Skip to main content
Schizophrenia Bulletin logoLink to Schizophrenia Bulletin
. 2018 Mar 9;45(2):377–385. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sby027

Hyper- and Hypomentalizing in Patients with First-Episode Schizophrenia: fMRI and Behavioral Studies

Vibeke Bliksted 1,2,3,, Chris Frith 4, Poul Videbech 5, Birgitte Fagerlund 6,7, Charlotte Emborg 1, Arndis Simonsen 1,2, Andreas Roepstorff 2,3, Daniel Campbell-Meiklejohn 8
PMCID: PMC6403062  PMID: 29534245

Abstract

Background

Historically, research investigating neural correlates of mentalizing deficits in schizophrenia has focused on patients who have been ill for several years with lengthy exposure to medication. Little is known about the neural and behavioral presentations of theory-of-mind deficits in schizophrenia, shortly after the first episode of psychosis.

Methods

We investigated social cognition in 17 recently diagnosed first-episode schizophrenia (FES) patients with little or no exposure to antipsychotic medication and 1:1 matched healthy controls. We recorded behavioral and neural responses to the Animated Triangles Task (ATT), which is a nonverbal validated mentalizing task that measures the ascription of intentionality to the movements of objects.

Results

FES patients under-interpreted social cues and over-interpreted nonsocial cues. These effects were influenced by current intelligence (IQ). Control group and FES neural responses replicated earlier findings in healthy adults. However, a region of anterior medial prefrontal cortex (amPFC) of FES patients showed a different response pattern to that of controls. Unlike healthy controls, patients increased activity in this social cognition region while studying “random” movements of shapes, as compared to the study of movements normally interpreted as “intentional”.

Conclusions

Mentalizing deficits in FES consists of hypo- and hypermentalizing. The neural pattern of FES patients is consistent with deficits in the ability to switch off mentalizing processes in potentially social contexts, instead increasing them when intentionality is not forthcoming. Overall, results demonstrate complexities of theory of mind deficits in schizophrenia that should be considered when offering social cognitive training programs.

Keywords: hypermentalizing, hypomentalizing, theory of mind, first-episode schizophrenia, social cognition, fMRI

Introduction

During recent years, there has been an increased focus on social cognitive deficits as core deficits in schizophrenia with a considerable impact on functional outcome.1–3 Specifically, theory of mind (ToM), social perception, social knowledge, attributional bias, and emotional processing have all been recognized as domains of particular interest in schizophrenia.4,5 In a meta-analysis of these social cognitive domains in schizophrenia, Savla et al6 concluded that theory of mind and social perception were the domains most severely affected.

Yet, patients with schizophrenia are a heterogeneous group and many speculations have been made regarding possible social cognitive subgroups.2,7–9 In this article, we refer to “mentalizing,” the act of inferring the mental states of others, which enables us to predict their actions.10 It has been suggested that one subgroup of patients perform poorly in social cognitive tests due to reduced ToM abilities (hypomentalizing) as seen in patients with autism, while another subgroup of patients perform poorly due to social interpretation of nonsocial events (hypermentalizing). The latter group has been associated with paranoia while the former group has been associated with negative symptoms.11,12 It has also been suggested that patients may in fact be hypo- and hypermentalizing at the same time.11–13 For example, a paranoid patient can over-interpret a neutral, casual interaction like people randomly passing by on the street as “evidence” of being persecuted by the government. At the same time, the patient can perceive only the literal meaning of a spoken dialogue and overlook sarcasm. This complexity is yet to be resolved.

In earlier literature, many brain regions have been consistently associated with various aspects of social cognition. These include anterior medial prefrontal cortex (amPFC), the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), temporal poles, the precuneus, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), superior temporal gyrus (STG), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), amygdala, fusiform face area (FFA), inferior parietal lobule (IPL), premotor cortex, anterior hippocampus, dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC), and ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC).14,15

More specifically, attribution of other people’s mental states, ToM, has been associated with a specific neural network of brain regions: the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), the left and right temporoparietal junction (TPJ), the temporal poles, the precuneus (PC), and the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC).14–17

Recently, Schilbach et al18 used a meta-analytically defined mentalizing network in a hypothesis-driven manner to compare functional connectivity in patients with schizophrenia and age- and gender-matched healthy controls based on Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) data. The researchers found indications of decreased functional connectivity between regions involved in mentalizing in schizophrenia patients. However, they found no significant associations between connectivity and symptoms, duration of illness, and chlorpromazine-equivalents. Literature reviews of functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) studies have also identified deficits within the mentalizing networks in schizophrenia.19,20

Many different fMRI paradigms have been used to measure ToM. In this study, we chose to use the Animated Triangles Task (ATT) which is a well validated nonverbal ToM paradigm.16,21,22 In the ATT, subjects are shown small film clips of 2 triangles moving either in a random way or interacting with apparent intentionality.

The classic version of AT, which we used in this study, has been used previously to investigate ToM brain activity in schizophrenia. For example, Das et al23 found that compared to the healthy control subjects, male patients with schizophrenia had a reduced neural activity in the right superior temporal gyrus (STG), temporoparietal junction (TPJ), and inferior frontal gyri (IFG). The authors suggested that this reduction of activity could reflect an impairment of reasoning about mental states of others, or “mentalizing.”24 In a similar vein, Koelkebeck et al25 did voxel-based morphometry analysis based on MRI scans. They found that in patients with schizophrenia, ToM deficits from the behavioral responses to the ATT correlated with gray matter volume reductions in pSTS and mPFC.

In a recent study, Martin et al26 identified a common network in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls that separate the viewing of intentional and random ATT animations. However, behavioral data showed that patients performed worse on the ToM tasks compared to the controls. It was concluded that mentalizing deficits in schizophrenia may be due to inefficient connections within these social brain networks.26

All studies thus far of ToM deficits on the ATT have been conducted on patients with lengthy exposure to medication. However, first-episode patients show differences in cognitive abilities compared to long-term patients,13,27 that could reflect long-term medication effects and/or the progression of the disorder.

In this study, we examined the neural basis of ToM impairments in recently diagnosed patients with first-episode schizophrenia (FES) receiving no or sparse antipsychotic medication. The patients had not been medicated for more than a maximum of 6 weeks over their lifetime, and had not been stigmatized by a diagnosis of schizophrenia. If ToM deficits are present early in the course of the disorder, we should be able to detect them at this early stage. Neural correlates would provide insight into how those deficits materialize. Our prediction, based on a recent meta-analysis of the ATT in patients with autism and schizophrenia,13 was that variability of performance on ToM tasks in FES are due to deficits of appropriate neural responses to social (hypomentalizing) and nonsocial (hypermentalizing) stimuli.

Methods and Materials

Subject Recruitment

Patients were recruited from OPUS, Clinic for people with schizophrenia, which is an intensive 2-year early-intervention program consisting of assertive community treatment, cognitive behavioral therapy, psychoeducational family treatment, and social skills training.28,29 Patients were recruited and tested a few days after receiving a diagnosis of schizophrenia and being included in OPUS, except for one patient who had been included for 403 days but never received any medication. Healthy control subjects were recruited via advertisements in 4 local newspapers.

Patients

Patients with FES were included in the study if they met the ICD-10 (International Classification of Disease 10th edition, WHO) criteria for schizophrenia; had no neurological disorder or severe head trauma according to ICD-10; or an ICD-10 diagnosis of drug- or alcohol-dependency. Patients were excluded if they had an estimated premorbid IQ <70 based on previous history or if they were not able to understand spoken Danish sufficiently to comprehend testing procedures. Patients had received less than 6 weeks of lifetime pharmacological treatment before the diagnostic interview. In all, 23 patients between the ages 18 and 30 were included. Four patients were unable to complete the fMRI scans due to worsening of their psychotic symptoms caused by scanner noise. Two patients stopped the scan midway. The 17 patients included in the fMRI analysis had the following medication histories: 8 patients were antipsychotic-naïve; 9 patients had been medicated with a low dose of atypical antipsychotics for less than 6 weeks; 4 patients had been medicated for less than 6 weeks with an antidepressant (1 without an antipsychotic), 3 received hypnotics (2 in combination with an antipsychotic).

Healthy Control Subjects

Healthy control subjects were matched one-to-one to patients on age, gender, handedness, educational level (based on the patients’ last commenced educational level), community of residence and parental socio economic status based on the highest parental education and expected parental income according to Statistics Denmark regarding wages (www.dst.dk/en). Healthy control subjects were excluded if they had a history of mental illness (self or among first-degree relatives), had psychotic symptoms, had a history of severe head injury or neurological illness (meeting ICD 10 criteria), or an ICD-10 diagnosis of drug- or alcohol-dependency. Nineteen healthy control subjects were included, however, one was excluded due to scanner problems, and one was excluded because the matched patient did not complete the session. This left 17 control subjects in the analysis, paired with 17 patients.

Ethics

All participants in this study received written and verbal information about the project and a written informed consent was obtained before inclusion. The study was approved by the Central Denmark Region Commitee on Health Research Ethics (Ref: M- 2009-0035) and the Danish Data Protection Agency. The project complied with the Helsinki-II-declaration.

Procedure

The patients underwent neuropsychological testing and were scanned with fMRI by VB a few days after the OPUS inclusion. Patients performed the ATT twice, once outside the scanner and once inside the scanner. Seven of the patients were psychologically tested (outside the scanner) at home and 10 were tested at VB’s office at Aarhus University Hospital Risskov.

Intelligence

Premorbid intelligence was estimated using DART (Danish Adult Reading Test), which is a Danish version of NART (The Nelson Adult Reading Test).30 The test consists of 50 rare words, which the subjects are asked to read aloud, and the number of correct pronunciations are scored. The NART has been shown to be a valid and reliable estimation of premorbid intelligence in schizophrenia.31,32 Estimation of current intelligence was done using 4 subtests from WAIS-III (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, third edition).33 The 4 subtests were chosen based on high correlation with the total WAIS-III IQ-score: Matrix Reasoning, Block Design, Vocabulary, Similarities.34

Psychopathology, Clinical Measures, and Drug Screening

At inclusion to the OPUS Clinic all FES patients were interviewed with the PSE-interview (Present State Examination, ICD-10) regarding Schizophrenia and drug dependency by psychiatrists.35 All healthy controls were interviewed with the entire PSE interview. All patients were rated with SANS and SAPS (Scale for the Assessment of Negative/Positive Symptoms).36,37 All subjects were tested for recent drug use using urine samples (testing for amphetamine, benzodiazepines, cannabis, codeine, morphine, cocaine) on the day of the fMRI scan. The neurocognitive testing and measures of psychopathology were done 1–3 days ahead of the fMRI scan.

Animated Triangles Task (ATT)

The ATT16,21 consists of short movie clips with 2 animated triangles. In the “random” movement condition, the triangles move in an arbitrary way, eg, bouncing. In the “intentional” (ToM) condition, 2 triangles interact in a socially complex way where one triangle appears, to most observers, to influence the mental state of the other triangle. The behavioral paradigm consisted of 8 animation clips, 4 of each condition lasting 38–41 s each. After each clip, the subjects were asked to tell what they thought was happening in the clips. Answers were recorded and transcribed. Two clinical psychologists, who were blinded to the subjects’ group status, evaluated each answer and mean scores were calculated for each subject. The subjects’ answers were scored regarding intentionality (degree of mental state attribution, range 0–5); and accuracy (how accurate was the description, range 0–3) as outlined by Castelli et al.16 Inter-rater agreement was moderate to almost perfect (intentionality for random animations: κ = 0.72, Z = 4.82, P < .0001; intentionality for ToM animations: κ = 0.85, Z = 9.69, P < .0001; accuracy for random animations: κ = 0.66, Z = 5.77, P < .0001; accuracy for ToM animations: κ = 0.51, Z = 7.37, P < .0001) (figure 1).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

The Animated Triangles Task (ATT). The scanning paradigm consisted of 4 blocks. Each block contained a presentation of a random and aToM movement sequence. Each clip lasted from 38 to 41 s. In addition, each block contained a still picture of a triangle scene, for 5 s. After each animation, subjects were asked a yes/no question (lasting 4 s) to ensure subjects paid attention to the task (supplementary material). Subjects pressed a button on a response box to indicate their response. There were no more than 128 s between 2 stimuli of the same type and condition. Stimuli were back projected onto a screen that could be seen by the subject in the scanner by way of a mirror placed above their eyes.

Behavioral Data Analysis

Statistical analysis was done with Stata IC 14 (64-bit) software. Patients with first-episode schizophrenia and controls (N = 17 pairs) were compared with regard to demographics, psychopathology, IQ, and social cognition. Continuous variables were examined by Wilcoxon rank-sum test (Mann–Whitney) and reported with mean and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Effect sizes of the continuous variables were reported by Harrell’s C and 95% CIs. Harrell’s C is a rank parameter measuring the ordinal predictive power of a model. Categorical variables were examined by Fisher’s exact test and reported with the counts and proportions of the total group in percentages. The social cognitive data were further analyzed by linear regression using current IQ as a co-variate.

MRI Acquisition

MRI imaging used a Siemens Magnetom Tim Trio scanner with a 16 channel head coil (Erlangen, Germany) at the Danish Neuroscience Centre. One hundred seventy-six slice whole brain T1 weighted images (265 × 256, 1 mm voxels, echo time [TE] 2.52 ms; repetition time [TR] 1900 ms) were obtained for anatomical registration of functional scans. Functional data were collected as T2-weighted echo planar images (EPI) in an interleaved slice acquisition order. Each volume (96 × 96 matrix, 2 mm voxels; TE, 27 ms; TR, 3300 ms) contained 61 slices.

Image Preprocessing

Preprocessing was carried out with FEAT v. 6.0 from FMRIB’s Software Library (FSL).38 Brain matter was segmented from nonbrain using a mesh deformation approach.39 High pass temporal filtering was applied using a Gaussian-weighted running lines filter, with a cut-off of 205s (twice the maximum period between trials of the same type).40 Each volume was motion corrected and smoothed with a Gaussian filter (full-width half-maximum of 5 mm). Independent component analysis was used to visually identify and remove obvious artifacts in the data using Multivariate Exploratory Linear Optimized Decomposition into Independent Components (MELODIC) software.41

FMRI Design

Single Subject Analysis

FSL (the FMRIB Software Library) was used to analyze the data.42 The general linear model contained 4 boxcar regressors for periods during: intentional movement videos, random movement videos, a still picture baseline, and question periods (see supplementary material for detail). Regressors were convolved with the default FSL hemodynamic response function (gamma function, delay = 6s, standard deviation = 3s) and filtered by the same high pass filter as the data. Contrasts were set up between the random and ToM movement conditions. Images were linearly registered to T1 structural images and standard MNI space.

Group Analysis

Patients were matched one-to-one with their control in a 2-sample paired t-test using a model that included a contrast between groups and additional dummy regressors for each pair of subjects. Patient groups were also analyzed separately. Group-level analysis was carried out with FLAME 1 + 243 with automatic outlier reduction. The Z statistic maps were cluster corrected (contiguous clusters conservatively defined by voxels with a conservative Z > 3) with a whole brain cluster significance level of P < .05.44–47 Correlations between neural responses to ATT conditions and positive/negative symptoms was also investigated (see supplementary material).

Results

Demographics, Psychopathology, IQ, and Social Cognition

Demographics, psychopathology, IQ, and social cognition are summarized in table 1. As expected FES subjects and healthy controls did not differ in age and gender. In spite of our carefully match based on parental education and social economic class and the last commenced education of the subjects the FES subjects had almost 3 years less education than the HCs (FES: 12.35, 95% CI [11.07, 13.64]; HC:15.18, 95% CI [13.66, 16.69], Z = −0.36, P = .72). Ten FES patients were unemployed and 2 on sick leave while all HC’s were either students or had a job. This could probably be explained by the fact that the FES subjects on average had experienced psychotic symptoms for several years (mean duration of untreated illness 13.35, 95% CI [9.40, 17.31] years). These symptoms were, eg, reported as hearing voices since kindergarten. The FES patients had a surprisingly long duration of untreated illness. Responses reflected duration of psychotic or psychotic-like symptoms, mainly auditory hallucinations. Future research may wish to elaborate on this and ask subjects of ultra-high risk criteria or basic symptoms.48

Table 1.

Comparison of Patients with First-Episode Schizophrenia (FES) and Controls on Demographics, Psychopathology, IQ, and Social Cognition

First-Episode Schizophrenia (N = 17) Healthy Controls (N = 17) Harrell’s C P Value
Age 23.94 (21.76, 26.11) 23.59 (21.50, 25.68) 0.46 (0.25, 0.68) .72a
Females 5 (29.41) 5 (29.41) 1.00b
Years of education 12.35 (11.07, 13.64) 15.18 (13.66, 16.69) 0.77 (0.61, 0.93) .01a
Current occupation <.01b
 Unemployed 10 (58.82) 0
 Work 0 6 (35.29)
 Student 5 (29.41) 11 (64.71)
 Sick leave 2 (11.76) 0
Days of FES-diagnosis 41.41 (3, 403)c
Years of untreated illness 13.35 (9.40, 17.31)
SANSd 9.76 (7.02, 12.51)
SAPSe 14.71 (12.63, 16.78)
DART (Est pre IQ)f 32 (30.32, 33.68) 34.35 (31.56, 37.15) 0.69 (0.49, 0.89) .06a
WAIS-III (Est func IQ)g 92.29 (80.68, 103.91) 112.47 (106.19, 118.75) 0.77 (0.60, 0.94) .01a
Animated triangles
 Intentionality ToM 12.06 (9.73, 14.39) 15.38 (14.11, 16.65) 0.72 (0.53, 0.90) .03a
 Intentionality random 1.32 (0.52, 2.13) 0.32 (−0.04, 0.68) 0.32 (0.15, 0.48) .04a
 Accuracy ToM 6.97 (5.77, 8.17) 9.29 (8.63, 9.96) 0.79 (0.62, 0.96) <.01a
 Accuracy random 10.24 (9.16, 11.32) 11.82 (11.67, 11.98) 0.72 (0.55, 0.89) .01a

Note: Continuous variables were examined by Wilcoxon rank-sum test (Mann–Whitney) and reported with mean (95% CI) and effect size by terms of Harrell’s C (95% CI). Categorical variables were examined by Fisher’s exact test and reported with the counts and proportions of group total, N (percentage).

aMann–Whitney test.

bFisher’s exact test.

cMinimum and maximum values.

dSANS, Scale for Assessment of Negative Symptoms.

eSAPS, Scale for Assessment of Positive Symptoms.

fDART, Danish Adult Reading test.

gWechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (Matrix Reasoning, Block Design, Vocabulary, Similarities).

FES subjects and HCs did not differ in estimated premorbid IQ (DART—FES: 32, 95% CI [30.32, 33.68]; HC: 34.35, 95% CI [31.56, 37.15], Z = 1.87, P = .06). However, they differed in estimated current IQ (WAIS—FES: 92.29, 95% CI [80.68, 103.91]; HC: 112.47, 95% CI [106.19, 118.75], Z = 2.72, P = .01).

FES subjects saw less intentionality compared to the HCs in the intentional movement animations (FES: 12.06, 95 % CI [9.73, 14.39]; HC: 15.38, 95% CI [14.11, 16.65], Z = 2.18, P = .03) which might be due to hypomentalizing. Importantly, they also saw more intentionality in the random animations than the HCs which might be due to hypermentalizing (FES: 1.32, 95% CI [0.52, 2.13]; HC: 0.32, 95% CI [−0.04, 0.68], Z = −2.09, P = .04) (figure 2).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Behavioral data from Animated Triangles Task. Ratings of subject reports on what occurred after watching videos of the ATT (moving shapes with apparent intentionality or random movement). All comparisons of FES patients and controls were significant (Ps < 0.04). Error bars are 95% CI.

Patients’ descriptions of animations were less accurate than the HC’s both with regard to the intentional movement animations (FES: 6.97, 95% CI [5.77, 8.17]; HC: 9.29, 95% CI [8.63, 9.96], Z = 2.89, P < .01) and the random animations (FES: 10.24, 95% CI [9.16, 11.32]; HC: 11.82, 95% CI [11.67, 11.98], Z = 2.45, P = .01) (figure 1). However, the above mentioned social cognitive differences did not remain significant when controlling for current IQ (all Ps > .11). There was an interaction between IQ and subject group (patient or control) in the accuracy score of the random movements, however conditional main effects between groups were nonsignificant in this model (β = −5.61, SE = 3.49, t = −1.61, P = .12).

fMRI Results

In the control group, intentional (ToM) movement activated greater temporal gyrus, occipital cortex and inferior frontal cortex. Activation overlapped with previous findings of a pSTS response on this contrast,16,49 assuring that the normal response was as expected and modeling of responses was correct. Qualitatively, patients had a similar pattern. Statistical comparison between groups revealed differences in another region, as described below. See supplementary material for individual group activations.

We focused on the interaction effect of patient (intentional–random)—control (intentional–random) to account for differences of baseline activation during task performance. Using a whole-brain search, this interaction effect was observed only in a region of amPFC (peak MNI coordinates [x y z in mm]: 6, 62, 16, Zmax = 4.2, 138 voxels, P = .01) (figure 3). Within this region, t tests showed that only the patient group showed a clear difference between conditions, with responses to random movement being higher than intentional movement (figure 2): Patients: mean difference 16.79, 95% CI (4.1, 29.5) (arbitrary units (a.u.); T(16) = 2.8, P = .013. Controls: mean difference: −7.4, 95% CI [−4.08, 18.92] (a.u). T(16) = −1.4, P = .19).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

(A) Activation in the medial prefrontal cortex corresponding to the interaction of patient group and movement condition (thresholded Z > 3.0, P <.05, cluster corrected). Activation overlaid on the MNI152 brain. (B) Within this region of MPFC, centered on the mean beta, the interaction is driven primarily by a greater activation in schizophrenia patients while viewing random movement, confirmed by pairwise T-test. Error bars are within-subject 95% CI.

Discussion

The behavioral data show that FES patients both hypo- and hypermentalize with neural evidence for the latter. Behaviorally, FES patients were less accurate in their descriptions of triangle actions in both conditions compared to the healthy controls. However, these differences were influenced by group differences in IQ.

In the control group, the contrast of intentional to random movement observation showed a pattern encompassing pSTS activation found in previous studies.16,49 This region did not respond differently in patients.

However, within another region that is historically associated with this contrast16 and other mentalizing processes,50 FES patients presented a different pattern to that of controls. Unlike the controls, an amPFC region of patients was more active while studying random movement, compared to studying movement normally described as intentional. Yet, they did not eventually ascribe more intentionality to this (vs the intentional) condition. This points to an association between amPFC and the act or effort of mentalizing, rather than its consequences.

From this perspective, we propose that patients tried to interpret what they saw, as they were instructed, in all situations where there was initially potential for intentionality. They continued to mentalize while observing the random movement, which would be a more prolonged and effortful process than the “intentional” condition (where the intentions were soon apparent). This would result in greater mentalizing-based neural activity. This could be a form of hypermentalizing, whereby patients fail to accept the absence of intentionality and turn off mentalizing processes—and instead ramp them up, despite evidence to the contrary. This response may be limited to contexts where intentionality is initially possible, whereby patients fail to change prior expectations when presented with evidence that intention is not present. If this process goes unabated, patients are likely, on more occasions than normal, to interpret nonsocial events (which might by chance look social), as having intentionality.

As with previous fMRI studies using the ATT,23–26,51 we found abnormal activity patterns in the mentalizing network of FES patients compared to healthy controls. However, in contrast to other ATT fMRI studies, our findings were restricted to dysfunctional activation in amPFC. A possible explanation for this might be that the previous fMRI studies using the ATT differed on key issues. These differences might explain divergent results of former studies, eg, sample sizes were small (N = 15–20), the Das et al23,24 sample comprised solely of male patients and Koelkebeck25 investigated a sample from a different culture.

Furthermore, in contrast to previous studies, we examined patients with minimal or no exposure to antipsychotic medication. It is known that antipsychotic medication influences the brain processes in schizophrenia.52–54

Having fewer positive symptoms and more negative symptoms as a patient was associated with neural responses to intentional (in reward associated ventral caudate and lateral frontal pole) and random movements. See supplementary material.

All, but one, of our patients were scanned within 5 days of receiving their diagnosis. At the other end of the scale, the Martin et al26 study investigated patients with a duration of the schizophrenia diagnosis of more than 21 years on average and twice as old as our sample. A recent meta-analysis showed more comprehensive mentalizing deficits in patients with long lasting schizophrenia compared to FES based on behavioral intentionality data from the ATT.13

Based on the 10-year follow-up studies of the OPUS FES patients, we also know that patients have very different prognosis, where some patients recover while others remain severely ill.29,55–57 Identification of deficits unique to FES can help us make predictions about the cause of the disorder, the experience of early symptoms, the prognosis of patients, and the changes that occur with years of ongoing treatment. A direct comparison between chronic patients and FES patients may not be valid given the range of cognitive differences between the 2 groups13 but one can inform the development of the other. Future research, paired with neuroimaging, can be used to further the relationship with longitudinal studies. One might also consider doing meta-analysis based on MRI data from FES patients with sparse exposure to medication to investigate functional connectivity of the mentalizing network as per Schilbach et al.18

FES patients have different ToM deficits (using the ATT) depending on their level of positive and negative symptoms.9 Our supplemental analysis lends neural support to this finding, which should be further investigated in a larger sample.

Historically, neuroimaging of the ATT has differed between studies. While some studies use an explicit type of responding (asking subjects to answers questions during scanning) other studies use a more implicit type of task administration (merely asking subjects to passively watch the film clips during scanning). Martin et al26 mentions how this might explain why some studies find under-activity in the same areas as other studies found over-activity. The ATT has recently been standardized and included as the social cognitive fMRI paradigm of the Human Connectome Project.49,58 This will help future research achieve more comparable fMRI data.

Clinical Implications of the Results

Results imply that FES patients have abnormal mentalizing abilities. Our results suggest the presence of simultaneous hyper- and hypomentalizing in patients. This means that a patient can both under-interpret and misunderstand intended social interaction from another human being. At the same time, the patient can over-interpret neutral noninteractions. This illustrates the complexity of social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia and results should be implemented in psychosocial interventions. For example, cognitive behavioral therapy tends to focus on the negative automatic thoughts of the patient. Our results imply that it is also crucial for therapy to focus on helping the patient find out in detail who-did-what-to-whom-and-why.

Limitations

Our study illustrates the challenges of scanning newly diagnosed, unmedicated patients. We had 6 patients who were not able to complete the scans due to worsening of symptoms because of scanner noise, which adds to the challenge of patients wishing to avoid social interaction with an unfamiliar person. While the NART has been found to be a valid and reliable measure of estimated premorbid intelligence in schizophrenia,31,32 it is possible that premorbid IQ was underestimated due to developmental delays, prodromal symptomatology, or early illness onset. While the 4 subtests selected from WAIS-III have been found to be highly correlated with full scale IQ,34 they do not capture the same variance as a full WAIS-III assessment. Therefore, matching of participants should be interpreted with some caution and fMRI results not interpreted as independent of the effects of schizophrenia on intelligence scores.

A larger sample size may reveal further neural differences within and between groups.

Conclusion

Our results imply that FES patients can have simultaneously hyper- and hypomentalizing tendencies. Neural correlates indicate that patients apply ToM processes despite low-level cues indicating that this would be inappropriate. Duration of illness needs to be taken into consideration when comparing fMRI results in schizophrenia. Results demonstrate the complexity of ToM deficits in schizophrenia and this should be taken into consideration when offering social cognitive training programs.

Funding

This work was supported by Det Frie Forskningsråd, Sundhed og sygdom, Ministry of Science, Innovation and Higher Education (09-071226 to C.F. and V.B.), Fonden til forskning af sindslidelser, Aarhus University, Faculty of Health Sciences (to V.B.) and Lundbeck Foundation grant (R54-2010-5434 to D.C.M., C.F., and A.R.).

Supplementary Material

Supplemental Material

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully thank the patients, psychiatrists, and staff at the OPUS Risskov Clinic; research assistant Tina Hoejsgaard Soerensen; research student Maria Lotus Thai, associate professor Torben Lund; MindLab Core Experimental Facility. The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.

References

  • 1. Fett AK, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez MD, Penn DL, van Os J, Krabbendam L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:573–588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Mancuso F, Horan WP, Kern RS, Green MF. Social cognition in psychosis: multidimensional structure, clinical correlates, and relationship with functional outcome. Schizophr Res. 2011;125:143–151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Schmidt SJ, Mueller DR, Roder V. Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(suppl 2):S41–S54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Green MF, Penn DL, Bentall R et al. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:1211–1220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Penn DL, Sanna LJ, Roberts DL. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an overview. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:408–411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Savla GN, Vella L, Armstrong CC, Penn DL, Twamley EW. Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophr Bull. 2012;39:979–992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Pickup GJ, Frith CD. Theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia: symptomatology, severity and specificity. Psychol Med. 2001;31:207–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Bell MD, Corbera S, Johannesen JK, Fiszdon JM, Wexler BE. Social cognitive impairments and negative symptoms in schizophrenia: are there subtypes with distinct functional correlates?Schizophr Bull. 2013;39:186–196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Bliksted V, Videbech P, Fagerlund B, Frith C. The effect of positive symptoms on social cognition in first-episode schizophrenia is modified by the presence of negative symptoms. Neuropsychology. 2017;31:209–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Frith CD, Frith U. The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron. 2006;50:531–534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Frith CD. Schizophrenia and theory of mind. Psychol Med. 2004;34:385–389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Abu-Akel A, Bailey AL. The possibility of different forms of theory of mind impairment in psychiatric and developmental disorders. Psychol Med. 2000;30:735–738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Bliksted V, Ubukata S, Koelkebeck K. Discriminating autism spectrum disorders from schizophrenia by investigation of mental state attribution on an on-line mentalizing task: a review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2016;171:16–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:267–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:620–631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Castelli F, Happé F, Frith U, Frith C. Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage. 2000;12:314–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Spotorno N, Koun E, Prado J, Van Der Henst JB, Noveck IA. Neural evidence that utterance-processing entails mentalizing: the case of irony. Neuroimage. 2012;63:25–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Schilbach L, Derntl B, Aleman A et al. Differential patterns of dysconnectivity in mirror neuron and mentalizing networks in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42:1135–1148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Lee KH, Farrow TF, Spence SA, Woodruff PW. Social cognition, brain networks and schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2004;34:391–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Brunet-Gouet E, Decety J. Social brain dysfunctions in schizophrenia: a review of neuroimaging studies. Psychiatry Res. 2006;148:75–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Abell F, Happé F, Frith U. Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cogn Dev. 2000;15:1–16. [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Schurz M, Radua J, Aichhorn M, Richlan F, Perner J. Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;42:9–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Das P, Lagopoulos J, Coulston CM, Henderson AF, Malhi GS. Mentalizing impairment in schizophrenia: a functional MRI study. Schizophr Res. 2012;134:158–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Das P, Calhoun V, Malhi GS. Mentalizing in male schizophrenia patients is compromised by virtue of dysfunctional connectivity between task-positive and task-negative networks. Schizophr Res. 2012;140:51–58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Koelkebeck K, Hirao K, Miyata J et al. Impact of gray matter reductions on theory of mind abilities in patients with schizophrenia. Soc Neurosci. 2013;8:631–639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Martin AK, Dzafic I, Robinson GA, Reutens D, Mowry B. Mentalizing in schizophrenia: a multivariate functional MRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93:158–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Hoff AL, Svetina C, Shields G, Stewart J, DeLisi LE. Ten year longitudinal study of neuropsychological functioning subsequent to a first episode of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2005;78:27–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Bertelsen M, Jeppesen P, Petersen L et al. Five-year follow-up of a randomized multicenter trial of intensive early intervention vs standard treatment for patients with a first episode of psychotic illness: the OPUS trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:762–771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Austin SF, Mors O, Secher RG et al. Predictors of recovery in first episode psychosis: the OPUS cohort at 10 year follow-up. Schizophr Res. 2013;150:163–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Nelson HE. National Adult Reading Test (NART): For the Assessment of Premorbid Intelligence in Patients with Dementia: Test Manual. London: NFER-Nelson; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Hjorthøj CR, Vesterager L, Nordentoft M. Test–retest reliability of the Danish adult reading test in patients with comorbid psychosis and cannabis-use disorder. Nord J Psychiatry. 2013;67:159–163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. O’Carroll R, Walker M, Dunan J et al. Selecting controls for schizophrenia research studies: the use of the National Adult Reading Test (NART) is a measure of premorbid ability. Schizophr Res. 1992;8:137–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Wechsler D. WAIS-III/WMS-III Technical Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Axelrod BN. Validity of the Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence and other very short forms of estimating intellectual functioning. Assessment. 2002;9:17–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.WHO. Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (Version 2.1). Geneva: World Health Organization; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Andreasen NC. Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms. Iowa City: University of Iowa; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Andreasen NC. Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms. Iowa City: University of Iowa; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23(suppl 1):S208–S219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;17:143–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage. 2002;17:825–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Beckmann CF, Smith SM. Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2004;23:137–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. Neuroimage. 2012;62:782–790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Woolrich MW, Behrens TE, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference. Neuroimage. 2004;21:1732–1747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Friston KJ, Worsley KJ, Frackowiak R, Mazziotta JC, Evans AC. Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent. Hum Brain Mapp. 1993;1:210–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Forman SD, Cohen JD, Fitzgerald M, Eddy WF, Mintun MA, Noll DC. Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn Reson Med. 1995;33:636–647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Worsley KJ, Marrett S, Neelin P, Vandal AC, Friston KJ, Evans AC. A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Hum Brain Mapp. 1996;4:58–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates (vol 113, pg 7900, 2016). Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113:E4929–E4929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Fusar-Poli P, Borgwardt S, Bechdolf A et al. The psychosis high-risk state: a comprehensive state-of-the-art review. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:107–120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Hillebrandt H, Friston KJ, Blakemore S-J. Effective connectivity during animacy perception–dynamic causal modelling of human connectome project data. Sci Reps. 2014;4:6240, 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Amodio DM, Frith CD. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:268–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Pedersen A, Koelkebeck K, Brandt M et al. Theory of mind in patients with schizophrenia: is mentalizing delayed?Schizophr Res. 2012;137:224–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Abbott CC, Jaramillo A, Wilcox CE, Hamilton DA. Antipsychotic drug effects in schizophrenia: a review of longitudinal FMRI investigations and neural interpretations. Curr Med Chem. 2013;20:428–437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Roder CH, Marie Hoogendam J, van der Veen FM. FMRI, antipsychotics and schizophrenia. Influence of different antipsychotics on BOLD-signal. Curr Pharm Des. 2010;16:2012–2025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Liemburg EJ, Knegtering H, Klein HC, Kortekaas R, Aleman A. Antipsychotic medication and prefrontal cortex activation: a review of neuroimaging findings. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012;22:387–400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Secher RG, Hjorthøj CR, Austin SF et al. Ten-year follow-up of the OPUS specialized early intervention trial for patients with a first episode of psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41:617–626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Bergh S, Hjorthøj C, Sørensen HJ et al. Predictors and longitudinal course of cognitive functioning in schizophrenia spectrum disorders, 10 years after baseline: the OPUS study. Schizophr Res. 2016;175:57–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Austin SF, Mors O, Budtz-Jørgensen E et al. Long-term trajectories of positive and negative symptoms in first episode psychosis: a 10 year follow-up study in the OPUS cohort. Schizophr Res. 2015;168:84–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Barch DM, Burgess GC, Harms MP et al. ; WU-Minn HCP Consortium. Function in the human connectome: task-fMRI and individual differences in behavior. Neuroimage. 2013;80:169–189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Material

Articles from Schizophrenia Bulletin are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES