Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Mar 7.
Published in final edited form as: Clin Psychol Rev. 2017 Nov 21;59:126–136. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.11.006

Cognitive deficits in bipolar disorders: Implications for emotion

Isabela MM Lima a,b,1, Andrew D Peckham a,1, Sheri L Johnson a,*,1
PMCID: PMC6404979  NIHMSID: NIHMS1015169  PMID: 29195773

Abstract

Prominent cognitive deficits have been documented in bipolar disorder, and multiple studies suggest that these deficits can be observed among non-affected first-degree relatives of those with bipolar disorder. Although there is variability in the degree of cognitive deficits, these deficits are robustly relevant for functional outcomes. A separate literature documents clear difficulties in emotionality, emotion regulation, and emotion-relevant impulsivity within bipolar disorder, and demonstrates that these emotion-relevant variables are also central to outcome. Although cognitive and emotion domains are typically studied independently, basic research and emergent findings in bipolar disorder suggest that there are important ties between cognitive deficits and the emotion disturbances observed in bipolar disorder. Understanding these relationships has relevance for fostering more integrative research, for clarifying relevant aspects related to functionality and vulnerability within bipolar disorder, and for the development of novel treatment interventions.


Bipolar disorder (BD) is a severe psychiatric illness that has been ranked as one of the 20 leading medical causes of disability (WHO, 2011). BD has been shown to be the psychiatric disorder with the highest rates of completed suicide across two major cohort studies (Ilgen et al., 2010; Nordentoft, Mortensen, & Pedersen, 2011). In a cross-national representative sample, one in four persons diagnosed with bipolar I disorder reported a suicide attempt (Merikangas et al., 2011). Rates of relapse remain high despite available treatments (Gitlin, Swendsen, Heller, & Hammen, 1995), and in the year after hospitalization for manic episode, two-thirds of patients do not return to work (Strakowski et al., 1998). Poverty, homelessness, and incarceration are all too common (Copeland et al., 2009).

Despite the often poor outcomes, there is also evidence for outstanding accomplishments and creativity among those with milder forms of the disorder and their family members (Coryell et al., 1989; Jamison, 1993; Murray & Johnson, 2010). Some individuals appear to achieve more than the general population, suggesting the importance of understanding the variables that predict differential outcome within bipolar disorder.

Within this paper, we focus on two key predictors of outcomes within bipolar disorder: cognition and emotionality. We review evidence that problems in cognition and emotionality are prominent among those diagnosed with the disorder, are not artifacts of symptom state, and relate substantively to poorer outcomes. Although traditionally studied separately, new work points toward the idea that cognition and emotionality are intricately linked within bipolar disorder. Drawing from research within bipolar disorder as well as outside of bipolar disorder, we build a model of how cognition and emotionality might be tied within bipolar disorder. We then provide suggestions for future research.

Before considering findings, it is worth noting that there are several forms of the disorder, defined by varying degrees and duration of manic symptoms (APA, 2013; WHO, 1993). Manic episodes are defined by abnormally elevated or irritable mood, accompanied by increased activity and at least three symptoms (four if mood is only irritable) such as decreased need for sleep, increased self-confidence, racing thoughts or flight of ideas, rapid speech, distractibility, goal-directed activity, and engagement in pleasurable activities without regard to potential negative consequences. To meet criteria for mania, these symptoms must persist for at least one week or require hospitalization, and must lead to difficulties with functioning. If functional impairment is not more than mild and duration is between 4 and 6 days, the episode is considered a hypomanic episode. Bipolar I disorder (BD I) is diagnosed on the basis of at least one lifetime manic episode within the DSM-5 and by at least two episodes within the ICD, whereas bipolar II disorder is diagnosed on the basis of at least one hypomanic episode (and no manic episodes) as well as major depressive episodes. Cyclothymic disorder is defined by chronic but milder fluctuations between manic and depressive symptoms. Most research focuses on BD I.

In addition to diagnosed samples, research has focused on those at high risk for bipolar disorder, including first-degree relatives of those with BD. This work draws on the evidence for extremely high heritability of BD I, with estimates from community-based twin studies of 0.85 (Kieseppä, Partonen, Haukka, Kaprio, & Lönnqvist, 2014). Other research has considered high risk for BD by virtue of lifetime subsyndromal symptoms, as measured by scales such as the Hypomanic Personality Scale (Eckblad & Chapman, 1986) or the General Behavior Inventory (Depue, Krauss, Spoont, & Arbisi, 1989). The study of high-risk individuals provides a way to decipher whether deficits are present before the onset of the disorder, of importance given models suggesting that episodes of the disorder may change brain function (Chang, Steiner, & Ketter, 2000; Strakowski, 2012) as well as individuals’ perceptions of their emotion regulation.

Beyond defining BD, it is worth defining some of the many different neuropsychological tasks that have been widely studied in BD. Perhaps no area has received more attention than executive function. Executive function is related to three core functions: 1) inhibition, the ability to suppress irrelevant information in working memory in order to accomplish an established goal; 2) working memory, the ability to hold and manipulate information in mind; and 3) cognitive flexibility, the ability to shift strategies in response to feedback (Diamond, 2013; Miyake et al., 2000). Attention (defined as the process of selecting information reception from internal or external cues) is implicated in all three of these aspects of executive function. Much of the literature we will discuss focuses on response inhibition, or the ability to suppress a prepotent response, which is considered a subtype of inhibition. Some tests measure multiple facets of executive function; for example the Trails B test likely requires working memory and cognitive flexibility (Sánchez-Cubillo et al., 2009).

Aside from executive function, multiple other facets of cognition have been widely studied in bipolar disorder. Verbal and non-verbal memory are related to the ability to register, store and retrieve verbal or visual information (Lezak, 1995). Verbal fluency is measured as the number of verbal responses a person can generate to a given target, such as a specific semantic category (e.g., animals, furniture) or phonetic category (e.g., words that begin with letter F) (Diamond, 2013). Although cognitive tasks have been designed to evaluate these specific functions, it is important to note that most measures are highly inter-correlated and may assess multiple overlapping functions to some extent (for example, the Trails B test is often described as an “executive function” task, although this task likely involves both working memory and cognitive flexibility. Not surprisingly, then, some authors label the function of certain tests differently, and this is particularly evident in meta-analyses of cognition. As we describe findings in this paper, we will use the terms proposed by the authors but will also identify key tests used to define a cognitive construct.

With this background in mind, we turn to a discussion of cognitive deficits, then of emotion-related traits. Our hope is that those concise summaries provide evidence for the importance of both domains, but also specificity regarding the facets of emotion and cognition that are most impaired in BD. This specificity then guides our consideration of models that integrate cognition and emotion.

1. Cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder

A large body of work identifies prominent cognitive deficits, as measured using neuropsychological tests, in BD (Bourne et al., 2013; Kurtz & Gerraty, 2009; Robinson et al., 2006). Although depressive and manic symptoms relate to lower performance on cognitive tests (Kurtz & Gerraty, 2009) and lower self-reported cognitive functioning (Peters et al., 2014), a more important question for mapping vulnerability is whether these deficits persist after remission. Findings of one meta-analysis suggest that the severity of impairment for those with remitted BD as compared to healthy control groups varies across domain and task, with higher effect sizes for verbal and nonverbal memory, attention, and the Trails B test, relevant to both attention and working memory (Kurtz & Gerraty, 2009). For verbal memory, the most frequently used task was the Rey and California Verbal Learning Test (deficits in the range of d = 0.81 across 18 studies). Parallel effect sizes were observed for a nonverbal memory task, the Delayed Recall Task of the Rey Complex Figure Test, (d = 0.80 across 3 studies). Executive function, as assessed using the Trails B measure, yielded an effect size of d = 0.73 across 18 studies. Comparable effect sizes were also observed for attention on the Trails A task (d = 0.65 across 17 studies) and Continuous Performance Task (d = 0.69 across 13 studies). Other domains appear to be less affected, with smaller between-group effect sizes, including Verbal Fluency (d = 0.51 across 15 studies, measured by FAS phonetic fluency and animal naming) and visuospatial abilities (d = 0.55 across five studies, assessed by Block Design). Findings of a different meta-analyses converge in suggesting that remitted BD was related to a similar profile of larger deficits in verbal memory (d > 0.8, assessed by California Verbal Learning Test in 10 studies), executive function (d > 0.8, assessed by Trail B in 10 studies) and and smaller deficits in visuoperception (d < 0.5, assessed by the copy version of the Rey Complex Figure Test in 4 studies) (Arts, Jabben, Krabbendam, & Van Os, 2008). A more recent meta-analysis confirmed this pattern, with deficits also evident on verbal learning, trail-making, and verbal working memory tasks (Bourne et al., 2013). This literature provides substantial evidence that cognitive deficits persist after remission.

Congruent with findings of the correlational research showing cognitive deficits during remission, longitudinal research documents that symptom fluctuations do not seem to explain most cognitive deficits. In one longitudinal study of individuals who were re-tested as episode status varied, attention and processing speed deficits appeared stable as manic and depressive symptoms fluctuated, although verbal fluency deficits appeared more prominent when depressive symptoms were present (Chaves et al., 2011). Another longitudinal study found that cognitive deficits in BD were stable across five years, with the exception of worsening in verbal memory (Santos et al., 2014); in parallel, a review of meta-analyses concluded that cognitive deficits were generally stable (Szmulewicz, Samamé, Martino, & Strejilevich, 2015). Taken together, evidence from the large number of cross-sectional studies and available longitudinal research indicates on average, those with BD experience cognitive deficits after remission.

Although fewer researchers have compared bipolar subtypes, meta-analytic findings indicate that persons with BD II also show cognitive deficits of the same form but slightly less severe than those observed in BD I (Bora, Yücel, Pantelis, & Berk, 2011). More specifically, those with BD II show less severe deficits than those with BD I, with moderate effect sizes for the contrast of BD I versus II for verbal memory (d > 0.5, seven studies), and smaller effect sizes for visual memory (d = 0.38, six studies), processing speed (d = 0.28, six studies) and general cognition (d = 0.26, eight studies). Findings did not indicate that BD I and II differed significantly in attention, planning, working memory, shifting and inhibition. The relatively greater impairment in verbal memory among those with BD I could reflect severity of the illness vulnerability, or the effects of antipsychotic medications, which are more commonly prescribed for BD I than II, as iatrogenic effects of antipsychotic medications have been observed on verbal memory and processing speed (Balanzá-Martínez et al., 2010).

These findings of cognitive deficits do not appear to be strictly an after-effect of years of illness, in that youth with BD show cognitive deficits that appear parallel to those observed among adults. As with adults, treatment and concomitant symptom improvements can improve cognitive performance among those with pediatric BD (Lera-Miguel, Andres-Perpina, Fatjo-Vilas, Fananas, & Lazaro, 2015). Nonetheless, in a meta-analysis of pediatric BD, effect sizes indicated greater deficits among the BD group compared to controls. The effect sizes varied, with larger effect sizes for verbal learning and memory (Z = 4.65, nine studies), followed by processing speed (Z = 3.61, seven studies), working memory (Z = 4.19, seven studies), executive function (Z = 4.07, nine studies) and attention (Z = 3.81, eight studies). Smaller, but significant, group differences of those with BD group compared to controls were observed in verbal fluency, visual memory, visuospatial skills, and general cognitive ability, with particularly small effects for motor skills (Z = 1.76), assessed in only one study (Nieto & Castellanos, 2011).

These results indicate that adults and children with BD demonstrate cognitive deficits, and that these deficits can be observed even during remission. The most prominent deficits involve executive function, attention, verbal memory and non-verbal memory, indicating that the profile of deficits is quite broad.

Although BD is clearly related to cognitive deficits, these deficits do not appear to be universal. It has been estimated that about 30% of patients with remitted BD will show cognitive performance levels within the normative range (Gualtieri & Morgan, 2008; Martino et al., 2014). This suggests the importance of considering how individual differences in cognition will relate to other domains, such as emotion and functioning.

1.1. Cognitive deficits before onset

Given the robust evidence that many adults and children with BD show cognitive deficits, a key question is whether cognitive deficits can be observed before onset. Much of this work has focused on IQ (Gale et al., 2013; Tiihonen et al., 2005). Here, though, we focus on executive function, as prospective research indicates that executive function more robustly predicts BD onset than does IQ (Meyer et al., 2004). In one study, offspring of mothers with BD, unipolar depression or no history of BD (n = 74) completed general intelligence tests between ages 8 and 15, executive function tests between ages 11 and 19, and diagnostic interviews (SCIDs) in early adulthood. Nine offspring were diagnosed with BD as young adults. Those 9 participants showed an average IQ score, but had lower performance on executive function tests (ds = 0.58–1.34)than did those who were not diagnosed with BD.These effects persisted when two offspring who had developed bipolar II disorder during adolescence were removed from analyses.

In a study of younger children at high risk for psychosis as indicated by symptom ratings on the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental Status, the 16 children who developed BD during the eight-year follow-up period showed substantially lower performance on Trails A and B tests compared to those who did not develop BD (Ratheesh et al., 2013). In sum, two small studies suggest that lower executive function and visuospatial ability can predict the onset of BD.

1.2. Cognitive deficits in relatives of those with bipolar disorder

Two meta-analyses have been conducted with first-degree family members of those with BD as compared to healthy controls (Arts et al., 2008; Bora, Yucel, & Pantelis, 2009). Although the effect sizes observed in unaffected relatives suggest less profound disturbance than the deficits observed in those diagnosed with the disorder, cognitive deficits can be consistently observed in unaffected relatives as compared to controls. These studies provide a different window into the nature of deficits that might be most tied to genetic vulnerability. In one meta-analysis of 17 studies, the family members of those with BD showed the largest deficits in response inhibition (measured by the Stroop Task), (d = 0.51 in 6 studies), followed by other executive function measures (d = 0.36–0.38), with smaller effects for verbal learning and memory measures (d ≤ 0.33)(Bora et al., 2009). A meta-analysis with slightly different selection criteria replicated findings for response inhibition (Stroop d = 0.49, four studies) and other executive function deficits (Trails B, d = 0.37, seven studies), and also identified significant bipolar deficits in verbal memory (CVLT immediate and delayed recall d = 0.42 and 0.56, respectively assessed in four studies) (Arts et al., 2008). Effects for IQ, FAS, digit span backward, Wisconsin Card Sort Task perseverative errors (assessing shifting, Miyake et al., 2000) and Trails did not reach statistical significance. Despite the clear evidence that deficits are common among unaffected relatives, there is evidence of cognitive heterogeneity in relatives of those with BD. For example, one twin study found that healthy twins with a bipolar sibling showed enhanced verbal fluency and verbal learning relative to controls (Higier et al., 2014).

Findings of multiple studies indicate that these cognitive deficits can be observed early in life, among child or adolescent offspring of those with BD, although these deficits are not as severe as those observed among probands of those with schizophrenia (for review, see Olvet, Burdick, & Cornblatt, 2013). In one study, deficits in executive function, as measured by the Wisconsin Card Sort Task were more severe among adolescent offspring of mothers with BD (n = 43) than among the offspring of mothers with major depressive disorder (n = 72) (Klimes-Dougan, Ronsaville, Wiggs, & Martinez, 2006), and these effects remained significant when controlling for IQ and current symptoms. In a second study, researchers focused on 45 offspring at risk for schizophrenia or BD, defined as at least one first-degree family member with the disorder and at least four relatives with the disorder (first degree or more distant). The 23 offspring from bipolar families demonstrated impairment in executive function measures of problem solving (d = 0.98), initiation (d = 0.72), planning (d = 0.58), verbal memory (d = 0.94 for immediate and 1.03 for delayed recall) and visual memory (d = 0.73 for immediate and d = 0.88 for delayed recall) as compared to controls (Maziade et al., 2009). Deficits in response inhibition have also been identified in offspring of those with BD I (Frangou, Haldane, Roddy, & Kumari, 2005).

In sum, multiple studies indicate that a broad range of cognitive measures are impaired among unaffected family members of those with BD, even at an early age, including executive function, verbal learning, and visual learning. Of note though, a clearer profile emerges in these at-risk studies of response inhibition as a particularly robust indicator of risk.

1.3. Cognition as a predictor of functional impairment

Given the cognitive deficits observed in those diagnosed with and at risk for BD, a key question is whether these deficits can help explain functional impairment. In a qualitative review of 52 studies, cognitive deficits were consistently associated with lower functioning within BD, in both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies (Baune, Li, & Beblo, 2013). Findings of a quantitative meta-analysis also indicated that cognitive deficits were tied to worse functional impairment, and effects did not appear to be moderated by clinical state, age, or study design (Depp et al., 2012). Multiple cognitive domains were tied to functional impairment, with aggregate rs ranging from 0.21 to 0.29, but working memory was the domain most specifically related to functional outcome.

The effects of cognition on functional impairment are also observed in longitudinal studies that control for baseline symptom severity. For example, when cognitive deficits were measured just after a first manic episode among young adults, more severe cognitive deficits significantly predicted lower quality of life six months later when controlling for baseline symptom severity (Mackala, Torres, Kozicky, Michalak, & Yatham, 2014). Although cognitive deficits tend to be stable over time (Samamé, Martino, & Strejilevich, 2014; Santos et al., 2014), a subgroup of patients show declines in cognitive function over time, and faster decline predicted lower functioning (measured via the FAST) at six-year follow-up (Mora, Portella, Forcada, Vieta, & Mur, 2013).

The strength of relationships between cognition and functional outcome likely depend on the measures of functioning employed. Baune et al. (2013) noted that effects were smaller when researchers relied on the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF), which is not surprising given the low inter-rater reliability of the GAF. In a meta-analysis of 22 studies of cognition and psychosocial function (N = 1344), stronger effects were observed for performance-based (r = 0.32) and milestone (e.g., achievements such as autonomy, marriage, employment, r = 0.33) measures of functioning as compared to clinician- or self-rated measures (r = 0.23 and r = 0.20 respectively). In this meta-analysis, an overall average correlation of 0.27 was observed between cognitive measures and everyday functioning (95% CI = 0.22–0.32, p < 0.001; (Depp et al., 2012).

Cognitive impairment can predict declines in functioning above and beyond the role of symptom status. Parallel with the profile of cognitive findings in BD, evidence implicates a broad range of cognitive variables tied to functional impairment but supports the key role of executive function measures such as working memory indices.

1.4. Summary of cognitive deficits

A large literature indicates that executive function and other facets of cognition are impaired in adults and youth with BD, even during remission. Such deficits can be observed in unaffected family members, with particularly robust evidence for deficits in response inhibition. Although not universally present, cognitive deficits can predict the onset of BD among those at-risk. Cognitive indices, including executive function measures, appear closely tied to functional impairment in cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, particularly when strong measures of function are used. Taken together, these findings suggest that cognition could serve as a vulnerability marker for BD.

2. Emotion in bipolar disorder

Euphoria and anger are defining features of mania, and accordingly, researchers have placed considerable emphasis on understanding emotionality in BD. Findings indicate that even after remission, those with BD display heightened or more frequent negative affectivity on self-report measures (Heerlein, Richter, Gonzalez, & Santander, 1998; Keitner et al., 1996), experience sampling (Knowles et al., 2007), and laboratory-based measures using standardized stimuli (Gruber, Harvey, & Johnson, 2009; Pavlova, Uher, Dennington, Wright, & Donaldson, 2011; Rich et al., 2010). This does not appear to be limited to negative valence, in that those with BD also display heightened or more frequent positive affectivity, again across self-report measures (Gruber et al., 2009), experience sampling (Knowles et al., 2007), and laboratory-based measures (Gruber et al., 2009; Gruber, Dutra, Eidelman, Johnson, & Harvey, 2011; Gruber, Harvey, & Purcell, 2011). Given the heterogeneous nature of emotion problems that have been observed, one study considered a broad range of emotion-related difficulties, and findings indicated that BD was more strongly tied to elevations of negative emotion as compared to positive affectivity problems (Johnson, Tharp, Peckham, & McMaster, 2016).

Beyond reactivity, researchers have found that those with BD tend to feel less confident in their ability to use adaptive emotion regulation strategies (Gruber, Harvey, & Gross, 2012). Disentangling the emotion regulation literature is not straight forward. Perhaps in response to the frequent emotion states, those with BD often report using many different emotion regulation strategies more frequently than those with no mood disorder do (Gruber, Kogan, Mennin, & Murray, 2013). In addition to the more frequent use of adaptive strategies, they tend to endorse using more maladaptive forms of emotion regulation, such as rumination, more than those without mood disorders do (Gruber et al., 2011; Gruber, Eidelman, & Harvey, 2008; Johnson, McKenzie, & McMurrich, 2008; Rowland et al., 2013; Thomas, Knowles, Tai, & Bentall, 2007). Beyond the large literature on responses to negative emotions, those with severe forms of the disorder endorse frequent use of strategies to dampen positive emotions (Edge et al., 2013; Gruber et al., 2011).

Beyond these typical domains of emotion and emotion regulation, findings have indicated that in the face of a given emotion state, those with BD report more difficulty controlling their speech and behavior. This phenomenon has been referred to as emotion-related impulsivity, and those with BD endorse significantly more concern about this form of impulsivity than other forms of impulsivity, even after remission (Muhtadie, Johnson, Carver, Gotlib, & Ketter, 2014). This tendency toward emotion-triggered impulsivity does not appear to be just the aftermath of the episodes, as it has also been observed among samples at risk by virtue of subsyndromal manic symptoms (Giovanelli, Hoerger, Johnson, & Gruber, 2013; Johnson, Carver, Mulé, & Joormann, 2013). Intriguingly, effects of emotion-related impulsivity do not appear to be just an effect of a higher level of emotionality or arousal, but rather, a more specific problem with constraint in the face of an emotion state (Johnson, Tharp, Peckham, Sanchez, & Carver, 2016).

There is also some mixed evidence that difficulty accurately identifying facial displays of emotion can be observed in adults and children with BD. Because these difficulties are not observed in most studies of remitted samples of adults with BD (Samamé, Martino, & Strejilevich, 2012), we focus on other facets of emotion here.

Given the well-established emotionality among those with BD, a key question is whether emotion problems can be observed before onset. Several studies suggest that problems with emotionality are apparent in the offspring of those with BD, including prolonged duration of emotion responses (Chang, Blasey, Ketter, & Steiner, 2003), and greater lability of negative emotions and to a smaller extent, positive emotions (Birmaher et al., 2013). Indeed, by preschool, offspring of those with BD tend to show increased reactivity to negative emotions in others and difficulty regulating their own negative emotions (Zahn-Waxler, Cummings, McKnew, & Radke-Yarrow, 1984; Zahn-Waxler, McKnew, Cummings, Davenport, & Radke-Yarrow, 1984). The TEMPS-A cyclothymic scale, comprised of items capturing the tendency to experience overly pronounced shifts in emotion states, has been shown to predict the onset of BD among youth (Kochman et al., 2005).

Given evidence that multiple emotion problems are observed in those diagnosed with and at risk for BD, researchers have considered how individual differences in emotion regulation relate to outcomes within BD. Within BD, more maladaptive approaches to regulating negative emotion relate to more severe depressive symptoms cross-sectionally (Gilbert, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Gruber, 2013; Green et al., 2011; Johnson et al., 2008; Rowland et al., 2013; Thomas et al., 2007) and longitudinally (Johnson et al., 2016; Van Rheenen & Rossell, 2014).

Beyond depression, emotion regulation also appears of import for functional outcomes. In one study, greater tendencies to engage in suppression were related to significantly lower function in BD, even after controlling for indices of negative and positive emotionality (Johnson et al., 2016). When those with BD I over-use dampening of positive mood states, lower functioning and quality of life are observed (Edge et al., 2013). Hence the ability to effectively regulate both positive and negative emotions appears important for outcomes in BD.

Higher emotion-related impulsivity has been found to relate to greater suicidality and aggression among those with remitted BD I. Given this, perhaps it is not surprising that emotion-related impulsivity is tied to substantially lower quality of life and functional outcomes for those with BD I (Johnson & Carver, 2016; Johnson, Carver, & Tharp, 2017; Muhtadie et al., 2014; Victor, Johnson, & Gotlib, 2011).

In sum, findings indicate that BD is related to increased negative emotion reactivity, and perhaps slightly less robustly degree, positive emotional reactivity. Those with BD may use emotion regulation strategies more often than others do, but they feel less confident about the effectiveness of these strategies. BD is also tied to emotion-related impulsivity. Many of these facets of emotionality can be observed before onset among those at risk for the disorder. Emotion variables also are important for outcomes. Those who use more rumination and suppression are prone to depression, and those who use more reappraisal may develop less depression over time. Negative emotionality, maladaptive strategies for regulating negative emotions, and dampening of positive emotion all relate to lower function. Emotion-related impulsivity relates to worse functional outcome, more aggression, and more suicidality in BD.

3. Links between cognition and emotion

Above, we have outlined evidence that both cognitive deficits and emotion disturbances are closely tied to BD, of central importance for functional outcomes in BD, observable before onset, and predictive of the onset of BD. Surprisingly, research in these two important domains has evolved in a largely separate manner within BD. That is, relatively little behavioral research has considered the interface of cognitive deficits and emotionality in BD. Accordingly, we focus on the rich literature outside of BD on the interface of cognition and emotion regulation.

3.1. Cognition and emotion outside bipolar disorder

Nearly all theories of emotion and emotion regulation involve cognitive processes. According to several theories of emotion, cognition provides a framework for appraising internal and external stimuli (Oatley & Johnson-Laird, 2014). Many experimental approaches provide insight into the ways in which variations in the valence and arousal of specific affective states influences cognition. Certainly, considerable research suggests that high levels of emotion may interfere with executive function, in part because of the resources consumed by prioritizing attention to highly salient, emotion-relevant stimuli (Pessoa, 2009). This prioritization of cognitive attentional resources is frequently observed when stimuli have high “motivational intensity” or induce high arousal (Harmon-Jones & Gable, 2009; Harmon-Jones & Gable, 2010). In contrast, low-arousal positive emotions have been theoretically and empirically linked with greater cognitive flexibility and broader attention, yet more difficulty inhibiting attention to distractions (Ashby, Isen, & Turken, 1999; Dreisbach & Goschke, 2004; Fredrickson, 2003).

In addition to effects of emotion on cognitive processes, experimental and correlational research in healthy populations and in other clinical disorders has provided understanding of the opposite direction–ways in which cognitive processing ability can influence emotion regulation (for review, see Hofmann, Schmeichel, & Baddeley, 2012; Schmeichel & Tang, 2015). Better skill in regulating emotions, as re-flected in the tendency to use reappraisal more and to gain more benefit from reappraisal, has been tied to higher performance on measures of executive function (von Hippel & Gonsalkorale, 2005), and more specifically working memory (Hendricks & Buchanan, 2016; McRae, Jacobs, Ray, John, & Gross, 2012; Schmeichel, Volokhov, & Demaree, 2008), cognitive flexibility (Malooly, Genet, & Siemer, 2013), and cognitive inhibition (see Joormann & Vanderlind, 2014), with particularly strong effects when cognitive inhibition is tested under conditions of stress (Quinn & Joormann, 2015). Other findings show that higher verbal fluency scores are related to greater emotion regulation success, as reflected in physiological measures and changes in facial affect (Gyurak et al., 2009; Gyurak, Goodkind, Kramer, Miller, & Levenson, 2012).

Other research focuses on cognitive abilities related to maladaptive regulatory strategies. The cognitive underpinnings of rumination have been correlated with each of the major domains of executive function: cognitive flexibility, inhibition, and working memory. Perhaps not surprisingly then, cognitive inflexibility, which involves the inability to switch strategies after negative feedback and related tendencies to perseverate, has been correlated with ruminative tendencies (Davis & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000). To assess other domains of executive function in relation to rumination, multiple studies have used tasks that contrast negatively valenced and neutral stimuli. Multiple studies suggest that rumination is related to poorer executive function—including inhibition, working memory updating, and switching—when valenced stimuli are used as distractors or when the stimuli to be processed are negatively valenced (Beckwé, Deroost, Koster, De Lissnyder, & De Raedt, 2014; Bernblum & Mor, 2010).

Finally, a large body of work has focused on executive function measures and emotion-related impulsivity. Across a set of studies, a recent meta-analysis indicated that emotion-related impulsivity was specifically tied to response inhibition, particularly when samples with more extreme deficits in this form of impulsivity were tested (Johnson et al., 2016).

Studies demonstrating a role of executive function in emotion regulation and emotion-related impulsivity dovetail with findings from functional MRI and electroencephalography (EEG) research. Although a full discussion of the rich literature on the neurophysiological basis of the emotion-cognition interactions is beyond the score of this review, recent approaches in this field increasingly highlight evidence showing substantial overlap in brain regions involved in both emotion and cognitive processes (e.g., Pessoa, 2009). Within this literature, much work has focused on the important role of prefrontal cortex (PFC) for emotion processing and emotion regulation(Banich et al., 2009; Beckwé et al., 2014; Bernblum & Mor, 2010; Davis & Nolen-hoeksema, 2000; Goodkind et al., 2015; Kahl, Winter, & Schweiger, 2012; Ochsner et al., 2004). The PFC is centrally implicated in executive function domains including cognitive control, cognitive flexibility and working memory (Diamond, 2013).

Some of this work has focused on brain regions that are activated when individuals are asked to inhibit attention to emotionally relevant content. These studies broadly show that activation of the right ventrolateral PFC is heavily involved in inhibition, both in the presence of affective and non-affective stimuli (Chiu, Holmes, & Pizzagalli, 2008; Dillon & Pizzagalli, 2007), while the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) may specifically support inhibition of emotional distractors (Chiu et al., 2008; Etkin, Büchel, & Gross, 2015). To test working memory in the face of emotion distractors, researchers assessed the ability to store goal-directed information after the presentation of an emotional interference (Banich et al., 2009). The results show heightened activation in PFC regions suggesting their engagement in down-regulating activity in emotion-related region, such as the amygdala.

Understanding neural underpinnings of effective emotion regulation rests on considering specific forms of emotion regulation (for review, see Ochsner & Gross, 2005). A large body of work has focused on the neural underpinnings of reappraisal, most commonly using experimental designs in which participants are asked to view emotionally-relevant stimuli and to engage in emotion regulation or as a control condition, to simply view the stimulus. In a meta-analysis of 48 studies, reappraisal was consistently associated the frontoparietal network, including activation of the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC), dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), and deactivation of the amygdala (Buhle et al., 2014; Messina, Bianco, Sambin, & Viviani, 2015). Emotion distancing, a form of reappraisal in which people try to distance themselves from negative emotions by thinking of the stimulus as far away, long ago, or less relevant to them, also has been found across seven studies to activate the frontoparietal network (Belden, Pagliaccio, Murphy, Luby, & Barch, 2015; Lewis, Todd, & Honsberger, 2007). Of interest, this same frontoparietal network, has been tied to response inhibition(Aron, 2011; Braver, Paxton, Locke, & Barch, 2009; Jamadar, Fielding, & Egan, 2013). Finally, although less is known, emotion-related impulsivity has been linked to diminished activation of some of these same regions, including dACC as well as bilateral inferior frontal gyrus/insula during response inhibition (Wilbertz et al., 2014). Taken together, the findings highlight the range of neural regions that must be coordinated to produce effective regulation of emotion.

Recent meta-analysis findings suggest that rumination is also tied to regions of the PFC, although in a different manner. That is, rumination appears related to increased functional connectivity of the subgenual prefrontal cortex with the default-mode network, and to elevated activation of the medial PFC in particular (Disner, Beevers, Haigh, & Beck, 2011; Hamilton, Farmer, Fogelman, & Gotlib, 2015).

Much of the research on neural and cognitive mechanisms in emotion, though, is correlational. Certainly, deficits in executive function, such as the ability to inhibit interference from emotion stimuli on a working memory task, can prospectively predict rumination (Zetsche & Joormann, 2011). Moreover, addressing deficits in executive function via computerized training (involving working memory training, or the combination of working memory training with attentional control training) can lead to reductions in rumination in those diagnosed with clinical depression (Siegle et al., 2014; Siegle, Thompson, Carter, Steinhauer, & Thase, 2007; Vanderhasselt et al., 2015), in people with remitted depression (Hoorelbeke, Koster, Demeyer, Loeys, & Vanderhasselt, 2016), and in student samples (Hoorelbeke, Koster, Vanderhasselt, Callewaert, & Demeyer, 2015). In addition, 20 sessions of training on a working memory task involving affective stimuli enhanced the efficiency of the frontoparietal network, and in turn, enhanced ability to down-regulate response to an emotion picture (Schweizer, Grahn, Hampshire, Mobbs, & Dalgleish, 2013). These findings indicate that deficits in executive functions and related neural activity appear to exert an important influence on emotion regulation effectiveness. With these models as backdrop, we turn to the smaller literature on cognition and emotion in BD.

3.2. Links between cognition and emotion in bipolar disorder

Considerable neurobiological evidence supports the premise that prefrontal control of affective states is impaired in BD (e.g., Green, Cahill, & Malhi, 2007; Strakowski, 2012). Reviewers consistently conclude that BD is tied to elevated neural activation in regions involved in responding to salient stimuli, such as the amygdala and ventral striatum compared to other, as well as deficient activation of regions involved in emotion regulation, such as prefrontal cortical areas (Chen, Suckling, Lennox, Ooi, & Bullmore, 2011; Green et al., 2007; Strakowski, Delbello, & Adler, 2012). Reviewers have also identified a consistent pattern of diminished connectivity of regions involved in top-down control with those involved in reactivity for those with BD (Phillips, Ladouceur, & Drevets, 2008). Not only are key emotion-relevant neural regions impaired in BD, but specific work confirms that activity in these regions is relevant to effective emotion regulation for those with BD. For example, frontoparietal activation has been shown to correlate negatively with amygdala activation levels during reappraisal in BD I (Kanske, Schönfelder, Forneck, & Wessa, 2015). The imaging findings fit with a profile of heightened emotionality and diminished emotion regulation in BD.

Although the biology of BD has been proposed to give rise to pervasive difficulties regulating emotion (Ghaznavi & Deckersbach, 2012), surprisingly little behavioral research in BD has considered how specific cognitive deficits relate to emotion and emotion regulation strategies. One approach has been to consider whether the deficits in executive function observed among those with BD are heightened in the context of emotion stimuli as compared to non-emotion stimuli. A second approach has been to more directly consider the facets of cognition that relate to effective emotion regulation within BD. We consider both approaches here.

We begin by considering research on affective stimuli during executive function tasks. The BD studies on this front have focused on response inhibition tasks such as the go-no go or anti-saccade tasks (Van Rheenen & Rossell, 2013). Those with BD, even during remission, have been found to have difficulty on the affective go-no go task compared to control participants, while showing less impairment on non-affective trials (Bauer, Frazier, Meyer, Youngstrom, & Zunta–Soares, G. B., & Soares, J. C., 2015; Gopin, Burdick, Derosse,Goldberg, & Malhotra, 2011). Similar effects have been observed with a version of the antisaccade task. That is, persons with BD and those with ADHD showed comparable deficits in a standard version of the anti-saccade task; for those with BD, though, emotion faces interspersed between trials of the antisaccade task led to significant decay, such that those with BD performed significantly more poorly than those with ADHD when the emotion faces were presented between trials(Soncin, Brien, Coe, Marin, & Munoz, 2016). Beyond the use of distractors with negative valence, one team used a variant of the antisaccade task in which participants were asked to rapidly look away from positive stimuli on some trials, in contrast to neutral stimuli on other trials. Those with BD showed particular deficits compared to controls when asked to rapidly look away from emotion stimuli (Mueller et al., 2010).

Drawing on the behavioral evidence for difficulties with executive function when emotion stimuli are present, one neuroimaging study examined neural responses to an executive function task involving emotion stimuli among those with BD. More specifically, participants were asked to complete a working memory task (the N-back task) with and without emotional distractors (Mullin et al., 2012). Patients diagnosed with BD showed reduced activity in several areas implicated in executive function and regulation, such as the dorsolateral PFC, dACC and inferior parietal cortex, compared to healthy controls. When emotion distracters were present, however, the bipolar patients showed increased activity in these regions as well as in emotion processing regions such as the amygdala and striatum; the authors argued that this increased activity reflected compensatory responses due to difficulties in regulating in the face of emotion stimuli. Atypical connectivity was also observed between the dACC and the amygdala when emotion distractors were present. The findings support the idea that executive function may be particularly difficult for those with BD when emotion-relevant stimuli are present.

Researchers have extended this approach to consider whether deficits in response inhibition for emotion stimuli can be observed among unaffected family members of those with BD. In the first study, 20 unaffected siblings of those with BD showed a nonsignificant trend toward poor performance on the affective go-no go task compared to controls (Brand et al., 2012). In the second study, seven unaffected offspring did not display significant deficits on the affective go-no go task (Bauer et al., 2015). The null findings are difficult to interpret, though, given the small sample size and meta-analytic evidence that emotion stimuli are not related to performance on the go/no-go task for those with emotion disorders (Wright, Lipszyc, Dupuis, Thayapararajah, & Schachar, 2014). It will be important to consider response inhibition and emotion in at-risk populations using other tasks and larger samples.

Early findings on whether cognitive indices can predict emotion regulation among those with BD have been more mixed. In one study, a composite measure of executive function based on Trails, Stroop, and Matrics scores did not predict self-rated Difficulties with Emotion Regulation scores among 51 persons with BD (Van Rheenen & Rossell, 2014). Although that study relied on “cold cognition” measures that did not integrate emotion-relevant stimuli, a measure of cognitive flexibility (task-switching) in the context of affective faces was found to predict self-rated ability to use Reappraisal, as measured by the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire among persons with remitted BD (Gul & Khan, 2014). Although preliminary, these findings suggest that it will be important to consider the influence of affective stimuli and contexts in using neuropsychological indices to predict emotion regulation within BD.

4. Summary of cognition and emotion

A large body of basic behavioral and neural research suggests that effective emotion regulation rests on strong executive function, and particularly inhibition. Beyond the evidence that cognitive ability supports effective emotion regulation, the presence of heightened emotion appears to lead to decays in executive function ability. One might expect that persons with clinically relevant deficits in cognition, then, could be prone to heightened emotion states, which in turn, would contribute to decays in cognitive performance. BD, then, is a natural place to consider the interface between cognition and emotion.

Nonetheless, emotionality has often been considered separately from the cognitive deficits in BD. The few integrative findings within BD suggest a more nuanced profile of cognitive deficits than has been observed in the general neuropsychological literature. That is, those with BD show more pronounced difficulties on cognitive tasks that integrate emotion stimuli than tasks without such stimuli. Moreover, performance on tasks with emotion stimuli may be more powerfully predictive of emotion regulation capacity than tasks without such stimuli. This research highlights that in the study of cognition, researchers would do well to include measures that incorporate emotion stimuli, as those may be a more powerful window into BD vulnerability.

5. Future directions

The few available findings support the idea that cognitive deficits are centrally involved in the emotion difficulties observed in BD. This has some overlap with transdiagnostic models of the importance of cognitive deficits (Goodkind et al., 2015), and particularly executive function deficits, with emotion-related symptoms across syndromes (Snyder, Miyake, & Hankin, 2015). If the idea that cognition can explain the emotion problems is supported, this model provides a different way to view the source and treatment of emotion difficulties within BD. Before considering those implications, we turn to several questions that have not been addressed in the bipolar literature.

To begin, it is often assumed that either cognition or emotion are central starting points for the eventual cascade into bipolar symptoms. Nonetheless, multiple variables may be involved in shaping both cognition and emotion. Of particular relevance, among those with BD, early trauma and adversity are tied to deficits in both cognition (Savitz, Van Der Merwe, Stein, Solms, & Ramesar, 2008) and emotion lability (Aas et al., 2014). Overall, it will be important to consider contextual factors that might contribute to the overlap between cognition and emotion before concluding that cognition is truly a central driver for other outcomes. Given that emotion and cognitive difficulties have been observed in unaffected family members, genetically-informed designs are likely to be of particular importance in understanding these links.

Beyond a better understanding of the context linking emotion and cognition, relatively little is known about whether cognition can explain the full range of emotion problems observed in BD. To date, researchers have been particularly focused on emotion regulation, and yet emotion experience and reactivity may be more closely tethered to disorder. Given how tightly correlated reactivity and regulation are, these facets of emotionality should be conjointly investigated in relation to cognition. Laboratory studies which examine responses to standardized emotion-relevant challenges can be particularly powerful ways to consider the time course of initial reactivity and recovery. Understanding this question could help disentangle whether it is appropriate to consider cognition as a driver of all facets of the emotionality profile in BD, or more strictly of relevance to emotion regulation, or even regulation only of negative stimuli.

Perhaps of most importance, there are key questions about which facets of cognition are most centrally involved in emotionality within BD. Although early research has focused on specific facets of cognitive inhibition or executive function, it remains possible that a much more general cognitive factor guides performance on specific cognitive tasks, as well as a range of emotional and functional outcomes. The facets of executive function are highly correlated, and that there may be general factors that explain much of the variance in specific facets of executive function and that explain emotion-relevant symptoms (Snyder et al., 2015). If true, then hunting for the singular cognitive process that explains these problems could be a flawed approach.

In addition to considering that there may be a general cognitive vulnerability to multiple facets of executive function deficits, the bipolar literature may be prematurely focused only on executive function. That is, multiple studies indicate that a range of cognitive functions beyond executive function are requisite to effective emotion regulation. For example, memory is required to consider alternative perspectives (a key aspect of reappraisal) (Schneider, Gur, Gur, & Shtasel, 1995). Similarly, processing speed is relevant for rapidly processing dynamic emotion stimuli, such as facial expressions as they occur in the real world (De Sonneville et al., 2002). Verbal fluency has been found to mediate the ability to down-regulate responses to expected, but not unexpected, laboratory stimuli (Gyurak et al., 2012; Schmeichel & Tang, 2014). Drawing on such findings, Van Rheenen and Rossell (2013) argued that language processing, processing speed, memory, and attention might all be relevant for understanding emotion in BD. Disentangling the true nature of the cognitive deficits underpinning emotionality in BD will require work using a battery of cognitive indices, as well as more sophisticated modeling that considers latent variables constructed to consider the role to address measurement error.

Beyond the need for greater specificity of the models linking cognition and emotion, there is a need for understanding how cognitive deficits and emotionality conjointly relate to functional outcomes, given that emotion and cognitive deficits are both clearly linked to functional outcomes in BD when examined separately. At the current time, studies have not examined whether the effects of cognitive deficits fully explain the functional outcomes observed among those with emotion dysregulation. That is, it remains unclear whether emotion qualities will explain functional outcomes above and beyond the role of the cognitive deficits. Understanding the overlapping and unique contributions of cognition and emotion to functionality will be important in choosing the most important targets for intervention. Overall, given the complexity of ways in which cognition and emotion may interface, experimental designs would be particularly helpful to understand these relationships.

Notwithstanding the gaps in testing a fuller model of cognition and emotion in BD, findings of the current review have several implications. Most importantly, the idea that cognition might drive emotionality in this disorder suggests that treatment might focus on relieving cognitive deficits. This is a marked shift from current approaches to treatment, which employ many emotion regulation techniques drawn from cognitive behavioral therapy or Dialectical Behavior Therapy (Linehan, 1993). Although such work remains a vital part of the research agenda in BD, if cognitive deficits precede and drive the emotionality observed in BD, it may be more important to focus on relieving cognitive concerns and hope that this leads to emotion improvements.

Certain pharmacological treatments may help reduce cognitive deficits in BD. For example, cognition has been found to improve with treatment lamotrigine (Pavuluri, Passarotti, Mohammed, Carbray, & Sweeney, 2010) or mifepristone (Young et al., 2004) in BD. It would be helpful to map the time course of changes in cognition and whether this precedes improvements in emotion with receipt of these medications.

To the extent that emotion difficulties improve when cognition problems are treated, this would also provide a clearer test of directionality of links between these two domains. In this way, some of the recent cognitive remediation work is particularly relevant and may provide cleaner information about the direction of effects. To date, a 24-session cognitive remediation program targeting a broad array of 21 cognitive tasks was shown to lead to significant improvements in global executive function and depression, but did not significantly shift working memory in BD. In one open trial of a treatment for BD that included mood monitoring, as well as cognitive coaching on how to improve planning, attention, and memory, participants self-reported improved cognitive and occupational function (Deckersbach et al., 2010). While these studies indicate that cognitive training could potentially address core cognitive concerns in BD, no research is available on whether this improves emotion regulation.

Again, the literature outside of BD provides helpful insights. That is, cognitive remediation has been shown to be helpful in addressing emotionality when applied with persons who were not diagnosed with BD. In part, these programs may have been successful because they targeted cognitive domains that seem particularly relevant for emotion outcomes. For example, several investigators have developed programs to improve working memory when viewing negative stimuli or completing a stressful task. These working memory training programs have been found to be efficacious in reducing rumination and depressive symptoms (Cohen, Mor, & Henik, 2015; Hoorelbeke et al., 2015; Porter et al., 2017). Taken together, the success of the more specific cognitive remediation programs outside of BD highlight how important it will be to understand which facets of emotionality are driven by cognition, and which facets of cognition make the most appropriate targets in BD. Studies applying cognitive remediation in BD would do well to consider whether improving “cold” cognitive processes alone are sufficient to improve emotion outcomes, as some evidence indicates is the case, or whether cognitive engagement with emotional stimuli amplifies improvement. This line of research may contribute to the development of optimized treatments, based on specific profiles of neurocognitive abilities or emotion dysregulation.

In sum, researchers have gained considerable traction in understanding both cognition and emotion in BD. Given the considerable support for both domains as key facets of vulnerability to the disorder, it is time for a more integrative perspective. We hope this review provides some backdrop that will foster this quest.

HIGHLIGHTS.

  • People with bipolar disorder and their relatives often show neurocognitive deficits.

  • Emotion concerns are observed in bipolar disorder and those vulnerable to the disorder.

  • New models outside of bipolar disorder suggest these domains are linked.

  • It is important to integrate neurocognition and emotion research within bipolar disorder.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Devon Sandel for assistance with formatting and references.

Role of funding sources

Andrew Peckham was supported by NIMH Grant T32-MH089919 during the preparation of this manuscript. NIMH had no role in writing the manuscript or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Isabela M. M. lima was supported by CAPES Scholarship BEX 7302/15–0 during the preparation of this manuscript. CAPES Scholarship had no role in writing the manuscript or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Footnotes

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Aas M, Aminoff SR, Lagerberg TV, Etain B, Agartz I, Andreassen OA, & Melle I (2014). Affective lability in patients with bipolar disorders is associated with high levels of childhood trauma. Psychiatry Research, 218(1), 252–255. 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.03.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
  3. Aron AR (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: Developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological Psychiatry. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arts B, Jabben N, Krabbendam L, & Van Os J (2008). Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives. Psychological Medicine, 38(6), 771–785. 10.1017/S0033291707001675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ashby FG, Isen AM, & Turken AU (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Balanzá-Martínez V, Selva G, Martínez-Arán A, Prickaerts J, Salazar J, González-Pinto A, … Tabarés-Seisdedos R (2010). Neurocognition in bipolar disorders—A closer look at comorbidities and medications. European Journal of Pharmacology, 626(1), 87–96. 10.1016/j.ejphar.2009.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Banich MT, Mackiewicz KL, Depue BE, Whitmer AJ, Miller GA, & Heller W (2009). Cognitive control mechanisms, emotion and memory: A neural perspective with implications for psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(5), 613–630. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bauer IE, Frazier TW, Meyer TD, Youngstrom E, & Zunta–Soares GB, & Soares JC (2015). Affective processing in pediatric bipolar disorder and offspring of bipolar parents. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 25(9), 684–690. 10.1089/cap.2015.0076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Baune BT, Li X, & Beblo T (2013). Short-and long-term relationships between neurocognitive performance and general function in bipolar disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35(7), 759–774. 10.1080/13803395.2013.824071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Beckwé M, Deroost N, Koster EHW, De Lissnyder E, & De Raedt R (2014). Worrying and rumination are both associated with reduced cognitive control. Psychological Research, 78(5), 651–660. 10.1007/s00426-013-0517-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Belden AC, Pagliaccio D, Murphy ER, Luby JL, & Barch DM (2015). Neural activation during cognitive emotion regulation in previously depressed compared to healthy children: Evidence of specific alterations. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(9), 771–781. 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bernblum R, & Mor N (2010). Rumination and emotion-related biases in refreshing information. Emotion Washington, D.C 10(3), 423–432. 10.1037/a0018427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Birmaher B, Goldstein BI, Axelson DA, Monk K, Hickey MB, Fan J, … Kupfer DJ (2013). Mood lability among offspring of parents with bipolar disorder and community controls. Bipolar Disorders, 15(3), 253–263. 10.1111/bdi.12060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bora E, Yucel M, & Pantelis C (2009). Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: A meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. Journal of Affective Disorders, 113, 1), 1–20. 10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bora E, Yücel M, Pantelis C, & Berk M (2011). Meta-analytic review of neurocognition in bipolar II disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 123(3), 165–174. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01638.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bourne C, Aydemir O, Balanzá-Martínez V, Bora E, Brissos S, Cavanagh JTO, … Goodwin GM (2013). Neuropsychological testing of cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar disorder: An individual patient data meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 128(3), 149–162. 10.1111/acps.12133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brand JG, Goldberg TE, Gunawardane N, Gopin CB, Powers RL, Malhotra AK, & Burdick KE (2012). Emotional bias in unaffected siblings of patients with bipolar i disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 136(3), 1053–1058. 10.1016/j.jad.2011.11.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Braver TS, Paxton JL, Locke HS, & Barch DM (2009). Flexible neural mechanisms of cognitive control within human prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(18), 7351–7356. 10.1073/pnas.0808187106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wage TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, … Ochsner KN (2014). Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: A meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 24(11), 2981–2990. 10.1093/cercor/bht154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chang KD, Blasey CM, Ketter TA, & Steiner H (2003). Temperament characteristics of child and adolescent bipolar offspring. Journal of Affective Disorders, 77(1), 11–19. 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00105-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chang KD, Steiner H, & Ketter TA (2000). Psychiatric phenomenology of child and adolescent bipolar offspring. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(4), 453–460. 10.1097/00004583-200004000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chaves OC, Lombardo LE, Bearden CE, Woolsey MD, Martinez DM, Barrett JA, … Glahn DC (2011). Association of clinical symptoms and neurocognitive performance in bipolar disorder: A longitudinal study. Bipolar Disorders, 13(1), 118–123. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00888.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Chen CH, Suckling J, Lennox BR, Ooi C, & Bullmore ET (2011). A quantitative meta-analysis of fMRI studies in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 13(1), 1–15. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00893.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Chiu PH, Holmes AJ, & Pizzagalli DA (2008). Dissociable recruitment of rostral anterior cingulate and inferior frontal cortex in emotional response inhibition. NeuroImage, 42(2), 988–997. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cohen N, Mor N, & Henik A (2015). Linking executive control and emotional response: A training procedure to reduce rumination. Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 3, 15–25. 10.1177/2167702614530114. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Copeland LA, Miller AL, Welsh DE, McCarthy JF, Zeber JE, & Kilbourne AM (2009). Clinical and demographic factors associated with homelessness and incarceration among VA patients with bipolar disorder. American Journal of Public Health, 99(5), 871–877. 10.2105/AJPH.2008.149989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Coryell W, Endicott J, Keller M, Andreasen N, Grove W, Hirschfeld RMA, & Scheftner W (1989). Bipolar affective disorder and high achievement: A familial association. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 983–988. 10.1176/ajp.146.8.983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Davis RN, & Nolen-Hoeksema S (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24(6), 699–711. 10.1023/A:1005591412406. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. De Sonneville LMJ, Verschoor CA, Njiokiktjien C, Op het Veld V, Toorenaar N, & Vranken M (2002). Facial identity and facial emotions: Speed, accuracy, and processing strategies in children and adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(2), 200–213. 10.1076/jcen.24.2.200.989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Deckersbach T, Nierenberg AA, Kessler R, Lund HG, Ametrano RM, Sachs G, … Dougherty D (2010). Cognitive rehabilitation for bipolar disorder: An open trial for employed patients with residual depressive symptoms. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 16(5), 298–307. 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2009.00110.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Depp CA, Mausbach BT, Harmell AL, Savla GN, Bowie CR, Harvey PD, & Patterson TL (2012). Meta-analysis of the association between cognitive abilities and everyday functioning in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 14(3), 217–226. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01011.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Depue RA, Krauss S, Spoont MR, & Arbisi P (1989). General behavior inventory identification of unipolar and bipolar affective conditions in a nonclinical university population. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98(2), 117–126. 10.1037/0021-843X.98.2.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Diamond A (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 64,135–168. 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dillon DG, & Pizzagalli DA (2007). Inhibition of action, thought, and emotion: A selective neurobiological review. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12(3), 99–114. 10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Disner SG, Beevers CG, Haigh EAP, & Beck AT (2011). Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(8), 467–477. 10.1038/nrn3027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dreisbach G, & Goschke T (2004). How positive affect modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 343–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Eckblad M, & Chapman LJ (1986). Development and validation of a scale for hypo-manic personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(3), 214–222. 10.1037/0021-843X.95.3.214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Edge MD, Miller CJ, Muhtadie L, Johnson SL, Carver CS, Marquinez N, & Gotlib IH (2013). People with bipolar I disorder report avoiding rewarding activities and dampening positive emotion. Journal of Affective Disorders, 146(3), 407–413. 10.1016/j.jad.2012.07.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Etkin A, Büchel C, & Gross JJ (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(11), 693–700. 10.1038/nrn4044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Frangou S, Haldane M, Roddy D, & Kumari V (2005). Evidence for deficit in tasks of ventral but not dorsal prefrontal executive function as an endophenotypic marker for bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 838–839. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Fredrickson BL (2003). The value of positive emotions. American Scientist, 91, 330–335. [Google Scholar]
  42. Gale CR, Batty GD, McIntosh AM, Porteous DJ, Deary IJ, & Rasmussen F (2013). Is bipolar disorder more common in highly intelligent people? A cohort study of a million men. Molecular Psychiatry, 18(2), 190–194. 10.1038/mp.2012.26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ghaznavi S, & Deckersbach T (2012). Rumination in bipolar disorder: Evidence for an unquiet mind. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 2(1), 2 10.1186/2045-5380-2-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gilbert KE, Nolen-Hoeksema S, & Gruber J (2013). Positive emotion dysregulation across mood disorders: How amplifying versus dampening predicts emotional reactivity and illness course. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51, 736–741. 10.1016/j.brat.2013.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gitlin MJ, Swendsen J, Heller TL, & Hammen C (1995). Relapse and impairment in bipolar disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(11), 1635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Goodkind M, Eickhoff SB, Oathes DJ, Jiang Y, Chang A, Jones-Hagata LB, … Etkin A (2015). Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(4), 305–315. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Gopin CB, Burdick KE, Derosse P, Goldberg TE, & Malhotra AK (2011). Emotional modulation of response inhibition in stable patients with bipolar I disorder: A comparison with healthy and schizophrenia subjects. Bipolar Disorders, 13(2), 164–172. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00906.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Green MJ, Cahill CM, & Malhi GS (2007). The cognitive and neurophysiological basis of emotion dysregulation in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 103(1–3), 29–42. 10.1016/j.jad.2007.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Green MJ, Lino BJ, Hwang EJ, Sparks A, James C, & Mitchell PB (2011). Cognitive regulation of emotion in bipolar I disorder and unaffected biological relatives. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 124, 307–316. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01718.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Gruber J, Dutra S, Eidelman P, Johnson SL, & Harvey AG (2011). Emotional and physiological responses to normative and idiographic positive stimuli in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 133(3), 437–442. 10.1016/j.jad.2011.04.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Gruber J, Eidelman P, & Harvey AG (2008). Transdiagnostic emotion regulation processes in bipolar disorder and insomnia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(9), 1096–1100. 10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Gruber J, Harvey AG, & Gross JJ (2012). When trying is not enough: Emotion regulation and the effort–success gap in bipolar disorder. Emotion, 12(5), 997 10.1037/a0026822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Gruber J, Harvey AG, & Johnson SL (2009). Reflective and ruminative processing of positive emotional memories in bipolar disorder and healthy controls. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(8), 697–704. 10.1016/j.brat.2009.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Gruber J, Harvey AG, & Purcell A (2011). What goes up can come down? A preliminary investigation of emotion reactivity and emotion recovery in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 133(3), 457–466. 10.1016/j.jad.2011.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gruber J, Kogan A, Mennin D, & Murray G (2013). Real-world emotion? An experience-sampling approach to emotion experience and regulation in bipolar I disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(4), 971–983. 10.1037/a0034425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Gualtieri CT, & Morgan DW (2008). The frequency of cognitive impairment in patients with anxiety, depression, and bipolar disorder: An unaccounted source of variance in clinical trials. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(7), 1122–1130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gul A, & Khan K (2014). Emotion regulation strategies can predict task-switching abilities in euthymic bipolar patients. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(847), 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Gyurak A, Goodkind MS, Kramer JH, Miller BL, & Levenson RW (2012).Executive functions and the down-regulation and up-regulation of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 103–118. 10.1080/02699931.2011.557291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Gyurak A, Goodkind MS, Madan A, Kramer JH, Miller BL, & Levenson RW (2009). Do tests of executive functioning predict ability to downregulate emotions spontaneously and when instructed to suppress? Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 144–152. 10.3758/CABN.9.2.144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Hamilton JP, Farmer M, Fogelman P, & Gotlib IH (2015). Depressive rumination, the default-mode network, and the dark matter of clinical neuroscience. Biological Psychiatry, 78, 224–230. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Harmon-Jones E, & Gable PA (2009). Neural activity underlying the effect of approach-motivated positive affect on narrowed attention. Psychological Science, 20, 406–409. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02302.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Harmon-Jones E, & Gable PA (2010). The blues broaden, but the nasty narrows: Attentional consequences of negative affects low and high in motivational intensity. Psychological Science, 21, 211–215. 10.1177/0956797609359622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Heerlein AS, Richter P, Gonzalez M, & Santander J (1998). Personality patterns and outcome in depressive and bipolar disorders. Psychopathology, 31(1), 15–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hendricks MA, & Buchanan TW (2016). Individual differences in cognitive control processes and their relationship to emotion regulation. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 912–924. 10.1080/02699931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Higier RG, Jimenez AM, Hultman CM, Borg J, Roman C, Kizling I, … Cannon TD (2014). Enhanced neurocognitive functioning and positive temperament in twins discordant for bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(11), 1191–1198. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13121683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, & Baddeley AD (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 174–180. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Hoorelbeke K, Koster EH, Demeyer I, Loeys T, & Vanderhasselt M-A (2016). Effects of cognitive control training on the dynamics of (mal) adaptive emotion regulation in daily life. Emotion, 16, 945–956. 10.1037/emo0000169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Hoorelbeke K, Koster EHW, Vanderhasselt M-A, Callewaert S, & Demeyer I (2015). The influence of cognitive control training on stress reactivity and rumination in response to a lab stressor and naturalistic stress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 69, 1–10. 10.1016/j.brat.2015.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Ilgen MA, Bohnert AS, Ignacio RV, McCarthy JF, Valenstein MM, Kim HM, & Blow FC (2010). Psychiatric diagnoses and risk of suicide in veterans. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(11), 1152–1158. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Jamadar SD, Fielding J, & Egan GF (2013). Quantitative meta-analysis of fMRI and PET studies reveals consistent activation in fronto-striatal-parietal regions and cerebellum during antisaccades and prosaccades. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1152–1158. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Jamison KR (1993). Touched with Fire: Manic-Depressive Illness and the Artistic Temperament. New York, NY: Free Press. [Google Scholar]
  72. Johnson SL, & Carver CS (2016). Emotion-relevant impulsivity predicts sustained anger and aggression after remission in bipolar I disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 189, 169–175. 10.1016/j.jad.2015.07.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Johnson SL, Carver CS, Mulé S, & Joormann J (2013). Impulsivity and risk for mania: Toward greater specificity. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 86(4), 401–412. 10.1111/j.2044-8341.2012.02078.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Johnson SL, Carver CS, & Tharp JA (2017). Suicidality in bipolar disorder: The role of emotion-triggered impulsivity. Suicide and Life-threatening Behavior, 47, 177–192. 10.1111/sltb.12274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Johnson SL, McKenzie G, & McMurrich S (2008). Ruminative responses to negative and positive affect among students diagnosed with bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(5), 702–713. 10.1007/s10608-007-9158-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Johnson SL, Tharp JA, Peckham AD, & McMaster KJ (2016). Emotion in bipolar I disorder: Implications for functional and symptom outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 40–52. 10.1037/abn0000116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Johnson SL, Tharp JA, Peckham AD, Sanchez AH, & Carver CS (2016). Positive urgency is related to difficulty inhibiting prepotent responses. Emotion, 16, 750–759. 10.1037/emo0000182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Joormann J, & Vanderlind WM (2014). Emotion regulation in depression the role of biased cognition and reduced cognitive control. Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 2(4), 402–421. 10.1177/2167702614536163. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  79. Kahl KG, Winter L, & Schweiger U (2012). The third wave of cognitive behavioural therapies. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 25, 522–528. 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328358e531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Kanske P, Schönfelder S, Forneck J, & Wessa M (2015). Impaired regulation of emotion: Neural correlates of reappraisal and distraction in bipolar disorder and unaffected relatives. Translational Psychiatry, 5(1), e497 doi.org/10.1038%2Ftp.2014.137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Keitner GI, Solomon DA, Ryan CE, Miller IW, Mallinger A, Kupfer DJ, & Frank E (1996). Prodromal and residual symptoms in bipolar I disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 37(5), 362–367. 10.1016/S0010-440X(96)90018-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Kieseppä T, Partonen T, Haukka J, Kaprio J, & Lönnqvist J (2014). High concordance of bipolar I disorder in a nationwide sample of twins. American Journal of Psychiatry. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Klimes-Dougan B, Ronsaville D, Wiggs EA, & Martinez PE (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in adolescent children of mothers with a history of bipolar or major depressive disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 60(9), 957–965. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Knowles R, Tai S, Jones SH, Highfield J, Morriss R, & Bentall RP (2007). Stability of self-esteem in bipolar disorder: Comparisons among remitted bipolar patients, remitted unipolar patients and healthy controls. Bipolar Disorders, 9, 490–495. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00457.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Kochman FJ, Hantouche EG, Ferrari P, Lancrenon S, Bayart D, & Akiskal HS (2005). Cyclothymic temperament as a prospective predictor of bipolarity and suicidality in children and adolescents with major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 10.1016/j.jad.2003.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Kurtz MM, & Gerraty RT (2009). A meta-analytic investigation of neurocognitive deficits in bipolar illness: Profile and effects of clinical state. Neuropsychology, 23, 551–562. 10.1037/a0016277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Lera-Miguel S, Andres-Perpina S, Fatjo-Vilas M, Fananas L, & Lazaro L (2015). Two-year follow-up of treated adolescents with early-onset bipolar disorder: Changes in neurocognition. Journal of Affective Disorders, 172, 48–54. 10.1016/j.jad.2014.09.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Lewis MD, Todd RM, & Honsberger MJ (2007). Event-related potential measures of emotion regulation in early childhood. Neuroreport, 18, 61–65. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328010a216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lezak MD (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). Neuropsychological assessment(3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford Univeristy Press. [Google Scholar]
  90. Linehan MM (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder.New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  91. Mackala SA, Torres IJ, Kozicky J, Michalak EE, & Yatham LN (2014). Cognitive performance and quality of life early in the course of bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 168, 119–124. 10.1016/j.jad.2014.06.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Malooly AM, Genet JJ, & Siemer M (2013). Individual differences in reappraisal effectiveness: The role of affective flexibility. Emotion, 13, 302–313. 10.1037/a0029980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Martino DJ, Strejilevich SA, Marengo E, Ibañez A, Scápola M, & Igoa A (2014). Toward the identification of neurocognitive subtypes in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 167, 118–124. 10.1016/j.jad.2014.05.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Maziade M, Rouleau N, Gingras N, Boutin P, Paradis M-E, Jomphe V, … Lefebvre A-A (2009). Shared neurocognitive dysfunctions in young offspring at extreme risk for schizophrenia or bipolar disorder in eastern quebec multigenerational families.Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35, 919–930. 10.1093/schbul/sbn058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. McRae K, Jacobs SE, Ray RD, John OP, & Gross JJ (2012). Individual differences in reappraisal ability: Links to reappraisal frequency, well-being, and cognitive control. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 2–7. 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.10.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  96. Merikangas KR, Jin R, He J-P, Kessler RC, Lee S, Sampson NA, … Karam EG (2011). Prevalence and correlates of bipolar spectrum disorder in the world mental health survey initiative. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 241–251. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Messina I, Bianco S, Sambin M, & Viviani R (2015). Executive and semantic processes in reappraisal of negative stimuli: Insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 6–956. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Meyer SE, Carlson GA, Wiggs EA, Martinez PE, Ronsaville DS, Klimes Dougan B, & Radke Yarrow M (2004). A prospective study of the association among impaired executive functioning, childhood attentional problems, and the development of bipolar disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 461–476. 10.1017/S095457940404461X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, & Wager TD (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Mora E, Portella MJ, Forcada I, Vieta E, & Mur M (2013). Persistence of cognitive impairment and its negative impact on psychosocial functioning in lithium-treated, euthymic bipolar patients: A 6-year follow-up study. Psychological Medicine, 43, 1187–1196. 10.1017/S0033291712001948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Mueller SC, Ng P, Temple V, Hardin MG, Pine DS, Leibenluft E, & Ernst M (2010). Perturbed reward processing in pediatric bipolar disorder: An antisaccade study. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 24, 1779–1784. 10.1177/0269881109353462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Muhtadie L, Johnson SL, Carver CS, Gotlib IH, & Ketter TA (2014). A profile approach to impulsivity in bipolar disorder: The key role of strong emotions. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 129, 100–108. 10.1111/acps.12136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Mullin BC, Perlman SB, Versace A, de Almeida JR, LaBarbara EJ, Klein C, … Phillips ML (2012). An fMRI study of attentional control in the context of emotional distracters in euthymic adults with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 201, 196–205. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Murray G, & Johnson SL (2010). The clinical significance of creativity in bipolar disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 721–732. 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Nieto RG, & Castellanos FX (2011). A meta-analysis of neuropsychological functioning in patients with early onset schizophrenia and pediatric bipolar disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, 266–280. 10.1080/15374416.2011.546049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Nordentoft M, Mortensen PB, & Pedersen CB (2011). Absolute risk of suicide after first hospital contact in mental disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 1058–1064. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Oatley K, & Johnson-Laird PN (2014). Cognitive approaches to emotions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 134–140. 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Ochsner KN, & Gross JJ (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 242–249. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC, Robertson ER, Chopra S, Gabrieli JDE, &Gross JJ (2004). For better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage, 23(2), 483–499. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Olvet DM, Burdick KE, & Cornblatt BA (2013). Assessing the potential to use neurocognition to predict who is at risk for developing bipolar disorder: A review of the literature. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 18, 129–145. 10.1080/13546805.2012.724193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Pavlova B, Uher R, Dennington L, Wright K, & Donaldson C (2011). Reactivity of affect and self-esteem during remission in bipolar affective disorder: An experimental investigation. Journal of Affective Disorders, 134, 102–111. 10.1016/j.jad.2011.04.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Pavuluri MN, Passarotti AM, Mohammed T, Carbray JA, & Sweeney JA (2010). Enhanced working and verbal memory after lamotrigine treatment in pediatric bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 12, 213–220. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00792.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Pessoa L (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 160–166. 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Peters AT, Peckham AD, Stange JP, Sylvia LG, Hansen NS, Salcedo S, &Deckersbach T (2014). Correlates of real world executive dysfunction in bipolar I disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 53, 87–93. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Phillips ML, Ladouceur CD, & Drevets WC (2008). A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: Implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 13, 833–857. 10.1038/mp.2008.65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Porter RJ, Hammar Å, Beevers CG, Bowie CR, Nodtvedt ØO, Peckham AD, … Johnson SL (2017). Cognitive and affective remediation training for mood disorders. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 51(4), 317–319. 10.1177/0004867416678079 (Epub 2016 Nov 12). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Quinn ME, & Joormann J (2015). Stress-induced changes in executive control are associated with depression symptoms examining the role of rumination. Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 3, 628–636. 10.1177/2167702614563930. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  118. Ratheesh A, Lin A, Nelson B, Wood SJ, Brewer W, Betts J, … Bechdolf A (2013).Neurocognitive functioning in the prodrome of mania - an exploratory study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 147(1–3), 441–445. 10.1016/j.jad.2012.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Rich BA, Holroyd T, Carver FW, Onelio LM, Mendoza JK, Cornwell BR, … Leibenluft E (2010). A preliminary study of the neural mechanisms of frustration in pediatric bipolar disorder using magnetoencephalography. Depression and Anxiety, 27(3), 276–286. 10.1002/da.20649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Robinson LJ, Thompson JM, Gallagher P, Goswami U, Young AH, Ferrier IN, & Moore PB (2006). A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 93, 105–115. 10.1016/j.jad.2006.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Rowland JE, Hamilton MK, Lino BJ, Ly P, Denny K, Hwang EJ, … Green MJ (2013). Cognitive regulation of negative affect in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research, 208, 21–28. 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.02.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Samamé C, Martino DJ, & Strejilevich SA (2012). Social cognition in euthymic bipolar disorder: Systematic review and meta-analytic approach. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125, 266–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Samamé C, Martino DJ, & Strejilevich SA (2014). Longitudinal course of cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder: A meta-analytic study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 164, 130–138. 10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Sánchez-Cubillo I, Perianez JA, Adrover-Roig D, Rodriguez-Sanchez JM, Rios-Lago M, Tirapu JEEA, & Barcelo F (2009). Construct validity of the trail making test: Role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 438–450. 10.1017/S1355617709090626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Santos JL, Aparicio A, Bagney A, Sánchez-Morla EM, Rodríguez-Jiménez R, Mateo J, & Jiménez-Arriero MÁ (2014). A five-year follow-up study of neuro-cognitive functioning in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 16, 722–731. 10.1111/bdi.12215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Savitz JB, Van Der Merwe L, Stein DJ, Solms M, & Ramesar RS (2008). Neuropsychological task performance in bipolar spectrum illness: Genetics, alcohol abuse, medication and childhood trauma. Bipolar Disorders, 10, 479–494. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00591.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Schmeichel BJ, & Tang D (2014). The relationship between individual differences in executive functioning and emotion regulation: A comprehensive review In Forgas JP, & Harmon-Jones E (Eds.). The control within: Motivation and its regulation. New York: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  128. Schmeichel BJ, & Tang D (2015). Individual differences in executive functioning and their relationship to emotional processes and responses. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 93–98. 10.1177/0963721414555178. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  129. Schmeichel BJ, Volokhov RN, & Demaree HA (2008). Working memory capacity and the self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1526–1540. 10.1037/a0013345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Schneider F, Gur RC, Gur RE, & Shtasel DL (1995). Emotional processing in schizophrenia: Neurobehavioral probes in relation to psychopathology. Schizophrenia Research, 17, 67–75. 10.1016/0920-9964(95)00031-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Schweizer S, Grahn J, Hampshire A, Mobbs D, & Dalgleish T (2013). Training the emotional brain: Improving affective control through emotional working memory training. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 5301–5311. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2593-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Siegle GJ, Price RB, Jones NP, Ghinassi F, Painter T, & Thase ME (2014). You gotta work at it: Pupillary indices of task focus are prognostic for response to a neurocognitive intervention for rumination in depression. Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 2, 455–471. 10.1177/2167702614536160. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  133. Siegle GJ, Thompson W, Carter CS, Steinhauer SR, & Thase ME (2007). Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in uni-polar depression: Related and independent features. Biological Psychiatry, 61(2), 198–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Snyder HR, Miyake A, & Hankin BL (2015). Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: Bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(328), 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Soncin S, Brien DC, Coe BC, Marin A, & Munoz DP (2016). Contrasting emotion processing and executive functioning in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and bipolar disorder. Behavioral Neuroscience, 130(5), 531–543. 10.1037/bne0000158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Strakowski S (2012). The bipolar brain: Integrating neuroimaging and genetics. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  137. Strakowski SM, Delbello MP, & Adler CM (2012). The functional neuroanatomy of bipolar disorder: A review of neuroimaging findings. Molecular Psychiatry, 10(1), 105–116. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Strakowski SM, Keck PE, McElroy SL, West SA, Sax KW, Hawkins JM, … Bourne ML (1998). Twelve-month outcome after a first hospitalization for affective psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 49–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Szmulewicz AG, Samamé C, Martino DJ, & Strejilevich SA (2015). An updated review on the neuropsychological profile of subjects with bipolar disorder. Archives of Clinical Psychiatry, 42, 139–146. 10.1590/0101-60830000000064. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  140. Thomas J, Knowles R, Tai S, & Bentall RP (2007). Response styles to depressed mood in bipolar affective disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 100, 249–252. 10.1016/j.jad.2006.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Tiihonen J, Haukka J, Henriksson M, Cannon M, Kieseppä T, Laaksonen I, … Lönnqvist J (2005). Premorbid intellectual functioning in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: Results from a cohort study of male conscripts. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1904–1910. 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Van Rheenen TE, & Rossell SL (2013). Genetic and neurocognitive foundations of emotion abnormalities in bipolar disorder. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 18, 168–207. 10.1080/13546805.2012.690938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Van Rheenen TE, & Rossell SL (2014). Objective and subjective psychosocial functioning in bipolar disorder: An investigation of the relative importance of neuro-cognition, social cognition and emotion regulation. Journal of Affective Disorders, 162, 134–141. 10.1016/j.jad.2014.03.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Vanderhasselt M-A, De Raedt R, Namur V, Lotufo PA, Bensenor IM, Boggio PS, & Brunoni AR (2015). Transcranial electric stimulation and neurocognitive training in clinically depressed patients: A pilot study of the effects on rumination. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 57, 93–99. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Victor SE, Johnson SL, & Gotlib IH (2011). Quality of life and impulsivity in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 13, 303–309. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00919.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. von Hippel W, & Gonsalkorale K (2005). “That is bloody revolting!” Inhibitory control of thoughts better left unsaid. Psychological Science, 16(7), 497–500. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Wilbertz T, Deserno L, Horstmann A, Neumann J, Villringer A, Heinze HJ, … Schlagenhauf F (2014). Response inhibition and its relation to multidimensional impulsivity. NeuroImage, 103, 241–248. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. World Health Organization; (1993). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: diagnostic criteria for research (Vol. 2). World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
  149. World Health Organization. World Report on Disability: Summary. (2011). 2011, WHO/NMH/VIP/11.01 www.refworld.org/docid/50854a322.html (accessed 9/292017). [PubMed]
  150. Wright L, Lipszyc J, Dupuis A, Thayapararajah SW, & Schachar R (2014). Response inhibition and psychopathology: A meta-analysis of go/no-go task performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123, 429–439. 10.1037/a0036295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Young AH, Gallagher P, Watson S, Del-Estal D, Owen BM, & Nicol Ferrier I (2004). Improvements in neurocognitive function and mood following adjunctive treatment with mifepristone (RU-486) in bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 1538–1545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Zahn-Waxler C, Cummings EM, McKnew DH, & Radke-Yarrow M (1984).Altruism, aggression, and social interactions in young children with a manic-depressive parent. Child Development, 55, 112–122. 10.2307/1129838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Zahn-Waxler C, McKnew DH, Cummings EM, Davenport YB, & Radke-Yarrow M (1984). Problem behaviors and peer interactions of young children with a manic-depressive parent. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 236–242. 10.1176/ajp.141.2.236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Zetsche U, & Joormann J (2011). Components of interference control predict depressive symptoms and rumination cross-sectionally and at six months follow-up. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42, 65–73. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES