Abstract
The power of human language derives not only from the precision of its signal or the complexity of its grammar, but also from its links to cognition. Infants as young as 3 months have begun to link language and core cognitive capacities. At 3 and 4 months, this link is not exclusive to human language: listening to vocalizations of nonhuman primates also supports infant cognition. By 6 months, infants have tuned this link to human speech alone. Here we provide evidence that infants’ increasing precision in speech perception shapes which signals they will link to cognition. Infants listening to German, a nonnative language that shares key rhythmic and prosodic properties with their own native language (English), successfully formed object categories. In contrast, those listening to Cantonese, a language that differs considerably in these suprasegmental properties, failed. This provides the first evidence that infants’ increasingly precise perceptual tuning to the sounds of their native language sets constraints on the range of human languages they will link to cognition: infants begin to specify which human languages they will link to core cognitive capacities even before they sever the link between nonhuman primate vocalizations and cognition.
Introduction
Language is a signature of our species. It enables us to express the contents of our own minds and to influence the minds of others. Human language is unparalleled in the precision of its signals and the complexity of its grammar, but the enduring power of language also derives from its inextricable links to cognition1. Incredibly, infants as young as 3 months of age are well on their way to establishing such links2–4. Most of the evidence for this precocious link comes from a simple yet powerful task designed to measure preverbal infants’ success in forming object categories, a core conceptual process that provides a groundwork for establishing meaning2,3,5–7. In the classic categorization task, infants first view a series of “familiarization” images from one object category (e.g., dinosaurs). Then they simultaneously view two new “test” images: a new exemplar from the familiarized category (e.g., another dinosaur; “familiar object”) and a new exemplar from a novel category (e.g., a fish; “novel object”). Infants’ ability to distinguish between the familiar and novel test images, measured by looking times, indicates whether they have formed the object category.
Crucially, infants’ success in this cognitive task depends upon the auditory information that accompanies the visual images presented during familiarization. When each image is paired with a segment of human language, infants as young as 3 months successfully form object categories; when the very same images are paired with tone sequences or a segment of backward speech, infants fail to do so2,8. But language is not the only signal that boosts infant cognition at 3–4 months: listening to the vocalizations of nonhuman primates (blue-eyed Madagascar lemur; Eulemur macaco flavifron) also supports object categorization8. This suggests that at 3–4 months, infants have a broad initial template that permits them to link the vocalizations of both humans and nonhuman primates to core conceptual capacities. However, the cognitive advantage conferred by listening to nonhuman primate vocalizations is short-lived: by 6 months, nonhuman primate vocalizations no longer support object categorization8.
This evidence offers new insights into the developmental mechanisms by which infants begin to establish an abstract link between language and cognition. After all, infants as young as 3–4 months of age do not yet parse individual words from the speech stream and assign them to visual referents (e.g., the visual images presented in the categorization task). Instead, very young infants must rely upon another, perhaps simpler mechanism to initiate the link to cognition. For example, simply listening to language may enhance 3- and 4-month-olds infants’ attention or arousal, and in this way boost core cognition9.
In addition to providing insights into the ontogenetic foundations for human language and its links to cognition, these findings raise a vital new question: Does infants’ precocious language-cognition link interface with their rapidly increasing precision in processing the human speech signal in the first months of life? Decades of research document infants’ increasing specialization for processing their native language in the first months of life. At birth, newborns distinguish broadly among human languages on the basis of rhythmic class (cf. stress-timed, syllable-timed, mora-timed)10. Moreover, the cries that newborns produce mirror the broad prosodic contours of their native language11. By 2–3 months, infants tune increasingly to the sounds of their language and eventually discriminate their own native language (cf. English) from other languages of the same rhythmic class (cf. Dutch or German)12. Considerable neural and behavioral evidence reveals that rhythm and prosody are instrumental as infants tune increasingly to the sounds of their own native language10,13–16. Moreover, evidence suggests that infants’ native language comes to serve as a “perceptual magnet” (cf.17), shaping infants’ perception of the sounds of native and nonnative languages.
But what remains unknown is whether and when infants’ exquisite perceptual tuning to their own native language interfaces with the more abstract link between language and core cognitive capacities, like object categorization. Here, we test the hypothesis that infants’ perceptual tuning to their native language in the first months of life10–16,18,19 has consequences beyond speech perception alone, setting constraints upon which language(s) they will link to core conceptual capacities. Does infants’ increasing sensitivity to various acoustic features of language—namely, those that demark their native language—guide them to link some languages, but not others, to core conceptual processes?
To address this question, we considered English-acquiring 3- to-4-month-old infants’ responses to one of two nonnative languages (German or Cantonese) in the context of the classic object categorization task. We selected German (Experiment 1) because it is a language that is phonologically ‘near’ to English; these two languages share key rhythmic (e.g., both have been classified as stress-timed) and suprasegmental, prosodic features20. We selected Cantonese (Experiment 2) because it is a language that is phonologically more ‘distant’ from English; Cantonese differs from English in rhythm (e.g., it has been classified as syllable-timed) and several other suprasegmental, prosodic features21. We reason as follows. If infants’ advances in speech perception do not yet interface with the still-emerging link language-cognition link, then listening to any human language should boost infant cognition. Alternatively, if infants’ rapid tuning to their native speech does interface with their emerging link to core cognition, then a more nuanced pattern should emerge: listening to German should boost infant cognition, yet listening to Cantonese should have a weaker effect, if any. Notice that this would constitute evidence that the developmental trajectory underlying infants’ precocious link between language and cognition unfolds along a different path than that underlying the link between nonhuman primate vocalizations and cognition.
Results
Does listening to German boost infant categorization?
In Experiment 1, infants participated in the object categorization task; each familiarization image was presented in conjunction with a segment of infant-directed German. Infants’ performance at test revealed that they successfully formed the object categories, favoring the novel over the familiar object at test (Fig. 1). This novelty preference was robust at both the group (Mnovelty preference = 0.59, SDnovelty preference = 0.13; t(30) = 3.7, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.69) and individual level (Z = 1.98, p = 0.048, sign test). There was no correlation between infants’ age and their test performance (r = 0.174, p = 0.324).
Does listening to Cantonese boost infant categorization?
In Experiment 2, infants participated in the same object categorization task as in Experiment 1, but this time each familiarization image was presented in conjunction with a segment of infant-directed Cantonese. Although these infants devoted the same amount of attention to the familiarization objects as their counterparts listening to German (p > 0.05), they failed to form object categories, showing no preference for either the novel or familiar object at test (Mnovelty preference = 0.51, SDnovelty preference = 0.15; t(37) = 0.38, p = 0.71, Cohen’s d = 0.067) (Fig. 1). Their chance-level test performance stands in sharp contrast to the success of their age-mates listening to either German, English2, or nonhuman primate vocalizations8 at 3–4 months. Infants’ performance at test did not correlate with their age (r = 0.085, p = 0.608). In addition, the test preferences of infants listening to Cantonese were significantly lower than those listening to German, t(66.6) = −2.3, p = 0.025, Cohen’s d = 0.57.
Discussion
These results advance our understanding of the origins of a uniquely human link between language and cognition. First, they reveal that infants’ rapid tuning to the sounds of their native language has an impact beyond speech perception alone: it serves as an anchor as infants forge their earliest links between human language and cognition. At 3–4 months, English-acquiring infants listening to German, a language that shares rhythmic and other suprasegmental features with English, successfully form object categories. In contrast, listening to Cantonese, a language from a different rhythmic class and exhibiting different suprasegmental features, confers no such cognitive advantage. This reveals that as early as 3–4 months, infants’ advances in speech processing play a central role in their establishment of a language-cognition link. In addition, this evidence is consistent with claims that rhythm may have likewise played a central role in the evolution of human speech22,23.
Second, infants’ responses to the two nonnative languages tested here, coupled with their previously reported responses to vocalizations of nonhuman primates8, show that infants begin to specify which languages they will link to core cognitive capacities even before they sever the link between nonhuman primate vocalizations and cognition. Although this pattern may appear paradoxical, it converges with neural and behavioral evidence indicating that infants begin tuning precisely among human vocalizations before they tune out nonhuman vocalizations18,24.
This developmental pattern also converges well with developmental evidence concerning the dedicated face processing system in humans. Newborn infants successfully distinguish among individual faces of both humans and nonhuman primates25. By 3 months, they exhibit perceptual tuning in their responses to human faces: their perception of faces of individuals from their own race (like their perception of speech sounds from their native language) is enhanced even as their perception of faces of individuals from other racial groups is diminished (a phenomenon often described as the “other-race effect”)26,27. However, during this same developmental window, they maintain their precision in distinguishing among faces of individual nonhuman primates28. Certainly, this parallel between the development of dedicated systems for face and for speech processing is intriguing. Notice, though, that the evidence we report here goes beyond perceptual tuning (to faces and voices) alone. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first evidence that infants’ perceptual tuning to their native speech signals has consequences for which signals—here, which languages—they will link to cognition.
Third, infants’ responses to a nonnative language (German) that is phonologically close to their own native language (English), coupled with their responses to their own native language (English)2, reveal that listening to both languages supports infant cognition, There is, however, a subtle difference in infants’ responses to these two languages at 3 months. Prior work established that infants listening to their native language exhibit a systematic shift in their expression of object categorization, preferring the familiar test image at 3 months, but the novel test image at 4 months and beyond2,5,6. Shifts like this, from familiarity to novelty preferences, are common in infant research2,8,29–39 and are thought to reflect developmental advances in infants’ efficiency in processing the stimulus materials2,8,40–43. Importantly, since both familiarity and novelty preferences reflect an ability to distinguish between the familiar and novel test images, both are taken as evidence of successful categorization.
But if infants listening to their native language shift from familiarity to novelty preferences between 3 and 4 months, why do those listening to German reveal novelty preferences at both 3 and 4 months? We suspect that this may be a consequence of the processing resources that very young infants devote when listening to their own native language: these infants, so actively engaged in tuning to the sounds of their own native language, may devote greater attentional resources to processing their own native language(s) than others. If this is the case, it would reduce the remaining cognitive resources available for processing the visual images in our categorization task. In future work, it will be possible to test this hypothesis by examining neural indices of attention and arousal in infants as they listen to their native and nonnative languages.
This work also opens several new avenues for research. For example, it will be important to extend the range of human languages and the range of language-learners examined. Another open question concerns the role of exposure. We know that infants adapt flexibly to the sounds of new languages (for review, see44). What remains unknown is whether exposing infants to naturalistic speech of nonnative, phonologically ‘distant’ languages (e.g., exposing English-acquiring infants to Cantonese and Cantonese-acquiring infants to English) is sufficient to enable these infants to link them to core cognitive capacities45.
Finally, this work offers new insights into how the uniquely human link between language and cognition may have emerged in evolution. Infants’ responses to the two nonnative languages tested here, considered in conjunction with their responses to English2 and to the vocalizations of nonhuman primates8, are consistent with the possibility that infants’ earliest links to cognition stem from two distinct systems—only one of which is dedicated specifically to human speech18,46–49. Perhaps listening to human languages, both native and nonnative, engages infants’ dedicated speech processing system, and infants’ exquisite tuning to their native language sets constraints upon which among these languages they will link to cognition. During this same developmental window, vocalizations of nonhuman primates may boost infant cognition by engaging another system, one not specialized for speech.
This possibility aligns with recent neuroanatomical evidence46,47 demonstrating that in humans, a cortical system for processing and producing articulate speech evolved atop a subcortical system that is shared among primates for processing and producing affective vocalizations. The neural reorganization that undergirds human infants’ rapid tuning to native speech may incidentally connect brain regions involved in speech processing with those involved in core conceptual processes. That is, human infants’ exquisite tuning to the speech signal and to other social communicative cues50,51 may recruit neural systems implicated in other cognitive processes, serving as a gateway for establishing increasingly precise and abstract links to meaning. (Interestingly, there is no evidence that nonhuman primates undergo such perceptual tuning)17,52. Indeed, infants that show greater neural commitment to the sounds of their native language experience more rapid language learning53. By extension, this interpretation would suggest that over evolutionary time, the neural reorganization underlying our ancestors’ increasing perceptual-motor precision with human speech likewise served as a gateway for establishing increasingly precise and abstract links to meaning.
Methods
Experiment 1
Participants
Thirty-one infants recruited from the Greater Chicago Area were included in the final analyses (19 females, Mage = 4.03 months, SDage = 0.609 months). Experiments were performed with approval and under the accordance of the relevant guidelines established by the Institutional Review Board at Northwestern University. We obtained informed consent from infants’ caregivers at the beginning of each lab visit, and all participants received a book and a toy as compensation for participation. Another 19 participated but were excluded from analyses due to fussiness (7), insufficient attention during familiarization (4), looking exclusively to one test image (2), or a test score more than 2 mean absolute deviations from the mean (6).
Materials
Visual stimuli: Line-drawn images of dinosaurs and fish formed two 8-item familiarization sets and two test pairs. Within each familiarization set, images varied in color; within each test pair, images were matched in color. Images were projected onto a white screen.
Auditory stimuli: A segment of infant-directed German speech spoken by a female adult (~5 s, Mpitch = 274 Hz, Rangepitch = 130–500 Hz) played from a hidden speaker below the screen.
Procedure
Infants were seated on a caregiver’s lap facing a screen. The visual images were projected onto the screen. Caregivers, who wore opaque glasses, were instructed not to talk to their infants or influence their attention in any way. Infants’ responses were recorded by a video camera hidden below the screen.
Familiarization phase: Visual stimuli (either 8 distinct dinosaurs or fish) were presented on alternating sides of the screen (20 s each). The left/right position of the first familiarization image was counterbalanced across infants. Acoustic stimuli were presented as each image appeared and were repeated 8 s later.
Test phase: Two images appeared side-by-side, in silence, and remained visible for 20 s. The left/right position of the test images was counterbalanced across infants.
Coding
Infants’ looking time at test served as our dependent measure. Infants’ left-right eye gaze directions were coded frame-by-frame by trained coders blind to the hypotheses. A second independent observer re-coded 25% of infants. Reliability between observers was high (r = 0.93).
Experiment 2
Participants
Thirty-eight infants recruited from the Greater Chicago Area were included in the final analyses (18 females, Mage = 4.07 months, SDage = 0.60 months). Experiments were performed with approval and under the accordance of the relevant guidelines established by the Institutional Review Board at Northwestern University. We obtained informed consent from infants’ caregivers at the beginning of each lab visit, and all participants received a book and a toy as compensation for participation. Another 15 participated but were excluded from analyses due to fussiness (2), insufficient attention during familiarization (9), looking exclusively to one test image (3), or a test score more than 2 mean absolute deviations from the mean (1).
Materials
Identical to Experiment 1, except the auditory stimuli was a segment of infant-directed Cantonese spoken by a female adult (~5 s, Mpitch = 308 Hz, Rangepitch = 229–473 Hz).
Procedure
Identical to Experiment 1.
Coding
Identical to Experiment 1. A second independent observer re-coded 25% of infants. Reliability between observers was high (r = 0.98).
Author Contributions
D.R.P. and S.R.W. designed the experiments, D.R.P. conducted the analyses, and D.R.P. and S.R.W. wrote the paper.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Miller GA. The place of language in a scientific psychology. Psychol. Sci. 1990;1:7–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00059.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ferry AL, Hespos SJ, Waxman SR. Categorization in 3- and 4-month-old infants: An advantage of words over tones. Child Dev. 2010;81:472–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01408.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Perszyk, D. R., Ferguson, B. & Waxman, S. R. Maturation constrains the effect of exposure in linking language and thought: Evidence from healthy preterm infants. Dev. Sci. 1–9, 10.1111/desc.12522 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 4.Ferguson, B. & Lew-Williams, C. Communicative signals promote abstract rule learning by 7-month-old infants. Sci. Rep. 6 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 5.Fulkerson AL, Waxman SR. Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization: Evidence from 6- and 12-month-olds. Cognition. 2007;105:218–28. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Balaban MT, Waxman SR. Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-old infants? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 1997;64:3–26. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1996.2332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Waxman SR, Markow DB. Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-month-old infants. Cognitive psychology. 1995;29:257–302. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1995.1016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ferry AL, Hespos SJ, Waxman SR. Nonhuman primate vocalizations support categorization in very young human infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:15231–35. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221166110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Perszyk DR, Waxman SR. Linking language and cognition in infancy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2018;69:231–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Nazzi T, Bertoncini J, Mehler J. Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1998;24:756–66. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mampe B, Friederici AD, Christophe A, Wermke K. Newborns’ cry melody is shaped by their native language. Curr. Biol. 2009;19:1994–1997. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.09.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Christophe A, Morton J. Is Dutch native English? Linguistic analysis by two-month-olds. Dev. Sci. 1998;1:215–219. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00033. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Peña M, Pittaluga E, Mehler J. Language acquisition in premature and full-term infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:3823–3828. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914326107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bosch L, Sebastián-Gallés N. Native-language recognition abilities in 4-month-old infants from monolingual and bilingual environments. Cognition. 1997;65:33–69. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00040-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Nazzi T, Jusczyk PW, Johnson EK. Language discrimination by English-learning 5-month-olds: Effects of rhythm and familiarity. J. Mem. Lang. 2000;43:1–9. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2698. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Abboub N, Nazzi T, Gervain J. Prosodic grouping at birth. Brain Lang. 2016;162:46–59. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kuhl PK. Human adults and human infants show a “ perceptual magnet effect” for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not. Percept. Psychophys. 1991;50:93–107. doi: 10.3758/BF03212211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Shultz S, Vouloumanos A, Bennett RH, Pelphrey K. Neural specialization for speech in the first months of life. Dev. Sci. 2014;17:766–74. doi: 10.1111/desc.12151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Moon C, Cooper RP, Fifer WP. Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behav. Dev. 1993;16:495–500. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80007-U. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Word prosodic systems in the languages of Europe. (Mouton de Gruyter, 1999).
- 21.Chan AYW, Li DCS. English and Cantonese phonology in contrast: explaining cantonese ESL learners’ english pronunciation problems. Lang. Cult. Curric. 2000;13:67–85. doi: 10.1080/07908310008666590. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ghazanfar AA, Takahashi DY. The evolution of speech: Vision, rhythm, cooperation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014;18:543–553. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ghazanfar AA, Takahashi DY. Facial expressions and the evolution of the speech rhythm. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014;26:1196–1207. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Minagawa-Kawai Y, et al. Optical brain imaging reveals general auditory and language-specific processing in early infant development. Cereb. Cortex. 2011;21:254–261. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Pascalis O, de Haan M, Nelson C. Is face processing species-specific during the first year of life? Science (80-.). 2002;296:1321–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1070223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kelly DJ, et al. The other-race effect develops during infancy: evidence of perceptual narrowing. Psychol. Sci. 2007;18:1084–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02029.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kelly DJ, et al. Three-month-olds, but not newborns, prefer own-race faces. Dev. Sci. 2005;8:F31–F36. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.0434a.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Pascalis O, et al. Plasticity of face processing in infancy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:5297–300. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0406627102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Uzgiris IC, Hunt JM. Attentional preference and experience: II. An exploratory longitudinal study of the effect of visual familiarity and responsiveness. J. Genet. Psychol. 1970;117:109–121. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1970.10533941. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hunt JM. Attentional preference and experience: I. Introduction. J. Genet. Psychol. 1970;117:99–107. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1970.10533940. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Rose SA, Feldman JF, Jankowski JJ. Infant visual recognition memory. Dev. Rev. 2004;24:74–100. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2003.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Perone, S. & Spencer, J. P. Autonomous visual exploration creates developmental change in familiarity and novelty seeking behaviors. Front. Psychol. 4 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 33.Slater A. Novelty, familiarity, and infant reasoning. Infant and Child Development. 2004;13:353–355. doi: 10.1002/icd.356. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Wetherford MJ, Cohen LB. Developmental changes in infant visual preferences for novelty and familiarity. Child Dev. 1973;44:416–424. doi: 10.2307/1127994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Roder BJ, Bushnell EW, Sasseville AM. Infants’ preferences for familiarity and novelty during the course of visual processing. Infancy. 2000;1:491–507. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0104_9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Shinskey JL, Munakata Y. Something old, something new: A developmental transition from familiarity to novelty preferences with hidden objects. Dev. Sci. 2010;13:378–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00899.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hunter MA, Ames EW. A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. Adv. Infancy Res. 1988;5:69–95. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Weizmann F, Cohen LB, Pratt RJ. Novelty, familiarity, and the development of infant attention. Dev. Psychol. 1971;4:149–154. doi: 10.1037/h0030432. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Colombo J, Bundy RS. Infant response to auditory familiarity and novelty. Infant Behav. Dev. 1983;6:305–311. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(83)80039-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Frick JE, Colombo J, Allen JR. Temporal sequence of global-local processing in 3-month-old infants. Infancy. 2000;1:375–386. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0103_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Colombo J. Infant attention grows up: The emergence of a developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2002;11:196–200. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00199. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Aslin RN. What’s in a look? Dev. Sci. 2007;10:48–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00563.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Reynolds GD, Romano AC. The development of attention systems and working memory in infancy. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2016;10:15. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kuhl PK. Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain? Dev. Sci. 2007;10:110–120. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00572.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Perszyk DR, Waxman SR. Listening to the calls of the wild: The role of experience in linking language and cognition in young infants. Cognition. 2016;153:175–181. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Owren MJ, Amoss RT, Rendall D. Two organizing principles of vocal production: Implications for nonhuman and human primates. Am. J. Primatol. 2011;73:530–44. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ackermann H, Hage SR, Ziegler W. Brain mechanisms of acoustic communication in humans and nonhuman primates: an evolutionary perspective. Behav. Brain Sci. 2014;37:529–604. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13003099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Dehaene-Lambertz G, Dehaene S, Hertz-Pannier L. Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science. 2002;298:2013–5. doi: 10.1126/science.1077066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.May L, Gervain J, Carreiras M, Werker JF. The specificity of the neural response to speech at birth. Dev. Sci. 2018;21:1–9. doi: 10.1111/desc.12564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Werker JF, Tees RC. Influences on infant speech processing: toward a new synthesis. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1999;50:509–35. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kuhl PK, Ramirez RR, Bosseler A, Lin J-FL, Imada T. Infants’ brain responses to speech suggest Analysis bySynthesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2014;111:11238–11245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1410963111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Zangenehpour S, Ghazanfar AA, Lewkowicz DJ, Zatorre RJ. Heterochrony and cross-species intersensory matching by infant vervet monkeys. PLoS One. 2009;4:e4302. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kuhl PK, et al. Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e) Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 2008;363:979–1000. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]