Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Violence Against Women. 2018 Aug 30;25(8):903–924. doi: 10.1177/1077801218796329

GENDER DIFFERENCES IN INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE (IPV): A PREDICTIVE ANALYSIS OF IPV BY CHILD ABUSE AND DOMESTIC VIOLENCE EXPOSURE DURING EARLY CHILDHOOD

Hyunzee Jung 1, Todd I Herrenkohl 2, Martie L Skinner 2, Jungeun Olivia Lee 3, J Bart Klika 4, Ashley N Rousson 5
PMCID: PMC6395526  NIHMSID: NIHMS995540  PMID: 30160199

Abstract

This study focused on gender differences in the prediction of adult intimate partner violence (IPV) by subtypes of child abuse and children’s exposure to IPV. Latent classes of adult IPV consisted of a no violence (20.3%), a psychological violence only (46.2%), a psychological and sexual violence (9.2%), a multi-type violence and intimidation (6.8%), and a psychological and physical violence with low intimidation class (17.5%). Physical-emotional child abuse and domestic violence exposure predicted a higher likelihood of multi-type violence for males. Sexual abuse predicted a higher likelihood of this same class for females. Implications for future research and prevention are discussed.

Keywords: child abuse, sexual abuse, domestic violence, inter-parental violence, spousal violence

INTRODUCTION

The goal of this study was to investigate the relationship between childhood exposure to abuse and other forms of violence in the home and the risk of later involvement in intimate partner violence (IPV). Although a relatively well-established pattern, we conducted this study to address several limitations and gaps in published research. First, research has to this point been dominated by cross-sectional studies with retrospective measures, which are problematic because they do not allow for the temporal ordering of variables and because responses about child abuse and other forms of early adversity are subject to recall bias (e.g., Capaldi, Knoble, Shortt, & Kim, 2012; Ehrensaft et al., 2003). Second, relevant studies have infrequently explored the connections between different types of exposure in children (e.g., exposure to physical and emotional abuse versus sexual abuse versus exposure to intimate partner violence) and outcomes of IPV perpetration and victimization in adulthood. Third, research on IPV in adults has used relatively narrow measures and typically does not attend to both perpetration and victimization in a single analysis. And, fourth, there has been a lack of attention to gender differences in the prediction of IPV. Before explaining the analysis model used in this study, we provide a brief overview of the literature and offer some of the key findings from recent reviews.

Review of the Literature

An intergenerational cycle of violence exists within families, such that children’s direct and indirect exposure to abuse (e.g., physical, emotional, sexual) and intimate partner violence when they are young tends to elevate their risk for perpetrating and being victimized by violence when they reach adulthood (Capaldi et al., 2012; Ehrensaft et al., 2003; T. I. Herrenkohl et al., 2004; Linder & Collins, 2005; Renner & Slack, 2006; Roberts, Gilman, Fitzmaurice, Decker, & Koenen, 2010; Stith et al., 2000). In fact, research shows that individuals exposed to violence as children often are exposed to other stressful events and are more likely than others to be victimized at other points in their lifetimes (David Finkelhor, Turner, Hamby, & Ormrod, 2011) (D. Finkelhor, Turner, Shattuck, & Hamby, 2013). According to Finkelhor and colleagues (2013), this pattern of “polyvictimization” affects a sizeable number of young people. According to results of their National Survey of Children’s Exposure to Violence, a cross-sectional telephone survey of children 17 years of age and younger, 15% of youth respondents experienced six or more victimization events, including maltreatment by a caregiver, witnessing violence, and being physically or sexually assaulted. Sadly, many polyvictimized youth continue to be vulnerable to violence in relationships when they enter and progress through adulthood.

While some research attends to the repetition of violence and vulnerability of children who have been victimized, most studies on intimate partner violence (IPV) are relatively basic in their design; that is, they focus on correlations between one, or perhaps several, predictors (e.g., physical child abuse, witnessing violence) and a single outcome (e.g., IPV victimization). In a meta-analysis of 39 studies published from 1978 to 1997, Stith et al. (2000) found that growing up in a violent home—experiencing child abuse and/or witnessing inter-parental violence—was significantly associated with physical intimate partner violence (IPV) perpetration and victimization in adult heterosexual marriages. For perpetration, the researchers documented an effect size of around .18, which is in the small to medium range. Tests of the effects sizes for abuse and witnessing violence in the home showed they did not differ. Similar size effects were documented for adult IPV victimization. Thus, both forms of exposure in children—direct and indirect—predict IPV in adulthood.

Findings summarized in other reviews reflect this pattern. For example, Gil-González, Vives-Cases, Ruiz, Carrasco-Portiño, and Álvarez-Dardet (2008) conducted a systematic review of 10 retrospective studies published from 1995 to 2004 focused on the association between childhood experiences of violence and later perpetration of verbal, physical, and sexual IPV. They found a consistent association between early exposure to violence and adult IPV perpetration, although the findings apply only to males. Similarly, a narrative review of 228 studies on risk factors for psychological, physical, and sexual IPV found evidence of a low to moderate association of child abuse and childhood exposure to IPV with later IPV perpetration and victimization (Capaldi et al., 2012). Capaldi et al. caution, however, that their results are mostly from retrospective studies. Further, Milaniak and Widom (2015) found those with substantiated abuse in childhood were significantly more likely to self-report adult IPV perpetration compared with non-abused matched controls. It is important to note, the study used an extremely limited measure of IPV consisting of only one item.

An interesting question raised in the literature is whether certain types of child maltreatment are more predictive of later IPV involvement than are others (T. I. Herrenkohl, Sousa, Tajima, Herrenkohl, & Moylan, 2008). However, because of the low base rate of certain forms of maltreatment, and because of the complications inherent in trying to differentiate subtypes of co-occurring risk influences (Capaldi et al., 2012; Dong et al., 2004; Ehrensaft et al., 2003; R. C. Herrenkohl & Herrenkohl, 2009; T. I. Herrenkohl et al., 2008), there have been relatively few well-designed studies on this topic. Casey, Beadnell, and Lindhorst (2009) found that experiences of physical and sexual abuse in childhood, particularly the combined experience of both forms of abuse, were associated with sexually coercive behavior towards an intimate partner for young adult males. Testing multiple forms of childhood abuse and a broad definition of adult IPV, Davis et al. (2015) found support for emotional and physical abuse in childhood, increasing the likelihood of perpetrating psychological and physical violence for adult males. In a review of the literature, Herrenkohl and colleagues (2008) did address the issue of how direct abuse and children’s exposure to domestic violence compare in their prediction of later outcomes for those who are impacted. They determined that both forms of exposure appear to have measurable, long-term consequences for children’s development and that when these exposures occur together, as they often can, the negative impact on children’s health and social functioning can be worse. In certain respects, this finding is similar to what Finkelhor and others describe in relation to the concept of polyvictimization, which fits a more general pattern of cumulative or additive risk (Dong et al., 2004).

Social learning theory is one perspective used to explain the intergenerational patterns of violence and abuse that exist within families (Akers, 1985; Bandura, 1977; Cochran, Sellers, Wiesbrock, & Palacios, 2011; Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1990). According to the theory, children who experience violence when they are young are at higher risk for perpetrating and being victimized by violence because they become socialized to certain models that breed hostility and aggression (Stith et al., 2000). Children incorporate styles of relating to others based on what they perceive as “normal” within the family. If violence is a common occurrence, children come to view it as just part of the way individuals express emotions, such as anger and frustration. While their own experiences of abuse may leave them emotionally harmed and vulnerable, adults with abuse histories are more apt to resort to, and become victims of, violence in their own relationships because it is what they know and have endured, sometimes over many years. The fact that individuals who were abused and neglected as children often lack problem-solving skills, are inclined to attribute hostile intentions to others, and also partner with others similarly prone to violence, adds to the likelihood of certain adult outcomes, including IPV (Ehrensaft et al., 2003). This perspective applies equally well to women and men, although systematic tests of gender differences in patterns leading to IPV are notably lacking (T. I. Herrenkohl et al., 2008).

Gender Differences

Whether child abuse and children’s exposure to domestic violence relate to later IPV similarly for males and females is indeed an open and unanswered question (T. I. Herrenkohl et al., 2008). Although there has been some research on the topic, findings are mixed (Capaldi et al., 2012). For example, Stith and colleagues (2000) found that, although significant associations of child abuse/IPV exposure and later IPV perpetration and victimization were evident for both genders, both child abuse and IPV exposure were stronger predictors of later IPV perpetration for males. Notably, Fang and Corso (2008) found a significant association between child physical abuse and adult IPV perpetration only for females, whereas child sexual abuse predicted later IPV perpetration only for males. Magdol, Moffitt, Caspi, and Silva (1998) found a significant association between harsh disciplining at age 7 – 9 and IPV perpetration and victimization (physical and psychological) at age 21 only among females. In multivariate analysis of that study, harsh disciplining was associated with physical perpetration only for females. Bensley, Van Eenwyk, and Wynkoop Simmons (2003) found no relation between retrospectively measured sexual abuse and past-year physical and psychological IPV victimization among adult women ages 18 – 64. In contrast, Thompson et al. (2006) found that retrospectively measured reports of child sexual abuse were, in fact, associated with IPV victimization for women. So, too, did Whitfield, Anda, Dube, and Felitti (2003). In that findings are very clearly mixed and based mainly on retrospective reports, more research on these topics is sorely needed.

Measuring IPV

Earlier published studies on the link between childhood abuse and later IPV typically examined perpetration and victimization separately (Ehrensaft et al., 2003; Fang & Corso, 2008; Renner & Slack, 2006). However, the perpetration and victimization of IPV are highly interrelated (e.g., Anderson, 2002), and many perpetrators are themselves past and present victims of violence. For example, in the National Surveys of Families and Households, about 64% of dyadic respondents who perpetrated IPV in marital and cohabiting relationships also reported having been victimized (Umberson, Anderson, Glick, & Shapiro, 1998). In the meta-analytic review by Stith, Smith, Penn, Ward, and Tritt (2004), IPV perpetration was moderately correlated with victimization (r = 0.41).

In measuring and studying violence perpetration and victimization, it is important to include covert as well as overt forms of abuse (P. H. Smith, Tessaro, & Earp, 1996). Doing so will provide a means by which to assess potential gender differences in the form and prediction of IPV. For example, Houry et al. (2008) found that more than a quarter (27.1%) of female victims of IPV reported being intimidated by their male partner, whereas only around 6% of men reported intimidation. At the same time, women and men were impacted by physical aggression in their relationships at comparable rates. Houry et al. (2008) found that 22% of women and 21% of men were victims of IPV, and 7% and 5%, respectively, were perpetrators of IPV; 2% of women and 13% of men were both victims and perpetrators.

Measures like the Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS; Capaldi et al., 2012; Straus, 1979; Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy, & Sugarman, 1996) have been criticized for not assessing power and control as a dimension of violence in intimate relationships. The addition of items from measures like the Women’s Experiences of Battering scale—an instrument designed to assess perceived susceptibility to physical and psychological danger and loss of power and control in intimate relationships—complement CTS-type items (P. H. Smith, Earp, & DeVellis, 1994; P. H. Smith, Smith, & Earp, 1999; P. H. Smith et al., 1996). Thus, we combine these items in order to assess IPV victimization and perpetration in the current study.

In summary, findings regarding the association between abuse and children’s exposure to violence and later IPV are generally consistent in that these experiences are indeed related. Yet, more nuanced questions about the link between different types of exposure and different types of IPV in adults—and about gender differences—have yet to be thoroughly addressed. In addition, measures of IPV are generally limited in the types of behaviors and experiences of violence they capture. Thus, our goal was to add to what is known by addressing some of these more nuanced questions, drawing on an expansive longitudinal dataset that is well suited to testing developmental (lifecourse) patterns of violence exposure and perpetration.

To capture variability in the experiences of IPV among adults in this study, we conducted a latent class analysis (LCA) of IPV victimization and perpetration. The analysis incorporates various types of self-reported IPV and perceptions of intimidation and control (see Measures). Measures of physical-emotional child abuse, sexual abuse, childhood exposure to IPV, and control variables were then added to the analysis as predictors of IPV class membership to determine the degree to which each form of exposure in children relates to the IPV outcomes. As described below, we also test for gender differences both in the composition of the IPV classes and in the prediction of those classes.

METHODS

Data and Procedure

Data are from the Lehigh Longitudinal Study, which began in 1973 – 1974 as the evaluation portion of a child abuse and neglect treatment and prevention program in two counties of eastern Pennsylvania (R.C. Herrenkohl, Herrenkohl, Egolf, & Wu, 1991). Selection of the sample was accomplished over a 2-year period by referrals, from two county child welfare agencies, of cases in which there was at least one abused or neglected child age 18 months to 6 years present in the home. The children served by child welfare agencies participated in one of several group settings (e.g., day care, Head Start). It was from these other settings that children from outside of the child welfare system were enrolled in the study. The original sample totals 457 children, and is composed of near equal numbers of males (n = 248) and females (n = 209). The racial and ethnic composition of the sample is consistent with the makeup of the two-county area from which participants were drawn: 1.3% (n = 6) American Indian/Alaska Native, 0.2% (n = 1) Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander, 5.3% (n = 24) Black or African American, 80.7% (n = 369) White, 11.2% (n = 51) more than one race, and 1.3% (n = 6) unknown. Eighty-six percent of children were from two-parent households. About 61% of families were in poverty according to income-to-needs ratio in 1976 (n = 276).

The first “preschool” wave of the study took place in 1976 – 1977 when children recruited to the study were 18 months to 6 years of age. A second “school-age” assessment was conducted in 1980 – 1982 when the children were 9 years old on average. A third “adolescent” assessment of all youth participants (91% of the original sample) was conducted in 1990 – 1992 when they were 14 to 22 years old. An adult wave of the study was completed in 2010 after intensive locating and interviewing efforts. Approximately 80% of the original sample still living (N = 356) was located and assessed via a comprehensive, interviewer-administered survey. In the adult assessment, participants were 36 years of age (range = 31 – 41) on average. The sample remains gender balanced: 186 (52.1%) males and 170 (47.9%) females. Although more of the original child welfare group was lost to attrition, there were no statistically significant group differences in gender, age, childhood socioeconomic status (SES), or ratings of neglect or parent-reported physically abusive discipline (T. I. Herrenkohl et al., 2013). Study procedures were approved by the Human Subjects Division at the University of Washington and the Office of Research and Sponsored Programs at Lehigh University.

The current sample was reduced to N = 326. Excluded are the cases of completely missing data on the IPV indicators because they were neither in marital nor romantic relationships at the time of the survey and in the previous year. Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for the study sample.

Table 1.

Demographics of the Sample and Descriptive Statistics and Gender Differences of the Study Variables.

Total (N = 326, 100.0%)
Males (n = 171, 52.5%)
Females (n = 155, 47.5%)
Yes No Yes No Yes No
N n (%) n (%) M (s.d.) N n (%) n (%) M (s.d.) N n (%) n (%) M (s.d.)
Demographics
    White (vs. minority1) 324 262 (80.9) 62 (19.1) - X2=2.45 170 143 (84.1) 27 (15.9) - 154 119 (77.3) 35 (22.7) -
    High school graduate or more 289 231 (79.9) 58 (20.1) - X2=1.18 148 122 (82.4) 26 (17.6) - 141 109 (77.3) 32 (22.7) -
    Age 326 - - 36.3 (2.1) t=.68 171 - - 36.2 (2.1) 155 - - 36.4 (2.1)
Child abuse
    Physical past 3 months 323 - - 7.3 (8.3) t=−2.82** 171 - - 8.5 (8.9) 152 - - 5.9 (7.5)
    Physical prior to past 3 months 325 - - 19.1 (18.3) t=−1.24 171 - - 20.3 (18.4) 155 - - 17.8 (18.1)
    Emotional past 3 months 323 - - 4.3 (6.4) t=−1.60 171 - - 4.9 (6.6) 152 - - 3.7 (6.1)
Child exposure to IPV 280 84 (30.0) 196 (70.0) - X2=.70 144 40 (27.8) 104 (72.2) - 136 44 (32.4) 92 (67.6) -
Sexual abuse 310 127 (41.0) 183 (59.0) X2=28.16*** 161 43 (26.7) 118 (73.3) 149 84 (56.4) 65 (43.6)
IPV in adulthood
    Intimidation (WEB) 309 43 (13.9) 266 (86.1) - X2=.008 160 22 (12.9) 138 (86.3) - 149 21 (14.1) 128 (85.9) -
    Psychological
        Perpetration 323 254 (78.6) 69 (21.4) - X2=.1 170 135 (79.4) 35 (20.6) - 153 119 (77.8) 34 (22.2) -
        Victimization 323 243 (75.2) 80 (24.8) - X2=.00 170 128 (75.3) 42 (24.7) - 153 115 (75.2) 38 (24.8) -
    Physical
        Perpetration 323 54 (16.7) 269 (83.3) - X2=9.69** 170 18 (10.6) 152 (89.4) - 153 36 (25.3) 117 (76.5) -
        Victimization 323 53 (16.4) 270 (83.6) - X2=.07 170 27 (15.9) 143 (84.1) - 153 26 (17.0) 127 (83.0) -
    Sexual
        Perpetration 323 38 (11.8) 285 (88.2) - X2=.48 170 22 (12.9) 148 (87.1) - 153 16 (10.5) 137 (89.5) -
        Victimization 323 45 (13.9) 278 (86.1) - X2=4.63* 170 17 (10.0) 153 (90.0) - 153 28 (18.3) 125 (81.7) -
    Injury
        Perpetration 323 11 (3.4) 312 (96.6) - X2=.02 170 6 (3.5) 164 (96.5) - 153 5 (3.3) 148 (96.7) -
        Victimization 323 17 (5.3) 306 (94.7) - X2=.22 170 8 (4.7) 162 (95.3) - 153 9 (5.9) 144 (94.1) -
Covariates
    Child welfare involvement 325 160 (49.1) 165 (50.9) - X2=.47 171 87 (50.9) 84 (49.1) - 154 73 (47.1) 81 (52.9) -
    Childhood SES 325 - - −1.3 (1.9) t=−.50 171 - - −1.2 (2.1) 154 - - −1.3 (1.7)

Note. IPV = Interpersonal Violence; WEB = Women’s Experiences with Battering Scale.

1

Racial minority group includes African American, Asian, Native American, Pacific Islander, and mixed race.

***

p < .001.

**

p < .01.

*

p < .05.

Variables

Child physical and emotional abuse was measured in the preschool assessment of the larger study by asking parents (mostly mothers) about their and other caregivers’ use of physically (12 items) and emotionally (7 items) abusive disciplining practices (See Appendix A for the list of items). Mothers responded to questions about their own, fathers’, and others’ physical disciplining of children for two time periods: (a) in the last three months and (b) prior to the last three months. Questions asked about emotionally abusive practices referred only to the 3 months prior.

Questions covered a range of practices, some of which were abusive (e.g., shaking a child, slapping a child’s face, hitting a child with a stick or paddle, taking meals away from a child, threatening to leave a child, threatening to send a child away). To differentiate abusive from non-abusive disciplining, each practice was assigned a rating of 0 to 5 to reflect its level of severity. Practices with a severity rating of 5 were considered abusive; those with a 4 were considered severely punishing; those with a 3, mildly punishing; those with a 2, mildly rewarding; and those with a 1, highly rewarding. For the current analysis, practices with a rating of 4 or 5 (severely punishing or abusive) were retained and then combined for each participant.

In scaling the data, our interest was in a child’s overall exposure to physically and emotionally abusive discipline practices across all possible caregivers. The overall scale score a child received on the combined measure can vary both in the number of abusive practices used by a caregiver and by the number abusive caregivers. Descriptive statistics for the physical and emotional abuse variables are as follows: Physical abuse for the last 3 months had a range of 0 to 42.9 with M = 7.3 and SD = 8.3, and physical abuse prior to the last 3 months had a range of 0 to 86.5 with M = 19.1 and SD = 18.3. Emotional abuse for the last 3 months had a range of 0 to 32.0 with M = 4.3 and SD = 6.4. These three abuse variables were modeled as indicators of a latent construct of child abuse in the preschool period.

Childhood exposure to intimate partner violence was measured by parental reports of their experience of intimate partner violence. In the preschool wave of the study, parents were asked (on a frequency of 1 = none of the time, 2 = rarely, 3 = frequently, 4 = most of the time) how often they or their partners/spouses (asked separately) threatened to physically harm the other; hit, pushed, or kicked; or destroyed something. Items were recoded (1 = frequent occurrence; 0 = no or rare occurrence) to identify frequently occurring behaviors. Any frequent occurrence of a behavior among the listed three by either partners/spouses was dichotomously coded as IPV exposure. In all, 30% (n = 84) of children were exposed to IPV and 70% (n = 196) were not.

Child sexual abuse was based on data from a number of sources, including child welfare case records and retrospective reports from the adolescent and adult assessments. All abuse occurred before age 18. Responses were coded yes (1) or no (0). In total, 127 participants (41.0%) were found to have been sexually abused. Of these, 84 (56.4%) were females.

Adulthood intimate partner violence was measured by nine indicators, eight of which pertain to the perpetration and victimization of physical, psychological, and sexual violence. Violence resulting in physical injury was also included. A separate indicator of perceived intimidation and control, based on the Women’s Experiences with Battering (WEB) Scale (P. H. Smith et al., 1994; 1999), was also included. Reports on the WEB indicate that it is an appropriate measure for both male and female victims and that the scale has good construct validity and strong internal consistency reliability (P. H. Smith et al., 1994; P. H. Smith, Thornton, DeVellis, Earp, & Coker, 2002). Respondents indicated their level of agreement or disagreement with items (1 = strongly agree to 6 = strongly disagree) that include: “He/she can scare me without laying a hand on me” and “He/she makes me feel like I have no control over my life, no power, no protection” (See Appendix B for the full list). Item scores were summed into a single continuous variable, which ranged from 10 to 60 (M = 14.5, SD = 9.2). A binary variable was created using the cutoff point of > 19 according to the original development of the scale and prior uses of WEB items (C. A. Smith & Thornberry, 1995; P. H. Smith et al., 2002). Scores > 19 were coded 1 (intimidation and control; n = 43 [13.9%]), and scores of 19 or lower were coded 0 (no intimidation and control; n = 266 [86.1%]).

Eight indicators for IPV perpetration and victimization were included. Respondents self-reported on the Revised Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS) whether they had engaged in certain behaviors (perpetration) or whether the same behaviors had been used against them (victimization). Subscales of physical assault, psychological aggression, sexual coercion, and physical injury were created, conforming to the Revised CTS guidelines, which newly included sexual coercion and physical injury given their importance in the context of IPV (Straus et al., 1996). The validity and the reliability of the scale is well established (Archer, 1999). Eight items of psychological aggression, 12 items of physical assault, seven items of sexual coercion, and six items of physical injury were dichotomized to indicate whether or not they had happened in the past year. Any occurrence of the listed behavior items was dichotomously coded as IPV for each type. As shown in Table 1, a majority of the sample experienced psychological IPV: 78.6% (n = 254) as perpetrators and 75.2% (n = 243) as victims. Physical IPV was perpetrated by 16.7% (n = 54) of the analysis sample, while 16.4% (n = 53) were determined to have been physically victimized. Nearly 12% (11.8%, n = 34) reported they perpetrated sexual IPV and 13.9% (n = 45) reported they have been sexually victimized by an intimate partner. Rates of IPV resulting in physical injuries was low: 3.4% (n = 11). Perpetration of IPV resulting in injuries to victims was also low: 5.3% (n = 17). Overall, there were no statistical differences in rates of IPV across males and females except for physical perpetration (X2 = 9.69, p < .01) and sexual victimization (X2 = 4.63, p < .05) being higher among females (25.3% and 18.3%, respectively).

Control variables included childhood SES and official child welfare involvement. Official child welfare involvement was included to account for the group composition of the sample, which distinguishes children involved with child welfare at the start of the study from those not involved: 1 = child welfare group (n = 160, 49.1%) and 0 = comparison (n = 166, 50.9%). Childhood SES is a standardized composite measure of parents’ occupational status, educational level, and family income, with a range of −5.43 to 6.30, M = −1.27, and SD = 1.90.

Analysis

Analyses were conducted in two phases. In Phase 1, we conducted a latent class analysis (LCA) of IPV victimization and perpetration. The analysis used maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors to classify participants on the nine IPV indicators. Models estimating one to six classes were compared on Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LMR) likelihood ratio tests (LMR-adjusted LRT), and the bootstrapped likelihood ratio test (BLRT) to gauge improvement in fit with each additional class. To examine the possibilities of gender differences in the LCA, we conducted a second analysis in which gender was added to the model as a covariate. Item thresholds and class probabilities were freely estimated across gender.

Phase 2 introduced measures of physical-emotional child abuse, sexual abuse, childhood exposure to IPV, and control variables as predictors of IPV class membership. Physical-emotional child abuse was analyzed as a latent factor indicated by three child abuse variables (two physical abuse measures assessed for the two different reference periods—the last 3 months and prior to the last 3 months—and one emotional abuse measure assessed for the last 3 months). A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) for the child abuse construct preceded the predictive models, and supported the fit of the child abuse construct to the data (RMSEA = 0, CFI = 1, SRMR = 0), with the standardized factor loadings in the total sample being 0.83 and 0.51 for physical abuse for the last 3 months and prior to the last 3 months, and 0.50 for emotional abuse for the last 3 months; 0.90, 0.53, and 0.50 for males, and 0.72, 0.49, and 0.47 for females. The loadings were all statistically significant (p < .001). Subsequently, the predictive models were assessed in a one-step approach where two models—the latent class model and the latent class regression model (regressing the latent classes on predictors and interaction terms)—were combined into a joint model using maximum-likelihood estimation (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2012). Gender interactions in predictors were also tested.

RESULTS

Latent Classes of IPV in Adulthood

Estimation of a series of latent class models determined that a five-class model best fit the data. As shown in Table 2, the sample-size adjusted BIC was the lowest (1745.8) for the five-class model. In addition, the BLRT and LMR-adjusted LRT rejected the hypothesis that the four-class model is preferable to the five-class model (p < 0.05), while the LMR adjusted LRT suggested that the five-class model was preferable to the six-class model (p = 0.09). The entropy was 0.90, indicating that probability of class assignment for the five-class model is high.

Table 2.

Latent Classification of Intimate Partner Violence in Adulthood (N = 325) – Latent Class Analysis Models with no Covariate.

1 class 2 classes 3 classes 4 classes 5 classes 6 classes
Loglikelihood −1124.98 −957.62 −863.01 −827.13 −808.85 −796.80
BIC 2302.04 2025.18 1893.84 1879.95 1901.26 1935.02
Adjusted BIC 2273.49 1964.91 1801.86 1756.24 1745.83 1747.88
Entropy 0.97 0.92 0.91 0.90 0.90
LMR adjusted LRT p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.05 p = 0.09
BLRT p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001

BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion; LMR adjusted LRT = Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LMR) adjusted likelihood ratio test; BLRT = Bootstrapped likelihood ratio test

Figure 1 provides a visual representation of the posterior probabilities of each type of violence for the five classes (See Appendix B for exact numeric values of these probabilities). The same five classes were generated across models with covariates (e.g., child abuse) included and excluded: Posterior probabilities of each indicator were similarly represented, and the sizes of the IPV classes were almost identical.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Posterior probabilities for latent interpersonal violence classes in a latent class analysis model with covariatesa (N = 325).

MVI = Multitype Violence and Intimidation; PHI = Psychological and Physical Violence with Lower Intimidation; PSV = Psychological and Sexual Violence; PVO = Psychological Violence Only; NOV = No Violence.

a Covariates include child physical-emotional abuse, sexual abuse, child exposure to IPV, gender, childhood SES, and child welfare involvement. Posterior probabilities for the latent class analysis model with no covariate were similar to those presented here.

Note. Original interpersonal violence (IPV) indicators based on the CTS have perpetration and victimization variables for each type of IPV (i.e. perpetration of physical violence, victimization of physical violence, etc.). Posterior probabilities for perpetration and victimization simultaneously increase or decrease, so average probabilities of perpetration and victimization for each type of violence are presented for parsimony. For example, the posterior probability for sexual victimization in PSV (Psychological and Sexual Violence) class was 0.895 and the corresponding perpetration was 0.930. (Other combinations of probabilities for perpetration and victimization are available upon request.)

Effects of Early Violence Experiences on Adult IPV

Two primary research questions were investigated: (1) whether early risk influences, including physical-emotional child abuse, sexual abuse, and childhood exposure to IPV, predict IPV class membership; and (2) whether gender interacts with risk influences in the prediction of later IPV. In analyses, the psychological violence only (PVO) and multi-type violence and intimidation (MVI) latent classes were specified as referents (Table 3).

Table 3.

Child Abuse Predicting Latent Interpersonal Violence Classes Moderated by Gender: Parameter Estimates With Standard Errors.

PVO
MVI
Reference category MVI PHI PSV NOV PHI PSV NOV
Total sample (N = 325)
    Main effects
        Physical-emotional (PE) abuse .08 (.06) b .04 (.05) c .09 (.05) .04 (.04) −.05 (.08) .00 (.07) −.04 (.06)
        Sexual abuse .85 (.66) −.13 (.46) −.54 (.52) −.04 (.37) −.98 (.74) −1.39 (.80) −.89 (.68)
        IPV exposure .20 (.73) .29 (.54) −.69 (.61) −.45 (.41) .10 (.93) −.88 (.87) −.65 (.74)
        Male −.71 (.60) −.57 (.49) −.98 (.48)* −.45 (.34) .14 (.81) −.28 (.69) .25 (.62)
        Child welfare 1.29 (.63)* .01 (.43) .50 (.45) .50 (.33) −1.29 (.74) −.79 (.71) −.79 (.65)
        Childhood SES .14 (.12) .04 (.09) −.19 (.12) .09 (.09) −.11 (.12) −.33 (.15)* −.05 (.12)
    Gender interaction a
        PE abuse .25 (.12)* −.06 (.13) .03 (.08) .04 (.08) −.30 (.17) −.22 (.12) −.20 (.11)
        Sexual abuse −.95 (.95) −8.78 (2.98)** −.61 (1.00) −1.04 (.72) −7.87 (3.03)** .34 (1.18) −.09 (1.00)
        IPV exposure 4.28 (2.08)* .08 (1.06) .32 (.88) .14 (.68) −4.20 (2.11)* −3.96 (2.10) −4.13 (2.05)*
Male (N = 171)
    PE abuse .19 (.07)** −.07 (.09) .03 (.05) .06 (.04) −.26 (.12)* −.16 (.07)* −.13 (.07)*
    Sexual abuse −.21 (.86) −16.8 (52.5) −.01 (.67) −.12 (.48) −15.23 (40.7) .19 (.98) .08 (.88)
    IPV exposure 1.22 (.85) .32 (.84) −.43 (.71) −.48 (.53) −.93 (1.19) −1.67 (.96) −1.71 (.86)*
    Child welfare .92 (.74) −.48 (.70) 1.28 (.68) .67 (.43) −1.41 (.97) .36 (.94) −.26 (.78)
    Childhood SES .59 (.15)* .05 (.15) .02 (.12) .20 (.11) −.24 (.18) −.27 (.17) −.09 (.16)
Female (N = 154)
    PE abuse .28 (.17) .61 (.32) .34 (.19) .26 (.15) .33 (.16)* .08 (.09) −.03 (.08)
    Sexual abuse 2.00 (.85)* 5.72 (1.93)** 1.55 (.94) 1.63 (.79)* 3.95 (1.97)* .07 (.86) −.46 (.77)
    IPV exposure −1.58 (1.10) .04 (1.92) −1.54 (1.17) −1.33 (.97) 2.46 (2.13) .35 (1.00) .35 (.86)
    Child welfare .92 (.84) −4.78 (1.17)* −.66 (.93) −.56 (.66) −6.59 (2.70)* −2.15 (1.04)* −1.46 (.87)
    Childhood SES −.14 (.18) −.93 (.50) −.57 (.23)* −.09 (.17) −1.00 (.58) −.55 (.22)* .05 (.17)

p < .10

*

p < .05

**

p < .01

Note. Analyses were repeated with alternate classes defined as a reference category. IPV = Interpersonal Violence; MVI = Multitype Violence and Intimidation; PHI = Psychological and Physical Violence with Lower Intimidation; PSV = Psychological and Sexual Violence; NOV = No Violence; PVO = Psychological Violence Only. Posterior probabilities for IPV classes in latent class analysis models with covariates were similar across the total sample and the subsamples of males and females.

a

Gender interaction effects were obtained in separate analyses from the main effects model. In addition, the three interaction terms were estimated from three separate models. Main effects in interaction models were free of remarkable changes from those in main effects model. b In the gender interaction model of sexual abuse, physical-emotional abuse predicted higher likelihood of classification into MVI relative to PVO at p < .05 (β = .11; se = .05). c In the gender interaction model of childhood IPV exposure, physical-emotional abuse predicted higher likelihood of classification into PHI relative to PVO at p < .01 (β = .17; se = .06).

As shown in Table 3, there were no statistically significant main effects of child abuse and child exposure to IPV on later adult IPV. However, significant gender interactions were found for physical-emotional child abuse and childhood exposure to IPV as well as sexual abuse. That is, physical-emotional child abuse was associated more strongly with an increased likelihood of MVI relative to PVO for males compared to females (β = .25, p < 0.05). Further exploration of this moderation effect in subgroup analyses showed that physical-emotional child abuse predicted a higher likelihood of MVI relative to other IPV classes for males, but not for females. For females, physical-emotional child abuse predicted a higher likelihood of psychological and physical violence with lower intimidation (PHI) relative to MVI (β = .33, p < 0.05). In sum, males who had been physically and/or emotionally abused in childhood appear more likely than females to be involved in MVI in intimate relationships, whereas females were more likely involved in PHI as compared to MVI.

Sexual Abuse

Sexual abuse effects on adult IPV were also moderated by gender. Sexual abuse was predictive of IPV for females, but not for males. Women with a sexual abuse history were more likely to be involved in IPV relationships of intimidation (MVI, β = 2.00, p < 0.05; PHI, β = 5.72, p < 0.01).

Childhood Exposure to IPV

Further, the effect of childhood exposure to IPV was also moderated by gender, and the pattern was similar to that of physical-emotional child abuse effects. Specifically, the positive association between childhood exposure to IPV and MVI was stronger for males than for females when compared to PVO (β = 4.28, p < 0.05), psychological and sexual violence (PSV) (β = 3.96, p = 0.06), PHI (β = 4.20, p < 0.05), and no violence (NOV) (β = 4.13, p < 0.05).

DISCUSSION

In this study, we used LCA to examine IPV as an outcome of physical and emotional child abuse, sexual abuse, and children’s exposure to intimate partner violence. Using LCA, a person-centered modeling approach, we identified five latent classes of adult IPV perpetration and victimization. These classes show considerable overlap in the various forms of perpetration and victimization, which is consistent with prior research (Richards, Tillyer, & Wright, 2017). In fact, previously published studies report co-occurrence rates of over 30% in some cases. Somewhat surprisingly, results of the current study found no evidence of gender differences in the constellation of the IPV classes, nor in their prediction by gender. Efforts to further understand the relationship between violence victimization and perpetration for individuals, and whether there are gender differences in this interplay, are important. To explain this dynamic, some experts attribute co-occurrence, in part, to the tendency for some victims (particularly women) to retaliate against their abusive partners (M. P. Johnson, 2005). Others note that measuring IPV changes across time and relationships is key to understanding gender differences (W. L. Johnson, Giordano, Manning, & Longmore, 2015). While a strength of the current study lies in its inclusion of both IPV perpetration and victimization, this exploration of qualitative differences in experiences of IPV and motivations for perpetration of IPV is left to future studies.

Findings on the intergenerational transmission of violence within families are increasingly well documented (T. I. Herrenkohl et al., 2016; T. I. Herrenkohl et al., 2008). In Capaldi et al.’s (2012) systematic review of the literature, results suggest a modest, but significant effect of children’s exposure to intimate partner violence and child abuse on later IPV risk. Results of the current study are generally consistent in that they provide evidence of this connection; however, they also point to possible gender moderation, which is less well-documented. In our study, sexual abuse appeared a stronger predictor of adult IPV classes for females, whereas physical-emotional child abuse and childhood exposure to IPV, to a certain extent, were stronger predictors of adult IPV for males. Interestingly, other studies, such as one by Fang and Corso (2008), found that sexual abuse was a particularly salient predictor of adult IPV for males. According to Capaldi et al.’s (2012) review, findings culled from over 200 articles (which are mostly retrospective studies) indicate that risk factors for IPV, including child abuse and children’s exposure to IPV, are similar overall for both genders.

One explanation for what appear to be inconsistent findings on gender differences in risk factors for adult IPV is that studies differ sometimes markedly in their sampling, measurement, and data analysis procedures. Whereas much prior research is based on clinical samples or samples from targeted settings, such as hospitals, shelters, and police stations (Hamberger & Larsen, 2015), the current study uses a community sample of individuals originally recruited from child welfare and other group settings (T. I. Herrenkohl, Klika, Herrenkohl, Russo, & Dee, 2012). Moreover, this study used a longitudinal design, and measures of IPV and childhood risks were based on several data sources, including parents’ reports of abuse and children’s exposure to violence in the home. Further, we intentionally broadened the conceptualization of IPV in this study to include experiences of control and domination using items from the Women’s Experiences with Battering Scale (P. H. Smith et al., 1994; 1999), which are not used routinely in other studies on the topic. Finally, differences might also stem from the manner in which the data were analyzed, which in our case centered on IPV classes rather than measured variables. In short, there are a host of factors that can impact the association between constructs that carry similar labels. Of course, findings in all cases should be replicated to assess the degree to which they extend beyond a particular research setting and sample.

While this study makes important contributions to the literature on the prediction of IPV and intergenerational patterns of violence within families, and on the measurement and analysis of IPV itself, it is not without limitations. Limitations include a reliance on self-reports of IPV from one partner rather than dyadic assessments, as recommended by some researchers (e.g., Capaldi et al., 2012). In addition, results pertain mainly to heterosexual couples and to those of a particular geographic region of the country with relatively limited racial and ethnic diversity. Thus, findings may not generalize to the larger U.S. population or to same-sex couples and should therefore be interpreted with those qualifications in mind. However, the study uses a longitudinal design and incorporates various types of child abuse and IPV perpetration and victimization. Additionally, it attends to possible gender differences in the prediction of IPV, which is important for prevention and intervention programs (T. I. Herrenkohl et al., 2008).

Acknowledgments

FUNDING: Funds for this project were provided by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01 HD049767) and the National Institute of Justice (2012-IJ-CX-0023). The content of this paper is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies. The funding agencies played no role in the study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

BIOGRAPHICAL STATEMENTS

Hyunzee Jung, PhD, is a research scientist in the School of Social Work, University of Washington. Her research includes the developmental impacts of child maltreatment and violence exposure with particular interests in prevention of antisocial behaviors and crimes in adolescence and adulthood. She also studies health disparities in aging at the intersection of socioeconomic status and contextual adversities, including discrimination based on sexual orientation.

Todd I. Herrenkohl, PhD, is a professor in the School of Social Work and Co-Director of the 3DL Partnership at the University of Washington. His scholarship focuses on the correlates and consequences of child maltreatment, risk and resiliency, and positive youth development. His funded studies and publications examine health-risk behaviors in children exposed to adversity, protective factors that buffer against early risk exposure, and prevention. An international scholar, Dr. Herrenkohl works with policymakers, school and child welfare professionals, and community partners to increase the visibility, application, and sustainability of evidence-based programs and practices in prevention, social-emotional learning, and trauma-responsive care.

Martie L. Skinner, PhD, is a research scientist at the Social Development Research Group, School of Social Work, University of Washington. She has over 20 years of experience studying human development and evaluating short- and long-term outcomes of preventive interventions. For the past 13 years she has been part of a multi-disciplinary team looking at risk and protective factors for the development of health risks in young adulthood. Recently she has focused on biological and social mechanisms that link exposure to stress to health risks. She has expertise in longitudinal data analysis techniques to study the complexities of psycho-bio-social models of development.

Jungeun Olivia Lee, PhD, is an assistant professor in the School of Social Work, University of Southern California. Her scholarship has broadly focused on the intersection between substance use and socioeconomic adversities across the life course and the way in which social and contextual risk influences impinge on healthy development. She is currently leading the Young Women and Child Development Study, a longitudinal study examining substance use and behavioral health for both teen mothers and children. She is also the leading investigator at the USC site of a multi-campus consortium that seeks to understand substance use and intimate partner violence.

J. Bart Klika, MSW, PhD, is the Chief Research and Strategy Officer with the national organization Prevent Child Abuse America. His research examines the causes and consequences associated with child abuse and neglect in an effort to prevent its occurrence. Dr. Klika is on the national Board of Directors for the American Professional Society on the Abuse of Children (APSAC) and is the chair of the APSAC prevention committee. Recently, Dr. Klika served as the senior editor for the APSAC Handbook on Child Maltreatment (4th ed.).

Ashley Rousson, MSW, LICSW, is a doctoral student in the School of Social Work, University of Washington. Ms. Rousson’s research interests focus on the etiology of intimate partner violence, trajectories of violence victimization and perpetration, and the impact of childhood exposure to violence. These research interests are rooted in her social work practice in the arenas of mental health, and intimate partner violence intervention.

Appendix A. Items measuring physical and emotional abuse

Physical abuse items n (%) Emotional abuse items n (%)
1 Pepper in mouth 78 (21.9) Take meals away 15 (4.2)
2 Slap face 220 (61.8) Threaten to leave 117 (32.9)
3 Shake 155 (43.5) Embarrass 119 (33.4)
4 Pull hair 172 (48.3) Threaten to send away 125 (35.1)
5 Hit with stick 213 (59.8) Isolate in dark room 4 (1.1)
6 Hit with strap 164 (46.1) Ridicule 120 (33.7)
7 Bite 93 (26.1) Lock out of house 8 (2.2)
8 Bite to bruise 14 (3.9)
9 Slap to bruise 89 (25.0)
10 Hit to bruise 63 (17.7)
11 Burn 21 (5.9)
12 Burn to leave mark 10 (2.8)

Appendix B. Posterior probabilities in latent interpersonal violence classes from latent class analyses with covariates

PSV NOV MVI PHI PVO
Total (N=326)
Entropy = 0.89 n = 30, 9.2% n = 66, 20.3% n = 22, 6.8% n = 57, 17.5% n = 150, 46.2%
Intimidation .06 .10 .56 .31 .02
Vic_Psychological .90 .00 1 1 .92
Vic_Physical .00 .00 1 .47 .00
Vic_Sexual 1 .01 .50 .05 .00
Vic_Injured .00 .00 .49 .09 .00
Perp_Psycho .93 .00 1 .98 1
Perp _Physical .04 .02 1 .45 .00
Perp _Sexual .69 .00 .51 .00 .04
Perp _Injured .00 .00 .31 .06 .00
Female (n=155)
Entropy = 0.93 n = 18, 11.7% n = 32, 20.8% n = 22, 14.3% n = 25, 16.2% n = 57, 37.0%
Intimidation .07 .07 .53 .15 .04
Vic_Psychological .94 .00 1 1 .91
Vic_Physical .00 .00 .88 .29 .00
Vic_Sexual 1 .03 .46 .00 .00
Vic_Injured .00 .00 .34 .07 .00
Perp_Psycho .88 .00 1 1 1
Perp _Physical .06 .03 1 .34 .07
Perp _Sexual .59 .00 .28 .00 .00
Perp _Injured .00 .00 .23 .00 .00
Male (n=171)
Entropy = 0.94 n = 16, 9.4% n = 39, 22.8% n = 13, 7.6% n = 20, 11.7% n = 83, 48.5%
Intimidation .06 .10 .38 .46 .04
Vic_Psychological .81 .00 1 1 1
Vic_Physical .00 .00 1 .65 .00
Vic_Sexual .63 .00 .32 .00 .04
Vic_Injured .00 .00 .40 .09 .01
Perp_Psycho 1 .13 1 .96 1
Perp _Physical .00 .00 1 .24 .00
Perp _Sexual 1 .00 .48 .00 .00
Perp _Injured .00 .00 .32 .00 .03

Note. MVI = Multitype Violence and Intimidation; PHI = Psychological and Physical Violence with Lower Intimidation; PSV = Psychological and Sexual Violence; PVO = Psychological Violence Only; NOV = No Violence; Vic_ = victimization; Perp_ = perpetration.

Appendix C. Patterns of perpetration and victimization of interpersonal violence (IPV) for each of the five latent IPV groups.

N (%)
IPV indicators Total PSV (n = 32) NOV (n = 66) MVI (n = 22) PHI (n = 57) PVO (n = 150)
Intimidation X2=96.9***
    No 266 (86.1) 27 (93.1) 47 (90.4) 10 (41.7) 33 (60.0) 149 (100.0)
    Yes 43 (13.9) 2 (6.9) 5 (9.6) 14 (58.3) 22 (40.0) 0 (0.0)
Psychological X2=330.6***
    None 67 (20.7) 1 (3.3) 66 (100.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0)
    Victimization only 2 (0.6) 1 (3.3) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (1.9) 11 (7.4)
    Perpetration only 13 (4.0) 2 (6.7) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0)
    Both 241 (74.6) 26 (86.7) 0 (0.0) 24 (100.0) 53 (98.1) 138 (92.6)
Physical X2=385.2***
    None 254 (78.6) 29 (96.7) 65 (98.5) 0 (0.0) 11 (20.4) 149 (100.0)
    Victimization only 15 (4.6) 1 (3.3) 1 (1.5) 0 (0.0) 14 (25.9) 0 (0.0)
    Perpetration only 16 (5.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 15 (27.8) 0 (0.0)
    Both 38 (11.8) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 24 (100.0) 14 (25.9) 0 (0.0)
Sexual X2=274.0***
    None 269 (83.3) 0 (0.0) 65 (98.5) 8 (33.3) 53 (98.5) 143 (96.0)
    Victimization only 16 (5.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 3 (12.5) 0 (0.0) 6 (4.0)
    Perpetration only 9 (2.8) 10 (33.3) 1(1.5) 4 (16.7) 1 (1.9) 0 (0.0)
    Both 29 (9.0) 20 (66.7) 0 (0.0) 9 (37.5) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0)
Injury X2=142.2***
    None 301 (93.2) 30 (100.0) 66 (100.0) 9 (37.5) 47 (87.0) 149 (100.0)
    Victimization only 11 (3.4) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 3 (12.5) 2 (3.7) 0 (0.0)
    Perpetration only 5 (1.5) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 7 (29.2) 4 (7.4) 0 (0.0)
    Both 6 (1.9) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 5 (20.8) 1 (1.9) 0 (0.0)
***

p < .001

Note. MVI = Multitype Violence and Intimidation; PHI = Psychological and Physical Violence with Lower Intimidation; PSV = Psychological and Sexual Violence; PVO = Psychological Violence Only; NOV = No Violence.

1

Racial minority group includes African American, Asian, Native American, Pacific Islander, and mixed race.

REFERENCES

  1. Akers RL (1985). Deviant behavior: A social learning approach (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson KL (2002). Perpetrator or victim? Relationships between intimate partner violence and well-being. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 851–863. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00851.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Archer J (1999). Assessment of the reliability of the Conflict Tactics Scales: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 1263–1289. doi: 10.1177/088626099014012003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Asparouhov T, & Muthén B (2012). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: 3-step approaches using Mplus. Retrieved November 9, 2017 from https://www.statmodel.com/download/webnotes/webnote15.pdf
  5. Bandura A (1977). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bensley L, Van Eenwyk J, & Wynkoop Simmons K (2003). Childhood family violence history and women’s risk for intimate partner violence and poor health. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 25, 38–44. doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(03)00094-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Capaldi DM, Knoble NB, Shortt JW, & Kim HK (2012). A systematic review of risk factors for intimate partner violence. Partner Abuse, 3, 231–280. doi: 10.1891/1946-6560.3.2.231 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Casey EA, Beadnell B, & Lindhorst TP (2009). Predictors of sexually coercive behavior in a nationally representative sample of adolescent males. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 24, 1129–1147. doi: 10.1177/0886260508322198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cochran JK, Sellers CS, Wiesbrock V, & Palacios WR (2011). Repetitive intimate partner victimization: An exploratory application of social learning theory. Deviant Behavior, 32, 790–817. doi: 10.1080/01639625.2010.538342 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Davis KC, Masters NT, Casey E, Kajumulo KF, Norris J, & George WH (2015). How childhood maltreatment profiles of male victims predict adult perpetration and psychosocial functioning. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, Advance online publication. doi: 10.1177/0886260515613345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dodge KA, Bates JE, & Pettit GS (1990). Mechanisms in the cycle of violence. Science, 250, 1678–1683. doi: 10.1126/science.2270481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dong M, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Dube SR, Williamson DF, Thompson TJ, . . . Giles WH (2004). The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 771–784. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ehrensaft MK, Cohen P, Brown J, Smailes E, Chen H, & Johnson JG (2003). Intergenerational transmission of partner violence: A 20-year prospective study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 741–753. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.71.4.741 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fang X, & Corso PS (2008). Gender differences in the connections between violence experienced as a child and perpetration of intimate partner violence in young adulthood. Journal of Family Violence, 23, 303–313. doi: 10.1007/s10896-008-9152-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Finkelhor D, Turner H, Hamby S, & Ormrod R (2011). Polyvictimization: Children’s exposure to multiple types of violence, crime, and abuse. OJJDP Juvenile Justice Bulletin, October. [Google Scholar]
  16. Finkelhor D, Turner HA, Shattuck A, & Hamby SL (2013). Violence, crime, and abuse exposure in a national sample of children and youth: An update. JAMA Pediatrics, 167, 614–621. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.42 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gil-González D, Vives-Cases C, Ruiz MT, Carrasco-Portiño M, & Álvarez-Dardet C (2008). Childhood experiences of violence in perpetrators as a risk factor of intimate partner violence: A systematic review. Journal of Public Health, 30, 14–22. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdm071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hamberger LK, & Larsen SE (2015). Men’s and women’s experience of intimate partner violence: A review of ten years of comparative studies in clinical samples; Part I. Journal of Family Violence, 30, 699–717. doi: 10.1007/s10896-015-9732-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Herrenkohl RC, Herrenkohl EC, Egolf BP, & Wu P (1991). The developmental consequences of child abuse: The Lehigh Longitudinal Study In Starr RHJ, & Wolfe DA (Eds.), The effects of child abuse and neglect: Issues and research (pp. 57–81). New York, NY: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  20. Herrenkohl RC, & Herrenkohl TI (2009). Assessing a child’s experience of multiple maltreatment types: Some unfinished business. Journal of Family Violence, 24, 485–496. doi: 10.1007/s10896-009-9247-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Herrenkohl TI, Hong S, Klika JB, Herrenkohl RC, Russo MJ, & Dee T (2013). Developmental impacts of child abuse and neglect related to adult mental health, substance use, and physical health. Journal of Family Violence, 28, 191–199. doi: 10.1007/s10896-012-9474-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Herrenkohl TI, Jung H, Klika JB, Mason WA, Brown EC, Leeb RT, & Herrenkohl RC (2016). Mediating and moderating effects of social support in the study of child abuse and adult physical and mental health. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 86, 573–583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Herrenkohl TI, Klika JB, Herrenkohl RC, Russo MJ, & Dee T (2012). A prospective investigation of the relationship between child maltreatment and indicators of adult psychological well-being. Violence and Victims, 27, 764–775. doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.27.5.764 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Herrenkohl TI, Mason WA, Kosterman R, Lengua LJ, Hawkins JD, & Abbott RD (2004). Pathways from physical childhood abuse to partner violence in young adulthood. Violence and Victims, 19, 123–136. doi: 10.1891/vivi.19.2.123.64099 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Herrenkohl TI, Sousa C, Tajima EA, Herrenkohl RC, & Moylan CA (2008). Intersection of child abuse and children’s exposure to domestic violence. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 9, 84–99. doi: 10.1177/1524838008314797 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Houry D, Rhodes KV, Kemball RS, Click L, Cerulli C, McNutt LA, & Kaslow NJ (2008). Differences in female and male victims and perpetrators of partner violence with respect to WEB scores. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 23, 1041–1055. doi: 10.1177/0886260507313969 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Johnson MP (2005). Domestic violence: It’s not about gender—or is it? Journal of Marriage and Family, 67, 1126–1130. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2005.00204.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Johnson WL, Giordano PC, Manning WD, & Longmore MA (2015). The age–IPV curve: Changes in the perpetration of intimate partner violence during adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44, 708–726. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0158-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Linder JR, & Collins WA (2005). Parent and peer predictors of physical aggression and conflict management in romantic relationships in early adulthood. Journal of Family Psychology, 19, 252–262. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.2.252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Magdol L, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, & Silva PA (1998). Developmental antecedents of partner abuse: A prospective-longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 375–389. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.107.3.375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Milaniak I, & Widom CS (2015). Does child abuse and neglect increase risk for perpetration of violence inside and outside the home? Psychology of Violence, 5, 246–255. doi: 10.1037/a0037956 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Renner LM, & Slack KS (2006). Intimate partner violence and child maltreatment: Understanding intra- and intergenerational connections. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30, 599–617. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2005.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Richards TN, Tillyer MS, & Wright EM (2017). Intimate partner violence and the overlap of perpetration and victimization: Considering the influence of physical, sexual, and emotional abuse in childhood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 67, 240–248. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.02.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Roberts AL, Gilman SE, Fitzmaurice G, Decker MR, & Koenen KC (2010). Witness of intimate partner violence in childhood and perpetration of intimate partner violence in adulthood. Epidemiology, 21, 809–818. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181f39f03 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Smith CA, & Thornberry TP (1995). The relationship between childhood maltreatment and adolescent involvement in delinquency. Criminology, 33, 451–481. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.1995.tb01186.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Smith PH, Earp JA, & DeVellis R (1994). Measuring battering: Development of the Women’s Experience with Battering (WEB) Scale. Women’s Health, 1, 273–288. doi: 10.1037/t02188-000 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Smith PH, Smith JB, & Earp JAL (1999). Beyond the measurement trap: A reconstructed conceptualization and measurement of woman battering. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 23, 177–193. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1999.tb00350.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Smith PH, Tessaro I, & Earp JAL (1996). Women’s experiences with battering: A conceptualization from qualitative research. Women’s Health Issues, 5, 173–182. doi: 10.1016/1049-3867(95)00615-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Smith PH, Thornton GE, DeVellis R, Earp J, & Coker AL (2002). A population-based study of the prevalence and distinctiveness of battering, physical assault, and sexual assault in intimate relationships. Violence Against Women, 8, 1208–1232. doi: 10.1177/107780102320562691 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Stith SM, Rosen KH, Middleton KA, Busch AL, Lundeberg K, & Carlton RP (2000). The intergenerational transmission of spouse abuse: A meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62, 640–654. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00640.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Stith SM, Smith DB, Penn CE, Ward DB, & Tritt D (2004). Intimate partner physical abuse perpetration and victimization risk factors: A meta-analytic review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10, 65–98. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2003.09.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Straus MA (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics (CT) Scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75–88. doi: 10.2307/351733 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Straus MA, Hamby SL, Boney-McCoy S, & Sugarman DB (1996). The revised Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283–316. doi: 10.1177/019251396017003001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Thompson RS, Bonomi AE, Anderson M, Reid RJ, Dimer JA, Carrell D, & Rivara FP (2006). Intimate partner violence: Prevalence, types, and chronicity in adult women. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 30, 447–457. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.01.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Umberson D, Anderson K, Glick J, & Shapiro A (1998). Domestic violence, personal control, and gender. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 442–452. doi: 10.2307/353860 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Whitfield CL, Anda RF, Dube SR, & Felitti VJ (2003). Violent childhood experiences and the risk of intimate partner violence in adults: Assessment in a large health maintenance organization. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 166–185. doi: 10.1177/0886260502238733 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES