Abstract
Although problematic media use among adolescents is of wide interest, less is known regarding problematic media use among younger children. The current study reports on the development and validation of a parent-report measure of one potential aspect of children’s problematic use-screen media addiction-via the Problematic Media Use Measure (PMUM). Items were based on the nine criteria for Internet Gaming Disorder in the DSM-5. The first study describes the development and preliminary validation of the PMUM in a sample of 291 mothers. Mothers (80.8% identified as White) of children 4 through 11 years of age completed the PMUM and measures of child screen time and child psychosocial functioning. EFA indicated a unidimensional construct of screen media addiction. The final versions of the PMUM (27 items) and PMUM Short Form (PMUM-SF, 9 items) evidenced high internal consistency (Cronbach α = .97 and α = .93, respectively). Regression analyses were conducted to examine convergent validity of the PMUM with indicators of child psychosocial functioning. Convergent validity was supported and the PMUM scales also independently predicted children’s total difficulties in functioning, over and above hours of screen time, indicating incremental validity. The second study sought to confirm the factor structure of the PMUM-SF and test for measurement invariance across gender. In a sample of 632 parents, we confirmed the factor structure of the PMUM-SF and found measurement invariance for boys and girls. These studies support the use of the PMUM-SF as a measure of screen media addiction in children ages 4 through 11 years old.
Keywords: problematic media use, screen addiction, internet gaming disorder, children, mobile device
At increasingly younger ages, children have access to mobile media devices (e.g., tablets and smartphones (Rideout, 2013) and evidence suggests that mobile device ownership and usage is growing even among toddler- and preschool-aged children (Kabali et al., 2015). Given the increased use of screen media, public health experts and researchers have advocated for increased research on “addiction” to the internet and electronic screen products in children (Felt & Robb, 2016; World Health Organization, 2015). Although research on problematic media use among adolescent and young adult samples has grown, limited research has considered whether younger children exhibit addictive media use. The aim of this study, therefore, is to develop and validate a parent-report measure that could be used to screen for addictive media use in children under age 12 years.
Defining Problematic and Addictive Media Use in Children
Problematic media use in adolescents has been studied across many types of screen media (e.g., pathological gaming or game addiction: (Gentile, 2009; Rehbein, Kleimann, & Mößle, 2010; Spekman, Konijn, Roelofsma, & Griffiths, 2013; problematic internet use: Jelenchick et al., 2014; Moreno, Jelenchick, Cox, Young, & Christakis, 2011; Moreno, Jelenchick, & Christakis, 2013; compulsive texting: Foerster, Roser, Schoeni, & Röösli, 2015; Lister-Landman, Domoff, & Dubow, 2015; and problematic mobile phone use: Foerster et al., 2015). One of the most widely studied types of problematic media use among adolescents is pathological gaming. Pathological gaming consists of excessive and persistent involvement with video games that interferes with a youth’s psychological, social, family, and school functioning (Gentile, 2009). Criteria used to measure pathological gaming include symptoms such as salience or preoccupation with playing video games, tolerance (the need to use video games for increasingly longer times to achieve the same effect), unsuccessful attempts to control use, loss of interest in other activities, and use to improve mood or escape negative feelings (Lemmens, Valkenburg, & Peter, 2011). Recently, the American Psychiatric Association (APA) has listed these criteria, and others, as proposed symptoms of Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) in Section 3 of the DSM-5 (APA, 2013).
The inclusion of IGD in Section 3 of the DSM-5 has provided the field with a consensus of criteria that may underlie internet- or screen-based behavioral addictions. As such, these criteria have been applied to other types of behavioral addictions (e.g., social media addiction: (van den Eijnden, Lemmens, & Valkenburg, 2016) and provide the theoretical basis for the present study. As has been noted by others (e.g., van den Eijnden et al., 2016), the underlying assumption of using IGD criteria to examine various screen media addictions is the premise that addictions to screen media consist of the same diagnostic criteria, because they reflect different types of the overarching construct of Internet Addiction (van den Eijnden et al., 2016). IGD criteria have primarily been examined in adolescents (> age 12 years) outside of the United States (e.g., Netherlands: Lemmens, Valkenburg, & Gentile, 2015; Hungary: Király et al., 2015; Finland: Männikkö, Billieux, & Kääriäinen, 2015; Spain: Fuster, Carbonell, Pontes, & Griffiths, 2016). As the etiology of IGD or other addictive screen media use in adolescents is unknown (Groves et al., 2015) it is important to determine whether signs of problematic media use can be identified and measured earlier in development.
Addictive media use symptoms in children (< 12 years) may present differently than in adolescents. Children are more reliant on parents for media access compared to adolescents, who have relatively more autonomy in their media use and have higher personal media ownership rates (Rideout, 2015). Therefore, the IGD symptom of “preoccupation” in younger children may be experienced by parents as frequent or persistent requests or strategies to access media, whereas “preoccupation” in adolescents is defined as intrusive cognitions about playing video games (Groves et al., 2015). Addictive use in children may also manifest as vehement resistance to limit setting by parents regarding media use. Such behaviors may interfere with home life (e.g., conflict with siblings about media use, disruption of family routines, parent-child conflict) as well as school functioning (e.g., delaying or avoiding school work in order to use media). Screen media addiction may also disrupt a child’s social functioning and possibly interfere with longer-term development of social competence (e.g., by displacing face-to-face peer interaction; Uhls et al., 2014). Unlike adolescents, who have the capacity to self-reflect and may be able to report on whether problematic media use disrupts their functioning in these areas, children are likely unaware of such impact. Therefore, adults in the child’s life (e.g., parents, teachers) who witness the potential dysfunction associated with the child’s media use are the preferred reporters for problematic media use in younger children. Taken together, assessment of problematic media use in children will need to consider the potential developmental differences in problematic use in this younger demographic and, like other measures of child psychosocial difficulties, is best measured using caregiver reports.
Current Studies
The primary goal of the first study was to develop a parent report measure of children’s addictive use of screen media, the Problematic Media Use Measure (PMUM). To do so, we use the DSM-5 criteria for IGD (see Method for full description of measure development). A critique of the prior development of measures of device or media addiction is the proliferation of measures that differ from each other, but purport to measure the same construct (e.g., inconsistently using substance use disorder criteria and/or pathological gambling criteria; Lemmens et al., 2015; Petry et al., 2014; van den Eijnden et al., 2016). With the expert consensus on IGD criteria, it has been emphasized that researchers use common methodology to assess IGD (Petry et al., 2014) and other screen-based behavioral addictions like social media disorder (van den Eijinden et al., 2016). As such, we use all nine IGD criteria to measure screen media addiction in young children. A second aim of this study was to test the reliability and validity of the PMUM in a sample of mothers of children ages 4 through 11 years old. To test convergent validity, we examined correlations between PMUM scores and other measures that have been previously associated with other types of addictive media use (i.e., screen time and psychosocial difficulties) or should theoretically associate with addiction (i.e., mothers’ reported concern about their child’s media use). To examine incremental validity, we tested whether PMUM scores uniquely predict child psychosocial functioning, over and above screen time.
In Study 2, our aim was to confirm the factor structure of the PMUM that emerged in Study 1, and to test for factor and structural invariance by gender. Finally, an additional examination of the PMUM’s convergent validity was conducted in this independent sample.
Study 1
Method
Participants and Procedure
The IRB of the authors’ institution reviewed this study and determined that this research was exempt. In December 2015, participants were recruited through Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk). MTurk has been found to be a reliable and inexpensive way to recruit participants (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; Casler, Bickel, & Hackett, 2013; Shapiro, Chandler, & Mueller, 2013). For example, Mturk respondents have been found to be more representative of the United States (US) population than convenience samples recruited for in-person data collection (Berinsky, Huber, & Lenz, 2012), and has been successfully used to study family processes and youth psychosocial functioning (Schleider & Weisz, 2015). MTurk workers who resided in the US and who had been qualified as “master” workers by Amazon Mturk were able to view the recruitment posting. Participants were compensated $0.40.
The participant had to be the mother of a child within the ages of 4 years through 11 years. We chose to include only mothers because prior research has indicated that mothers are more likely than fathers to engage in certain media parenting practices (e.g., restrict TV and explain TV content (Valkenburg, Krcmar, Peeters, & Marseille, 1999). Demographic questions screened out ineligible responders. Participants were unable to go back to the demographic questions in order to prevent response changes to the eligibility questions.
Participants who answered one or more of the validation checks incorrectly were not included in analyses (n = 5). Participants who demonstrated incoherent response patterns (e.g., answering 1’s or 2’s for all items and not altering their responses for reverse-scored questions; n = 2) were also excluded from analyses, reducing the sample size from 298 to 291 participants. See Table 1 for descriptive statistics.
Table 1.
Demographic variable | Mean (SD) or % (n) |
---|---|
Child age (years) | 7 (2) |
Child sex (female) | 50.0% (145) |
| |
Child race and ethnicity | |
White | 76.8% (222) |
Black | 9.0% (26) |
Biracial | 8.9% (26) |
Asian or Pacific Islander | 4.2% (12) |
Other | 1.0% (3) |
Hispanic or Latino/a (any race) | 11.0% (32) |
| |
Mother race and ethnicity | |
White | 80.8% (235) |
Black | 9.6% (28) |
Biracial | 3.8% (11) |
Asian or Pacific Islander | 4.1% (12) |
Other | 1.7% (5) |
Hispanic or Latina (any race) | 7.6% (22) |
| |
Past Year Family Income | |
$0–$20,000 | 16.6% (48) |
$20,001–$40,000 | 26.5% (77) |
$40,001–$60,000 | 19.3% (56) |
$60,001–$80,000 | 17.9% (52) |
$80,000–$100,000 | 10.7% (31) |
Greater than $100,000 | 9.0% (26) |
| |
Mother education level | |
Did not graduate high school | 0.3% (1) |
High school diploma or GED only | 13.8% (40) |
Some college courses | 30.9% (90) |
2-year college degree | 12.0% (35) |
4-year college degree | 30.2% (88) |
More than a 4-year college degree | 12.7% (37) |
| |
Most Commonly Used Screen Media by the Child | |
Tablet | 32.4% (94) |
Television | 31.0% (90) |
Video games | 13.8% (40) |
Computer/laptop | 11.0% (32) |
Mobile phone | 8.6% (25) |
Handheld video game device | 2.7% (8) |
| |
Child has his/her own mobile device (yes) | 69.3% (201) |
Age when child received his/her own mobile device (years) | 7 (2) |
| |
Daily Screen Timea | |
| |
Television | 3.18 (1.29) |
Mobile device | 2.22 (1.60) |
Video game | 1.69 (1.47) |
Computer/laptop | 1.20 (1.46) |
| |
Total Daily Screen Time | 8.24 (3.91)b |
Note. Frequencies may not add up to n = 291 due to missing values.
Due to the use of categorical response options for screen time assessment, means reflect these transformed continuous values: 0 = 0 hours, 1 = Less than 1 hour, 2 = 1 hour, 3 = 2 hours, 4 = 3 hours, 5 = 4 hours, and 6 = More than 4 hours.
This value reflects the sum of the transformed continuous scores (0–6 for each of the 4 types of media, with a possible range of 0–24, corresponding to 0 hours each day to more than 4 hours each day for each of the four types of media) and not actual hours.
Additional Measures
Demographics
Mother’s age, race/ethnicity, highest level of education, relationship to child (e.g., biological mother, adoptive mother, other), and family income were assessed. Mothers also reported on their child’s sex, age, race/ethnicity, and type of school or child-care setting.
Screen media use
Mothers reported on their child’s average weekday and weekend screen time for the following types of screen media: (1) television (TV) shows (broadcast or streaming), DVDs, or videotapes on a TV set; (2) video games on a handheld game player or game system; (3) mobile devices, such as smartphones and tablets, not including time required for school assignments or homework; and (4) desktop computer or laptop use. Response options ranged from None to More than 4 hours per day. Categorical responses were classified on a scale from 0 to 6 to be used in the regression analyses.
To calculate average daily screen time, responses to the weekday items were multiplied by 5 and responses to the weekend items were multiplied by 2. These products were added together and divided by 7 to calculate total screen time across all devices.
Mothers were asked (from Rideout, 2013): “What age did your child first get his/her OWN mobile device, such as a smartphone or tablet (e.g., iPad, Kindle Fire)?”, and “What type of screen media does your child use the most (not including screen media used for school or homework)?” with the following options: TV, video games, mobile phone, tablet, handheld video game device, and computer/laptop.
Child psychosocial functioning
Mothers completed the 25-item Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, 1997), a widely-used instrument (Stone, Otten, Engels, Vermulst, & Janssens, 2010) which assesses five domains of child functioning (e.g., emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, and prosocial behaviors) and provides a total difficulty score (sum of all subscales except prosocial behaviors). These subscales of the SDQ reflect constructs that have been used to test the validity of measures of IGD and Social Media Disorder (e.g., Lemmens et al., 2015 and van den Eijnden et al., 2016). Mothers received the version of the SDQ (parent-report for 4–10 year olds or for mothers of 11 year olds, the 11–17 year old version) appropriate for the age of the child. Response options for both SDQ versions are: Not True (= 0), Somewhat True (= 1), and Certainly True (= 2). Current Cronbach’s alphas for the total difficulties score (20 items) were .86 and .91 for the 4–10 year old version and 11–17 year old versions, respectively (see Table 2 for descriptive statistics for SDQ subscales).
Table 2.
SDQ Scale | Range | Mean (SD) | Cronbach’s alpha (items) 4– 0 year old version | Cronbach’s alpha (items) 11–17 year old version |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total score | 0–31 | 9.39 (6.48) | .86 (20) | .91 (20) |
Emotional symptoms | 0–9 | 1.88 (2.10) | .76 (5) | .80 (5) |
Conduct Problems | 0–8 | 1.47 (1.77) | .67 (5) | .82 (5) |
Hyperactivity/Impulsivity | 0–10 | 3.90 (2.47) | .75 (5) | .87 (5) |
Peer Relationship Problems | 0–8 | 2.15 (1.85) | .59 (5) | .77 (5) |
Prosocial Behaviors | 0–10 | 7.45 (2.28) | .78 (5) | .81 (5) |
Note. SDQ items ranged from Not True (= 0), Somewhat True (= 1), and Certainly True (= 2). The sum of the items was calculated to generate subscale scores.
Concern about child media use
We used one item to assess mothers’ concern about their child’s screen media use: “How often do you worry about your child’s screen media use (TV, computer, video games, or mobile device)?” Response options ranged from Never (1) to Always (5).
Problematic Media Use Measure (PMUM) Development
PMUM items were generated based on criteria suggested for IGD in the DSM-5 (APA, 2013). With slight modifications to account for parents reporting on their child’s media use compared to youth reporting on their own use, we created items reflecting the following IGD criteria items (APA, 2013): (1) preoccupation (5 items), (2) withdrawal (11 items), (3) tolerance (4 items), (4) unsuccessful attempts by parent to control use (10 items), (5) loss of interest in previous hobbies and entertainment (7 items), (6) deceived others about use (5 items), (7) use to escape or relieve a negative mood (4 items), (8) jeopardized/lost a relationship or had compromised functioning in school due to use (4 items), and (9) continued use despite psychosocial problems (10 items). Content used to generate items that correspond to the DSM criteria were drawn from literature on problematic media use in adolescents, clinical experience, and interviews with mothers of children between the ages of 4–8 years old that had been collected in a separate study (Author removed for peer review).
The resulting pool of 60 items was sent to five experts on addiction, internet gaming disorder, internet addiction, and children’s media use. Experts represented a variety of disciplines, including pediatrics, developmental psychology, clinical psychology, communications, and adolescent medicine. Experts provided feedback on measure instructions, item content and wording, and indicated whether criteria were not included or adequately captured. Revisions to the measure instructions and items were made based on expert feedback.
The 60-item measure, instructed participants to respond to questions based on any type of screen media their child used (see online supplemental material for instructions) and to “select the option that is true for your child in the past month.” We chose the more general term “screen media” instead of specifying screen media format in order to identify problematic use of any screen media. We did so for two reasons. First, given the multiple formats of screen media used by children, problems could arise for more than one media format (e.g., video game console and tablet) and/or problems could arise for different formats in different contexts. For example, a parent may report problems with his/her child wanting to use the computer at home, but persistent requests to use mobile devices primarily outside the home. Second, we chose the term screen media instead of a specific format (e.g., internet games, Youtube videos), as parents may be unable to reliably report on the specific programs their child uses. For example, parents may not be able to distinguish whether their child has problems due to wanting to play internet games on his Ipad versus stream TV programs (or both). Thus, the term “screen media” was chosen to be broad enough to capture a variety of media formats and to facilitate parent report of general problems due to use, and with an eye to the fact that media formats will change over time. We chose “past month” for parents to report on their child’s problematic media use, instead of one year (as used for IGD; APA, 2013), as we believed that accurately characterizing a young child’s media use over one year would be challenging for parents. Responses were based on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from Never (1) to Always (5).
Data Analytic Plan
In Study 1, data reduction analyses were conducted to decrease the number of items on the PMUM for a full scale and short form version. Inter-item correlations were first examined to identify items that were poorly correlated with the other items in the scale. Next, exploratory factor analyses (EFAs) were conducted to aid in item reduction and finalize the scale content. Then, analyses on the final PMUM scale and PMUM Short Form (PMUM-SF) were conducted to establish the convergent validity by examining correlations between the PMUM and PMUM-SF and children’s screen time and correlations between the PMUM and PMUM-SF and mothers’ reported concern about their child’s screen media use. We tested incremental validity of the PMUM and PMUM-SF via multiple regression analyses to determine whether the PMUM and PMUM-SF accounted for significant variance in child functioning (i.e., subscales of the SDQ), over and above screen time.
Results
Factor Structure and Reliability
After eliminating four items based on low inter-item correlations, the final set of 56 items was then entered into an EFA with Oblimin rotation. Evaluation of the scree plot indicated a one-factor solution, with all items loading > .40 on the factor. To further reduce the items, in order to decrease redundancy among items measuring the same symptom, we retained the three highest loading items from each IGD symptom for the PMUM full scale, and the highest loading item for the PMUM-SF (based on prior measure development utilizing three items to assess each symptom; Lemmens et al., 2015; van den Eijnden et al., 2016). The EFA for the final set of PMUM items is presented in Table 3 and the EFA for the PMUM-SF is presented in Table 4. Internal reliability for the final PMUM and PMUM-SF were strong (Cronbach α = .97 and α = .93, respectively). The PMUM Full Scale and PMUM-SF correlated highly with each other (r = .98, p < .01).
Table 3.
PMUM Items (with original item numbers) | Factor Loading |
---|---|
26. It is hard for my child to stop using screen media. Unsuccessful control. | .85 |
56. It is increasingly difficult to pull my child away from screen media. Unsuccessful control. | .85 |
53. It is really difficult to get my child to stop using screen media. Unsuccessful control. | .84 |
47. Screen media is the only thing that seems to motivate my child. Loss of interest | .83 |
32. My child is always thinking about using screen media. Preoccupation | .83 |
23. Screen media is all that my child seems to think about. Preoccupation | .83 |
57. My child becomes frustrated when he/she cannot use screen media. Withdrawal | .82 |
58. My child’s screen media use interferes with family activities. | .82 |
44. My child gets upset when he/she cannot use screen media. Withdrawal | .82 |
31. There is nothing my child enjoys as much as screen media. Loss of interest | .80 |
54. My child becomes angry when he/she cannot use screen media. Withdrawal | .79 |
33. My child’s screen media use causes problems for the family. | .78 |
34. The amount of time my child wants to use screen media keeps increasing. Tolerance | .76 |
25. My child attempts to use screen media for increasing amounts of time. Tolerance | .75 |
52. Problems occur for our family when my child cannot use screen media. | .75 |
40. My child would find life boring without screen media. Loss of interest | .74 |
55. Life would be easier if my child was not so attached to screen media. | .74 |
36. The first thing my child asks to do when he/she comes home from school is to use screen media. preoccupation | .73 |
24. My child’s screen media use negatively affects his/her friendships. | .73 |
7. My child uses screen media for increasing amounts of time. Tolerance | .73 |
6. My child loses sleep due to screen media use. | .72 |
10. My child sneaks using screen media. deception | .71 |
19. My child lies about doing chores or school work in order to use screen media. deception | .69 |
29. When my child has had a bad day, screen media seems to be the only thing that helps him/her feel better. escape/relieve mood | .65 |
20. My child feels better when he/she uses screen media. Escape/relieve mood. | .64 |
2. My child uses screen media to feel better. Escape/relieve mood | .62 |
1. My child lies in order to use screen media. deception | .60 |
| |
% of Variance | 57.78% |
Eigenvalue | 15.60 |
Cronbach alpha | .97 |
M (SD) | 2.24 (0.83) |
Note. Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization rotation method. The PMUM ranged from 1 = Never to 5 = Always.
Table 4.
PMUM Items (with original item numbers) | Factor Loading |
---|---|
26. It is hard for my child to stop using screen media. (Unsuccessful control) | .86 |
47. Screen media is the only thing that seems to motivate my child. (Loss of interest) | .85 |
23. Screen media is all that my child seems to think about. (Preoccupation) | .85 |
58. My child’s screen media use interferes with family activities. (Psychosocial consequences) | .84 |
33. My child’s screen media use causes problems for the family. (Serious problems due to use) | .81 |
57. My child becomes frustrated when he/she cannot use screen media. (Withdrawal) | .81 |
34. The amount of time my child wants to use screen media keeps increasing. (Tolerance) | .77 |
10. My child sneaks using screen media. (Deception) | .72 |
29. When my child has had a bad day, screen media seems to be the only thing that helps him/her feel better. (Escape/relieve mood) | .68 |
| |
% of Variance | 64.24% |
Eigenvalue | 5.78 |
Cronbach alpha | .93 |
M (SD) | 2.16 (0.87) |
Note. Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization rotation method. The PMUM ranged from 1 = Never to 5 = Always.
Convergent and Incremental Validity
Correlations between the PMUM Full Scale and total daily screen time and the one-item “worry about child’s media use” were significant (p < .01) and moderate in size (r = .49 and r = .59, respectively), supporting convergent validity of the scale. Similar correlations (p < .01) were found between the PMUM-SF and total daily screen time (r = .47) and the one-item “worry about child’s media use” (r = .58).
Incremental validity was examined by using multiple regression to test whether the PMUM and PMUM-SF predicted child psychosocial functioning over and above screen time. Age and total screen time were entered in the first step and PMUM total score (or PMUM-SF score) was entered in the second step (see Tables 5 and 6). Total screen time and child age predicted child psychosocial functioning difficulties (B = .37 and B= −.17, respectively, p < .01), explaining 13% of the variance. In the second step, the PMUM explained an additional 24% of the variance in child psychosocial functioning difficulties (see Table 5), and screen time was no longer significantly associated with child functioning difficulties (B = .09, p = .12). Similar results were found for the SDQ subscales: Hyperactivity and Inattention and Prosocial Behaviors, which are also presented in Table 5. Screen time remained significantly associated with the Peer Relationship Problems, Conduct Problems, and Emotional Symptoms subscales in Step 2, albeit with a lower standardized Beta value (see Table 5). Similar findings were found for the PMUM-SF (see Table 6).
Table 5.
SDQ Outcomes | ||
---|---|---|
| ||
SDQ Total | ||
| ||
ΔR2 | β | |
Step 1 | .13 | |
Total Screen Time | .37** | |
Child age | −.17** | |
F(2, 274) = 20.97** | ||
| ||
Step 2 | .24 | |
Total Screen Time | .09 | |
Child age | −.15** | |
PMUM Score | .56** | |
F(3, 273) = 54.53** | ||
| ||
SDQ Peer Relationship Problems | ||
| ||
Predictors | ΔR2 | B |
| ||
Step 1 | .10 | |
Total Screen Time | .32** | |
Child age | −.14* | |
F(2, 274) = 15.09** | ||
| ||
Step 2 | .09 | |
Total Screen Time | .16* | |
Child age | −.13* | |
PMUM Score | .34** | |
F(3, 273) = 20.66** | ||
| ||
SDQ Hyperactivity and Inattention | ||
| ||
Predictors | ΔR2 | B |
| ||
Step 1 | .04 | |
Total Screen Time | .16** | |
Child age | −.14* | |
F(2, 274) = 4.97** | ||
| ||
Step 2 | .19 | |
Total Screen Time | −.09 | |
Child age | −.13* | |
PMUM Score | .50** | |
F(3, 273) = 26.56** | ||
| ||
SDQ Emotional Symptoms | ||
| ||
Predictors | ΔR2 | B |
| ||
Step 1 | .11 | |
Total Screen Time | .35** | |
Child age | −.06 | |
F(2, 274) = 17.44** | ||
| ||
Step 2 | .14 | |
Total Screen Time | .13* | |
Child age | −.04 | |
PMUM Score | .43** | |
F(3, 273) = 31.12** | ||
| ||
SDQ Conduct Problems | ||
| ||
Predictors | ΔR2 | B |
| ||
Step 1 | .15 | |
Total Screen Time | .38** | |
Child age | −.21** | |
F(2, 274) = 23.71** | ||
| ||
Step 2 | .19 | |
Total Screen Time | .13* | |
Child age | −.20** | |
PMUM Score | .50** | |
F(3, 273) = 45.90** | ||
| ||
SDQ Prosocial Behaviors | ||
| ||
Predictors | ΔR2 | B |
| ||
Step 1 | .07 | |
Total Screen Time | −.22** | |
Child age | .23** | |
F(2, 274) = 10.82** | ||
Step 2 | .10 | |
| ||
Total Screen Time | −.04 | |
Child age | .22** | |
PMUM Score | −.36** | |
F(3, 273) = 19.06** |
p < .05,
p < .01
Table 6.
SDQ Outcomes | ||
---|---|---|
| ||
SDQ Total | ||
| ||
ΔR2 | β | |
Step 1 | .13 | |
Total Screen Time | .37** | |
Child age | −.17** | |
F(2, 274) = 20.97** | ||
| ||
Step 2 | .24 | |
Total Screen Time | .10 | |
Child age | −.15** | |
PMUM-SF | .56** | |
F(3, 273) = 54.74** | ||
| ||
SDQ Peer Relationship Problems | ||
| ||
Predictors | ΔR2 | β |
| ||
Step 1 | .10 | |
Total Screen Time | .32** | |
Child age | −.14* | |
F(2, 274) = 15.09** | ||
| ||
Step 2 | .09 | |
Total Screen Time | .16* | |
Child age | −.13* | |
PMUM-SF | .34** | |
F(3, 273) = 21.12** | ||
| ||
SDQ Hyperactivity and Inattention | ||
| ||
Predictors | ΔR2 | B |
| ||
Step 1 | .04 | |
Total Screen Time | .16** | |
Child age | −.14* | |
F(2, 274) = 4.97** | ||
| ||
Step 2 | .18 | |
Total Screen Time | −.07 | |
Child age | −.13* | |
PMUM-SF | .49** | |
F(3, 273) = 25.37** | ||
| ||
SDQ Emotional Symptoms | ||
| ||
Predictors | ΔR2 | B |
| ||
Step 1 | .11 | |
Total Screen Time | .35** | |
Child age | −.06 | |
F(2, 274) = 17.44** | ||
Step 2 | .15 | |
| ||
Total Screen Time | .14* | |
Child age | −.04 | |
PMUM-SF | .43** | |
F(3, 273) = 31.67** | ||
| ||
SDQ Conduct Problems | ||
| ||
Predictors | ΔR2 | B |
| ||
Step 1 | .15 | |
Total Screen Time | .38** | |
Child age | −.21** | |
F(2, 274) = 23.71** | ||
| ||
Step 2 | .19 | |
Total Screen Time | .09 | |
Child age | −.20** | |
PMUM-SF | .50** | |
F(3, 273) = 46.88** | ||
| ||
SDQ Prosocial Behaviors | ||
| ||
Predictors | ΔR2 | B |
| ||
Step 1 | .07 | |
Total Screen Time | −.22** | |
Child age | .23** | |
F(2, 274) = 10.81** | ||
| ||
Step 2 | .11 | |
Total Screen Time | −.04 | |
Child age | .22** | |
PMUM-SF | −.38** | |
F(3, 273) = 20.53** |
p < .05,
p < .01
Results from Study 1 support the reliability and convergent and incremental validity of the PMUM Full Scale and PMUM-SF. Best practices in scale development include confirmation of the factor structure in a separate, independent sample (Worthington & Whittaker, 2006), which we pursue in Study 2. Given that the PMUM-SF demonstrated similar psychometric properties, was highly correlated with the PMUM Full Scale, similarly predicted child psychosocial difficulties, and was shorter (i.e., less burdensome to participants), we chose to confirm the validation of the PMUM-SF in Study 2.
Study 2
Method
Participants and Procedure
Study 2 participants were recruited through Qualtrics; the data presented for Study 2 are drawn from a larger study on parent and child media use. Eligible participants had to be the primary caregiver (mother or father) of a child between the ages of 4 years and 14 years. Demographic questions screened out ineligible responders (n = 154). Data was screened for implausible scores or if two or more participants had the same IP address (indicating that the same parent reported on more than one child or if two parents reported on the same child; n = 2). See Table 7 for descriptive statistics for the final sample (N = 632).
Table 7.
Demographic variable | Mean (SD) or % (n) |
---|---|
Child age (years) | 8.62 (4.12) |
Child sex (female) | 44.8% (336) |
| |
Child race and ethnicity | |
White | 84.0% (630) |
Black | 8.0% (60) |
Asian | 3.9% (29) |
American Indian or Alaska Native | 2.0% (15) |
Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander | 0.3% (2) |
Hispanic or Latino/a (any race) | 12.0% (90) |
| |
Caregiver relationship to child | |
Parent | 92.5% (694) |
Grandparent | 5.5% (41) |
Aunt/Uncle | 5 (0.7%) |
Sibling | 4 (0.5%) |
Other caregiver | 6 (0.8%) |
| |
Caregiver age (years) | 40.36 (10.01) |
| |
Caregiver sex (female) | 449 (59.9%) |
| |
Caregiver race and ethnicity | |
White | 82.3% (617) |
Black | 6.8% (51) |
Asian | 3.6% (27) |
American Indian or Alaska Native | 1.3% (10) |
Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander | 0.3% (2) |
Hispanic or Latino/a (any race) | 13.1% (98) |
| |
Caregiver education level | |
Did not graduate high school | 1.0% (7) |
High school diploma or GED only | 12.0% (90) |
Some college (< 4 years) or technical school | 29.1% (218) |
College graduate (>= 4 years) | 40.4% (303) |
Post-graduate work (e.g., MD, MA, PhD, JD) | 17.5% (131) |
| |
Screen Media Owned by Child or in Child’s Bedroom | |
Television | 63.7% (478) |
Tablet | 56.0% (420) |
Video game system | 45.3% (340) |
Smart phone or other mobile phone | 42.9% (322) |
Computer/laptop | 42.0% (315) |
| |
PMUM-Short Form (Cronbach alpha = .96) | 2.20 (1.12) |
Measures
In addition to the PMUM-SF and demographic questions, items related to parental conflict with child over screen media use were administered. These items consisted of the stem, “How often do you have a conflict with your child because your child has to turn off” and included the following types of media: television, computer, tablet, smartphone, and video games. Parents responded on a scale from “once a year or never” to “many times a day.”
Data Analytic Plan
To confirm the factor structure of the PMUM-SF in this new sample, we conducted a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) using maximum likelihood estimation via Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 2012). Model fit was assessed using cut-off values suggested by Hu and Bentler (1999): root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) less than or equal to 0.06 and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) less than or equal to 0.08 indicates good fit. Model fit was also assessed using CFI (above .90), squared mean residuals above .1, and significant factor loadings. Another aim of this study was to determine if there was measurement invariance of the PMUM-SF by gender. To determine equivalence, we examined change in CFI and RMSEA values between measurement models (as recommended by Cheung & Rensvold, 2002 and Timmons, 2010). Finally, as a test of convergent validity, we examined the association between PMUM-SF scores and parent-child conflict over screen media use, using bivariate correlation analysis.
Results
Confirmatory Factor Analysis
A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to test the factor structure of the PMUM-SF. The one-factor model demonstrated an acceptable fit to the data (RMSEA = .085; CFI = .961; SRMR = .024; see Table 8). The 9 items loaded significantly on the factor (above .70, p < .001; see Figure 1) and squared mean residuals were above .1. To test for measurement invariance between boys and girls, we conducted a multiple-group analysis in Mplus. The fit for the configural invariance model did not significantly change from the overall model (see Table 8), indicating that the factor structure of the PMUM-SF is the same for boys and girls. Successive tests of model fit were also conducted to test for other indicators of measurement invariance (Table 8). Utilizing the ΔCFI and RMSEA tests (i.e., ΔCFI and ΔRMSEA < .01 indicates a non-significant difference in fit), support was found for metric, scalar, and strict invariance. In other words, factor loadings, intercepts, and residuals were equivalent across groups. Finally, factor-level invariance was also demonstrated in that the factor variance and factor mean were equivalent for boys and girls. In sum, strong evidence was found for measurement invariance of the PMUM-SF.
Table 8.
χ2 (df) | RMSEA | SRMR | CFI | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Unifactorial Model | χ2 (27) = 149.193** | 0.085 | .024 | .961 |
Multiple Group Analyses | ||||
Configural invariance | χ2 (54) = 196.778** | 0.092 | .028 | .954 |
Weak (metric) invariance | χ2 (62) = 220.838** | 0.090 | .060 | .949 |
Strong (scalar) invariance | χ2 (70) = 235.456** | 0.087 | .059 | .947 |
Strict invariance | χ2 (79) = 252.667** | 0.083 | .071 | .944 |
Factor variance invariance | χ2 (80) = 254.725** | 0.083 | .082 | .944 |
Factor mean invariance | χ2 (81) = 272.893** | 0.087 | .073 | .938 |
Note. Root mean square error of approximation: RMSEA, Standardized root mean square residual: SRMR, Comparative fit index: CFI.
p < .01
Convergent Validity
Bivariate correlations between the parent-child conflict over screen media use and PMUM-SF scores were conducted. Across each type of media, greater parent-child conflict associated with higher PMUM-SF scores, ranging from .41 to .50 (p < .01; see Table 9), further supporting the convergent validity of the PMUM-SF.
Table 9.
Parent-Child Conflict | PMUM-SF |
---|---|
Over Turning Off: | |
Television | .41** |
Computer | .45** |
Tablet | .42** |
Smartphone | .49** |
Video games | .50** |
p < .01
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to develop and test the reliability and validity of a measure of screen media “addiction” in a sample of children between the ages of 4 and 11 years old. We found, in Study 1, that the PMUM Full Scale and its shorter version, the PMUM-SF, evidenced strong psychometric properties, with good internal consistency and convergent validity. Incremental validity was demonstrated as well, with the PMUM and PMUM-SF independently predicting children’s total difficulties in functioning, over and above hours of screen time. In Study 2, with a separate, larger sample, we confirmed the factor structure, reliability, and convergent validity of the PMUM-SF; we also found that the PMUM-SF works well for both girls and boys via multiple group analyses. These studies support the use of the PMUM-SF as a measure of problematic media use in children under age 12 years.
The PMUM assesses a unidimensional construct of problematic media use, consisting of 27 items that reflect all nine criteria proposed for Internet Gaming Disorder (APA, 2013). The PMUM-SF uses 9 items corresponding to these criteria. To our knowledge, this is the first parent-report measure of problematic media use in children ages 4–11 years old. Certain qualities of this measure make it highly suitable for use by clinicians and researchers alike. The PMUM-SF is short enough to be completed during intake appointments with psychologists. With its strong psychometrics and face validity, the PMUM-SF could also be used by researchers seeking to identify children with problematic media use who may be too young to self-report on their symptoms. Another promising feature of the PMUM-SF is its use of the term “screen media” instead of specifying certain devices. Given how quickly new mobile media devices are developed, and the diversity of media platforms used by children today, it was important to create a screening tool that was broad enough to capture any screen media. As is done with other clinical measures, the clinician can assess for what media (e.g., video games, online gaming) is of most concern to the parents, after administering this screener. The PMUM-SF can be completed by parents whose child is having problems related to video game use, tablet use, and/or other mobile device use (or heretofore unknown platforms). By not focusing on a specific device, the PMUM-SF can be used to capture any problematic media use. We recommend that researchers and/or clinicians ask parents about the type of media their child uses the most to complement the PMUM-SF total score.
There were limitations of this study that should be addressed in the future. First, in this cross-sectional study, we could not test whether problematic media use preceded problems in psychosocial functioning or vice versa. This relationship may also be bidirectional (Gentile, Swing, Lim, & Khoo, 2012). Future research using multiple time points is needed in order to understand the development of problematic media use in children. A limitation, that should be addressed in the future, regarded our method for assessing screen time. Although future studies should use Time Use Diaries or passive sensing technology, we were limited to parent report given that it was a one-time online study. Another limitation is the racial/ethnic diversity of the sample was fairly homogenous; thus, the findings may not generalize to all parent populations. Finally, there are multiple strategies that can be taken to reduce the items, each of which carries assumptions such as whether IGD is a singular unifactor disorder or a complex issue that may present heterogeneous sets of symptoms across patients. Future research should consider the whether certain approaches are more valid for clinical diagnosis in different populations.
Another limitation of this measure is that it is focused on “addictive” screen media use; other aspects of problematic media use have been articulated (see a Pathway Model of Problematic Mobile Phone Use: Billieux, 2012; Billieux, Maurage, Lopez-Fernandez, Kuss, & Griffiths, 2015), that this measure does not assess. Thus, future research should consider other aspects of problematic media use (e.g., antisocial and risky patterns of use; Billieux et al., 2015) as appropriate to the developmental stage of the child.
These studies presents a first attempt to measure problematic media use in children. Future studies are encouraged in order to validate the PMUM-SF in more diverse samples. Future research can test whether the PMUM Full Scale or PMUM-SF associates with other indices of child adjustment. Validating the PMUM in clinical samples is an important next step, as well as establishing clinical cut-off scores. Pending further validation, the PMUM has the potential to identify children at greater risk for later screen media dependence and problems associated with excessive screen media use.
Supplementary Material
Public Significance Statement.
Children’s screen time and mobile device use has grown markedly. Concerns have been raised about whether children are “addicted” to screen media; however, no measures have been developed to assess screen media addiction in younger children. This manuscript describes the development and validation of a parent-report measure of screen media addiction in children under age 12 years.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, grant numbers F32HD085684 and R01HD061356. We would also like to acknowledge the experts who provided feedback on the content of the PMUM. We would like to thank the Momentum Center and Center for Statistical Consultation and Research at the University of Michigan for their feedback on this study.
Footnotes
Author Disclosure Statement
The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
References
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5. Washington, DC: Author; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Berinsky AJ, Huber GA, Lenz GS. Evaluating Online Labor Markets for Experimental Research: Amazon.com's Mechanical Turk. Political Analysis. 2012;20:351–368. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpr057. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Billieux J. Problematic use of the mobile phone: a literature review and a pathways model. Current Psychiatry Reviews. 2012;8:299–307. [Google Scholar]
- Billieux J, Maurage P, Lopez-Fernandez O, Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Can Disordered Mobile Phone Use Be Considered a Behavioral Addiction? An Update on Current Evidence and a Comprehensive Model for Future Research. Current Addiction Reports. 2015;2:156–162. doi: 10.1007/s40429-015-0054-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon's Mechanical Turk a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on psychological science. 2011;6:3–5. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casler K, Bickel L, Hackett E. Separate but equal? A comparison of participants and data gathered via Amazon’s MTurk, social media, and face-to-face behavioral testing. Computers in Human Behavior. 2013;29:2156–2160. [Google Scholar]
- Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural equation modeling. 2002;9:233–255. [Google Scholar]
- Felt L, Robb M. Technology Addiction: Concern, Controversy, and Finding a Balance. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Foerster M, Roser K, Schoeni A, Röösli M. Problematic mobile phone use in adolescents: derivation of a short scale MPPUS-10. International journal of public health. 2015;60:277–286. doi: 10.1007/s00038-015-0660-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster H, Carbonell X, Pontes HM, Griffiths MD. Spanish validation of the Internet Gaming Disorder-20 (IGD-20) Test. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;56:215–224. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.11.050. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gentile DA. Pathological Video-Game Use Among Youth Ages 8 to 18: A National Study. Psychological Science. 2009;20:594–602. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02340.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gentile DA, Swing EL, Lim CG, Khoo A. Video game playing, attention problems, and impulsiveness: Evidence of bidirectional causality. Psychology of Popular Media Culture. 2012;1:62. [Google Scholar]
- Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A Research Note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groves CL, Blanco-Herrera JA, Prot S, Berch ON, McCowen S, Gentile DA. Game and Internet Addiction After DSM-5. The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Psychology, Technology and Society. 2015:502. [Google Scholar]
- Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
- Jelenchick LA, Eickhoff J, Christakis DA, Brown RL, Zhang C, Benson M, Moreno MA. The Problematic and Risky Internet Use Screening Scale (PRIUSS) for adolescents and young adults: Scale development and refinement. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014;35:171–178. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kabali HK, Irigoyen MM, Nunez-Davis R, Budacki JG, Mohanty SH, Leister KP, Bonner RL., Jr Exposure and Use of Mobile Media Devices by Young Children. Pediatrics. 2015;136:1044–1050. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Király O, Sleczka P, Pontes HM, Urbán R, Griffiths MD, Demetrovics Z. Validation of the Ten-Item Internet Gaming Disorder Test (IGDT-10) and evaluation of the nine DSM-5 Internet Gaming Disorder criteria. Addictive Behaviors. 2015;64:253–260. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemmens JS, Valkenburg PM, Gentile DA. The Internet Gaming Disorder Scale. Psychological assessment. 2015;27:567. doi: 10.1037/pas0000062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemmens JS, Valkenburg PM, Peter J. Psychosocial causes and consequences of pathological gaming. Computers in Human Behavior. 2011;27:144–152. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.07.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lister-Landman KM, Domoff SE, Dubow EF. The Role of Compulsive Texting in Adolescents’ Academic Functioning. Psychology of Popular Media Culture. 2015 Advance online publication. [Google Scholar]
- Moreno MA, Jelenchick L, Cox E, Young H, Christakis DA. Problematic internet use among US youth: a systematic review. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2011;165:797–805. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreno MA, Jelenchick LA, Christakis DA. Problematic internet use among older adolescents: A conceptual framework. Computers in Human Behavior. 2013;29:1879–1887. [Google Scholar]
- Muthén L, Muthén B. Mplus statistical modeling software: Release 7.0. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Männikkö N, Billieux J, Kääriäinen M. Problematic digital gaming behavior and its relation to the psychological, social and physical health of Finnish adolescents and young adults. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. 2015;4:281–288. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petry NM, Rehbein F, Gentile DA, Lemmens JS, Rumpf HJ, Mossle T, … O'Brien CP. An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction. 2014;109:1399–1406. doi: 10.1111/add.12457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rehbein F, Kleimann M, Mößle T. Prevalence and Risk Factors of Video Game Dependency in Adolescence: Results of a German Nationwide Survey. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2010;13:269–277. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rideout V. Zero to Eight Children's Media Use in America. Common Sense Media; 2013. Retrieved from https://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/zero-to-eight-childrens-media-use-in-america-2013. [Google Scholar]
- Rideout V. The Common Sense Census: Media Use by Tweens and Teens. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media; 2015. Retrieved from https://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/the-common-sense-census-media-use-by-tweens-and-teens. [Google Scholar]
- Schleider JL, Weisz JR. Using Mechanical Turk to Study Family Processes and Youth Mental Health: A Test of Feasibility. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2015;24:3235–3246. [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro DN, Chandler J, Mueller PA. Using Mechanical Turk to study clinical populations. Clinical Psychological Science. 2013 2167702612469015. [Google Scholar]
- Spekman MLC, Konijn EA, Roelofsma PHMP, Griffiths MD. Gaming addiction, definition and measurement: A large-scale empirical study. Computers in Human Behavior. 2013;29:2150–2155. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.05.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stone LL, Otten R, Engels RC, Vermulst AA, Janssens JM. Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire for 4-to 12-year-olds: a review. Clinical child and family psychology review. 2010;13:254–274. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0071-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timmons A. Establishing factorial invariance for multiple-group confirmatory factor analysis. KUant Guide. 2010:22. [Google Scholar]
- Uhls YT, Michikyan M, Morris J, Garcia D, Small GW, Zgourou E, Greenfield PM. Five days at outdoor education camp without screens improves preteen skills with nonverbal emotion cues. Computers in Human Behavior. 2014;39:387–392. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.036. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Valkenburg PM, Krcmar M, Peeters AL, Marseille NM. Developing a scale to assess three styles of television mediation: “Instructive mediation,” “restrictive mediation,” and “social coviewing”. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. 1999;43:52–66. [Google Scholar]
- van den Eijnden RJJM, Lemmens JS, Valkenburg PM. The Social Media Disorder Scale. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;61:478–487. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.038. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. Public health implications of excessive use of the internet, computers, smartphones and similar electronic devices: meeting report, Main Meeting Hall, Foundation for Promotion of Cancer Research, National Cancer Research Centre; Tokyo, Japan. 27–29 August 2014; World Health Organization; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Worthington RL, Whittaker TA. Scale development research: A content analysis and recommendations for best practices. The Counseling Psychologist. 2006;34:806–838. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.