Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Autism Dev Disord. 2020 May;50(5):1816–1821. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3736-1

“Um” Fillers Distinguish Children with and without ASD

Karla K McGregor 1, Rex R Hadden 1
PMCID: PMC6395582  NIHMSID: NIHMS1505541  PMID: 30171507

Abstract

Two laboratories have reported that children with ASD are less likely than their typical peers to fill pauses with um but their use of uh is unaffected (Irvine, Eigsti, & Fein, 2016; Gorman et al., 2016). In this brief report, we replicated this finding by comparing the discourse of 7-to-15-year-olds with ASD (N = 31) to that of their typically developing same-age peers (N =32). The robustness of this easily documented difference in discourse suggests a potentially useful clinical marker of ASD.

Keywords: Autism Spectrum Disorder, fillers, disfluency, discourse


Spoken language contains frequent pauses and, in English, these pauses are often filled with uh or um. Arguably, these forms are not mere disfluencies, rather, they are conventional in phonological form, meaning, and use (Clark & Fox Tree, 2002, cf. Corley & Stewart, 2008). Investigations of um or uh (but not both) have revealed multiple pragmatic discourse functions served by these fillers. For example, listeners take um to mean that speakers are having difficulty planning or remembering what they want to say (Fox Tree, 2007) and uh to mean that speakers are introducing information that is new to the discourse (Arnold, Fagnano, & Tanenhaus, 2003; Arnold, Tanenhaus, Altmann, & Fagnano, 2004). There is debate over whether fillers might also serve as an indicator of truthfulness. A meta-analysis of 41 studies found that filled pauses, in general, bore no reliable relationship with the truthfulness of the utterances in which they occurred (Sporer & Schwandt, 2006), but more recent work suggests that um use is lower during lying than truth telling (Arciuli, Mallard, & Villar, 2010; Villar, Arciuli, & Mallard, 2012). Adults and children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) produce fewer filled pauses relative to content word repetitions than their typical peers do (Lake, Humphreys, & Cardy, 2011; MacFarlane, Gorman, Ingham, Hill, Papadakis, Kiss, & van Santen, 2017). Given that pragmatic deficit is a core symptom of ASD, this finding lends credence to the hypothesis that ums and uhs serve pragmatic functions, whatever they may be.

There is increasing evidence that uh and um are not interchangeable. Higher um use is associated with female sex, higher education and younger age, whereas higher uh use is associated with male sex, lower education, and older age, and this is true of all of the (Germanic) languages that have been studied (Wieling et al., 2016). Compared to uh, um is more likely to occur at the beginning of a prosodic phrase, and more likely to be followed by a longer pause (Clark & Fox Tree, 2002). As such, these forms may serve distinct pragmatic functions (Fox Tree, 2001).

Irvine et al. (2016) collected verbal descriptions of paintings under dual load (tapping a keyboard while talking for 10 seconds) from 64 people ages 8 to 21 years. Some had ASD, some had typical development, and some had histories of ASD but had achieved optimal outcomes. They found that the uh rate (total uhs relative to total fluent words produced) did not distinguish the groups but um rate did. The ASD group had a lower um rate than either the typical or optimal outcomes groups. Moreover, um rate was negatively correlated with autism severity as measured by the Social Communication Questionnaire but it bore no relationship to chronological age, overall language ability, executive function, or IQ. Given the relation to the severity of social-communicative deficits, the authors conclude that um serves a listener-oriented pragmatic function; whereas uh may be more speaker-directed.

Gorman et al. (2016) conducted a parallel study. Their participants were 4-to-8-year olds who had ASD, and same age peers who had specific language impairment (SLI), or typical development. SLI is a neurodevelopmental disorder that compromises language ability—and in that way shares a challenge that is also characteristic of ASD—but people with SLI do not present with restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior or the same deficits in social-communicative reciprocity that characterize ASD. The investigators prepared transcripts of each child’s discourse during the play, picture description, story-telling, and conversation associated with the administration of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Survey (ADOS). From these transcripts, they found that uh rate (uhs relative to total fluent words) did not vary with participant group but um rate did. Again, the ASD group used um at a lower rate than the typical group. The ASD and SLI groups did not differ in um rate (nor did the SLI and typical group differ). When um was considered in relation to uh (ums/[ums + uhs]), the ASD group had lower ratios than both the ASD and SLI groups. Like Irvine et al. (2016), Gorman et al. (2016) found no association between um rate and chronological age, overall language ability, executive function, or IQ. There was a trend towards a correlation between scores on the Social Communication Questionnaire and um-uh ratio in the ASD group such that those who performed more poorly on social communication had lower ratios than those who performed better. Together, the difference between the ASD and SLI groups in um-uh ratio and the pattern of correlations within the ASD group suggest that um use reflects social-communicative skill rather than language ability per se.

More boys than girls are affected by ASD, and the Irvine et al. (2016) and Gorman et al. (2016) studies reflected that; their ASD samples were 88% and 90% boys, respectively. There is some evidence that low um use is specific to boys with ASD. In a third study of 65 children with ASD (49 boys, 16 girls) ages 6 to 17 and 17 typical peers, Parish-Morris, et al. (2017) also reported lower filled pause rates among children with ASD. However, with reasonable power to detect sex differences, they found that the girls with ASD used fewer uhs than the boys but a similar number of ums. Thus their um-uh ratios were higher and not distinguishable from those of the typical peers.

Together, these three studies demonstrate that low um rates may distinguish children (or at least boys) with ASD from their typical age-mates, both at younger ages (Gorman et al., 2016) and older ages (Irvine et al., 2016) and in a range of discourse tasks. If this finding is robust, um rate may provide a useful clinical marker of ASD. Whereas previous studies have mined the pragmatic deficits associated with ASD to determine the discourse functions of fillers, we might now be able to mine the distinct functions of um and uh fillers to aid in the identification of ASD.

In the current study, we aimed to replicate the lower rate of um usage on the part of children with ASD as reported by Irvine et al. (2016) and Gorman et al. (2016) and confirm the significant association between um usage and social-communication ability as reported by Irvine et al. (2016) in an independent sample of children participating in a different discourse activity. Our sample of ASD children was 94% boys, so we did not attempt to replicate Parish-Morris et al. (2017).

Method

Participants

This study involved analysis of discourse samples collected in conjunction with (but subsequently not used in) a project exploring associations between syntax and the lexicon among children with and without ASD (REMOVED FOR BLIND REVIEW). The ASD group comprised 31 children (29 boys); the group of typically developing (TD) peers comprised 32 children (16 boys), all were monolingual English speakers. Participants with ASD had been diagnosed with ASD prior to the study. Participants in the TD group did not have ASD or any other neurodevelopmental disabilities per parent report. In both groups, ages ranged from 7 to 15 years with a mean of 10;9.

All participants had normal hearing acuity and nonverbal intelligence as determined by passing scores on a pure-tone hearing screening (American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 1990) and standard scores above 80 on the matrices subtest of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test-2 (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004), respectively. An assistant trained in reliable administration for research purposes verified the diagnosis of ASD via scores on Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 1999). To further describe the participants, we collected demographic information and administered the Clinical Evaluation of Language Functions (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 2003), the Expressive Vocabulary Test (Williams, 2007), the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-3 (Dunn & Dunn, 1997), and the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ, Rutter, Bailey, Berument, Lord, & Pickles, 2003).

The two groups were matched for chronological age and nonverbal intelligence (Table 1). They did not differ significantly on maternal education. As is characteristic of the disorder, the children with ASD scored more poorly on measures of language and social communication. On the SCQ, 29 of the 31 children with ASD met the recommended cut-off of 12 as a threshold for ASD (Lee, David, Rusyniak, Landa, & Newschaffer, 2007). We did not exclude the two children who did not meet this threshold from the ASD group as we had already verified their independent diagnosis with the ADOS. All participants with TD scored below 12 on the SCQ.

Table 1.

Demographic Information and Test Scores for the ASD and TD Groups

TD (N = 32) ASD (N = 31)
Measure Median (−/+ quartile) Median (−/+ quartile) U p r
Age in Months 129(106, 147) 132(105, 151) 454.5 0.57 .07
Years Maternal Education 16(14,17) 16(15,18) 406.5 0.29 .16
KBIT Matrices SS 111 (106, 122) 113(100, 117) 462.5 0.64 .06
CELF Core SS 116(112,120) 100(84,114) 242.5 0.0005 .44
EVTSS 112(106, 120) 96(84,116) 286.5 .01 .36
PPVT-3 SS 120(112,126) 111(99,119) 326.5 .02 .29
SCQ 2(1,5) 22 (16, 28) 18.5 < 0.001 .83
ADOS Algorithm
 Communication 4 (3, 6)
 Social Interaction 9(7,11)
 Imagination 1(1,2)
 Stereotyped 4 (3, 5)

Note. KBIT = Kaufman Brief Intelligence Scale, SS = Standard Score, CELF = Clinical Evaluation of Language Functions, EVT = Expressive Vocabulary Test, PPVT= Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-3, SCQ = Social Communication Questionnaire, ADOS = Autism Diagnostic Observation Survey.

Procedure

To elicit expository discourse, the examiner asked what is your favorite game or sport; why is X your favorite game/sport; How do you play X; and how do you win? (Nippold, Hesketh, Duthie, & Mansfield, 2005). The examiner provided general prompts and as much time as needed for the child to complete the task.

From digital audio files, a research assistant transcribed the discourse according to conventions in the Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (Miller & Chapman, 1995). A second assistant independently transcribed 20% of the samples. Inter-transcriber reliability exceeded 90% for utterance boundaries, mazes, morphemes, and uh vs. um distinctions.

The discourse of the two groups did not differ in volubility as measured by seconds (ASD M = 327, SD = 204, TD M = 279, SD = 126, t = 1.12, df = 61, p = .27), or words (ASD M = 382, SD = 325, TD M = 354, SD = 267, t = 0.37, df = 61, p = .72).

Results

Given non-normal distributions per Kolmogorov-Smirnov Tests, we used nonparametric Mann-Whitney U tests to compare the um and uh use of the TD and ASD groups. Just as in Irvine et al. (2016) and Gorman et al. (2016), the groups did not differ in uh use, but they did differ in um use (Table 2). Whether measured as the total number of ums, the ratio of ums to total fluent words, or the ratio of ums to total um+uh, the TD group used more ums than the ASD group and these were medium to large effects. A cut-off of fewer than two ums per every 100 words yielded a sensitivity to ASD of .77, a specificity of .69, a positive likelihood ratio of 2.5 (95% CI 1.4–4.3) and a negative likelihood ratio of .33 (95% CI .17-.65).

Table 2.

Um and Uh Use by the TD and ASD Groups

TD (N = 32) ASD (N = 31)
Measure Mdn (−/+ quartile) Mdn (−/+ quartile) U p r
Total urns 7.5 (5.5, 14.5) 3.0(1.0,6.0) 225.0 .0002 .47
Total uhs 1 (0.0, 2.0) 1 (0.0, 4.0) 397.0 .17 .17
ums/fluent words .03 (.02, .06) .009 (.002, .02) 241.0 .0005 .44
Uhs/fluent words .002 (0.0, .007) .005 (0.0, .02) 407.5 .22 .15
Ums/(ums + uhs) .92 (.78, 1.0) .70 (.33, 1.0) 263.0 .009 .34

Figure 1 presents a comparison of um and uh use by diagnosis and sex. Given that our sample was not powered to discern both effects, we refrained from going beyond this descriptive analysis. Note that one girl with ASD used um frequently and the other did not. Neither used uh at all, so their um/(um+uh) ratios were 1, equivalent to the upper quartile for the TD girls.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Comparison of filler use by sex and diagnostic category expressed as Mdn, −/+ quartile, and outliers. The scores of the two girls with ASD are plotted individually.

There was no relationship between the SCQ and either the um/fluent words ratio, Spearman rank R = −.06, p > .05, or the um/(um+uh) ratio, Spearman rank R = .02, p > .05.

Discussion

This study replicated the primary finding of Irvine et al. (2016) and Gorman et al. (2016) in an independent sample of children and extended that finding to expository discourse. Children with ASD filled pauses with um at a lower rate than their typical peers. Their rate of uh fillers was unaffected. Given the expository task applied here, a person using fewer than two ums per every 100 fluent words is 2.5 times as likely to have ASD as a person who uses more; a person who uses more than two is 1/3 as likely to have ASD as a person who uses less.

For a group of 24 adults with ASD, Irvine et al. (2016) reported a significant correlation of −.45 between the SCQ lifetime severity score and the um/fluent words ratio (lower scores on the SCQ equate to more severe ASD symptomology). For a group of 50 children with ASD, Gorman et al. (2016) reported a nonsignificant correlation of −.29 between the SCQ Communication Total Score and the um/(um+uh) ratio. In the group of 29 children with ASD examined here, there was no relationship between the SCQ and um usage whether measured as um/fluent words or um/(um+uh). Although low um use is associated with ASD, the evidence that it reflects severity of the social-communication deficits that characterize ASD is inconclusive.

Future Considerations

The female sex in general (Wieling et al., 2016) and girls with ASD in particular (Parish-Morris et al., 2016) appear to use uh less than males. As concluded by Parish-Morris et al. (2016), this tendency serves to mask low um usage in girls with ASD, especially when um usage is determined as the ratio of um/(um+uh). The two girls with ASD in the current study never used uh so their um/(um+uh) ratios were 1. This high ratio did indeed mask low um usage in one of the girls. Future examinations of filler use among girls with ASD should consider using um relative to total fluent words, rather than um relative to uh, as it might prove to be a more sensitive measure of girls’ deficits in pragmatic discourse performance. That said, it was the um/(um+uh) ratio, not the um/fluent words ratio, that differentiated the ASD and SLI groups in Gorman et al. (2016).

The verbal task used to elicit discourse merits consideration as well. In our data set, the overall number of filled pauses was noticeably lower for both um and uh in both ASD and TD groups than in the Irvine et al. (2016) data set. We attribute this to task difference. Fillers are more frequent when discourse is difficult (Arnold, Hudson-Kam, & Tanenhaus, 2007; Bortfeld, Leon, Bloom, Schober, & Brennan 2001), and the time-constrained, high-load picture description task in Irvine et al. was likely more challenging than the untimed, no-load favorite game or sport description elicited here. Cognitive load was also offered as an explanation for the variation in filler use across discourse activities reported in Gorman et al. (2016). Those activities, arranged from most to least use of um, were conversation, picture description, play, and story-telling. Those wanting to use um rate as a variable should plan the discourse activity with care to ensure maximum sensitivity to between-group differences.

Conclusions

The current study had two primary limitations. The relatively small sample of children with ASD meant that the study was underpowered to test potential relationships between um use and the participants’ performance on tests of cognition, language, and autism symptomology. The few female participants in the ASD group prevented conclusions about sex differences in um usage among individuals with ASD. The contribution of the current study is that it replicated the main finding of Irvine et al. (2016) and Gorman et al. (2016): a low rate of pauses filled with um differentiates children with ASD from their typical age-mates across a variety of discourse tasks. Low um use is a robust pattern and one that is relatively quick and easy to document, requiring discourse samples of only minutes in length. Although the sensitivity and specificity of this measure are not adequate to support its use as a sole index of ASD, the likelihood ratios are promising. As such, presentation of a low um rate in the presence of a typical uh rate holds potential as a clinical marker of ASD.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

This study was funded by NIH-NIDCD 2 R01 DC003698 together with an augmentation award from Autism Speaks. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

We thank the children for their participation and Nichole Eden and Jacob Oleson for their assistance.

Footnotes

Karla McGregor declares that she has no conflict of interest. Rex Hadden declares that he has no conflict of interest.

References

  1. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1990). Guidelines for screening for hearing impairments and middle ear disorders. Asha, 32(Suppl 2), 17–24. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arciuli J, Mallard D, & Villar G (2010). “Um, I can tell you’re lying”: Linguistic markers of deception versus truth-telling in speech. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31(3), 397–411. [Google Scholar]
  3. Arnold JE, Fagnano M, & Tanenhaus MK (2003). Disfluencies signal theee, um, new information. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(1), 25–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arnold JE, Hudson-Kam CL, & Tanenhaus MK (2007). If you say thee uh you are describing something hard: The on-line attribution of disfluency during reference comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33(5), 914–930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Arnold JE, Tanenhaus MK, Altmann RJ, & Fagnano M (2004). The old and thee, uh, new: Disfluency and reference resolution. Psychological Science, 15(9), 578–582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bortfeld H, Leon SD, Bloom JE, Schober MF, & Brennan SE (2001). Disfluency rates in conversation: Effects of age, relationship, topic, role, and gender. Language and Speech, 44(2), 123–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Clark HH, & Fox Tree JE (2002). Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking. Cognition, 84(1), 73–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Corley M & Stewart OW (2008). Hesitation disfluencies in spontaneous speech: The meaning of um. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2(4), 589–602. [Google Scholar]
  9. Dunn LM, & Dunn LM (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Third Edition Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
  10. Fox Tree JE (2001). Listeners’ uses of um and uh in speech comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 29(2), 320–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fox Tree JE (2007). Folk notions of um and uh, you know, and like. Text & Talk, 27(3), 297–314. [Google Scholar]
  12. Gorman K, Olson L, Hill AP, Lunsford R, Heeman PA, & van Santen JP (2016). Uh and um in children with autism spectrum disorders or language impairment. Autism Research, 9(8), 854–865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Irvine CA, Eigsti IM, & Fein DA (2016). Uh, um, and autism: Filler disfluencies as pragmatic markers in adolescents with optimal outcomes from autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(3), 1061–1070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Kaufman AS, & Kaufman NL (2004). The Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test-2. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  15. Lake JK, Humphreys KR, & Cardy S (2011). Listener vs. speaker-oriented aspects of speech: Studying the disfluencies of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 135–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Lee LC, David AB, Rusyniak J, Landa R, & Newschaffer CJ (2007). Performance of the Social Communication Questionnaire in children receiving preschool special education services. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(2), 126–138. [Google Scholar]
  17. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, & Risi S (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule – WPS Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
  18. MacFarlane H, Gorman K, Ingham R, Hill AP, Papadakis K, Kiss G, & Van Santen J (2017). Quantitative analysis of disfluency in children with autism spectrum disorder or language impairment. PloS one, 12(3), e0173936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Miller JF, & Chapman RS (1995). SALT: Systematic analysis of language transcripts. Madison: University of Wisconsin, Language Analysis Laboratory. [Google Scholar]
  20. Nippold MA, Hesketh LJ, Duthie JK, & Mansfield TC (2005). Conversational versus expository discourse: A study of syntactic development in children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1048–1064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Parish-Morris J, Liberman MY, Cieri C, Herrington JD, Yerys BE, Bateman L, … & Schultz RT (2017). Linguistic camouflage in girls with autism spectrum disorder. Molecular Autism, 8(1), 48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Rutter M, Bailey A, Berument SK, Lord C, & Pickles A (2003), Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
  23. Semel E, Wiig EH, & Secord WA (2003). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals —Fourth Edition (CELF-4). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  24. Sporer SL, & Schwandt B (2006). Paraverbal indicators of deception: A meta‐ analytic synthesis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(4), 421–446. [Google Scholar]
  25. Villar G, Arciuli J, & Mallard D (2012). Use of “um” in the deceptive speech of a convicted murderer. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(1), 83–95. [Google Scholar]
  26. Wieling M, Grieve J, Bouma G, Fruehwald J, Coleman J, & Liberman M (2016). Variation and change in the use of hesitation markers in Germanic languages. Language Dynamics and Change, 6(2), 199–234. [Google Scholar]
  27. Williams KT (2007). Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second Edition. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES