Abstract
Objectives
Our aim was to identify message characteristics for cigarette pack inserts that aim to help smokers quit.
Methods
US adult smokers from an online consumer panel (N = 665) participated in a discrete choice experiment with a 2x2x2x2x4 within-subjects balanced incomplete block design, manipulating: image (vs no image), text type (testimonial vs informational), cessation resource information (vs none), call to action (vs none), and message topic (well-being, financial benefit, cravings, social support). Participants evaluated 9 choice sets, each with 4 inserts, selecting: (1) the most and least helpful for quitting; and (2) the most and least motivating to quit. Linear models regressed choices on insert characteristics, controlling for sociodemographics and smoking-related variables. We assessed interactions between insert characteristics and smoker attributes (ie, education, quit intention, self-efficacy).
Results
Inserts were most helpful and motivating when they included an image, provided cessation resource information, or referenced well-being and financial benefits. Significant interactions indicated that inserts with cessation resource information were relatively more helpful and motivating among smokers with low self-efficacy, an intention to quit, or lower education.
Conclusion
Cigarette pack inserts with imagery and cessation resource information may be particularly effective in promoting smoking cessation.
Keywords: tobacco control, health communication, smoking cessation, health policy
In 2000, Canada became the first country to mandate cigarette pack inserts that complemented the pictorial health warnings required on the outside of the packs by elaborating on the risks associated with smoking or providing cessation resource information. Numerous studies have assessed the effects of pictorial warnings1,2 and over 100 countries require them on cigarette packs.3 Few studies have explored health-directed cigarette pack inserts, however,4–6 and Canada remains the only country in which they are required. In 2012, Canada amended its insert requirement, adding color graphics and cessation efficacy messages (www.tobaccolabels.ca/countries/canada). This approach was supported by theory-based research suggesting the importance of complementing fear-arousing messages, such as those that portray warnings about smoking-related harms, and that contain efficacy messages about the benefits of quitting (response efficacy) that increase confidence to quit (self-efficacy).7,8 Consistent with these expectations, observational studies of Canadian smokers have found that more frequent reading of inserts was positively associated with stronger subsequent self-efficacy to quit and more downstream quit attempts, including attempts sustained for at least a month.4,5 Little is known about which insert characteristics are most helpful and motivating for smokers to quit, although the Canadian inserts suggest several characteristics to evaluate: cessation message topics, imagery, type of text, and calls to action.
Cessation Message Topics
Some research suggests that health messages are most effective if they promote both response efficacy (ie, benefits of quitting) and self-efficacy (ie, confidence that one can successfully quit).8,9 An observational study of Canadian smokers found that those who read inserts that included both types of efficacy messages were more likely than those who did not read them to increase their self-efficacy to quit and to engage in quit attempts that lasted 30 days or more.4 The researchers were unable to compare the effectiveness of reading inserts with both messages compared to reading inserts with only one or the other; however, an experimental study of cessation advertisements10 found that those that provided a rationale for quitting (response efficacy) were perceived as more motivating and more likely to encourage quit attempts than those that provided advice for quitting (self-efficacy). There is also evidence that the association between reading inserts and sustained cessation attempts is at least partly independent of individuals’ response efficacy or self-efficacy.5 A goal of our study was to provide further insight into the relationship between types of efficacy message and their impact on recipients.
Text-only versus Text with Images
Cigarette warning research provides abundant evidence for the superior effectiveness of warnings with images compared to text-only warnings in experimental studies,2,11,20 observational studies,21–29 and randomized trials.14,30,31 Warnings with imagery appear to have greater impact among more disadvantaged smokers,12 which may be because pictures can communicate more effectively than only text with low-literacy audiences.19,32 However, studies of pictorial warnings have focused primarily on images illustrating the harmful effects of smoking, with considerably less research exploring imagery that could improve messages about the benefits of cessation and strategies for quitting. Some studies have found that messages about cessation are generally perceived as less effective than those about smoking-related risks, perhaps because evocative, graphic imagery does not “fit” with cessation messages.18,33 In our study we explore the extent to which cessation-specific images can enhance the effectiveness of cessation messages.
Testimonial versus Informative Texts
Messages that build a connection with smokers by presenting personal testimonials from smokers who have quit might be more effective at supporting or prompting cessation than those that merely convey factual information, as warnings commonly do. Testimonials can facilitate information processing, offer social connections, and overcome resistance. 34 Indeed, testimonials have been found to aid information processing among smokers with low education35,36 and engage smokers with high self-efficacy37 or intentions to quit.38 However, research is mixed regarding the effectiveness of testimonials versus didactic messages that convey factual information about the risks of smoking through pictorial warnings.18,33,36 Our study explores the relative effectiveness of testimonials versus didactic messages in the context of pack inserts.
Cessation Resources
Pictorial warnings in at least 30 countries include information about cessation resources, such as a quitline number and/or a cessation website. By going beyond providing information about smoking-related risks, these warnings not only serve as cues for thinking about quitting, but also offer resources for help with making the next steps towards cessation. Previous studies have found that provision of cessation resources in warnings can be effective in informing smokers about their existence4,39 and promoting their use.40–42 However, cessation-focused warnings can be perceived as less effective than warnings that highlight smoking harms.18,33,43 Nevertheless, cessation resource information is likely to be particularly relevant for smokers who intend to quit (ie, due to stage of change), with relatively lower self-efficacy (ie, due potential need for assistance), and who have lower educational attainment (eg, due to relatively lower access to cessation resources). Research is needed to explore whether the inclusion of cessation resource information can enhance the effectiveness of cessation messages, and perhaps, even increase the relatively low utilization rates for some cessation assistance resources.44
Calls to Action
Warnings in some countries include a “call to action” message that exhorts smokers to engage in a recommended behavior (eg, “Call Quitline” in Australia). Consistent with action identification theory (or construal level theory), a call to action is a concrete, low construal message (as opposed to an abstract, high construal message), which promotes an immediate response.45 Calls to action are regularly recommended as an accompaniment and closure to public health messages and are common in anti-smoking media campaigns.46,47 However, it appears that no scientific studies have systematically manipulated calls to action to assess their effectiveness, as our study aims to do.
Study Aims
Our study aimed to assess the effects of different insert characteristics on smokers’ selections of inserts as helpful or motivating for smoking cessation. The hypotheses, based on the preceding literature review, were: (1) smokers will select inserts with response efficacy messages as more motivating to quit but less helpful than self-efficacy messages; (2) smokers will select inserts with images as more motivating and helpful than those without images, with stronger effects for smokers with relatively lower educational attainment; (3) smokers will select inserts with testimonial messages as more motivating and helpful than those with informative texts, with stronger effects for smokers who intend to quit, have high self-efficacy, or relatively lower educational attainment; (4) smokers will select inserts with cessation resource information as more motivating and helpful than inserts without such information, with stronger effects for smokers who intend to quit, have relatively lower self-efficacy, and with lower educational attainment; and (5) smokers will select inserts with a call to action as more motivating and helpful than those without a call to action.
To test these hypotheses, discrete choice experiments (DCEs) were used. DCEs use fractional factorial designs to determine the independent effects of multiple stimulus characteristics on choice behavior, and are widely used in marketing research about decision making.48 The tobacco industry uses DCEs in premarket research,49–51 and tobacco industry experts in tobacco lawsuits claim that DCEs are less biased than other methods used to study tobacco packaging and labeling.52 By showing participants multiple stimuli and asking them to pick their top choice, DCEs may better represent consumer choices in natural settings.53 Previous studies using DCE methods have assessed variations in characteristics of cigarette pack design, health warnings, and cigarette sticks,54,55 brand variants on cigarette packs,56 and strategies for communicating harmful cigarette chemicals through package inserts.57 We applied this approach to evaluate different characteristics on inserts that contain efficacy messages.
METHODS
Sample
We recruited adult smokers 18 to 50 years old in the United States (US) using participants provided by Lightspeed GMI (www.lightspeedresearch.com), a commercial provider of online panels for conducting consumer research. Eligible participants had smoked at least 100 cigarettes in their lifetime and smoked at least once in the past 30 days, comparable to smoker definitions used for the US National Health Interview Survey.58 Data were collected between November 4 and November 12, 2016. Invitation emails were sent to panel members, which included a link to the online survey. Lightspeed GMI provided standard compensation to panelists for participation in this type of study (ie, baseline range = $0.30 – $0.75; follow-up = $0.50 – $1.00). Sample targets were established to recruit a minimum of 600 participants, 50% of whom intended to quit smoking within the next 6 months, and 50% of whom did not. In DCEs, sample sizes of around 400 typically have adequate power for hypothesis testing around meaningful stimulus attributes, which is consistent with power analyses based in multinomial logit models.59 This sample size target allowed for expected elimination of observations from the analytic sample of those participants who did not indicate preferences for any inserts. However, the study protocol also required individuals to make choices across multiple-choice sets, increasing statistical power to reject the null. This increase in statistical power was necessary for assessing interactions, where the sample size was less than 400 when running analyses that were stratified along key variables (ie, intention to quit, self-efficacy, education).
Study Design and Protocol
The experiment used a 2x2x2x2x4 within-subjects balanced incomplete block design, which systematically manipulated the following insert characteristics: image provision (vs no image), text type (testimonial vs informative), cessation resource information (vs no information about cessation resources), call to action (vs no call to action), and 4 message topics, including 2 response efficacy messages (benefits of cessation for wellbeing; financial benefits of quitting) and 2 self-efficacy messages (how to deal with cravings; capitalizing on social support when quitting). By testing multiple messages on different topics, the study aimed to produce more insightful and generalizable of findings than those produced through single-message evaluations, as recommended for media effects research.60
A team of researchers, health communication experts, and a graphic designer developed the stimuli to be tested, including the selection of texts and images. Text selection was based on a content analysis of efficacy messages in smoking cessation materials (including Canadian inserts), a qualitative pre-test with 20 adult smokers, and a quantitative pre-test with 300 adult smokers. Images were selected from different image databases to match each text. For testimonial inserts, images showed people who could credibly give the testimonial. For example, a testimonial about social support for quitting used an image of 2 women close together (Figure 1). The text of the testimonial was made as similar as possible in meaning to the didactic version, put into quotes, and attributed the portrayed individual who was given a sex-matched first name. The cessation resource information included a quit line number (1-800-784-8669) and a smoking cessation website URL (http://smokefree.gov). The call to action message was a modification of the informational message (eg, “if you want help to quit, call 1-800-784-8669”) that encouraged smokers to engage in the recommended behavior immediately (eg, “Quit Now! Call 1-800-QUIT-NOW”). For these messages, the statement on the banner of the insert was also changed from a factual statement to an exhortatory message (eg, “Quitting is easier with active support” vs “Get support to quit now!”).
The full factorial design generated 64 possible combinations of insert characteristics; however, to optimize the design, a balanced incomplete block design was used. As we did not have any a priori hypotheses around interactions between insert characteristics, the design was not set up to assess such interactions. This allowed for 16 distinct insert profiles to be used, with each one appearing multiple times in the context of 24 “choice sets” that included 4 different inserts. To reduce response burden, each participant was randomized to evaluate one of 4 blocks that included 9 choice sets, 5 of which were unique to a particular block, and 4 of which were common across all blocks. Within any block, all attribute levels appeared multiple times. The 4 common sets allowed evaluation of possible differences in choices across groups assigned to each of the 4 blocks. To evaluate this, the unique choice sets (training dataset) were cross-validated using data from the common choice sets as the validation dataset (results not shown). Cross-validation showed no significant differences in choices across groups assigned to different blocks.
Measures
Dependent variables
“Best-worst” scaling was used, in which participants were asked to select messages preferred most and least (rather than selecting just one option), which increases the precision of estimates and statistical power.61 For each choice set, participants were presented with the 4 images of inserts (Figure 1) and asked: “Which insert would be MOST helpful and which would be LEAST helpful for you if you decided to quit smoking?” after which the participant was able to choose one insert as “most helpful” and one as “least helpful,” with only mutually exclusive options allowed. After selecting these, participants were asked: “Thinking about these inserts, do you actually think that: (a) None would be helpful if you decided to quit, or (b) At least one would be helpful if you decided to quit.” Then, participants were asked: “Which insert would MOST motivate you and LEAST motivate you to quit smoking?” and asked to indicate which was most and which was least motivating. Afterwards, participants were asked: “Do you actually think that: (a) None would be motivating if you decided to quit, or (b) At least one would be motivating if you decided to quit.” Participants could view each choice set for as long as they wished. For each choice set, the insert selected as most helpful/motivating to quit smoking was given a value of 1, the least helpful/motivating to quit was given a value of -1. The remaining inserts in that set were given a value of 0. If the participant indicated that none would be helpful/ motivating, all inserts in that choice set were assigned a value of 0.
Independent variables
Insert characteristics were effects coded such that coefficients reflected deviations of the group from the grand mean. Participant characteristics were assessed with a self-administered survey. Sociodemographics included age group in years (18–29, 30–39, 40–50), sex (men/ women), race (white, African-American, other), and education (some university or less, completed university or higher). Smoking-related variables included frequency (every day, some days), nicotine dependence, self-efficacy to quit (low, high), intention to quit in the next 6 months (yes/no), and at least one quit attempt in the past 4 months (yes/no). Nicotine dependence was quantified using the heaviness of smoking index (HSI), based on the reported number of cigarettes smoked per day and time to first cigarette.62 Self-efficacy to quit was measured by asking: “If you decided to give up smoking completely in the next 6 months, how sure are you that you would succeed?” with responses on a 9 point scale ranging from “not at all” to “extremely.”63 Due to the skewed distribution of the responses to this variable, a median split was used to dichotomize the variable into low (ie, 1–6), and high (ie, 7–9).64–66
Data Analysis
For each outcome (ie, helpful to quit, motivating to quit), participants indicating that none of the inserts would be helpful/motivating (ie, “no-difference” option) for all 9 choice sets were excluded from the primary analysis as they did not contribute meaningful information for assessing the relative effectiveness of different insert characteristics. Those who selected at least one insert as more or less helpful/motivating were included in the analysis. For choice sets where participants indicated “no difference” (see measurement, above), responses were recoded to “0.” Pearson’s chi-square tests were used to compare the demographic and smoking-related characteristics of excluded participants with the analytic sample. To assess the impact of each insert characteristic on choice, a utility range (the difference between each characteristic’s highest and lowest estimated part-worth utility) was calculated. Part-worth utility refers to the relative contribution of each level of an insert characteristic on choice (ie, as approximated by the estimated coefficients in the model). The relative importance of each characteristic on making a choice was then calculated as the utility range for each characteristic, divided by the sum of all the characteristics’ utility ranges for a given outcome.
As recommended for the best-worst scaling approach for DCE, we analyzed data using mixed linear regression to control for repeated measures.61 Dependent variables were the selection of an insert as motivating to quit and helpful to quit, with each assessed in separate models. Independent variables included insert characteristics, controlling for block, sociodemographics, and smoking-related participant characteristics. Where indicated by related research (see introduction), we also tested for interactions between insert characteristics and key participant characteristics (ie, quit intention, self-efficacy, and education). Interaction terms were entered into the full model (described above) one at a time, after which they were removed, another interaction term included and the model re-estimated. Because models were re-estimated 10 times for each outcome, we used a Bonferroni correction procedure to determine statistical significance (ie, p < .05/10 = .005). Only significant interactions are reported. When a statistically significant result was found, data were stratified by the participant characteristic and models re-estimated for each group. All data analyses were conducted using Stata v13.1.67
RESULTS
Sample Characteristics
Of those completing the study (N = 665), approximately one-fifth selected the no-difference option for all choice sets when evaluating helpfulness for quitting (20%, N = 136) and when evaluating motivation to quit (21%, N = 141). These participants were excluded from the respective analytic samples for evaluating the relative effectiveness of the different insert characteristics. Compared to those retained in the analyses (Table 1), participants who were excluded from either analytic sample were more likely to: be older than 40; be women; have lower education; have low self-efficacy; have no quit intention; and have no recent quit attempt. Most participants in the analytic samples were women (57%), white (79%), and smoked every day in the past 30 days (86%). A high proportion of the sample was 30–39 years old (44%) and completed university or higher (46%).
Table 1.
Characteristics | Total (N = 665) | Helpful for Quitting | p - value | Motivating to Quit | p - value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Opted outa (N = 136) | Analytic (N = 529) | Opted outa (N = 141) | Analytic (N = 524) | ||||
Age (in Years) | .002 | < .001 | |||||
| |||||||
18–29 | 20% | 18% | 20% | 16% | 20% | ||
30–39 | 44% | 33% | 47% | 33% | 47% | ||
40–50 | 36% | 49% | 33% | 50% | 33% | ||
| |||||||
Sex | .021 | .013 | |||||
| |||||||
Men | 40% | 32% | 43% | 31% | 43% | ||
Women | 60% | 68% | 57% | 69% | 57% | ||
| |||||||
Race | .629 | .493 | |||||
| |||||||
White | 79% | 82% | 79% | 82% | 79% | ||
African American | 8% | 7% | 8% | 6% | 9% | ||
Other | 13% | 11% | 13% | 12% | 13% | ||
| |||||||
Education | < .001 | < .001 | |||||
| |||||||
Less than high school | 3% | 7% | 3% | 6% | 3% | ||
Completed high school | 20% | 25% | 19% | 26% | 19% | ||
Some university or college | 30% | 42% | 27% | 40% | 27% | ||
Completed university or higher | 46% | 26% | 52% | 28% | 51% | ||
| |||||||
Smoking Frequency | .624 | .403 | |||||
| |||||||
Every day | 86% | 85% | 86% | 84% | 86% | ||
Some days | 14% | 15% | 14% | 16% | 14% | ||
| |||||||
HSI [mean (SD)] | 2.81 (1.32) | 2.89 (1.23) | 2.79 (1.35) | .788 | 2.91 (1.21) | 2.79 (1.35) | .824 |
Self-Efficacy | .003 | .006 | |||||
| |||||||
Low | 53% | 66% | 51% | 65% | 52% | ||
High | 45% | 34% | 49% | 35% | 48% | ||
| |||||||
Quit Intention in Next 6 Months | .011 | .008 | |||||
| |||||||
Yes | 55% | 41% | 57% | 41% | 57% | ||
No | 45% | 59% | 43% | 59% | 43% | ||
| |||||||
Quit Attempt in Past 4 Months | < .001 | < .001 | |||||
| |||||||
Yes | 51% | 28% | 56% | 28% | 56% | ||
No | 49% | 72% | 44% | 72% | 44% |
Note.
HSI – Heaviness of Smoking Index
Opted out indicates that the participant did not choose any options from any of the choice sets in the experiment; all p-values were calculated using chi-square tests for all variables except HSI which was calculated using a t-test.
Relative Impact of Insert Characteristics on Choices
For both outcomes (ie, perceptions of helpfulness and motivation to quit), message topic (41% and 43%, respectively), provision of image (36% and 31%, respectively), and inclusion of cessation resource information (22% and 22%, respectively) were the most influential insert characteristics on choices (Figure 2). Text type (0% and 3%) and the inclusion of a call to action (1% and 2%) were substantially less influential.
Effects of Message Topic on Choices
The insert message topics on craving and social support (self-efficacy) were evaluated as less helpful and motivating, as their coefficients were both significantly less than 0 (Table 2). The messages on financial benefits and well-being (response efficacy) were evaluated as relatively more helpful and motivating, as their coefficients were significantly greater than 0.
Table 2.
Helpful (N = 529) | Motivating (N = 524) | |
---|---|---|
Insert’s Characteristics | Coef. [95% CI] | Coef. [95% CI] |
Image | ||
| ||
Not present | −0.11 [−0.12, −0.10]*** | −0.10 [−0.11, −0.09]*** |
Present | 0.11 [0.10, 0.12]*** | 0.10 [0.09, 0.11]*** |
| ||
Text Type | ||
| ||
Informative | 0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] | 0.01 [−0.00,0.02] |
Testimonial | 0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] | −0.01 [−0.02, 0.00] |
| ||
Cessation Information | ||
| ||
Not present | −0.07 [−0.08, −0.06]*** | −0.07 [−0.08, −0.06]*** |
Present | 0.07 [0.06, 0.08]*** | 0.07 [0.06, 0.08]*** |
| ||
Call to Action | ||
| ||
Not present | 0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] | 0.00 [−0.01, 0.00] |
Present | 0.00 [−0.01, 0.01] | 0.00 [−0.00, 0.01] |
| ||
Message Topic | ||
| ||
Well-being | 0.07 [0.05, 0.09]*** | 0.07 [0.05, 0.08]*** |
Financial | 0.14 [0.12, 0.16]*** | 0.16 [0.14, 0.18]*** |
Craving | −0.10 [−0.11, −0.08]*** | −0.12 [−0.13, −0.10]*** |
Social support | −0.11 [−0.13, −0.10]*** | −0.11 [−0.13, −0.09]*** |
p < .05;
p < .01;
p < .001
Note.
Models adjust for sociodemographic variables including age, sex, race, education, HSI, self-efficacy, quit intention, and quit attempt.
Effects of Imagery on Choices
Inserts with an image present were selected as more helpful and motivating to quit than text alone (Table 2), as coefficients associated with the presence of imagery were significantly greater than 0 and those associated with its absence were significantly less than 0. Interactions between image and education were statistically significant for both outcomes (p < .001). After stratifying models by education, smokers with higher educational attainment selected inserts with an image as more motivating to quit (B = 0.11, p < .001) than smokers with lower educational attainment (B = 0.09, p < .001), but the presence of images positively influenced both groups.
Effects of Textual Type on Choices
Testimonial and didactic text did not differentially influence participant choices (Table 2; Figure 2), as their coefficients were not significantly different from the grand mean of 0. Interactions between text type and smoker characteristics were not statistically significant in any model.
Effects of Cessation Resource Information on Choices
Inserts that included cessation resource information were selected as more helpful and motivating to quit (Table 2; Figure 2). Statistically significant interactions were observed between cessation resource information and all 3 smoker characteristics for both outcomes (p < .001 for all). In stratified models, results indicated that inclusion of cessation resource information was selected as helpful and motivating for all subgroups, but the effect was stronger for those with: no intention to quit (B = 0.09, p < .001 for helpful and B = 0.09, p < .001 for motivating) compared to those with an intention to quit (B = 0.05, p < .001 and B = 0.06, p < 0., respectively); low self-efficacy (B = 0.10, p < .001 and B = 0.09, p < .001, respectively) compared to high self-efficacy (B = 0.03, p < .001 and B = 0.05, p < .001, respectively); and lower education (B = 0.08, p < .001 and B = 0.09, p < .001, respectively) compared to higher education (B = 0.05, p < .001 and B = 0.05, p < .001, respectively).
Effects of Call to Action on Choices
Inserts with a call to action were no more or less likely to be selected than inserts without them (Table 2; Figure 2). There were no significant interactions between call to action and any of the 3 smoker characteristics for both outcomes.
DISCUSSION
This first-time use of DCEs for studying smoking cessation messages found that smokers’ selection of messages as helpful and motivating for cessation were primarily driven by message topic (41% and 43% of the variance, respectively), the inclusion of imagery (36% and 31%, respectively), and information about cessation resources (22% and 22%, respectively). As expected, response efficacy messages about enhanced well-being and financial benefits that accompany cessation were perceived by smokers as more motivating to quit than self-efficacy messages that aimed to boost smokers’ confidence about quitting by providing tips around dealing with cravings and capitalizing on existing social support. Against our expectations, these response efficacy messages also were selected as more helpful for quitting than self-efficacy messages – which we expected would work by being perceived as helpful. As the general pattern of results was the same for both outcomes, it is possible that participants did not distinguish between what would be helpful if they decided to quit and what is motivating to quit now. Future research in this area may consider asking about each of these outcomes in separate blocks of stimuli, to facilitate distinction between the 2 domains. Also, future studies should consider study designs based in the Extended Parallel Processing Model and Prospect Theory, from which specific hypotheses can be derived around the effectiveness of gain-framed messages, which are like response efficacy messages about benefits of cessation, relative to and in combination with loss-framed messages, which are more akin to the messages about smoking-relevant consequences portrayed in pictorial warnings on the outside of packs.68,69 Cigarette packaging may present a unique opportunity for evaluating these theories in the context of repeated exposure. Nevertheless, meta-analyses indicate that these types of messages have additive effects on behavior change,70,71 suggesting that the assessment of one message type – as was done in this study – is likely to capture message effects that apply in the presence of the other message type.
Our findings are consistent with past studies that have found text with images generally works better than text-only messages,72,73 including for cigarette warnings.2,25 We found an interaction between image and educational level, although contrary to expectations, the inclusion of images had a slightly stronger influence among smokers with higher than lower education. This contradicted our hypothesis about stronger effects among smokers with lower education, perhaps because our “low education” group did not have particularly low education. Few people with high school education or less participated, so the low category combined educational attainment up to some college, but without completing it. Therefore, this low education group appears unlikely to adequately represent low-literacy audiences for whom imagery is hypothesized to be most effective. Future research on cessation messages should find ways to recruit smokers from more disadvantaged groups who are less likely to participate in online consumer panels, as smoking is concentrated among these groups.19
We found no meaningful difference between the impacts of informational and testimonial textual types on choices (<1% for helpful; 3% for motivating). The literature on the effectiveness of testimonial compared to informational warning labels for cigarette packs is also mixed.18,33 As in warning label studies, the testimonial messages we used were relatively short. Such brief narratives might not be sufficient to facilitate identification with the characters in the story nor to transport readers into the story, which are 2 key means by which narrative communication is hypothesized to be more effective than other genres.35,74 The lack of difference in effects by textual type also may be due to the quality of the narratives or the fact that the informational messages addressed the smoker as “you,” which might have made the message more personal – one of the key ways that testimonials presumably work. Although interactions with smoker characteristics were not statistically significant, future research could explore ways to improve testimonials, including pairing with different types of imagery, to see if they can be optimally designed to work with more disadvantaged smokers.
Consistent with our hypothesis and past research on the value of including cessation resource information on health warnings,39–42 we found that smokers perceived inserts with this information (ie, a quit line and smoking cessation website) as more helpful and motivating to quit. As hypothesized, the provision of this information was perceived as even more helpful and motivating among smokers who had lower self-efficacy and lower educational attainment relative to their counterparts. That those who did not intend to quit had stronger responses than those who intended to quit was against our hypothesis. Compared to those who intend to quit, those who do not intend to quit or who had only vague quit intentions may be less likely to attend to information about cessation resources, which are promoted through state and national media campaigns; 75 thus, the relative novelty of the resource information may help account for the greater perceived motivation and utility associated with cessation support. For all subgroups, however, the inclusion of cessation information enhanced the likelihood that inserts would be chosen as helpful or motivating. Including this information on inserts may be particularly relevant in the US, where a federal appellate court ruling76 halted implementation of pictorial warnings partly because they included exhortations to quit. Consequently, the US Food and Drug Administration may have to refrain from requiring references to cessation or cessation resource information on any future warnings it requires on the outside of packs. Including such cessation messaging and information in the inserts should face weaker legal constraints.77
We found no support for our hypothesis that including a call to action would make inserts more effective – this message attribute explained little variability in insert choices (1%–2%). These low construal messages that urge an immediate response45 may work best in videos or electronic media that are more engaging than print. Their effects also may be enhanced for messages delivered through channels that facilitate immediately engaging in the recommended behavior (ie, smart-phone to call the provided number; online video to access the provided URL). As described above, messages that do not include exhortations to quit may be easier to implement in the context of the US legal system, where the tobacco industry has used exhortatory language to support its argument that FDA messages go against its rights to avoid compelled speech.77
Our findings have several limitations. Insert stimuli were presented to participants on a computer screen, which would be different than their interaction with inserts in real life. To minimize this incongruence, an animated video demonstrating how inserts are found inside of cigarette packs was presented before exposure to the stimuli. Nevertheless, responses to real inserts, which would be delivered in every pack the smoker opens, may be different. Trials that examine smokers’ responses to inserts under more natural conditions of exposure are needed to determine their effects. However, recent randomized controlled trials examining the effects of pictorial versus text-only warnings for cigarette pack exteriors have produced results that are consistent with those found in observational studies and short-term experiments.30,31 Hence, the results here may generalize to other study modalities and more natural conditions of exposure. Although DCE is regarded as a method that reduces demand effects while closely resembling real life choices, the predictive validity of DCE for stimuli like ours remains to be evaluated and should be researched. Nevertheless, identifying insert characteristics that smokers perceive as most helpful and motivating is much like smokers’ perceived effectiveness ratings for different anti-smoking media, which studies have linked to actual changes in behavioral intentions and behavior.78
Our analytic samples differed on several characteristics from the roughly one-fifth of the total sample that was excluded because they selected “no difference” for all choice sets. However, the sample that opted out was different from the analytic samples in expected ways: smokers who were older and had lower education, lower self-efficacy, lower intention to quit, and had not recently tried to quit were less likely to find any efficacy information helpful or motivating. It is encouraging that relatively lighter smokers, which represent a growing proportion of the smoker population,79 were no more likely to opt out. Although relatively lighter smokers are likely to purchase packs less often than heavier smokers, it is not clear how often they purchase packs, if at all. Nevertheless, our results indicate that pack inserts that reach lighter smokers may help motivate them to quit. Indeed, approximately 80% of smokers in our sample found at least one insert to be more helpful or more motivating than others. This result indicates that inserts, especially those with characteristics having stronger influences on choices, could have a significant impact on a large portion of smokers who see them, including those across the spectrum of nicotine dependence.
Receptivity to insert messages could change over time, as observational research in Canada found that smokers’ attention to inserts with efficacy messaging increased over time, whereas attention to warnings on the outside of packs decreased.5 Warnings may become less effective because smokers cannot avoid seeing them repeatedly and becoming habituated to their content, whereas smokers may attend more to inserts when thinking about quitting, making them especially impactful. Future research is needed to test this hypothesis.
Finally, the generalizability of this study may be limited due to differences between our sample of online consumer panelists and US smokers who tend to be more disadvantaged. Some experimental studies of cigarette warning characteristics have found similar patterns of results across study samples from online panels, purposive samples that target more disadvantaged populations, and population-based representative samples.18,19,33,80 Nevertheless, this effect may not be found when examining cessation efficacy messages. Indeed, we found some evidence that more disadvantaged populations are more likely not to view any message as helpful or useful. Finding messages that work for this group is critical for ensuring the health equity impact of any proposed interventions.
IMPLICATIONS FOR TOBACCO REGULATION
Our study suggests that print messages with imagery and cessation resource information may motivate and help smokers to quit. These findings, along with observational research in Canada and dominant communication theory, indicate that countries could enhance the cessation effects of warnings they require on cigarette packs by also requiring inserts with complementary cessation efficacy messages that include imagery and resource information. Implications of this study are especially relevant to the US, where tobacco industry litigation successfully halted implementation of FDA-proposed warnings. Courts ruled that the emotion-evoking graphic imagery of warnings – and, possibly, their exhortations that smokers quit by offering a 1-800-QUIT-NOW phone number – went beyond providing purely non-controversial, factual information and, therefore, violated applicable First Amendment constraints.76,81 Requiring inserts with content much like that evaluated in this study likely would be less constrained by the First Amendment. This is because they would provide only factual information about cessation without any exhortations to quit. Also, inserts, unlike external warnings, are not readily seen by non-smokers or smokers prior to purchase and do not prevent cigarette companies from using large portions of the valuable space on their cigarette packs for their own purposes.77 Moreover, this study suggests that adding a call to action (eg, “1-800-QUIT-NOW”) to purely informational cessation assistance information, which could increase their legal vulnerability, does not necessarily increase the inserts’ perceived cessation utility or motivational effect. By contrast, this study indicates that more important content involves imagery with cessation information to enhance the effectiveness of the inserts.
When considered in the context of other available research and communication theory, this study suggests that other countries should learn from and follow the example of Canada (and the tobacco industry) and begin using cigarette inserts to deliver important information and messages to smokers more effectively. Prior research suggests that maximizing the effectiveness of labeling policy will likely require both fear arousing warnings on the outside of packs, as well as inserts with complementary efficacy messages.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by a grant from the US National Cancer Institute (R01 CA167067). The funder had no role in the design, analysis, preparation, or decision to publish the manuscript.
Footnotes
Human Subjects Statement
The IRB at the University of South Carolina approved study protocols used in this research.
Conflict of Interest Statement
All authors of this article declare they have no conflicts of interest.
Contributor Information
James F. Thrasher, Professor, Department of Health Promotion, Education & Behavior, Arnold School of Public Health, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC.
Dien Anshari, Assistant Professor, Faculty of Public Health, University of Indonesia, Depok, Indonesia.
Victoria Lambert-Jessup, MSPH Student, Department of Health Promotion, Education & Behavior, Arnold School of Public Health, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC.
Farahnaz Islam, PhD Student, Department of Epidemiology & Biostatistics, Arnold School of Public Health, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC.
Erin Mead, Assistant Professor, Department of Medicine, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT.
Lucy Popova, Assistant Professor, School of Public Health, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA.
Ramzi Salloum, Assistant Professor, College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.
Crawford Moodie, Senior Research Fellow, Institute for Social Marketing, University of Stirling, Stirling, United Kingdom.
Jordan Louviere, Research Professor, Institute for Choice and School of Marketing, University of South Australia, Adelaide, SA, Australia.
Eric N. Lindblom, Director, O’Neill Institute for National & Global Health Law, Georgetown University Law Center, Washington, DC.
References
- 1.Hammond D. Health warning messages on tobacco products: a review. Tob Control. 2011;20(5):327–337. doi: 10.1136/tc.2010.037630. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Noar SM, Hall MG, Francis DB, et al. Pictorial cigarette pack warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Tob Control. 2016;25(3):341–354. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-051978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Canadian Cancer Society. [Accessed January 14, 2018];Cigarette Package Health Warnings: International Status Report. (5). 2016 Available at: http://www.tobaccolabels.ca/wp/wp-content/uploads/2016/11/Cigarette-Package-Health-Warnings-International-Status-Report-English-CCS-Oct-2016.pdf.
- 4.Thrasher JF, Osman A, Abad-Vivero EN, et al. The use of cigarette package inserts to supplement pictorial health warnings: an evaluation of the Canadian policy. Nicotine Tob Res. 2015;17(7):870–875. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntu246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Thrasher JF, Swayampakala K, Cummings KM, et al. Cigarette package inserts can promote efficacy beliefs and sustained smoking cessation attempts: a longitudinal assessment of an innovative policy in Canada. Prev Med. 2016;88:59–65. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Moodie C. Adult smokers’ perceptions of cigarette pack inserts promoting cessation: a focus group study. Tob Control. 2018;27(1):72–77. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2016-053372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Popova L. Scaring the snus out of smokers: testing effects of fear, threat, and efficacy on smokers’ acceptance of novel smokeless tobacco products. Health Commun. 2014;29(9):924–936. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2013.824063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Witte K, Allen M. A meta-analysis of fear appeals: implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ Behav. 2000;27(5):591–615. doi: 10.1177/109019810002700506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Strahan EJ, White K, Fong GT, et al. Enhancing the effectiveness of tobacco package warning labels: a social psychological perspective. Tob Control. 2002;11(3):183–190. doi: 10.1136/tc.11.3.183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Duke JC, Nonnemaker JM, Davis KC, et al. The impact of cessation media messages on cessation-related outcomes: results from a national experiment of smokers. Am J Health Promot. 2014;28(4):242–250. doi: 10.4278/ajhp.120920-QUAN-452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bansal-Travers M, Hammond D, Smith P, et al. The impact of cigarette pack design, descriptors, and warning labels on risk perception in the U.S. Am J Prev Med. 2011;40(6):674–682. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2011.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cantrell J, Vallone DM, Thrasher JF, et al. Impact of tobacco- related health warning labels across socioeconomic, race and ethnic groups: results from a randomized web-based experiment. PLoS One. 2013;8(1):e52206. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Fathelrahman AI, Omar M, Awang R, et al. Impact of the new Malaysian cigarette pack warnings on smokers’ awareness of health risks and interest in quitting smoking. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2010;7(11):4089–4099. doi: 10.3390/ijerph7114089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.McQueen A, Kreuter MW, Boyum S, et al. Reactions to FDA-proposed graphic warning labels affixed to U.S. smokers’ cigarette packs. Nicotine Tob Res. 2015;17(7):784–795. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntu339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Nan X, Zhao X, Yang B, et al. Effectiveness of cigarette warning labels: examining the impact of graphics, message framing, and temporal framing. Health Commun. 2015;30(1):81–89. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2013.841531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Rousu MC, Marette S, Thrasher JF, et al. The economic value to smokers of graphic warning labels on cigarettes: evidence from combining market and experimental auction data. J Econ Behav Organ. 2014;108:123–134. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Veer E, Rank T. Warning! The following packet contains shocking images: the impact of mortality salience on the effectiveness of graphic cigarette warning labels. J Consum Behav. 2012;11(3):225–233. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hammond D, Thrasher J, Reid JL, et al. Perceived effectiveness of pictorial health warnings among Mexican youth and adults: a population-level intervention with potential to reduce tobacco-related inequities. Cancer Causes Control. 2012;23(Suppl 1):57–67. doi: 10.1007/s10552-012-9902-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Thrasher JF, Carpenter MJ, Andrews JO, et al. Cigarette warning label policy alternatives and smoking-related health disparities. Am J Prev Med. 2012;43(6):590–600. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2012.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Thrasher JF, Rousu MC, Hammond D, et al. Estimating the impact of pictorial health warnings and “plain” cigarette packaging: evidence from experimental auctions among adult smokers in the United States. Health Policy. 2011;102(1):41–48. doi: 10.1016/j.healthpol.2011.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Agaku IT, Filippidis FT, Vardavas CI. Effectiveness of text versus pictorial health warning labels and predictors of support for plain packaging of tobacco products within the European Union. Eur Addict Res. 2015;21(1):47–52. doi: 10.1159/000366019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Alaouie H, Afifi RA, Haddad P, et al. Effectiveness of pictorial health warnings on cigarette packs among Lebanese school and university students. Tob Control. 2015;24(e1):e72–e80. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2012-050745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Borland R, Wilson N, Fong GT, et al. Impact of graphic and text warnings on cigarette packs: findings from four countries over five years. Tob Control. 2009;18(5):358–364. doi: 10.1136/tc.2008.028043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hitchman SC, Driezen P, Logel C, et al. Changes in effectiveness of cigarette health warnings over time in Canada and the United States, 2002–2011. Nicotine Tob Res. 2014;16(5):536–543. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntt196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Noar SM, Francis DB, Bridges C, et al. The impact of strengthening cigarette pack warnings: systematic review of longitudinal observational studies. Soc Sci Med. 2016;164:118–129. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Swayampakala K, Thrasher JF, Hammond D, et al. Pictorial health warning label content and smokers’ understanding of smoking-related risks-a cross-country comparison. Health Educ Res. 2015;30(1):35–45. doi: 10.1093/her/cyu022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Thrasher JF, Hammond D, Fong GT, et al. Smokers’ reactions to cigarette package warnings with graphic imagery and with only text: a comparison between Mexico and Canada. Salud Publica Mex. 2007;49(Suppl 2):S233–S240. doi: 10.1590/s0036-36342007000800013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Yong HH, Fong GT, Driezen P, et al. Adult smokers’ reactions to pictorial health warning labels on cigarette packs in Thailand and moderating effects of type of cigarette smoked: findings from the international tobacco control southeast Asia survey. Nicotine Tob Res. 2013;15(8):1339–1347. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nts241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Thrasher JF, Perez-Hernandez R, Arillo-Santillan E, et al. Towards informed tobacco consumption in Mexico: effect of pictorial warning labels in smokers. Salud Publica Mex. 2012;54(3):242–253. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brewer NT, Hall MG, Noar SM, et al. Effect of pictorial cigarette pack warnings on changes in smoking behavior: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern Med. 2016;176(7):905–912. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.2621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Evans AT, Peters E, Strasser AA, et al. Graphic warning labels elicit affective and thoughtful responses from smokers: results of a randomized clinical trial. PLoS One. 2015;10(12):e0142879. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Thrasher JF, Villalobos V, Szklo A, et al. Assessing the impact of cigarette package health warning labels: a cross-country comparison in Brazil, Uruguay and Mexico. Salud Publica Mex. 2010;52(Suppl 2):S206–S215. doi: 10.1590/s0036-36342010000800016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hammond D, Reid JL, Driezen P, et al. Pictorial health warnings on cigarette packs in the United States: an experimental evaluation of the proposed FDA warnings. Nicotine Tob Res. 2013;15(1):93–102. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nts094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kreuter MW, Green MC, Cappella JN, et al. Narrative communication in cancer prevention and control: a framework to guide research and application. Ann Behav Med. 2007;33(3):221–235. doi: 10.1007/BF02879904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kreuter MW, Holmes K, Alcaraz K, et al. Comparing narrative and informational videos to increase mammography in low-income African American women. Patient Educ Couns. 2010;81(Suppl):S6–S14. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2010.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Thrasher JF, Arillo-Santillan E, Villalobos V, et al. Can pictorial warning labels on cigarette packages address smoking-related health disparities? Field experiments in Mexico to assess pictorial warning label content. Cancer Causes Control. 2012;23(Suppl 1):69–80. doi: 10.1007/s10552-012-9899-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Berg CJ, Thrasher JF, Westmaas JL, et al. College student reactions to health warning labels: sociodemographic and psychosocial factors related to perceived effectiveness of different approaches. Prev Med. 2011;53(6):427–430. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kim HS, Bigman CA, Leader AE, et al. Narrative health communication and behavior change: the influence of exemplars in the news on intention to quit smoking. J Commun. 2012;62(3):473–492. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01644.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Thrasher JF, Osman A, Moodie C, et al. Promoting cessation resources through cigarette package warning labels: a longitudinal survey with adult smokers in Canada, Australia and Mexico. Tob Control. 2015;24(e1):e23–e31. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-051589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Li J, Grigg M. New Zealand: new graphic warnings encourage registrations with the quitline. Tob Control. 2009;18(1):72. doi: 10.1136/tc.2008.027649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Miller CL, Hill DJ, Quester PG, et al. Impact on the Australian quitline of new graphic cigarette pack warnings including the quitline number. Tob Control. 2009;18(3):235–237. doi: 10.1136/tc.2008.028290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Willemsen MC, Simons C, Zeeman G. Impact of the new EU health warnings on the Dutch quit line. Tob Control. 2002;11(4):381–382. doi: 10.1136/tc.11.4.381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.International Agency for Research on Cancer (IARC) IARC Handbooks of Cancer Prevention, Tobacco Control, Vol. 12: Methods for Evaluating Tobacco Control Policies. Lyon, France: IARC; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kaufman A, Augustson E, Davis K, et al. Awareness and use of tobacco quitlines: evidence from the health information national trends survey. J Health Commun. 2010;15(Suppl 3):264–278. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2010.526172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kim H, Rao AR, Lee AY. It’s time to vote: the effect of matching message orientation and temporal frame on political persuasion. J Consum Res. 2008;35(6):877–889. [Google Scholar]
- 46.US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. [Accessed January 14, 2018];The CDC Clear Communication Index User Guide. 2014 Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/ccindex/tool/index.html.
- 47.US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. [Accessed January 8, 2018];Media Campaign Resource Center. Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/tobacco/multimedia/media-campaigns/index.htm.
- 48.Lancsar E, Louviere J. Conducting discrete choice experiments to inform healthcare decision making: a user’s guide. Pharmacoeconomics. 2008;26(8):661–677. doi: 10.2165/00019053-200826080-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Coupon optimization at individual level conjoint analysis. Philip Morris; 1998. [Accessed January 15, 2018]. Available at: https://www.industrydocumentslibrary.ucsf.edu/tobacco/docs/rfxd0175. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Dennis & Company Research. Cigarette visual comparison study: concept evaluation. RJ Reynolds; 1999. [Accessed January 15, 2018]. Available at: https://www.industrydocumentslibrary.ucsf.edu/tobacco/docs/qlpy0225. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hirji T. Project Sky Conjoint. British American Tobacco; 1996. [Accessed January 15, 2018]. Available at: https://www.industrydocumentslibrary.ucsf.edu/tobacco/docs/qlbp0197. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Devinney T. [Accessed January 14, 2018];Analysis of Consumer Research Evidence on the Impact of Plain Packaging for Tobacco Products (Updated to 2014) 2014 Available at: https://www.jti.com/sites/default/files/key-regulation-pdfs/submissions/uk/3-2014-01-03---devinney---analysis-of-consumer-researchevidence-on-impact-of-plain-packaging.pdf.
- 53.Mangham LJ, Hanson K, McPake B. How to do (or not to do) ... Designing a discrete choice experiment for application in a low-income country. Health Policy Plan. 2009;24(2):151–158. doi: 10.1093/heapol/czn047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hoek J, Gendall P, Eckert C, et al. Dissuasive cigarette sticks: the next step in standardised (‘plain’) packaging? Tob Control. 2016;25(6):699–705. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2015-052533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kotnowski K, Fong GT, Gallopel-Morvan K, et al. The impact of cigarette packaging design among young females in Canada: findings from a discrete choice experiment. Nicotine Tob Res. 2016;18(5):1348–1356. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntv114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hoek J, Gendall P, Eckert C, et al. Effects of brand variants on smokers’ choice behaviours and risk perceptions. Tob Control. 2016;25(2):160–165. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-052094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Salloum RG, Louviere JJ, Getz KR, et al. Evaluation of strategies to communicate harmful and potentially harmful constituent (HPHC) information through cigarette package inserts: a discrete choice experiment. Tob Control. 2017 Jul 13; doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2016-053579. pii: tobaccocontrol-2016-053579. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) National Health Interview Survey - Glossary - Adult Tobacco Use Information. Atlanta, GA: National Center for Health Statistics, CDC; 2017. [Accessed January 8, 2018]. Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhis/tobacco/tobacco_glossary.htm. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Newey W, McFadden D. Handbook of Econometrics. Vol. 4. Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland Publishing Co; 1994. Large Sample Estimation and Hypothesis Testing; pp. 2111–2245. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Jackson SA. Message Effects Research: Principles of Design and Analysis. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Louviere JJ, Flynn TN, Marley AA. Best-Worst Scaling: Theory, Methods and Applications. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, et al. Measuring the heaviness of smoking: using self-reported time to the first cigarette of the day and number of cigarettes smoked per day. Br J Addict. 1989;84:791–800. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1989.tb03059.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.International Agency for Research on Cancer (IARC) IARC Handbooks of Cancer Prevention, Tobacco Control, Vol. 12: Methods for Evaluating Tobacco Control Policies. Lyon, France: IARC; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 64.McMahan S, Witte K, Meyer J. The perception of risk messages regarding electromagnetic fields: extending the extended parallel process model to an unknown risk. Health Commun. 1998;10(3):247–259. doi: 10.1207/s15327027hc1003_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Roberto AJ, Goodall CE. Using the extended parallel process model to explain physicians’ decisions to test their patients for kidney disease. J Health Commun. 2009;14(4):400–412. doi: 10.1080/10810730902873935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Witte K, Berkowitz JM, Cameron KA, et al. Preventing the spread of genital warts: using fear appeals to promote self-protective behaviors. Health Educ Behav. 1998;25(5):571–585. doi: 10.1177/109019819802500505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 13. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Toll B, O’Malley S, Katulak N, et al. Comparing gainand loss-framed messages for smoking cessation with sustained- release bupropion: a randomized controlled trial. Psychol Addict Behav. 2007;21(4):534–544. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.4.534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Mays D, Turner M, Zhao X, et al. Framing pictorial cigarette warning labels to motivate young smokers to quit. Nicotine Tob Res. 2015;17(7):769–775. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntu164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Tannenbaum M, Hepler J, Zimmerman R, et al. Appealing to fear: a meta-analysis of fear appeal effectiveness and theories. Psychol Bull. 2015;141(6):1178–1204. doi: 10.1037/a0039729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Witte K, Allen M. A meta-analysis of fear appeals: implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ Behav. 2000;27:608–632. doi: 10.1177/109019810002700506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Chang C. Seeing is believing: the direct and contingent influence of pictures in health promotion advertising. Health Commun. 2013;28(8):822–834. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2012.726403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Houts PS, Doak CC, Doak LG, et al. The role of pictures in improving health communication: a review of research on attention, comprehension, recall, and adherence. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;61(2):173–190. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2005.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Green MC, Brock TC. The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;79(5):701–721. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.5.701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Huang L, Thrasher J, Abad E, et al. The US National Tips From Former Smokers anti-smoking campaign: promoting awareness of smoking-related risks, cessation resources, and cessation behaviors. Health Educ Behav. 2015;42(4):480–486. doi: 10.1177/1090198114564503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. [Accessed January 15, 2018];RJ Reynolds vs FDA. 696 F3d 1205, DC Cir. 2012 Available at: https://www.leagle.com/decision/infco20120824144.
- 77.Lindblom EN, Berman ML, Thrasher JT. FDA-required tobacco product inserts & onserts–and the First Amendment. Food Drug Law J. 2017;72(1):1–25. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Davis KC, Duke J, Shafer P, et al. Perceived effectiveness of antismoking ads and association with quit attempts among smokers: evidence from the tips from former smokers campaign. Health Commun. 2017;32(8):931–938. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2016.1196413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Schauer G, Malarcher A, Mowery P. National trends in frequency and amount of nondaily smoking, and relation to quit attempts, 2000–2012. Nicotine Tob Res. 2016;18(6):1539–1544. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntv258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Huang L-L, Thrasher JF, Reid JL, et al. Predictive and external validity of a pre-market study to determine the most effective pictorial health warning label content for cigarette packages. Nicotine Tob Res. 2016;18(5):1376–1381. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntv184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Goodman EP. Visual gut punch: persuasion, emotion, and the constitutional meaning of graphic disclosure. Cornell Law Rev. 2014;99(3):513–569. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]