also importantly pointed out that based on animal and human studies, single or relatively brief exposures to these types of agents do not appear to pose substantial risk to the developing brain. Results from a meta-analysis conducted on epidemiological evidence linking anesthesia/surgery with poor neurodevelopmental outcomes in children showed a modestly elevated risk of adverse behavioral or developmental outcomes in children who were exposed to anesthesia/surgery early in childhood3. Subsequent research suggested longer, but not shorter, durations of anesthesia during a single exposure are associated with adverse outcomes4,5. Results from investigations into long-term academic and cognitive performance, as well as risk of mental disorders, developmental delay, and ADHD diagnoses following childhood surgery/anesthesia suggest that there may be subgroups vulnerable to adverse outcomes following early anesthesia exposure6,7. In addition, multiple, but not single, exposures to anesthesia have been linked to later learning disabilities and ADHD811, suggesting a particular risk to repeated exposures to anesthesia in early childhood. The fact that these drugs are often required for surgeries in young children or pregnant women underscores the necessity to identify conditions which pose risk for maldevelopment so that therapeutic strategies may be developed to mitigate adverse outcomes.

Animal studies have shown that one type of acute damage to the developing brain induced by AASDs is increased apoptotic neurodegeneration, which likely involves N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) glutamate receptor antagonism or potentiation of γ-aminobutyric acid A (GABAA) receptors12. Recent animal studies have shown AASDs also impair neuronal function by disturbing timely axonal pruning and cues that promote accurate directional growth of axons during development13,14. Preclinical research has been valuable in defining the spatial and temporal patterns of the acute apoptotic damage, which is widespread and includes cell-type-specific patterns13. The degree to which specific brain areas are impacted depends on the developmental age each area is undergoing synaptogenesis, creating a window of vulnerability to AASD-induced apoptosis15. Neurons are most vulnerable to AASD-induced apoptosis from embryonic day (E)19 to postnatal day (P)1416 in rodents. While damage can be observed across many brain regions at any age in this window, stages of heightened vulnerability within distinct brain regions have been observed17.

Both rodent and non-human primate (NHP) models have been useful for elucidating long-term functional consequences of AASD-induced acute apoptotic damage and for identifying specific parameters that confer this risk (See Maloney et al.16 for full review). The majority of rodent studies devoted to characterizing the behavioral impairments related to AASD-induced apoptotic damage have involved treating mice or rats at P7 or later. This age corresponds to the peak of synaptogenesis within the rodent brain, and is analogous to the third trimester of pregnancy through the first few years of infancy in humans18,19. Only four rodent studies have involved behavioral analyses following exposure to an anesthetic agent at an age earlier than P7. Learning and memory deficits were reported in all four studies when mice or rats were tested at 1–2 months of age, and a longer-duration exposure to sevoflurane also produced disturbances in social interactions20. The behavioral effects of repeated exposures to an AASD at P7 and/or later have also been assessed in the rodent2127. Only one out of eight of the repeated exposure studies failed to demonstrate functional impairments, which was likely due to a very short exposure duration (30 min)28. Studies involving the behavioral outcomes resulting from longer-duration developmental exposures to volatile anesthetics in rodents P7 or older, sometimes coupled with systemic sedative agents2935, have also been conducted. Three out of ten of these studies reported no behavioral impairments, perhaps due to comparatively low concentrations of the anesthetic agent and the absence of any additional sedative agent3638. Together, the findings from rodent studies on the functional consequences of developmental AASD exposure confirm that repeated and longer-duration exposures typically confer the greatest risk of behavioral impairments. However, there is scant evidence available regarding the impact of multiple exposures at earlier ages in the rodent, and very little work has been done to identify any sex-related effects in any murine models.

The relevance of rodent studies on the functional consequences of developmental AASD exposures has been confirmed by work using the infant NHP model. Of the five studies known to us, all have reported functional deficits later in life following infantile exposure to volatile general or systemic anesthetics. Behavioral perturbations include learning and memory deficits39, increased anxiety4042, impaired motor reflexes, and altered social affiliation42. Results from NHP studies point out that the effects of early AASD exposure on levels of anxiety-like behaviors, motor performance, and social interactions have not been thoroughly examined in rodent models16. Thus, while the negative impact of repeated and longer-duration exposures on learning and memory performance have been fairly well documented during the middle stages of vulnerability (i.e., at P7) in rodents, the question of whether these same exposure parameters also produce deficits in other behavioral domains remains to be fully characterized. In addition, the few studies examining very early exposure to AASDs suggest this is a critical period for inducing long-term functional consequences, but the impact of repeated exposures at early ages has not yet been adequately assessed.

We have designed the present study to yield information with translational relevance to identifying aspects of AASD developmental exposure protocols that most likely produce negative neuropathological and functional outcomes. The experimental design includes a comparison of the effects resulting from a single, putatively nontoxic exposure to a prototypical anesthetic agent, with those accruing from repeated exposures of the same agent on levels of apoptotic degeneration in the developing brain and on subsequent behavioral functions. Specifically, we exposed mouse pups to isoflurane (ISO) at one or multiple ages and quantified the acute apoptotic response. In addition, we characterized the effects of repeated exposures to ISO at very young postnatal ages (P3+5+7) on a variety of behavioral functions including learning and memory, motor/sensorimotor capabilities, emotionality, and social interactions. Our behavioral studies also incorporated an analysis of sex-related effects since this variable has received little attention within the research area16.

Results

Neonatal ISO exposures induce elevated levels of neuroapoptosis in mice

Following published methods regarding ISO inhalational exposure in rodents43, C57BL/6 pups were exposed to 1.5% ISO or oxygenated air (AIR) for 3 h at P3, P5, P7 or P3+5+7. Acute neuroapoptotic response was assessed using activated-caspase-3 (AC3) immunohistochemistry (IHC) with degrees of neurodegeneration quantified by the optical dissector and fractionator method44. Exposure of neonatal mice to a single dose of ISO induced abnormally high levels of neuroapoptosis in the cortex, thalamus, dorsal striatum, and dorsal hippocampus in an age-dependent manner (Fig. 1; Table 1). ISO exposure at P3 significantly increased AC3+ profiles compared to AIR in the thalamus (Fig. 2A), in contrast to exposure at P5, when levels of AC3+ profiles were significantly increased in all brain regions (Fig. 2B). At P7, the cortex was the only brain region to show significantly increased neuroapoptosis for ISO relative to AIR (Fig. 2C). Thus, our single-age ISO data suggests the pattern and degree of drug-induced neuroapoptosis varies as a function of age, which may be related to the ontogenetic development of the structures involved19.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Age at greatest vulnerability to increased apoptosis induced by ISO in the developing mouse brain is region-specific. Mouse pups were exposed to 1.5% ISO or AIR only for 3 h on P3, P5, or P7, and processed for AC3+ IHC beginning 6 h after initiation of exposure. Representative AC3+ IHC images of mouse pup brain at the ages at which the greatest increase in apoptotic neurodegenerative response induced by ISO above typical developmental levels (AIR) was observed. Thalamus, P3; Hippocampus, P5; Striatum, P5; Cortex; P5. Scale bar is 50 µm.

Table 1.

Statistical results of AC3+ IHC quantification in brains of ISO- and AIR-exposed mouse pups.

Age At Exposure rmANOVA interaction Within Region Comparisons (Drug)
Region × Drug Cortex Thalamus Striatum Hippocampus
P3 F(3,33) = 3.313, p = 0.032 F(1,44) = 1.173, p = 1.000 F(1,44) = 18.289, p = 0.0004 F(1,44) = 2.622, p = 0.448 F(1,44) = 3.271, p = 0.308
P5 F(2.82,31.02) = 5.443, p = 0.005 F(1,44) = 10.746, p = 0.008 F(1,44) = 8.491, p = 0.022 F(1,44) = 51.644, p < 0.000005 F(1,44) = 8.179, p = 0.026
P7 F(3,42) = 4.644, p = 0.007 F(1,56) = 25.553, p = 0.00002 F(1,56) = 1.987, p = 0.656 F(1,56) = 1.851, p = 0.716 F(1,56) = 0.379, p = 1.000
P7, P5+7, P3+5+7 Region × Exposure F(3,360) = 24.762, p < 0.000005 F(3,360) = 2.768, p = 0.176 F(3,360) = 13.851, p < 0.000005 F(3,360) = 5.518, p = 0.004
F(5.89,78.5) = 4.280, p = 0.0009

All distributions were normal and p-values reflect Bonferroni correction.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Neonatal ISO exposure increased the density of AC3+ profiles following single and multiple exposures in an age-dependent manner. Mouse pups were exposed to 1.5% ISO for 3 h or AIR only on P3, P5, P7, P5+7 or P3+5+7 and the density of AC3+ profiles was quantified in four distinct brain regions. (A) At P3, increased AC3+ densities were observed in the thalamus only of ISO-exposed brains compared to AIR-exposed controls. (B) At P5, an increase in AC3+ densities was observed in all brain regions examined. (C) At P7, increased AC3+ densities were observed only in the cortex of ISO-exposed brains compared to AIR-exposed controls. (D) In the cortex, enhanced AC3+ densities were observed following ISO exposure on P7, P5+7, and P3+5+7 compared to AIR only exposure. No change in AC3+ density over AIR controls was observed in the thalamus at P7 regardless of number of exposures. Only following ISO exposure on P5+7 were increased AC3+ densities observed in the striatum and hippocampus over AIR controls. Means ± SEM are shown. All data points represented by open gray circles.

One of the main goals of this study was to investigate the behavioral effects of ISO on the developing brain. Since the human literature suggested that repeated, but not single, anesthetic exposures during childhood were associated with adverse developmental and behavioral outcomes811, we evaluated the effects of multiple administrations of ISO during the early neonatal period (P3-P7). To accomplish this goal, we conducted parallel studies in separate cohorts of mice exposed to 1.5% ISO or AIR for 3 h at P3+5+7: one cohort was used for histopathological studies while another cohort was used for behavioral analyses. The mice used for histopathology studies were euthanized 3 h following the final exposure at P7 and brain tissue was processed for AC3-IHC and quantification of AC3+ profiles as described above. The mice used in the behavioral cohort were studied on a battery of behavioral tests beginning in the early post-weaning period up into adulthood.

Analysis of the AC3+ profile data from the multiple ISO dosing study (Fig. 2D; Table 1) showed, in good agreement with results from the P7 group in the single ISO dosing study, that the cortex was the only brain region to have a significant increase in neuroapoptosis relative to the AIR group. Moreover, the cortex was the only region that showed significantly more AC3+ cells relative to AIR, regardless of the number of prior exposures (i.e., in the P5+7 and P3+5+7 ISO groups). However, AC3+ profiles were found to be significantly elevated in the cortex, striatum, and hippocampus in the P5+7 ISO group relative to their respective AIR groups. Interestingly, AC3+ profiles were significantly decreased in the cortex, striatum, and hippocampus of the P3+5+7 ISO groups relative to the P5+7 groups; although the apoptosis response in the hippocampus was less than that observed in these other two brain regions. We hypothesize that this effect is likely due to very large deletions of neurons resulting from previous ISO exposures, and the number of surviving neurons vulnerable to drug-induced apoptosis was greatly reduced. It is possible the reduced degree of apoptotic response observed within the hippocampus at P3+5+7 is attributable to the higher levels of neurogenesis in this area, and this may contribute to the decreased density of AC3+ profiles. Previous research suggests progenitor proliferation in the hippocampus is decreased for up to five days following neonatal ISO exposure30, although the role of perturbed neurogenesis and its interaction with apoptosis following neonatal anesthesia exposure remains to be evaluated in future studies45. It is noteworthy that the levels of AC3+ profiles were unaltered in the thalamus at P7 following the repeated ISO exposures. We interpret this lack of effect of multiple ISO doses on the thalamus to at least partially reflect that P7 is an age beyond the peak sensitivity of ISO-induced apoptosis in the thalamus.

Multiple neonatal exposures of ISO induce only mild behavioral deficits

To investigate the possible longitudinal impact of ISO-induced increased neuroapoptosis response on behavioral function, we exposed male and female C57BL/6 mouse pups to ISO or AIR on P3+5+7, using the same exposure parameters as reported above. We assessed behaviors across multiple domains to characterize the influence of multiple ISO exposures on learning and memory and other behavioral functions not yet comprehensively examined in the rodent, including neuromotor, emotionality and social interactions. Our behavioral characterization was initiated during the juvenile stage and continued into adulthood (see Table 2 for order of and ages at testing). Based on the findings in previous rat studies suggesting important sex-specific behavioral consequences following neonatal ISO exposure23,46, behavioral performance of males and females was also analyzed separately to identify sex-dependent behavioral effects of neonatal ISO exposure in the mouse.

Table 2.

Order of and age at testing to evaluate behavioral effects of multiple neonatal ISO exposures.

Behavioral Tests Age At Testing
1- h locomotor activity P31/P137
Sensorimotor battery P32–33
Morris water maze P34–42
Rotarod P45, P49, P53
Elevated plus maze P54–56/P144–146
Social approach P60/P178
Conditioned fear P194–196

To identify any gross developmental delay induced by multiple ISO exposures that might underlie long-term alterations in behavioral functions, we inspected the mice daily for any signs of gross physical abnormality and monitored body weight from P3 through weaning at P21. We did not observe physical anomalies in the treated mice and analysis of pre-weaning body weights showed the mice were unaffected by ISO exposure (Supplementary Table S1), suggesting multiple ISO exposures did not induce gross developmental delay. In addition, we did not observe weight differences between ISO and AIR mice at later ages corresponding to behavioral testing (P30, P45 and P60). These results thereby eliminate ISO-induced delays in growth and development as a confounding factor.

Functional characterization of ISO-exposed mice revealed subtle, sex-specific abnormalities across several behavioral domains. Behavioral assessment began with tests to measure general activity or sensorimotor disruptions that may interfere with interpretation of later behavioral test results. On P31, a 1-h locomotor activity/exploration test was used to assess general activity, exploratory behavior, and emotionality of the mice. Two days later (P32–33) a sensorimotor test battery was used to evaluate balance, strength, and coordination as previously described47. No differences between groups were observed for any of the sensorimotor tests for males or females (Supplementary Table S1). Also, although no significant overall effects for ISO were found for ambulatory activity or vertical rearing, sex-dependent differences were found with regard to general activity. Specifically, male ISO mice displayed significantly fewer total ambulations (whole body movements) on average across time compared to the male AIR control group (Fig. 3A; Table 3), although pair-wise comparisons did not reveal significant differences for any given time blocks. Similarly, while there were no significant overall drug effects for time spent at rest, the male ISO group showed significantly increased rest times compared to the male AIR mice (Fig. 3B; Table 3). No differences were observed in females regarding these two variables. In addition, there were no significant overall drug effects, nor any sex-specific differences found for vertical rearing or any emotionality (center of chamber) variables (Table 3).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Multiple neonatal ISO exposures results in subtle, sex-specific behavioral disruptions. Male and female mice were exposed to 1.5% ISO for 3 h or AIR only on P3+5+7 and behaviorally characterized across multiple domains beginning during the juvenile stage and continuing into adulthood. (A,B) During the 1-h locomotor activity/exploration test, male, but not female, ISO mice exhibited (A) decreased total ambulations across the six 10-min blocks (*p = 0.031) and (B) increased total time at rest. (C) ISO mice spent less time on the accelerating rotarod during the first trial on Day 2 and second trial on Day 3 compared to AIR mice (*p = 0.003, Day 3). (D) These deficits were most pronounced between male mice (*p = 0.012, Day 3). (E,F) Male ISO mice spent less (E) absolute (*p = 0.028) and (F) percent total time in the open arms (*p = 0.032) of the EPM compared to male AIR mice. Female mice displayed comparable EPM performance across groups. (G) During the preference for social novelty trial of the social approach task, both male ISO and AIR mice exhibited a preference to spend more time investigating the novel conspecific and female AIR mice exhibited a trend for this preference, while female ISO mice spent a similar amount of time investigating both the familiar and novel conspecifics. (H) Only male AIR mice exhibited a greater number of entries into the investigation zone around the novel mouse compared to that around the familiar mouse. Means ± SEM are shown. All data points represented by open gray circles.

Table 3.

Statistical results of behavioral testing of ISO- and AIR-exposed mice.

Variable Comparison Statistical test Output P value Nonparametric
Total ambulations (across 10-min blocks) Drug two-way rmANOVA F(1,31) = 2.497 p = 0.124 normal
Males only, Drug rmANOVA F(1,18) = 5.485 p = 0.031 normal
Females only, Drug rmANOVA F(1,13) = 0.005 p = 0.952 normal
Rearing (across 10-min blocks) Drug two-way rmANOVA F(1,31) = 2.625 p = 0.115 normal
Males only, Drug rmANOVA F(1,18) = 0.447 p = 0.512 n/a
Females only, Drug rmANOVA F(1,13) = 4.360 p = 0.057 normal
Time at rest Drug two-way ANOVA F(1,31) = 2.100 p = 0.157 normal
Male only, Drug ANOVA F(1,18) = 5.148 p = 0.036 normal
Females only, Drug ANOVA F(1,13) = 0.017 p = 0.898 normal
Time in center area Drug two-way ANOVA F(1,31) = 0.049 p = 0.823 normal
Males only, Drug ANOVA F(1,18) = 0.162 p = 0.692 normal
Females only, Drug ANOVA F(1,13) = 0.013 p = 0.911 normal
Entries into center area Drug two-way ANOVA F(1,31) = 1.343 p = 0.255 normal
Males only, Drug ANOVA F(1,18) = 4.072 p = 0.059 normal
Females only, Drug ANOVA F(1,13) = 0.046 p = 0.883 U(15) = 21, p = 0.529
Accelerating rotarod (across 3 days/6 trials) Drug × Day two-way rmANOVA F(1,62) = 3.153 p = 0.049 n/a
Day 2, Drug multiple comparison F(1,186) = 5.774 p = 0.051 U(35) = 89, p = 0.037
Day 3, Drug multiple comparison F(1,186) = 10.739 p = 0.003 U(35) = 92, p = 0.048
Males only, Drug × Day rmANOVA F(2,36) = 4.648 p = 0.016 normal
Males only, Day 2, Drug multiple comparison F(1,108) = 1.602 p = 0.208 U(20) = 36, p = 0.315
Males only, Day 3, Drug multiple comparison F(1,108) = 8.594 p = 0.012 normal
Females only, Drug × Day rmANOVA F(2,26) = 0.357 p = 0.703 normal
Females only, Day 2, Drug multiple comparison F(1,78) = 3.946 p = 0.050 U(15) = 11, p = 0.066
Females only, Day 3, Drug multiple comparison F(1,78) = 3.307 p = 0.073 normal
Time in open arm (across 3 days) Drug two-way rmANOVA F(1,31) = 1.095 p = 0.303 n/a
Males only, Drug rmANOVA F(1,18) = 5.750 p = 0.028 n/a
Females only, Drug rmANOVA F(1,13) = 0.007 p = 0.934 normal
Percent time in open arm (across 3 days) Drug two-way rmANOVA F(1,31) = 1.219 p = 0.278 n/a
Day × Sex two-way rmANOVA F(1.95,60.28) = 3.481 p = 0.038 n/a
Males only, Drug rmANOVA F(1,18) = 5.425 p = 0.032 n/a
Females only, Drug rmANOVA F(1,13) = 0.024 p = 0.879 normal
Sociability, investigation zone time (social vs. empty) Drug × Zone two-way rmANOVA F(1,31) = 0.010 p = 0.920 n/a
AIR, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 51.581 p < 0.000005 Z = −3.516, p = 0.0004
ISO, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 67.629 p < 0.000005 normal
AIR males only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 41.580 p < 0.000005 Z = −2.201, p = 0.028
AIR females only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 16.726 p = 0.0003 normal
ISO males only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 38.926 p = 0.000001 normal
ISO females only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 29.340 p = 0.000007 normal
Sociability, investigation zone entries (social vs. empty) Drug × Zone two-way rmANOVA F(1,31) = 1.165 p = 0.289 normal
AIR, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 54.161 p < 0.000005 normal
ISO, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 44.190 p < 0.000005 normal
AIR males only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 43.581 p < 0.000005 normal
AIR females only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 17.602 p = 0.0002 normal
ISO males only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 21.073 p = 0.00007 normal
ISO females only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 23.118 p = 0.00004 normal
Social novelty, investigation zone time (familiar vs. novel) Drug × Zone two-way rmANOVA F(1,31) = 2.882 p = 0.099 n/a
Sex × Zone two-way rmANOVA F(1,31) = 5.545 p = 0.025 normal
AIR, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 19.266 p = 0.0001 normal
ISO, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 5.662 p = 0.024 Z = −1.771, p = 0.077
AIR males only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 23.975 p = 0.00003 normal
AIR females only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 3.095 p = 0.088 normal
ISO males only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 8.939 p = 0.005 Z = −1.988, p = 0.047
ISO females only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 0.197 p = 0.660 normal
Social novelty, investigation zone entries (familiar vs. novel) Drug × Zone two-way rmANOVA F(1,31) = 2.457 p = 0.127 normal
Sex × Zone two-way rmANOVA F(1,31) = 5.462 p = 0.026 normal
AIR, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 14.146 p = 0.0007 normal
ISO, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 3.493 p = 0.071 normal
AIR males only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 25.172 p = 0.00002 normal
AIR females only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 0.759 p = 0.390 normal
ISO males only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 3.864 p = 0.058 normal
ISO females only, Zone planned comparison F(1,31) = 0.506 p = 0.482 normal
Males only, Novel Zone, Drug multiple comparison F(1,62) = 5.173 p = 0.052 normal

For non-normal distributions, Mann-Whitney U and Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks nonparametric tests for two independent and paired samples, respectively, are also reported. Equivalent nonparametric test for two-way rmANOVA not available (n/a). Bonferroni correction reflected in p-values, where appropriate.

Because learning disabilities were linked to multiple anesthesia exposures in children8,9 and learning and memory deficits were observed previously in rodents following neonatal anesthesia exposure23,29,30,33,46,4851, we evaluated our mice in the Morris water maze (MWM) to assess spatial learning and memory following our standard protocols47. Beginning on P34, MWM testing started with two days of cued trials to evaluate possible nonassociative dysfunctions induced by ISO that might compromise swimming ability. The mice then received acquisition training during five days of place trials, with a 60-s probe trial conducted 1 h after the last place trial on day 5 to evaluate retention of the platform location. No differences were observed between groups for male or female mice during the cued, place or probe trials (Supplementary Fig. S1, Supplementary Table S1). These data suggest that multiple ISO exposures on P3+5+7 did not produce demonstrable deficits in acquisition or retention capabilities on a spatial reference memory-based test in juvenile mice.

Mice were tested on the rotarod to assess the effects of ISO on fine motor coordination according to our standardized procedures52. The mice received 1 stationary rod trial, 2 constant rotational speed trials, and 2 accelerating rotational speed trials on P45, 49, 53. No significant overall effects involving drug were observed for the stationary rod or constant speed rotarod trials, nor were any sex-specific effects found during these procedures. However, analysis of the data from the accelerating rotarod trials yielded a significant Drug x Day interaction (Fig. 3C; Table 3), and a marginally nonsignificant Drug effect. Subsequent comparisons showed ISO mice remained on the rod for a significantly shorter duration compared to the AIR control group on day 3, according to the Bonferroni criterion (critical alpha p < 0.008), and large, yet marginally significant, differences were also observed for day 2. A major component of the differences observed on trial 2, day 3 were due to inferior performance by male ISO mice compared to the male AIR control group (Fig. 3D; Table 3). Deficits exhibited by the ISO mice on the accelerating rotarod reflect either impaired coordination or motor learning disturbances. Although our protocol was designed to minimize the effects of motor learning by separating test sessions by four intervening days, motor learning still readily occurred, thus making it difficult to separate motor performance from acquisition effects.

Anxiety-like behaviors were assessed in the elevated plus maze (EPM) for 3 consecutive days on P54–56, using our standardized procedures47. No significant overall effects involving Drug were found concerning time spent in the open arms (Table 3). However, planned analyses conducted on each sex revealed a significant Drug effect for the male groups indicating male ISO mice spent significantly less time in the open arms on average across test days compared to the male AIR mice (Fig. 3E; Table 3). However, pair-wise comparisons did not show any significant differences for any given test day, and no significant effects were found for the female groups (Fig. 3E; Table 3). We also examined the percent of total time spent in the open arms out of the total time spent in both sets of arms (% open arm time) to help control for confounding influences due to differences in activity levels between groups. Analysis of these data yielded a significant Sex x Test Day interaction for % open arm time, while planned analyses according to sex revealed results similar to those found for open arm time. Specifically, a significant Drug effect was observed for the male groups showing that, on average across test days, the male ISO mice spent significantly lower percentage of time in the open arms compared to the male AIR controls (Fig. 3F; Table 3). Again, no significant pair-wise comparisons were observed for any given test day, and analysis of the female groups failed to demonstrate any significant effects (Fig. 3F; Table 3).

Sociability and preference for social novelty were assessed on P60 through the use of our standardized social approach procedure47, which was developed with minor adaptation from methods previously described5356. As defined in this measure of restrictive interactions, sociability is reflected by increased social investigation of a novel conspecific over an empty withholding cage, while preference for social novelty is indicated by increased social investigation of a novel versus familiar conspecific. No chamber or zone biases were observed during the habituation trial for either group of males or females, suggesting these variables were not confounding influences on investigatory behavior. ISO and AIR control mice of both sexes each displayed robust sociability in terms of spending a greater time in the investigation zone surrounding the social stimulus (conspecific) compared to the zone time for the non-social stimulus (Table 3). The same results were found for entries made into the investigation zones surrounding the social and non-social stimuli (Table 3). During the preference for social novelty trial, male ISO and AIR control mice spent more time in the investigation zone surrounding the novel mouse relative to the time spent in the zone surrounding the familiar mouse (Fig. 3G; Table 3). Among female mice, neither the ISO nor the AIR groups displayed a significant preference for the novel versus the familiar mouse, although the AIR controls showed a trend toward spending more time in the investigation zone surrounding the novel conspecific compared to the zone times observed for the familiar mouse (Fig. 3G; Table 3). The failure to reach statistical significance in the female control group may be due to the reduced power resulting from the low sample size (n = 6). Results from analyzing the investigation zone entries data were similar, except that male AIR mice exhibited significantly more entries into the novel mouse zone compared to the familiar mouse zone, while the ISO males did not (Fig. 3H; Table 3). The male AIR group also showed a nonsignificant trend toward more entries into the zone surrounding the novel mouse compared to entries of the ISO males (Fig. 3H; Table 3). No differences in entries between investigation zones were observed for female groups (Fig. 3H; Table 3).

To determine whether sex-dependent deficits in the ISO mice described above changed with age, we retested both groups of mice later in adulthood (P137-P178) using the locomotor activity, EPM and social approach tests. Analysis of the ambulatory activity data from the re-test revealed a significant Drug x Sex x Time interaction (Supplementary Table S2), although additional contrasts involving males and females and subsequent pair-wise comparisons did not show significant differences between groups. Analysis of the data from the EPM and social approach retest did not reveal any significant effects involving Drug or any sex-dependent effects (Supplementary Table S2). Lastly, we also assessed possible deficits in Pavlovian conditioning by evaluating the mice on our conditioned fear procedure52. No significant overall effects involving Drug or any sex-dependent effects were found with regard to freezing levels observed during a two-min baseline period or during CS-US training on day one, during the contextual fear test on day two, or on the altered context baseline or auditory cue test on day three (Supplementary Fig. S1; Supplementary Table S1).

Discussion

To elucidate the acute neurological and functional impact of repeated exposures to a prototypical general anesthetic during synaptogenesis, we repeatedly exposed mouse pups to ISO and examined the acute neuroapoptotic response and long-term behavioral responses across several domains. As expected, the degree of drug-induced neuroapoptosis varied according to brain region. The thalamic response peaked earliest at P3, the hippocampus and striatum around P5, and the cortex remained vulnerable through P7. These results are consistent with temporal and spatial patterns of neuroapoptosis susceptibility shown in previous studies16,17. Our histopathological analysis suggests the striatum and cortex are the most vulnerable to repeated ISO exposure because these areas showed additional drug-induced apoptosis with subsequent exposures. Abnormally increased neuroapoptosis was observed only in the cortex following three ISO exposures. The neuroapoptotic response was greatly reduced in other areas examined, likely due to large deletions of neurons having occurred from previous exposures.

Our results provide additional information about the risk conferred by multiple anesthesia exposures for inducing possible functional disturbances. We evaluated the behavioral consequences of exposure to a prototypical general anesthetic at early neonatal ages, and also included sex differences in our analyses. Specifically, we observed hypoactivity and increased anxiogenic behavior in males repeatedly exposed to ISO, as well as motor coordination disruptions in both sexes. Social preference behavior was impacted only in females. The impairments observed were mild compared to those found following more neurotoxic agents such as anesthetic cocktails29, high doses of ethanol57, or multiple doses of PCP58. Of note, we did not observe any learning and memory deficits. This was unexpected given the large number of studies reporting learning and memory impairments following AASD drug exposure in rodents16. It is possible the large amount of handling during development, i.e. daily weight assessments P3 – P21, masked our ability to observe deficits in these mice by providing enrichment during the pre-weaning period57,59,60. Re-testing our mice failed to replicate the findings observed in the younger mice. This may be due, again, to enriching effects of extensive handling and behavioral testing of the mice throughout their early lives. We have reported a similar effect in mice following neonatal exposure to high doses of ethanol where drug-treated mice exhibited profound spatial learning and memory deficits in the MWM as juveniles, but performed similarly to control mice when tested again in young adulthood57. An independent cohort tested only in young adulthood again displayed MWM deficits57. We have also reported the presence of spatial learning deficits in 4-month old rats following neonatal exposure to an anesthetic cocktail29, suggesting the potential to recover cognitive functions following exposure to AASD agents during development may depend on the magnitude of apoptotic degenerative response and degree of initial behavioral impairment. If the experience of extensive handling and behavioral testing have enriching effects on recovery of behavioral functions, subtle behavioral deficits resulting from neonatal administration of anesthetics might be mitigated throughout a study as a function of increasing experience with these parameters.

Considering the results from the present study, an appropriate next step might be to test an independent cohort without extensive handling during development or prior behavioral testing. We recently recommended this approach in a review of the use of animal models to evaluate functional consequences of anesthesia during early development16. Nevertheless, we decided against attempting to replicate our results with an independent cohort because of the relatively small effect sizes we observed and due to the infrequent clinical use of ISO currently. While preclinical research into the effects of ISO increases our knowledge about the functional consequences of exposure to a prototypical general anesthetic during development, and is thus important, we feel a more appropriate use of our resources is to design and conduct additional studies using more up-to-date AASD protocols such as those that involve sevoflurane or desflurane alone or in combination with other AASDs.

Results from the few studies that have examined the sex-specific impact of neonatal ISO exposure on later behavioral function in the rat are inconsistent, likely due to concentration/duration of ISO used. Consistent with increased vulnerability in males observed in our study, impaired learning and memory performances in males but not females were reported for novel object recognition, contextual fear conditioning, RAM, and MWM46,50,51. Lee et al. reported male rats exposed to ISO displayed deficits in social recognition (memory) whereas female rats were unaffected46. Our data, together with findings from these previous studies, suggest that early exposure to AASD drugs may result in sex-specific neurobehavioral outcomes and that biological sex should be a variable included in future AASD animal research.

Of considerable note to interpreting our present ISO results are recent findings suggesting that mouse and NHPs might be similarly affected by multiple ISO exposures in terms of behavioral impairments outside the domains of learning and memory. Specifically, rhesus macaques were exposed to ISO on one or three early postnatal days42, and when tested in infancy, the monkeys exposed to ISO repeatedly displayed disrupted motor reflexes and decreased activity during a free play test. When tested later in development (i.e., 12 months old), monkeys exposed to ISO across multiple days displayed increased affiliative and anxiety-related behaviors. These findings parallel those we observed in our mouse model of multiple neonatal ISO exposures: decreased activity levels, impaired motor coordination, increased anxiety-related behavior and subtle social preference disruptions.

Other noteworthy commonalities between species are underscored by results from NHP studies on the apoptotic effects of exposure to AASDs in the infant rhesus macaque across development, which have demonstrated patterns of regional vulnerability similar to those observed in rodents, with neuroapoptosis peaking at early ages in subcortical regions and lasting into later ages in the neocortex12,17,6164. Functional consequences attributed to the neuroapoptotic response following exposure to AASDs during development may also involve apoptosis of glia, particularly oligodendrocytes, extending the age of vulnerability to this drug-induced toxicity beyond that observed for neurons6265. Oligoapoptosis is markedly greater than neuroapoptosis in the NHP during later65, as opposed to earlier64, stages of development. We recently provided preliminary evidence that anesthetic exposure might also induce enhanced oligoapoptosis in the rodent16. While peak oligodendrocyte maturation likely occurs at later ages than when the mice were treated in the present study, it is possible oligoapoptosis played some role in disrupted function, as seen in the NHP42,65. It is also possible that the more severe behavioral phenotypes observed with exposures at later ages than used here may reflect an influence from enhanced oligoapoptosis. Although beyond the scope of the present study, our recent qualitative analysis of oligoapoptosis in P14–15 mice16 supports the need to investigate a window of vulnerability mediating oligo- versus neuro-apoptosis throughout rodent development. Such research would advance our understanding of how age- and cell-specific apoptotic events following AASD exposure might contribute to neurobehavioral outcomes.

In conclusion, we found that exposure to the general anesthetic ISO during early neonatal ages results in substantial acute apoptotic degeneration and that multiple exposures induce subtle sex-dependent effects on behavior. More work is needed to further clarify the mechanisms underlying AASD-induced sex-specific outcomes, and the role of cell-type-specific enhanced apoptosis in disrupting behavioral circuits. Identification of specific circuit disruptions will lead to novel adjunctive strategies that may help ameliorate the potential acute cell loss and long-term functional disruptions induced by AASD exposure in the developing brain.

Methods

Animals

All experimental protocols were approved by and performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Washington University. Experimentally naïve C57BL/6 mouse pups were used from 15 litters for acute neuroapoptotic histopathology, 5 independent litters, 15 females and 20 males, for long-term behavioral analyses. The colony room lighting was 12:12 h light/dark cycle; room temperature (~20–22 °C) and relative humidity (50%) controlled automatically. Standard lab diet and water was freely available. Pregnant dams were individually housed in translucent plastic cages measuring 28.5 × 17.5 × 12 cm with corncob bedding. Upon weaning at P21, mice for behavioral testing were group housed according to sex and random distribution across treatment groups. All mice were carefully monitored throughout the experiment for weight and general appearance.

Anesthesia exposure

Age- and litter-matched mouse pups were exposed to 1.5% ISO USP (Butler Animal Health Supply, Dublin, OH) or oxygenated air only (AIR), for 3 h at P3, P5, P7 or P3+5+7 following established methods43 with normoxic and normothermic conditions maintained. The concentration and duration of ISO exposure were chosen based on previous pilot studies demonstrating 1.5% for 3 h induced an apoptotic response without high mortality or excessive weight loss, and because it is substantially below that which has been cited as the MAC for the infant mouse (2.26% ISO)66. Oxygen was flowing at 30% for 6 liters/min. Pups were placed back with dam once consciousness was regained, and the nest was monitored for restored maternal care.

Tissue processing, immunohistochemistry, and density quantification

Six h after initial exposure to ISO or AIR, pups were deeply anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (i.p.) and perfused with a heparinized saline flush followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in a sodium phosphate buffer. Immediately following perfusion, the whole brain was removed and post-fixed in perfusate for 4 days, then serially sectioned (70 µm) on a Vibratome. AC3-IHC processing and quantification of AC3 positive profiles were conducted as previously described44. Sections were quenched in methanol containing 3% hydrogen peroxide, then washed in PBS, incubated in blocking solution (2% bovine serum albumin + 0.2% dry milk in 0.1% triton-X PBS) for 60 min, and then incubated overnight in rabbit anti-AC3 antiserum (1:1000; Cell Signaling Technology, Beverly, MA). The next day, sections were washed in PBS, incubated for 60 min in goat anti-rabbit secondary antiserum (1:200; Vector Labs, Burlingame, CA), and washed in PBS. Sections were then reacted in the dark with ABC reagents (Vectastain ABC Elite Kit, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) for 60 min, washed in PBS, and the color developed with VIP reagents (Vector VIP substrate kit for peroxidase, Vector Labs, Burlingame, CA). Finally, sections were dehydrated with a series of graded ethanols and cleared in Citrisolv before coverslipping with Permount. For apoptotic quantification, Stereo Investigator 7 (MBF Bioscience, Inc., Williston, VT) on a Pentium III PC, connected to a Prior Optiscan motorized stage (ES103 XYZ system, Prior Scientific Inc., Rockland MA) mounted on a Nikon Labophot-2 microscope was used. The boundaries of the brain regions of interest were manually traced, then AC3+ profiles were marked and the area of each region was calculated with reference to a standard atlas. To estimate the total number of AC3+ profiles in each brain region, regional counts were summed and then divided by the total area counted, and a mean density score was derived for each animal.

Behavioral tests

All behavioral testing was conducted during the light cycle, by a female experimenter blinded to experimental group. All equipment was cleaned with 2% chlorhexidine diacetate or 70% ethanol between animals.

Locomotor activity

The mice were evaluated over a 1 h period in transparent enclosures (47.6 × 25.4 × 20.6 cm) surrounded by metal frames containing 4 × 8 matrices of photobeam pairs as previously described47. Computer software (MotorMonitor, Hamilton-Kinder, LLC, Poway, CA) quantified horizontal and vertical beam breaks as ambulations and rearings, respectively, in a 33 × 11 cm central zone and a bordering 5.5 cm peripheral zone. General activity variables (total ambulations, rearings, time at rest) along with measures of emotionality, including time spent, distance traveled and entries made into the central zone were analyzed.

Sensorimotor battery

Walking initiation, ledge, platform, pole, and inclined and inverted screen tests were performed as previously described67. Time in each task was manually recorded. The average for two trials was used for analyses. To avoid exhaustion effects, the order of the tests during the first set of trials was reversed for the second set of trials. Tests lasted 60 s, except for the pole test, which lasted 120 s. For walking initiation, time for an animal to leave a 21 × 21 cm square on a flat surface was recorded. For ledge and platform tests, the time the animal was able to balance on an acrylic ledge (0.75 cm wide and 30 cm high), and on a wooden platform (1.0 cm thick, 3.0 cm in diameter and elevated 47 cm) was recorded, respectively. The pole test was used to evaluate fine motor coordination by quantifying time to turn 180° and climb down a vertical pole. The screen tests assessed a combination of coordination and strength by quantifying time to climb up or hang onto a mesh wire grid measuring 16 squares per 10 cm, elevated 47 cm and inclined (60° or 90°) or inverted.

Morris water maze

MWM was conducted as previously described47. Briefly, cued, place and probe trials were conducted in a galvanized steel pool, measuring 120 cm in diameter, and filled with opaque water. The PVC escape platform measured 11.5 cm in diameter. A digital video camera connected to a PC computer and the computer software program ANY-maze (Stoelting Co., Wood Dale, IL) tracked the swimming pathway of the mouse to the escape platform and quantified path length, latency to find escape platform, and swimming speeds.

On two consecutive days, animals received four cued trials, separated by 1 h, during which a red tennis ball atop a rod was attached to the escape platform and served as a visual cue. To prevent spatial learning, the escape platform was moved to a different quadrant location for each trial. The mouse was released from the quadrant opposite to the platform location, allowed 60 s to locate the platform, and then allowed to sit on it for 30 s before being returned to its home cage. Three days later, animals received two blocks of two consecutive place trials on five consecutive days, with an intertrial interval between 30–90 s and approximately 2 h separating trial blocks. The escape platform remained in the same quadrant location for all place trials and distal cues were placed on the walls of the room to support spatial learning. The mouse was released from a different location for each trial on each day. The mouse was allowed 60 s to find the escape platform, and allowed to sit on it for 30 s before being returned to its home cage. Cued and place trials were combined into blocks of two or four trials for analyses, respectively. Immediately following completion of place trials, one 60 s probe trial was conducted to assess memory retention of the escape platform location, which was removed from the pool yet the distal cues remained.

Rotarod

For the rotarod (Economex, Columbus Instruments, Columbus, OH), animals were placed on a 9.5 cm section of grooved rod measuring 3.81 cm in diameter surrounded by plastic walls and elevated 52 cm from the floor. Animals received five trials on three test days separated by four days to minimize motor learning. The rod was stationary for trial 1 and continuously rotating at 2.5 rpm for the trials 2 and 3 for 60 s. The rod accelerated by 0.13 rpm per second for trials 4 and 5 for 180 s. Time the animal remained on the rod was used for analyses.

Elevated plus maze

EPM was conducted as previously described47. Briefly, a 5 min trial was conducted on each of 3 consecutive days. Animals were placed on a dimly lit black acrylic surface measuring 5 × 5 cm and elevated 63 cm above the floor equipped with photo beam pairs. Four arms (35 cm long, 5 cm wide; two open and two with 15 cm high walls) extended from a central area. The MotorMonitor software (Kinder Scientific, LLC, Poway, CA) quantified beam breaks as duration, distance traveled, entries, and time at rest in each zone (closed arms, open arms and center area).

Social approach

As previously described47, social approach apparatus was evaluated in a rectangular acrylic box divided into three separate chambers each measuring 19.5 × 39 × 22 cm including dividing walls with 5 × 8 cm doorways to allow for movement between chambers. Each outer chamber contained a stainless steel conspecific cage (inverted Galaxy Pencil Cup, Spectrum Diversified Designs, Inc, Streetsboro, OH) with vertical bars to allow minimal contact, measuring 10 cm in base diameter. A digital video camera and ANY-maze software (Stoelting Co., Wood Dale, IL.) tracked behavior of the mouse and time spent in 2 cm investigation zone surrounding the conspecific cages. Four consecutive 10 min trials were conducted. The first two trials served as apparatus habituation (trial 1, middle chamber only; trial 2, all three chambers and empty conspecific cages). During trial 3 (sociability), a sex- and age-matched conspecific was placed in one cage with the other empty. During trial 4 (social novelty preference), the now familiar conspecific remained, and an unfamiliar sex- and age-matched conspecific was placed in the other cage. Placement of conspecifics was counterbalanced between exposure groups.

Fear conditioning

All mice were evaluated for fear conditioning as described previously52. Briefly, animals were habituated to and tested in acrylic chambers, measuring 26 × 18 × 18 cm with a metal grid floor, an LED light bulb and an inaccessible peppermint odorant. Day 1 testing lasted 5 min, and an 80 dB tone sounded for 20 s at 100 s, 160 s and 220 s, paired with a 1.0 mA shock during the last 2 s of the tone. The freezing behavior was quantified through the computerized image analysis software program FreezeFrame (Actimetrics, Evanston, IL). Freezing was defined as no movement except for normal respiration. Day 2 lasted 8 min, and the light was illuminated yet no tones or shocks were presented to evaluate freezing behavior in response to the contextual cues associated with the shock from day 1. Day 3 lasted 10 min and the context of the chamber was changed to an opaque acrylic, walled chamber containing a coconut odorant. Baseline freezing behavior to the new context was quantified during the first 2 min. The 80 dB tone sounded 120–600 s. Freezing behavior to the auditory cue associated with the shock stimulus from day 1 was quantified. Shock sensitivity was evaluated following testing as previously described52.

Statistical Analyses

All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistic software (v.24). Prior to analyses, all data were screened for fit of distributions with assumptions underlying univariate analyses, which included the Shapiro-Wilk test and qqplot investigations for normality. Means and standard errors were computed for each measure. Analysis of variance (ANOVA), including repeated measures, were used to analyze the histological and behavioral data. For social approach data, a priori planned comparisons were used. With a statistically significant interaction between main factors, simple main effects were calculated to provide clarification of statistically significant between-treatment and within-treatment differences. Where appropriate, the Huynh-Feldt adjustment was used to protect against violations of sphericity, the Bonferroni correction was applied to multiple pairwise comparisons, and Tukey’s HSD method was used as the post hoc test. Statistical results were confirmed with two-tailed non-parametric testing, when available, for any datasets with violations of the univariate assumptions. As the ANOVA is robust to violations of normality68, we are confident in our results and feel the ANOVA provides a more sensitive and powerful statistical test. However, we included non-parametric test results in the statistical tables where appropriate. The critical alpha value for all analyses was p < 0.05, unless otherwise stated. Test statistics and other details for each analysis are provided in Tables 1, 3, S1 and S2.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information (181.6KB, pdf)

Acknowledgements

We thank the late Dr. John Olney for his support and mentorship during this project. Support for this work was provided by the following grants: NIH grants HD052664 (K.N.N.), HD052664 (K.N.N.), HD037100 (JWO). Partial support for this work was provided by the Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center at Washington University (NIH/NICHD U54 HD087011; D.F.W., K.K.N.).

Author Contributions

S.E.M., C.E.C., G.T.T. and D.F.W. designed research; S.E.M., C.M.Y., C.E.C., S.W. and J.H. performed research; S.E.M. and D.F.W. analyzed data; S.E.M., J.H., K.N.N. and D.F.W. wrote the manuscript.

Data Availability

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-019-39174-6.

References

  • 1.FDA Safety Communication. FDA Drug Safety Communication: FDA review results in new warnings about using general anesthetics and sedation drugs in young children and pregnant women. https://www.fda.gov/Drugs/DrugSafety/ucm532356.htm (2016).
  • 2.FDA Safety Communication. FDA Drug Safety Communication: FDA approves label changes for use of general anesthetic and sedation drugs in young children. https://www.fda.gov/Drugs/DrugSafety/ucm554634.htm (2017).
  • 3.DiMaggio C, Sun LS, Ing C, Li G. Pediatric anesthesia and neurodevelopmental impairments: a Bayesian meta-analysis. J Neurosurg Anesthesiol. 2012;24:376–381. doi: 10.1097/ANA.0b013e31826a038d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Davidson AJ, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome at 2 years of age after general anaesthesia and awake-regional anaesthesia in infancy (GAS): an international multicentre, randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2016;387:239–250. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00608-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ing C, et al. Duration of general anaesthetic exposure in early childhood and long-term language and cognitive ability. Br J Anaesth. 2017;119:532–540. doi: 10.1093/bja/aew413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Glatz P, et al. Association of Anesthesia and Surgery During Childhood With Long-term Academic Performance. JAMA Pediatr. 2017;171:e163470–e163470. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.3470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ing C, et al. Age at Exposure to Surgery and Anesthesia in Children and Association With Mental Disorder Diagnosis. Anesth. Analg. 2017;125:1988–1998. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000002423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wilder RT, et al. Early exposure to anesthesia and learning disabilities in a population-based birth cohort. Anesthesiology. 2009;110:796–804. doi: 10.1097/01.anes.0000344728.34332.5d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Flick RP, et al. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes after early exposure to anesthesia and surgery. Pediatrics. 2011;128:e1053–61. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-0351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Sun LS, et al. Association Between a Single General Anesthesia Exposure Before Age 36 Months and Neurocognitive Outcomes in Later Childhood. JAMA. 2016;315:2312–2320. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.6967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sprung J, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder after early exposure to procedures requiring general anesthesia. Mayo Clin. Proc. 2012;87:120–129. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2011.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ikonomidou C, et al. Blockade of NMDA Receptors and Apoptotic Neurodegeneration in the Developing Brain. Science. 1999;283:70–74. doi: 10.1126/science.283.5398.70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mintz CD, Barrett KMS, Smith SC, Benson DL, Harrison NL. Anesthetics interfere with axon guidance in developing mouse neocortical neurons in vitro via a γ-aminobutyric acid type A receptor mechanism. Anesthesiology. 2013;118:825–833. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318287b850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Obradovic AL, et al. Early Exposure to Ketamine Impairs Axonal Pruning in Developing Mouse Hippocampus. Mol. Neurobiol. 2018;55:164–172. doi: 10.1007/s12035-017-0730-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Olney JW, Young C, Wozniak DF, Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Ikonomidou C. Do pediatric drugs cause developing neurons to commit suicide? Trends in pharmacological sciences. 2004;25:135–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2004.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Maloney, S. E. et al. Using animal models to evaluate the functional consequences of anesthesia during early neurodevelopment. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.03.014 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 17.Dikranian K, Qin YQ, Labruyere J, Nemmers B, Olney JW. Ethanol-induced neuroapoptosis in the developing rodent cerebellum and related brain stem structures. Brain research. Developmental brain research. 2005;155:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.devbrainres.2004.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Dobbing J, Sands J. Comparative aspects of the brain growth spurt. Early human development. 1979;3:79–83. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(79)90022-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.West JR. Fetal alcohol-induced brain damage and the problem of determining temporal vulnerability: a review. Alcohol and drug research. 1987;7:423–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Satomoto M, et al. Neonatal exposure to sevoflurane induces abnormal social behaviors and deficits in fear conditioning in mice. Anesthesiology. 2009;110:628–37. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181974fa2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Zhu C, et al. Isoflurane anesthesia induced persistent, progressive memory impairment, caused a loss of neural stem cells, and reduced neurogenesis in young, but not adult, rodents. Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism: official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism. 2010;30:1017–30. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2009.274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Huang L, Liu Y, Jin W, Ji X, Dong Z. Ketamine potentiates hippocampal neurodegeneration and persistent learning and memory impairment through the PKCγ–ERK signaling pathway in the developing brain. Brain Research. 2012;1476:164–171. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.07.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Murphy, K. L. & Baxter, M. G. Long-Term Effects of Neonatal Single or Multiple Isoflurane Exposures on Spatial Memory in Rats. Front. Neurol. 4 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 24.Yu D, Jiang Y, Gao J, Liu B, Chen P. Repeated exposure to propofol potentiates neuroapoptosis and long-term behavioral deficits in neonatal rats. Neuroscience Letters. 2013;534:41–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.12.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gao J, Peng S, Xiang S, Huang J, Chen P. Repeated exposure to propofol impairs spatial learning, inhibits LTP and reduces CaMKIIα in young rats. Neuroscience Letters. 2014;560:62–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.11.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gonzales ELT, et al. Repeated Neonatal Propofol Administration Induces Sex-Dependent Long-Term Impairments on Spatial and Recognition Memory in Rats. Biomol Ther (Seoul) 2015;23:251–260. doi: 10.4062/biomolther.2014.120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Jeevakumar V, et al. Ketamine administration during the second postnatal week induces enduring schizophrenia-like behavioral symptoms and reduces parvalbumin expression in the medial prefrontal cortex of adult mice. Behavioural Brain Research. 2015;282:165–175. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Rosenholm M, Paro E, Antila H, Võikar V, Rantamäki T. Repeated brief isoflurane anesthesia during early postnatal development produces negligible changes on adult behavior in male mice. PLOS ONE. 2017;12:e0175258. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jevtovic-Todorovic V, et al. Early exposure to common anesthetic agents causes widespread neurodegeneration in the developing rat brain and persistent learning deficits. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2003;23:876–82. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-03-00876.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Stratmann G, et al. Isoflurane differentially affects neurogenesis and long-term neurocognitive function in 60-day-old and 7-day-old rats. Anesthesiology. 2009;110:834–48. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e31819c463d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Boscolo A, et al. The abolishment of anesthesia-induced cognitive impairment by timely protection of mitochondria in the developing rat brain: The importance of free oxygen radicals and mitochondrial integrity. Neurobiology of Disease. 2012;45:1031–1041. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.12.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Boscolo A, Ori C, Bennett J, Wiltgen B, Jevtovic-Todorovic V. Mitochondrial protectant pramipexole prevents sex-specific long-term cognitive impairment from early anaesthesia exposure in rats. Br J Anaesth. 2013;110:i47–i52. doi: 10.1093/bja/aet073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Shu Y, et al. Nociceptive stimuli enhance anesthetic-induced neuroapoptosis in the rat developing brain. Neurobiology of Disease. 2012;45:743–750. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Man Y-G, Zhou R-G, Zhao B. Efficacy of rutin in inhibiting neuronal apoptosis and cognitive disturbances in sevoflurane or propofol exposed neonatal mice. Int J Clin Exp Med. 2015;8:14397–14409. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Xiao H, Liu B, Chen Y, Zhang J. Learning, memory and synaptic plasticity in hippocampus in rats exposed to sevoflurane. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 2016;48:38–49. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2015.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Loepke AW, et al. The effects of neonatal isoflurane exposure in mice on brain cell viability, adult behavior, learning, and memory. Anesthesia and analgesia. 2009;108:90–104. doi: 10.1213/ane.0b013e31818cdb29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Liang G, et al. Isoflurane causes greater neurodegeneration than an equivalent exposure of sevoflurane in the developing brain of neonatal mice. Anesthesiology. 2010;112:1325–34. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181d94da5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Zhou Z-B, et al. Subclinical concentrations of sevoflurane reduce oxidative stress but do not prevent hippocampal apoptosis. Molecular Medicine Reports. 2016;14:721–727. doi: 10.3892/mmr.2016.5336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Paule MG, et al. Ketamine anesthesia during the first week of life can cause long-lasting cognitive deficits in rhesus monkeys. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33:220–230. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Raper J, Alvarado MC, Murphy KL, Baxter MG. Multiple anesthetic exposure in infant monkeys alters emotional reactivity to an acute stressor. Anesthesiology. 2015;123:1084–1092. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Raper J, Bush A, Murphy KL, Baxter MG, Alvarado MC. Multiple sevoflurane exposures in infant monkeys do not impact the mother-infant bond. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2016;54:46–51. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2016.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Coleman K, et al. Isoflurane Anesthesia Has Long-term Consequences on Motor and Behavioral Development in Infant Rhesus Macaques. Anesthes. 2017;126:74–84. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000001383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Johnson SA, Young C, Olney JW. Isoflurane-induced neuroapoptosis in the developing brain of nonhypoglycemic mice. Journal of neurosurgical anesthesiology. 2008;20:21–8. doi: 10.1097/ANA.0b013e3181271850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Young C, Straiko MM, Johnson SA, Creeley CE, Olney JW. Ethanol causes and lithium prevents neuroapoptosis and suppression of pERK in the infant mouse brain. Neurobiology of disease. 2008;31:355–60. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2008.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kang E, et al. Neurogenesis and Developmental Anesthetic Neurotoxicity. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2017;60:33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2016.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Lee BH, Chan JT, Kraeva E, Peterson K, Sall JW. Isoflurane exposure in newborn rats induces long-term cognitive dysfunction in males but not females. Neuropharmacology. 2014;83:9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.03.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Dougherty JD, et al. The disruption of Celf6, a gene identified by translational profiling of serotonergic neurons, results in autism-related behaviors. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:2732–53. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4762-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kang E, et al. Early postnatal exposure to isoflurane causes cognitive deficits and disrupts development of newborn hippocampal neurons via activation of the mTOR pathway. PLOS Biology. 2017;15:e2001246. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2001246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Ramage TM, et al. Distinct long-term neurocognitive outcomes after equipotent sevoflurane or isoflurane anaesthesia in immature rats. Br J Anaesth. 2013;110:i39–i46. doi: 10.1093/bja/aet103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Kodama M, et al. Neonatal desflurane exposure induces more robust neuroapoptosis than do isoflurane and sevoflurane and impairs working memory. Anesthesiology. 2011;115:979–91. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318234228b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Rothstein S, Simkins T, Nunez JL. Response to neonatal anesthesia: effect of sex on anatomical and behavioral outcome. Neuroscience. 2008;152:959–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.01.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wozniak DF, Xiao M, Xu L, Yamada KA, Ornitz DM. Impaired spatial learning and defective theta burst induced LTP in mice lacking fibroblast growth factor 14. Neurobiology of disease. 2007;26:14–26. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2006.11.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lazar NL, Neufeld RW, Cain DP. Contribution of nonprimate animal models in understanding the etiology of schizophrenia. Journal of psychiatry & neuroscience: JPN. 2011;36:E5–29. doi: 10.1503/jpn.100054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Moy SS, et al. Sociability and preference for social novelty in five inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like behavior in mice. Genes, brain, and behavior. 2004;3:287–302. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-1848.2004.00076.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Naert A, Callaerts-Vegh Z, D’Hooge R. Nocturnal hyperactivity, increased social novelty preference and delayed extinction of fear responses in post-weaning socially isolated mice. Brain research bulletin. 2011;85:354–62. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Silverman JL, Yang M, Lord C, Crawley JN. Behavioural phenotyping assays for mouse models of autism. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. 2010;11:490–502. doi: 10.1038/nrn2851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Wozniak DF, et al. Apoptotic neurodegeneration induced by ethanol in neonatal mice is associated with profound learning/memory deficits in juveniles followed by progressive functional recovery in adults. Neurobiology of disease. 2004;17:403–14. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2004.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Yuede CM, et al. Behavioral consequences of NMDA antagonist-induced neuroapoptosis in the infant mouse brain. PloS one. 2010;5:e11374. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Luchetti A, et al. Early handling and repeated cross-fostering have opposite effect on mouse emotionality. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9:93. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Flanigan TJ, Cook MN. Effects of an Early Handling-Like Procedure and Individual Housing on Anxiety-Like Behavior in Adult C57BL/6J and DBA/2J Mice. PLOS ONE. 2011;6:e19058. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Ikonomidou C, Stefovska V, Turski L. Neuronal death enhanced by N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonists. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2000;97:12885–90. doi: 10.1073/pnas.220412197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Creeley CE, et al. Isoflurane-induced apoptosis of neurons and oligodendrocytes in the fetal rhesus macaque brain. Anesthesiology. 2014;120:626–638. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Brambrink AM, et al. Isoflurane-induced neuroapoptosis in the neonatal rhesus macaque brain. Anesthesiology. 2010;112:834–41. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e3181d049cd. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Brambrink AM, et al. Isoflurane-induced apoptosis of oligodendrocytes in the neonatal primate brain. Ann. Neurol. 2012;72:525–535. doi: 10.1002/ana.23652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Schenning KJ, et al. Isoflurane exposure leads to apoptosis of neurons and oligodendrocytes in 20- and 40-day old rhesus macaques. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2017;60:63–68. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2016.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Loepke AW, McCann JC, Kurth CD, McAuliffe JJ. The physiologic effects of isoflurane anesthesia in neonatal mice. Anesthesia and analgesia. 2006;102:75–80. doi: 10.1213/01.ANE.0000181102.92729.B8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Maloney, S. E. et al. Examining the Reversibility of Long-Term Behavioral Disruptions in Progeny of Maternal SSRI Exposure. eNeuro ENEURO.0120-18.2018 10.1523/ENEURO.0120-18.2018 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 68.Blanca, M. J., Alarcón, R. & Arnau, J. Non-normal data: Is ANOVA still a valid option? Psicothema 552–557 10.7334/psicothema2016.383 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information (181.6KB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES