Abstract
Objective
To see to what extent executive function limitations at age 10 years among children born extremely preterm (EP) are associated with blood levels of inflammatory and neurotrophic proteins during the first postnatal month.
Design
Longitudinal cohort
Setting
Children were enrolled shortly before or after birth.
Subjects
692 children born before the 28th week of gestation who had an IQ ≥ 70 at age 10 years.
Exposures
Blood concentrations during the first four weeks of proteins with inflammatory and neurotrophic properties.
Main outcome measures
Z-scores ≤ −1 on the Differential Ability Scales–II working memory (WM) assessment) (N=164), the NEPSY-II (A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment-II) Inhibition-Inhibition assessment) (N=350), the NEPSY-II Inhibition-Switching assessment) (N=345), as well as a Zscore ≤ −1 on all three assessments (identified as the executive dysfunction composite (EDC) (N=104).
Results
Increased risks of the EDC associated with high concentrations of inflammatory proteins (IL-8, TNF-α, and ICAM-1) were modulated by high concentrations of neurotrophic proteins. This pattern was also seen for the working memory limitation, but only with high concentrations of IL-8 and TNF-α, and the switching limitation, but only with high concentrations of ICAM-1.
Conclusions
Among children born EP, risks of executive function limitations might be explained by perinatal systemic inflammation in the absence of adequate neurotrophic capability.
1. Introduction
Children born very preterm are often at higher risk of executive function limitations than their term-born peers (Farooqi et al., 2016; Taylor and Clark, 2016), especially if they have early-identified structural abnormalities of the brain (Kalpakidou et al., 2014). Because inflammation appears to increase the risk of brain damage in the very preterm newborn (Dammann and Leviton, 2014), we reasoned that perhaps systemic inflammation provides information about the risk of executive dysfunctions (EDs). Similarly, because growth factors with neurotrophic and/or angiogenic properties have the potential to minimize the occurrence of brain damage (Chew and DeBoy, 2016; Krityakiarana et al., 2016), we also reasoned that high concentrations of such proteins might ameliorate the risks of executive dysfunctions associated with systemic inflammation.
The three-factor structure of executive function, which consists of working memory (WM), ability to inhibit responses, and ability to shift/switch tasks, was based on findings among adults, and applies to older children, adolescents (Friedman and Miyake, 2017). To avoid any controversy about what we consider executive function, we considered separately the three components when evaluating the relationship between the concentrations of neonatal biomarkers and execution dysfunctions. We also created an ED composite consisting of low scores on each of the three assessments.
Here we report our findings of the relationships between low scores on assessments of executive functions (WM, inhibition, and switching) and elevated blood concentrations of inflammation-related proteins and proteins with angiogenic and neurotrophic properties. We restricted our analyses to children with IQ ≥ 70 so that the outcome we studied was executive functions limitations in the context of normal intelligence.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants (Table 1a)
Table 1.
1a. sample description | ||
Yes | No | |
Enrolled at birth | 1506 | |
Survived to age 10 years | 1198 | 308 |
Proteins measured in ≥ 2 blood spots collected in first postnatal month | 1192 | 6 |
Returned for an assessment at age 10 years | 857 | 335 |
DAS-II IQ ≥ 70 | 713 | 144 |
Had all 3 executive function assessments. | 692 | 21 |
1b. Children classified by whether or not they had Z-scores ≤ −1 on each of three assessments of executive functions. | ||||
Working Memory |
Inhibition Inhibition |
Inhibition Switching |
N | Description |
≤ −1 | ≤ −1 | ≤ −1 | 104 | All 3 (Exec Dysfunction Composite) (EDC) |
≤ −1 | ≤ −1 | 14 | ||
≤ −1 | ≤ −1 | 28 | ||
≤ −1 | 18 | Isolated Working Memory (WM) | ||
≤ −1 | ≤ −1 | 138 | ||
≤ −1 | 94 | Isolated Inhibition Inhibition (INI) | ||
≤ −1 | 78 | Isolated Inhibition Switching (INS) | ||
221 | Referent (no Z-score ≤ −1) | |||
Summary | ||||
All ≤ −1 | 164 | All Working Memory | ||
All ≤ −1 | 350 | All Inhibition Inhibition | ||
All ≤ −1 | 345 | All Inhibition Switching | ||
Total N | 692 |
The ELGAN (Extremely Low Gestational Age Newborn) study is a multi-center prospective, observational study of the risk of structural and functional neurologic disorders in infants born before the 28th week of gestation.(O’Shea et al., 2009) Of the 1506 infants born before the 28th week and recruited before the end of the first postnatal day, 966 children were actively recruited for follow-up at age 10 (because of the availability of circulating protein markers from the first month of life). 857 (89%) returned for a neurocognitive assessment at age 10 years. The sample for this study consists of the 692 of these 857 children who had a DAS-II mean IQ ≥ 70 and were administered the DAS-II WM assessment and NEPSY-II Inhibition-Inhibition and Inhibition-Switching subtests.
2.2. Newborn variables
The gestational age estimates were based on a hierarchy of the quality of available information. Most desirable were estimates based on the dates of embryo retrieval or intrauterine insemination or fetal ultrasound before the 14th week (62%). When these were not available, reliance was placed on a ≥ 14 weeks fetal ultrasound (29%), LMP without fetal ultrasound (7%), and gestational age recorded in the admission log (1%). The birthweight Z-score is the number of standard deviations the infant’s birthweight is above or below the median weight of infants at the same gestational age in a standard data set (Yudkin et al., 1987).
2.3. Procedures for the assessments at age 10 years
A subset of the families (n=966) who participated in the follow up at two years of age were contacted by mail and then by phone to invite them to participate in the 10-year follow up. This subset was selected because we had measurements of inflammation-related proteins in neonatal blood. Lost to follow-up families were searched for on state vaccination registries, and other openly-available websites. Facebook was also used where approved by the local institution’s IRB. Families willing to participate were scheduled for one visit during which all of the measures reported here were administered in 3 to 4 hours, including breaks.
2.4. General cognitive ability.
General cognitive ability (or IQ) was assessed with the School-Age DAS-II Verbal and Nonverbal Reasoning scales (Elliott, 2007). We classified children based on the mean of their verbal and non-verbal components into two groups: <70 and ≥ 70. Only those in the higher IQ group are included in this report.
2.5. Indicators of executive function
Executive functions were assessed with both the DAS-II (Elliott, 2007) and the NEPSY-II (Korkman et al., 1998). The DAS Recall of Digits Backward and Recall of Sequential Order measured verbal WM, while the NEPSY-II Inhibition-Inhibition and Inhibition-Switching measured simple inhibition and inhibition in the context of set shifting, respectively. We created an executive dysfunction composite (EDC), which we defined by a Z-score ≤ −1 on all three assessments (i.e., the DAS-II WM, and the NEPSY inhibition and switching components) among children with DAS-II mean IQ ≥ 70.
2.6. Blood spot collection, storage, and protein measurement
Drops of blood were collected on filter paper on the first postnatal day (range: 1–3 days), the 7th postnatal day (range: 5–8 days), the 14th postnatal day (range: 12–15 days), the 21st postnatal day (range: 19–23 days), and the 28th postnatal day (range: 26–29). All blood was from the remainder of specimens obtained for clinical indications. Dried blood spots were stored at −70 °C in sealed bags with a desiccant until processed. After 10 years of storage at −24 °C, median protein abundance in dried blood spots decreases only 7%.(Bjorkesten et al., 2017) Details about the elution of proteins from the blood spots are provided elsewhere.(Fichorova et al., 2015)
All protein measurements were made by Dr. Fichorova’s Genital Tract Biology Laboratory at the Brigham and Women’s Hospital in Boston MA. This laboratory is accredited by The College of American Pathologists. The following proteins were measured with the Meso Scale Discovery (MSD) electrochemiluminescence multiplex platform and Sector Imager 2400, which has high analytic(Fichorova et al., 2008) and clinical validity(Hecht et al., 2011; Leviton et al., 2011a; McElrath et al., 2011; Leviton et al., 2012b; Bose et al., 2013): C-Reactive Protein (CRP), serum amyloid A (SAA), myeloperoxidase (MPO), Interleukin-1 β (IL-1β), Interleukin-6 (IL-6), Interleukin-6 Receptor (IL-6R), Tumor Necrosis Factor-α (TNF-α), Tumor Necrosis Factor Receptor-1 (TNFR-1), TNFR-2, IL-8 (CXCL8), Regulated upon Activation, Normal T-cell Expressed, and Secreted (RANTES; CCL5), Intercellular Adhesion Molecule −1 (ICAM-1; CD54), Vascular Cell Adhesion Molecule-1 (VCAM-1; CD106), matrix metalloproteinase-9 (MMP-9), Thyroid Stimulating Hormone (TSH), Erythropoietin (EPO),Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor (VEGF), Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor Receptor-1 (VEGFR-1, also known as sFLT-1), Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor Receptor-2 (VEGFR-2; KDR), and IGF Binding Protein-1 (IGFBP-1).
The Laboratory optimized an ELISA assay for measuring IGF-1 in the dry blood spots using Duoset assay reagents. In brief, each dry blood spot elution was incubated with an acidic solution in a ratio of 1:20 (R&D Systems Catalog Buffer 08, pH 2.15) for 10 minutes at room temperature, followed by pH neutralization with 1:50 1.2 N NaOH (Fisher) in 0.5M HEPES Buffer (Invitrogen). The plates were incubated with capture antibody overnight, blocked with a 5% Tween 20 reagent (R& D Systems) for 1 hour, washed and incubated with 100 μl standards and neutralized samples for 2h, followed by automated wash in 0.05% Tween20/PBS, 100 μl biotinylated goat antihuman IGF-1 antibody for 2h, automated wash, streptavidin-HRP solution for 20 min, automated wash, substrate reagent for 20 minutes, and neutralization with stop solution. The plates were read at 450 nm filter and reference filter 570 nm using Victor2 reader (Perkin Elmer, Boston, MA). The inter-assay coefficient of variation assessed by a quality control sample run on each plate was 17%. The pg/ml values intrapolated from the standard curve of each assay were normalized to mg of total protein measured by the Pierce BCA assay (Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA).
The Laboratory used a multiplex immunobead assay manufactured by R&D Systems (Minneapolis, MN) and a MAGPIX Luminex reader (R&D Systems) to measure placenta growth factor (PIGF), Neurotrophin-4 (NT-4), Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF), basic Fibroblastic Growth Factor (bFGF), and angiopoietin-1 (Ang-1), and angiopoietin-2 (Ang-2).
The total protein concentration in each eluted sample was determined by BCA assay (Thermo Scientific, Rockford, IL) using a multi-label Victor 2 counter (Perkin Elmer, Boston, MA) and the measurements of each individual protein were normalized to mg total protein.
2.7. Data analyses
In the ELGAN Study cohort, children who had top quartile concentrations of inflammation-related proteins were no more likely than their peers who had lower concentrations to have a mother who had limited educational achievement, a low score on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition™ (KBIT-2™), or was eligible for government-provided medical care insurance (Medicaid) (Leviton et al., 2016a). Thus, confounding by social class is not an issue. Similarly, other potential confounders such as sex were also not associated with systemic inflammation.
We evaluated the following generalized null hypotheses:
1. Individual days: single proteins
We evaluated the hypothesis that children who had a top quartile concentration of each protein on each day were not at greater risk of low scores (i.e., Z-score ≤ −1) on the three NEPSY assessments of working memory, the inhibition-inhibition, inhibition-switching, and were not at greater risk of the EDC (defined as a Z-score ≤ −1 on all three assessments) than children who had a lower concentration of each protein on each of five days during the first postnatal month (Appendix Tables 1–4, which are summarized in Table 2).
Table 2.
Day 1 | Day 7 | Day 14 | Day 21 | Day 28 | ||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
W | I | S | E | W | I | S | E | W | I | S | E | W | I | S | E | W | I | S | E | |
CRP | Λ | Λ | Λ | |||||||||||||||||
SAA | Λ | |||||||||||||||||||
MPO | ||||||||||||||||||||
IL-1β | ||||||||||||||||||||
IL-6 | Λ | Λ | Λ | |||||||||||||||||
IL-6R | V | V | V | V | V | |||||||||||||||
TNF-α | Λ | Λ | Λ | |||||||||||||||||
TNF-R1 | ||||||||||||||||||||
TNF-R2 | Λ | Λ | ||||||||||||||||||
IL-8 | Λ | Λ | Λ | Λ | Λ | Λ | Λ | Λ | Λ | |||||||||||
RANTES | V | V | ||||||||||||||||||
ICAM-1 | Λ | Λ | Λ | |||||||||||||||||
VCAM-1 | V | |||||||||||||||||||
MMP-9 | V | |||||||||||||||||||
TSH | ||||||||||||||||||||
EPO | Λ | Λ | ||||||||||||||||||
NT-4 | V | V | ||||||||||||||||||
BDNF | V | V | ||||||||||||||||||
bFGF | V | V | V | V | ||||||||||||||||
IGF-1 | Λ | |||||||||||||||||||
IGFBP-1 | V | |||||||||||||||||||
VEGF | ||||||||||||||||||||
VEGF-R1 | Λ | |||||||||||||||||||
VEGF-R2 | Λ | |||||||||||||||||||
PIGF | ||||||||||||||||||||
Ang-1 | V | V | V | V | V | |||||||||||||||
Ang-2 | 9 | 2 | 3 | 11 | 4 |
This table summarizes Supplement Tables 2, 4, 6, and 8. Λ indicates increased risk of a Z-score < −1 on the executive dysfunction assessment(s) identified at the top of each column, while V indicates decreased risk. W indicates a DAS-II Working Memory Z-score ≤ −1, I indicates a NEPSY-II Inhibition-inhibition Z- score ≤ −1, S indicates a NEPSY-II Inhibition-Switching Z- score ≤ −1, and E indicates the executive dysfunction composite defined as a Z-score ≤ −1 on all three assessments.
2. Early and late epochs: single proteins
We evaluated the hypotheses that children who had a top quartile concentration of each protein on at least two days during the first two postnatal weeks (when specimens were obtained on days 1, 7, and 14) and during the second two postnatal weeks (when specimens were obtained on days 21 and 28) were not at greater risk of low scores on the working memory, inhibition-inhibition, and the inhibition-switching assessments, and of the EDC than children who had lower concentrations (Appendix Table 5 and summarized in Table 3).
Table 3.
Early epoch | Late epoch | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Proteins | W | I | S | E | W | I | S | E |
CRP | Λ | |||||||
SAA | ||||||||
MPO | ||||||||
IL-1β | ||||||||
IL-6 | ||||||||
IL-6R | ||||||||
TNF-α | Λ | Λ | Λ | |||||
TNF-R1 | ||||||||
TNF-R2 | ||||||||
IL-8 | Λ | Λ | Λ | Λ | Λ | |||
RANTES | ||||||||
ICAM-1 | Λ | |||||||
VCAM-1 | ||||||||
MMP-9 | ||||||||
TSH | Λ | |||||||
EPO | Λ | |||||||
NT-4 | ||||||||
BDNF | V | V | ||||||
bFGF | V | |||||||
IGF-1 | ||||||||
IGFBP-1 | ||||||||
VEGF | ||||||||
VEGF-R1 | V | Λ | Λ | |||||
VEGF-R2 | ||||||||
PIGF | ||||||||
Ang-1 | V | V | ||||||
Ang-2 |
This table summarizes Appendix Tables 3, 5, 7 and 9, which deal with elevated concentrations on two days during the early epoch (first two postnatal weeks) and during the late epoch (third and fourth two postnatal weeks). Λ indicates increased risk of a Z-score < −1 on the assessment(s) identified at the top of each column, while V indicates decreased risk. W indicates a DAS-II Working Memory Z-score ≤ −1, I indicates a NEPSY-II Inhibitioninhibition Z- score ≤ −1, S indicates a NEPSY-II Inhibition-Switching Z- score ≤ −1, and E indicates the executive dysfunction composite defined as a Z-score ≤ −1 on all three assessments.
3. Early and late epochs: Combinations of inflammation-related and angio-neurotrophic proteins
We evaluated the hypotheses that children who had top quartile concentrations of one or both of two proteins (one an inflammation-related protein and the other an angio-neurotroph) on at least two days during the first and second epochs were not at higher or lower risk of low scores on the working memory, inhibition-inhibition, and the inhibition-switching assessments, and of the EDC than children who had lower concentrations (Appendix Tables 6–13) and summarized in Table 4).
Table 4.
4a. early epoch | ||||||||||||
TNF-α | IL-8 | ICAM-1 | ||||||||||
Proteins | W | I | S | E | W | I | S | E | W | I | S | E |
IL-6R | V | V | V | V | V | V | ||||||
MMP-9 | V | Λ | V | Λ | V | |||||||
RANTES | V | V | V | V | V | V | ||||||
EPO | Λ | Λ | Λ | Λ | ||||||||
NT-4 | V | V | V | V | ||||||||
BDNF | V | V | V | V | V | V | ||||||
bFGF | Λ | V | V | V | V | |||||||
IGF-1 | V | V | V | V | V | |||||||
VEGF | V | V | V | V | V | V | V | V | ||||
VEGF-R1 | V | V | V | V | V | V | V | V | ||||
VEGF-R2 | V | V | V | V | V | |||||||
PIGF | V | V | V | V | V | V | V | |||||
Ang-1 | V | V | V | V | V | V | V | V | ||||
Ang-2 | ||||||||||||
4b. late epoch | ||||||||||||
TNF-α | IL-8 | ICAM-1 | ||||||||||
Proteins | W | I | S | E | W | I | S | E | W | I | S | E |
IL-6R | V | V | ||||||||||
MMP-9 | V | V | ||||||||||
RANTES | V | V | V | V | ||||||||
EPO | V | V | V | V | V | V | ||||||
NT-4 | V | V | V | V | ||||||||
BDNF | V | V | ||||||||||
bFGF | V | V | ||||||||||
IGF-1 | V | V | V | V | ||||||||
VEGF | V | V | ||||||||||
VEGF-R1 | V | V | V | Λ | V | |||||||
VEGF-R2 | Λ | V | ||||||||||
PIGF | V | V | V | V | ||||||||
Ang-1 | V | |||||||||||
Ang-2 | V | V | V | V |
The inflammation-related protein is identified at the top of each set of columns, and the neurotrophic proteins are identified on the left. Λ indicates increased risk of a Z-score < −1 on the executive dysfunction assessment(s) identified just below the inflammation-related protein, while V indicates decreased risk. W indicates a DAS-II Working Memory Z-score ≤ −1, I indicates a NEPSY-II Inhibition-inhibition Z- score ≤ −1, S indicates a NEPSY-II Inhibition-Switching Z- score ≤ −1, and E indicates the executive dysfunction composite defined as a Z-score ≤ −1 on all three assessments.
The concentrations of inflammation-related proteins in the ELGAN Study varied with gestational age, and with the postnatal day of collection (Leviton et al., 2011d; Leviton et al., 2012a). In addition, one set of measurements was made in 2009–2010 and the second in 2015, and, while the distributions of each were similar, they were not identical. Consequently, we divided our sample into 30 groups defined by three gestational age categories (23–24, 25–26, 27 weeks), five postnatal days of blood collection (1, 7, 14, 21 and 28), and two measurement sets (20092010, 2015). Because we were interested in the contribution of low and high concentrations, and the concentrations of most proteins did not follow a normal distribution, the distribution of each protein’s concentration was divided into four quartiles among children in each of the 30 groups.
In the ELGAN Study population, both low gestational age (Leviton et al., 2011d) and fetal growth restriction (Landis and Koch, 1977) were associated with protein concentrations, as well as with low IQ (Joseph et al., 2016). Consequently, we adjusted for gestational age category (23–24, 25–26, 27 weeks) and birth weight Z-score < −1. We created logistic regression models of the risk of our indicators of executive dysfunctions that enabled us to calculate odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals associated with protein concentrations in the top quartile relative to the risk among children with lower concentrations. We selected variables as confounders if identified in the literature or if in our data they were associated with both the exposure and the outcome with probabilities ≤ .25 (Dales and Ury, 1978). The final logistic regression models adjusted for gestational age category (23–24, 25–26, and 27 weeks) and birth weight Z-score < −1.
In separate multinomial logistic regression models, we evaluated the risk of each executive dysfunction associated with top quartile concentrations of two proteins simultaneously relative to the risk among children who had lower concentrations of both proteins. The indicator that an angiogenic, neurotrophic, or anti-inflammatory protein appears to have protective effects is a statistically significant increased risk of the executive dysfunction among children whose blood had a top quartile concentration of the inflammation-related protein and a lower concentration of the angiogenic, neurotrophic, or anti-inflammatory protein, while the risk was not increased among children whose blood had top quartile concentrations of both proteins
Enrollment and consent procedures for this follow up study were approved by the institutional review board of every participating institution.
3. Results
3.1. Sample description (Table 1b)
Of the 692 children who at age 10 years had an IQ ≥ 70, and an assessment of WM, inhibition and switching, 164 had a WM Z-score ≤ −1, while 350 had an inhibition Z-score ≤ −1, and 345 a switching Z-score ≤ −1. Only 104 children had a Z-score ≤ −1 on all three assessments, which we identify as the Executive Dysfunction Composite (EDC). Only 221 children had a Z-score >−1 on all three assessments.
3.2. Tables 2–4
Detailed results are included in the tables in the Supplement, and the details of these tables are summarized in Tables 2–4. Table 2 summarizes Supplement Tables 1–4, while Table 3 summarizes Supplement Table 5. Table 4 summarizes Supplement Tables 6 through 13.
3.2.1. Individual days (Table 2)
For each protein, we evaluated the relationship between elevated concentrations in specimens collected on each of five occasions and four indicators of executive dysfunction, for a total of 20 significance tests for each protein. We found associations between a high concentration of CRP, IL-6, TNF-α, and IL-8 on at least two days and one or more indicators of executive dysfunction. High concentrations of IL-6R, bFGF, BDNF, RANTES, and Ang-1 on at least two days were associated with a decreased risk of one or more executive function limitations.
3.2.2. Multiple days in the early and late epochs (Table 3)
3.2.3. Early epoch
We classify the first two postnatal weeks as the early epoch and the second two postnatal weeks as the late epoch, and classify children as having increased concentrations during an epoch if the top quartile concentrations were evident on two (or more) days during that epoch. In the early epoch, the risks of low WM Z-scores were increased among children who had elevated concentrations of TNF-α, IL-8, and EPO. For example, the odds ratio for a low WM score was 2.2 (95% confidence interval: 1.4, 3.3) among children who had elevated concentrations of TNF-α, and (OR: 1.6; (1.01, 2.6)) among children who had elevated concentrations of IL-8 on two days during the early epoch. Increased risks of low Z-scores on the inhibition assessment were associated with top-quartile concentrations of CRP (OR: 1.5 (1.02, 2.3)), while low Z-scores on the switching component were associated with top-quartile concentrations of TNF-α (OR: 1.6; (1.03, 2.3)) and TSH (OR: 1.5; (1.02, 2.2)). Increased risks of the EDC were associated with top quartile concentrations of TNF-α (OR: 1.8; (1.1, 2.9)) and IL-8 (OR: (1.9; (1.1, 3.2)) during the early epoch.
Reduced risks of low Z-scores on the inhibition assessment were associated with high concentrations of Ang-1 during the early epoch (OR: 0.6; (0.4, 0.9)), while reduced risks of the EDC were associated with top quartile concentrations of Ang-1 (OR: 0.4; (0.2, 0.8)).
3.2.4. Late epoch
The risks of low WM Z-scores were increased among children who had elevated concentrations of IL-8 (OR: 2.1; (1.1, 3.8)) and ICAM-1 (OR: 1.9; (1.03, 3.3)) on two days during the late epoch. Increased risks of low Z-scores on the inhibition assessment were associated with top-quartile concentrations of IL-8 (OR: 2.0; (1.1, 3.8)) and VEGFR1 (OR: 2.6; (1.3, 3.8)), while increased risks of low Z-scores on the switching assessment were associated with top-quartile concentrations of VEGF-R1 only (OR: 1.9; (1.03, 3.4)), and increased risks of the EDC were associated with top-quartile concentrations of IL-8 only (OR: 2.0; (1.03, 3.9)).
Top quartile concentrations of BDNF during the late epoch were associated with reduced risks of low Z-scores on the switching assessment (OR: 0.6; (0.3, 0.96)), as well as reduced risks of the EDC (OR: 0.4; (0.2, 0.9)).
3.3. Sets of inflammation and neurotrophic proteins (Table 4)
3.3.1. Early epoch
The increased risks of low scores on the WM and inhibition assessments and of the EDC associated with top quartile concentrations of TNF-α, and separately, associated with top quartile concentrations of IL-8, were moderated by top quartile concentrations of most neurotrophic proteins, whereas low scores on the switching assessment were not. The increased risks of low scores on the switching assessment and of the EDC associated with high concentrations of ICAM-1 were also moderated by top quartile concentrations of most neurotrophic proteins, whereas this was not the case for low scores on the WM and inhibition assessments.
The risk of the EDC (defined as a Z-score ≤ −1 on the working memory, inhibition, and switching assessments) associated with a top quartile concentration of TNF-α was lower when the high concentration of TNF-α was accompanied by a top quartile concentrations of each of 11 of the 14 proteins that had neurotrophic, angiogenic, or anti-inflammatory properties. Similarly, the risk of the EDC associated with a top quartile concentration of IL-8 was lower when the high concentration of TNF-α was accompanied by a top quartile concentrations of 12 of these 14 proteins, and the risk of the EDC associated with a top quartile concentration of ICAM-1 was lower when the high concentration of ICAM-1 was accompanied by a top quartile concentrations of 8 of these 14 proteins. With 95% confidence intervals (p < 0.05), 2 of these 42 ORs calculated would be statistically significant by chance alone. In contrast, we found that 31 ORs were statistically-significant, about 15 times more than expected.
3.3.2. Late epoch
The pattern of moderation by neurotrophins of increased risks associated with high concentrations of TNF-α during the early epoch is not seen in the late epoch. In contrast, low scores on all three assessments and EDC associated with high concentrations of IL-8 appear to be moderated by high concentrations of neurotrophic proteins. Similar, but less prominent examples of this modulation were associated with high concentrations of ICAM-1 and risks of low scores on assessments of WM, switching, and the EDC.
4. Discussion
4.1. What we found
Our main findings are that extremely low gestational age newborns who had sustained/recurrent systemic inflammation during the first postnatal month are at increased risk of impairments on assessments of WM, inhibition, the ability to switch tasks (and responses), and the EDC, and that these increased risks appear to be ameliorated by (presumably concomitant) abundant amounts of proteins with neurotrophic and angiogenic properties.
4.2. What others found in humans. How our findings are similar or different
We have not found any other report that assessed the contribution of neonatal systemic inflammation to the occurrence of executive dysfunctions at school age among children born extremely or very preterm. Consequently, we advise caution in drawing inferences from our findings.
4.3. Executive dysfunctions
4.3.1. Categorization of executive dysfunctions
No sharp break in assessment scores (or discontinuity) separates those with an executive dysfunction from their peers (Branum-Martin et al., 2013). The preference of epidemiologists to study categorical entities rather than continua is exemplified by the establishment of cut-offs for continuous measures of function/dysfunction (e.g., hypertension, diabetes mellitus, glaucoma). In keeping with this preference, we dichotomized Z-scores on the WM, inhibition, and switching assessments.
4.3.2. Executive dysfunctions in general or does the blood profile associated with one dysfunction differ from that of other limitations?
Each executive function limitation is most often characterized by increased risks associated with systemic inflammation, which is attenuated in the presence of high concentrations of neurotrophic proteins. The differences among the protein profiles of the three limitations are relatively minor in comparison to their similarities. This might reflect the tendency for the three dysfunctions to co-occur. On the other hand, the variations on a theme might also reflect some differences that if not unique, at least reveal biologic differences.
4.4. Categorization of exposure.
4.4.1. top-quartile concentrations
In the absence of information about what level of inflammation is needed to result in brain damage or increased risk of executive dysfunctions, we have assumed that relatively high levels are needed. Consequently, we have compared children whose concentrations of inflammation-related proteins are in the top quartile to children who had lower concentrations. This has allowed us to identify inflammation as an antecedent of ventriculomegaly when the newborn was in the intensive care nursery (Leviton et al., 2011b; Leviton et al., 2018b), and low Bayley Scales of Infant Development-II (O’Shea et al., 2012), an attention problem (O’Donnell et al., 2014), cerebral palsy (Kuban et al., 2014), and microcephaly (Leviton et al., 2011c) at age 2 years, and low IQ (Kuban et al., 2017), attention deficit hyperactive disorder at age 10 years (Allred et al., 2017), and learning limitations (Leviton et al., 2018a).
4.4.2. high concentrations on multiple days
Transient inflammation is less likely to damage the newborn EP brain than recurrent or sustained inflammation (Dammann and Leviton, 2014). In addition, systemic inflammation tends to be sustained over the first postnatal weeks in our ELGAN Study population. These observations prompted us to examine not only single-day elevated concentrations, but also elevated concentrations on multiple days.
4.5. Inflammation is associated with increased risk of executive dysfunctions
When we did not consider neurotrophins, we found that the risks of executive dysfunctions were associated with single-day high concentrations of IL-8 (Table 2: days 1 and 21), and only meager associations with top quartile, single-day concentrations of IL-6, TNF-α, TNF-R2, and ICAM-1. Recurrent or sustained high concentrations of TNF-α during the first two weeks, however, were associated with increased risks of executive dysfunction, while recurrent or sustained high concentrations of IL-8 during the first two weeks, and separately during the second two weeks were also associated with increased risks of executive dysfunction (Table 3). This documentation of the highest risks of damage/dysfunction when the inflammation is sustained or recurrent is in keeping with our experience with other disorders.(Dammann and Leviton, 2014; Kuban et al., 2015; Leviton et al., 2016b; Kuban et al., 2017) We now have additional evidence that in some cases, the inflammation can be sustained for a month.(Dammann et al., 2016)
4.5.1. Neurotrophins protect
We found strong evidence that elevated concentrations of neurotrophins modulate the risk of executive dysfunctions (Table 4). Indeed, the inference from the discrepancy between Tables 3 and 4 suggests that the potential contribution of inflammation to dysfunctions is best evaluated in light of the potentially modulating effects of neurotrophic, angiogenic, and anti-inflammatory influences.
4.5.2. Failure of neurotrophins to protect
In isolated instances, increased risk occurred when the concentrations of both the inflammatory and neurotrophic proteins were in the top quartile. This pattern has several interpretations. One is that high concentrations of both proteins are needed to cause damage. Another is that as a consequence of damage, both proteins are released into the circulation. The third interpretation borrows from both of these possibilities and postulates that the putative damage promoter does indeed promote damage, and in doing so also promotes the synthesis and/or release of the putative protector/repair-enhancer (Xing and Lo, 2017). Because we are unable to distinguish which of these three possibilities applies to our findings, we identify this pattern as the “damage cause and/or consequence” pattern.
4.6. Co-occurrence of inflammation and neurotrophic proteins
The term “help-me signaling” has been applied to the release by damaged or diseased neurons of inflammation and neurotrophic proteins that are able to recruit cells to assist in repair/regeneration.(Xing and Lo, 2017) The cooccurrence of elevated concentrations of inflammation and neurotrophic proteins has been documented in ELGANS.(Leviton et al., 2017) Others have emphasized the cross-talk between neurotrophins and inflammation.(da Silva Meirelles et al., 2017)
4.7. Developmental regulation
In general, compared to older children and adults, newborns tend to have diminished inflammatory and protective immune responses to inflammatory/infectious stimuli (Dammann and Leviton, 2014). This can be especially pronounced among those born preterm (Kan et al., 2016).
Despite differences in gestational age, newborns with the same postconceptional age have similar profiles of gene expression (Zasada et al., 2014). Nevertheless, among extremely low gestational age newborns, the lower the gestational age, the higher the concentrations of inflammation-related cytokines in the blood (Leviton et al., 2011d), while the availability of neurotrophins increases with gestational age (van Tilborg et al., 2016). The result is that the more “mature” the newborn (along the developmental regulation schedule), the more s/he should be able to resolve inflammation (Dammann and Leviton, 2014), and promote brain growth and well-being (Murase, 2014). With these thoughts in mind, we offer the possibility that the high concentrations of the proteins we measured are surrogates for proteins we did not measure, and for developmental processes we did not identify.
4.8. Limitations and strengths of this study
Perhaps our main weakness is the limited number of children in some analyses. With 445 children who provided two specimens for the late epoch (Appendix Table 3), only 28 children are expected to have top-quartile concentrations of both proteins (445 × .25 × .25). As half of all children had a low score on the switching assessment (345/692), we would expect only 14 of these 28 children to have a switching impairment. On occasion, however, the cell representing the top-quartile concentrations of both proteins might be empty or have only one child, by chance alone, resulting in our inability to estimate the confidence interval. So long as, such empty cells are accompanied by statistically-significantly high risks for the cell represented by the top quartile concentration of the inflammationrelated protein and a lower concentration of the neurotrophic protein, they can be viewed as support for the ability of neurotrophic proteins to modulate the increased risks associated with inflammation.
The interrelatedness of early and late systemic inflammation (Dammann et al., 2016), limits our ability to tease apart the contributions of each epoch to the occurrence of each dysfunction. We are also limited by the relatively small number of proteins measured. Inflammation is a broad and complex phenomenon (Zak and Aderem, 2009), and not one fully assessed by our methods. In addition, the proteins we measured might not be in the causal and/or repair chains, but merely surrogates for other proteins in their broad group. For example, although we found inflammatory signals for TNF-α, IL-8, and ICAM-1, we prefer not to focus on these specific proteins, but rather on broader inflammatory processes (Becher et al., 2017). Similarly, we view each neurotrophin as a surrogate for other proteins that possess neurotrophic characteristics (Oliveira et al., 2013).
We relied on blood specimens obtained for clinical indications. As their cardio-pulmonary function and blood gas exchange stabilized, infants were less likely than their sicker peers to have blood drawn on days 14, 21, and 28. Consequently, selection bias probably occurred to some extent.
What we measured was peripheral blood. The “periphery as a window to the brain” concept assumes that what is measured in the blood conveys valuable information about what is happening in the brain (Fernandes et al., 2015). Our study is further limited if this assumption is not true.
With more than 600 children and our exposure defined by a protein concentration in the top quartile, and identification of a WM limitation in 24% of children, we have power to appreciate risk ratios of 1.8 as statistically significant. Other strengths are the selection of infants based on gestational age (and not birth weight) (Arnold et al., 1991), prospective collection of all data, modest attrition, and finally, protein data of high quality (Fichorova et al., 2008), and high content validity (Fichorova et al., 2011; Hecht et al., 2011; Leviton et al., 2011d; McElrath et al., 2011)
4.9. Conclusions
In this sample of children born extremely preterm, systemic inflammation conveys information about heightened risks of executive dysfunctions, while high concentrations of neurotrophic proteins appear to modulate these increased risks.
Protein-concentration profiles in blood obtained during the first postnatal month discriminate minimally among the executive dysfunctions we assessed.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors express their gratitude to the children and their families who participated in this study. They also gratefully acknowledge the contributions of the ELGAN Study Investigators, listed below.
Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston MA
Janice Ware, Taryn Coster, Brandi Henson, Rachel Wilson, Kirsten McGhee, Patricia Lee, Aimee Asgarian, Anjali Sadhwani
Brigham and Women’s Hospital- Genital Tract Biology, Boston, MA
Hidemi Yamamoto, Stanthia Ryan, Damilola Junaid, Hassan Dawood, Noah Beatty, Ngan Luu, Vanessa Tang, Rosaria Rita Sassi, Jenna-Malia Pasicznyk
Tufts Medical Center, Boston MA
Ellen Perrin, Emily Neger, Kathryn Mattern, Jenifer Walkowiak, Susan Barron
University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester MA
Jean Frazier, Lauren Venuti, Beth Powers, Ann Foley, Brian Dessureau, Molly Wood, Jill Damon-Minow
Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT
Richard Ehrenkranz, Jennifer Benjamin, Elaine Romano, Kathy Tsatsanis, Katarzyna Chawarska, Sophy Kim, Susan Dieterich, Karen Bearrs
Wake Forest University Baptist Medical Center, Winston-Salem NC
T. Michael O’Shea, Nancy Peters, Patricia Brown, Emily Ansusinha, Ellen Waldrep, Jackie Friedman, Gail Hounshell, Debbie Allred (deceased)
University Health Systems of Eastern Carolina, Greenville, NC
Stephen C. Engelke, Nancy Darden-Saad, Gary Stainback
North Carolina Children’s Hospital, Chapel Hill, NC
Diane Warner, Janice Wereszczak, Janice Bernhardt, Joni McKeeman, Echo Meyer
Helen DeVos Children’s Hospital, Grand Rapids, MI
Steve Pastyrnak, Wendy Burdo-Hartman, Julie Rathbun, Sarah Nota, Teri Crumb,
Sparrow Hospital, Lansing, MI
Madeleine Lenski, Deborah Weiland, Megan Lloyd
University of Chicago Medical Center, Chicago, IL
Scott Hunter, Michael Msall, Rugile Ramoskaite, Suzanne Wiggins, Krissy Washington, Ryan Martin, Barbara Prendergast, Megan Scott
William Beaumont Hospital, Royal Oak, MI
Judith Klarr, Beth Kring, Jennifer DeRidder, Kelly Vogt
Financial Support:
This study was supported by grants from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (5U01NS040069; 2R01NS040069), the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (5P30HD018655), The National Eye Institute (1-R01-EY021820), and the Office of the NIH Director (1UG3OD023348).
Abbreviations
- Ang-1
Angiopoietin-1
- Ang-2
Angiopoietin-2
- BDNF
Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor
- bFGF
basic Fibroblast Growth Factor
- CRP
C-Reactive Protein
- DAS-II
Differential Ability Scales–II
- EP
Extremely preterm
- EPO
Erythropoietin
- ICAM-1
Intercellular Adhesion Molecule-1
- IGF-1
Insulin-like growth factor-1
- IGFBP-1
Insulin-like growth factor binding protein-1
- IL-1β
Interleukin-1β
- IL-6
Interleukin-6
- IL-6R
Interleukin-6 Receptor
- IL-8
Interleukin-8
- KBIT-2
Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test– 2
- MMP-9
Matrix Metalloproteinase-9
- MPO
Myeloperoxidase
- NEPSY-II
A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment
- NT-4
Neurotrophin-4
- PIGF
Placenta Growth Factor
- RANTES
Regulated upon Activation, Normal T-cell Expressed, and Secreted
- SAA
Serum Amyloid A
- TNF-R1
Tumor Necrosis Factor-α Receptor-1
- TNF-R2
Tumor Necrosis Factor-α Receptor-2
- TNF-α
Tumor Necrosis Factor-α
- TSH
Thyroid-Stimulating Hormone
- VCAM-1
Vascular Cell Adhesion Molecule-1
- VEGF
Vascular endothelial growth factor
- VEGF-R1
Vascular endothelial growth factor Receptor-1
- VEGF-R2
Vascular endothelial growth factor Receptor-2
- DAS-II
Differential Ability Scales–II
- WM
Working memory
- NEPSY-II
A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment-II
- EDC
Executive Dysfunction Composite
Footnotes
5. Competing interests:
The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Conflict of Interest Statement:
The authors report no conflict of interest, financial or otherwise.
6. References
- Allred EN, Dammann O, Fichorova RN, Hooper SR, Hunter SJ, Joseph RM, Kuban K, Leviton A, O’Shea TM, Scott MN (2017) Systemic Inflammation during the First Postnatal Month and the Risk of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Characteristics among 10 year-old Children Born Extremely Preterm. Journal of neuroimmune pharmacology : the official journal of the Society on NeuroImmune Pharmacology 12:531543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold CC, Kramer MS, Hobbs CA, McLean FH, Usher RH (1991) Very low birth weight: a problematic cohort for epidemiologic studies of very small or immature neonates. Am J Epidemiol 134:604–613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becher B, Spath S, Goverman J (2017) Cytokine networks in neuroinflammation. Nat Rev Immunol 17:49–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjorkesten J, Enroth S, Shen Q, Wik L, Hougaard DM, Cohen AS, Sorensen L, Giedraitis V, Ingelsson M, Larsson A, Kamali-Moghaddam M, Landegren U (2017) Stability of Proteins in Dried Blood Spot Biobanks. Molecular & cellular proteomics : MCP 16:1286–1296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bose CL, Laughon MM, Allred EN, O’Shea TM, Van Marter LJ, Ehrenkranz RA, Fichorova RN, Leviton A (2013) Systemic inflammation associated with mechanical ventilation among extremely preterm infants. Cytokine 61:315–322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branum-Martin L, Fletcher JM, Stuebing KK (2013) Classification and identification of reading and math disabilities: the special case of comorbidity. Journal of learning disabilities 46:490–499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chew LJ, DeBoy CA (2016) Pharmacological approaches to intervention in hypomyelinating and demyelinating white matter pathology. Neuropharmacology 110:605–625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; da Silva Meirelles L, Simon D, Regner A (2017) Neurotrauma: The Crosstalk between Neurotrophins and Inflammation in the Acutely Injured Brain. International journal of molecular sciences 18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dales LG, Ury HK (1978) An improper use of statistical significance testing in studying covariables. Int J Epidemiol 7:373–375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dammann O, Leviton A (2014) Intermittent or sustained systemic inflammation and the preterm brain. Pediatr Res. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dammann O, Allred EN, Fichorova RN, Kuban K, O’Shea TM, Leviton A (2016) Duration of Systemic Inflammation in the First Postnatal Month Among Infants Born Before the 28th Week of Gestation. Inflammation 39:672–677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott CD (2007) Differential Ability Scales, 2nd Edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Farooqi A, Adamsson M, Serenius F, Hagglof B (2016) Executive Functioning and Learning Skills of Adolescent Children Born at Fewer than 26 Weeks of Gestation. PLoS ONE 11:e0151819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandes BS, Steiner J, Berk M, Molendijk ML, Gonzalez-Pinto A, Turck CW, Nardin P, Goncalves CA (2015) Peripheral brain-derived neurotrophic factor in schizophrenia and the role of antipsychotics: meta-analysis and implications. Molecular psychiatry 20:1108–1119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fichorova RN, Beatty N, Sassi RRS, Yamamoto HS, Allred EN, Leviton A, investigators ftE (2015) Systemic inflammation in the extremely low gestational age newborn following maternal genitourinary infections. American Journal of Reproductive Immunology 73:162–174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fichorova RN, Onderdonk AB, Yamamoto H, M.L. D, Dubois AM, Allred E, Leviton A, Investigators ftES (2011) Maternal microbe-specific modulation of inflammatory response in extremely low gestational age newborns. mBio 2:e00280–00210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fichorova RN et al. (2008) Biological and technical variables affecting immunoassay recovery of cytokines from human serum and simulated vaginal fluid: a multicenter study. Anal Chem 80:4741–4751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman NP, Miyake A (2017) Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior 86:186–204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hecht JL, Fichorova RN, Tang VF, Allred EN, McElrath TF, Leviton A (2011) Relationship between neonatal blood protein profiles and placenta histologic characteristics in ELGANs. Pediatr Research 69:68–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joseph RM, O’Shea TM, Allred EN, Heeren T, Hirtz D, Jara H, Leviton A, Kuban KC (2016) Neurocognitive and Academic Outcomes at Age 10 Years of Extremely Preterm Newborns. Pediatrics 137:pii: e20154343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalpakidou AK, Allin MP, Walshe M, Giampietro V, McGuire PK, Rifkin L, Murray RM, Nosarti C (2014) Functional neuroanatomy of executive function after neonatal brain injury in adults who were born very preterm. PLoS One 9:e113975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kan B, Razzaghian HR, Lavoie PM (2016) An Immunological Perspective on Neonatal Sepsis. Trends in molecular medicine 22:290–302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp S (1998) NEPSY: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment.. New York: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Krityakiarana W, Zhao PM, Nguyen K, Gomez-Pinilla F, Kotchabhakdi N, de Vellis J, Espinosa-Jeffrey A (2016) Proof-of Concept that an Acute Trophic Factors Intervention After Spinal Cord Injury Provides an Adequate Niche for Neuroprotection, Recruitment of Nestin-Expressing Progenitors and Regeneration. Neurochemical research 41:431–449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuban KC, O’Shea TM, Allred EN, Paneth N, Hirtz D, Fichorova RN, Leviton A (2014) Systemic inflammation and cerebral palsy risk in extremely preterm infants. J Child Neurol 29:1692–1698.24646503 [Google Scholar]
- Kuban KC, O’Shea TM, Allred EN, Fichorova RN, Heeren T, Paneth N, Hirtz D, Dammann O, Leviton A (2015) The breadth and type of systemic inflammation and the risk of adverse neurological outcomes in extremely low gestation newborns. Pediatr Neurol 52:42–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuban KC, Joseph RM, O’Shea TM, Heeren T, Fichorova RN, Douglass L, Jara H, Frazier JA, Hirtz D, Rollins JV, Paneth N (2017) Circulating Inflammatory-Associated Proteins in the First Month of Life and Cognitive Impairment at Age 10 Years in Children Born Extremely Preterm. J Pediatr 180:116–123 e111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landis JR, Koch GG (1977) The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33:159–174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leviton A, Allred EN, Yamamoto H, Fichorova RN, Investigators. ftES (2012a) Relationships among the concentrations of 25 inflammation-associated proteins during the first postnatal weeks in the blood of infants born before the 28th week of gestation.. Cytokine 57:182–190.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leviton A, Hecht JL, Allred EN, Yamamoto H, Fichorova RN, Dammann O (2011a) Persistence after birth of systemic inflammation associated with umbilical cord inflammation. J Reprod Immunol 90:235–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leviton A, O’Shea TM, Bednarek FJ, Allred EN, Fichorova RN, Dammann O (2012b) Systemic responses of preterm newborns with presumed or documented bacteraemia. Acta Paediatr 101:355–359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leviton A, Allred EN, Fichorova RN, Kuban KC, O’Shea TM, Dammann O (2016a) Antecedents of inflammation biomarkers in preterm newborns on days 21 and 28. Acta Paediatr 105:274–280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leviton A, Allred EN, Fichorova RN, Kuban KC, Michael O’Shea T, Dammann O (2016b) Systemic inflammation on postnatal days 21 and 28 and indicators of brain dysfunction 2years later among children born before the 28th week of gestation. Early Hum Dev 93:25–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leviton A, Kuban K, O’Shea TM, Paneth N, Fichorova R, Allred EN, Dammann O (2011b) The Relationship between early concentrations of 25 blood proteins and cerebral white matter injury in preterm newborns: The ELGAN Study. J Pediatr 158:897–903.e895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leviton A, Kuban KC, Allred EN, Fichorova RN, O’Shea TM, Paneth N, Investigators ES (2011c) Early postnatal blood concentrations of inflammation-related proteins and microcephaly two years later in infants born before the 28th post-menstrual week. Early Hum Dev 87:325–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leviton A, Allred EN, Yamamoto H, Fichorova RN, Kuban K, O’Shea TM, Dammann O (2017) Antecedents and correlates of blood concentrations of neurotrophic growth factors in very preterm newborns. Cytokine 94:21–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leviton A, Dammann O, Allred EN, Joseph RM, Fichorova RN, O’Shea TM, Kuban KCK (2018a) Neonatal systemic inflammation and the risk of low scores on measures of reading and mathematics achievement at age 10 years among children born extremely preterm. Int J Dev Neurosci 66:45–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leviton A, Allred EN, Fichorova RN, O’Shea TM, Fordham LA, Kuban KCK, Dammann O (2018b) Circulating biomarkers in extremely preterm infants associated with ultrasound indicators of brain damage. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leviton A, Fichorova R, Yamamoto Y, Allred EN, Dammann O, Hecht J, Kuban K, McElrath T, O’Shea TM, Paneth N (2011d) Inflammation-related proteins in the blood of extremely low gestational age newborns. The contribution of inflammation to the appearance of developmental regulation. Cytokine 53:66–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McElrath TF, Fichorova RN, Allred EN, Hecht JL, Ismail MA, Yuan H, Leviton A (2011) Blood protein profiles of infants born before 28 weeks differ by pregnancy complication. Am J Obstet Gynecol 204:418.e411418.e412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murase S (2014) A new model for developmental neuronal death and excitatory/inhibitory balance in hippocampus. Mol Neurobiol 49:316–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Donnell KJ, Glover V, Barker ED, O’Connor TG (2014) The persisting effect of maternal mood in pregnancy on childhood psychopathology. Development and psychopathology 26:393–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Shea TM, Allred EN, Dammann O, Hirtz D, Kuban KC, Paneth N, Leviton A (2009) The ELGAN study of the brain and related disorders in extremely low gestational age newborns. Early Hum Dev 85:719–725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Shea TM, Allred EN, Kuban K, Dammann O, Paneth N, Fichorova R, Hirtz D, Leviton A, Investigators FtES (2012) Elevated concentrations of inflammation-related proteins in postnatal blood predict severe developmental delay at two years in extremely premature infants. J Pediatr 160:395–401.e394 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliveira SL, Pillat MM, Cheffer A, Lameu C, Schwindt TT, Ulrich H (2013) Functions of neurotrophins and growth factors in neurogenesis and brain repair. Cytometry Part A : the journal of the International Society for Analytical Cytology 83:76–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor HG, Clark CA (2016) Executive function in children born preterm: Risk factors and implications for outcome. Semin Perinatol. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; van Tilborg E, Heijnen CJ, Benders MJ, van Bel F, Fleiss B, Gressens P, Nijboer CH (2016) Impaired oligodendrocyte maturation in preterm infants: Potential therapeutic targets. Prog Neurobiol 136:28–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xing C, Lo EH (2017) Help-me signaling: Non-cell autonomous mechanisms of neuroprotection and neurorecovery. Prog Neurobiol 152:181–199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yudkin PL, Aboualfa M, Eyre JA, Redman CW, Wilkinson AR (1987) New birthweight and head circumference centiles for gestational ages 24 to 42 weeks. Early Hum Dev 15:45–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zak DE, Aderem A (2009) Systems biology of innate immunity. Immunol Rev 227:264–282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zasada M, Kwinta P, Durlak W, Bik-Multanowski M, Madetko-Talowska A, Pietrzyk JJ (2014) Development and maturation of the immune system in preterm neonates: results from a whole genome expression study. BioMed research international 2014:498318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.