Abstract
Our study sought to assess whether parental monitoring and their associations with health behaviors differed for heterosexual girls compared to sexual minority girls (girls who identified as lesbian or bisexual, endorsed same-sex attraction, or had same-sex romantic or sexualpartners). We analyzed three components of parental monitoring—adolescent disclosure, parental solicitation, and parental knowledge—between heterosexual and sexual minority girls.We also tested if the associations between these three constructs and adolescent relationship abuse, suicidality, heavy drinking, binge drinking, anxiety symptoms, and depressive symptoms were different for heterosexual girls compared to sexual minority girls. Sexual minority girls were less likely to disclose accurately to their parents their location and activities and perceived their parents asked less and knew less about their location and activities than did heterosexual girls. Heterosexual girls who reported higher levels of adolescent disclosure were less likely than were sexual minority girls to report suicidality and anxiety symptoms. Additionally, heterosexual girls who reported higher levels of parental knowledge were also less likely than were sexual minority girls to report anxiety and depressive symptoms. These findings suggest that parental monitoring may not be as protective forsexual minority girls as it is for heterosexual girls.
Keywords: familial relationships, mental health, sexual minorities, parental monitoring, substance use
Introduction
Sexual minority youth—youth who identify as gay, lesbian, or bisexual, endorse same-sex attraction or have same-sex romantic or sexual partners—are atgreater risk for adverse health outcomes such as victimization, suicidality, substance use, anxiety, and depressive symptoms compared to their heterosexual peers(Kann et al., 2011; Liu & Mustanski, 2012; Marshal et al., 2011; Marshal et al., 2008). Several professional organizations interested in reducing health care disparities—such as the Institute of Medicine(2011)—have called for research to determine what drives these differential outcomes between sexual minority and heterosexual youth. Meyer’s Minority Stress Theory (2003) posits that stigma and discrimination against sexual minorities are stressors that create a hostile social environment that may ultimately lead to health problems, contributingto these health disparities.
Sexual minority girls (SMGs) are a subpopulation of sexual minority youth who are particularly vulnerable and deserve special focus. First, disparities in substance use(Marshal et al., 2008; Talley, Hughes, Aranda, Birkett, & Marshal, 2014) and peer victimization (Friedman et al. 2011)are more pronounced between SMGs and heterosexual girls compared todisparities seen between sexual minority and heterosexual boys. Second, girls may respond to stress differently than do boys, which may indicate a different pathway to maladaptive behaviors.They tend to have greater investment in interpersonal relationships than do boys(Rose & Rudolph, 2006) andhave a stronger physiological response to social stress than do boys (Rudolph, Troop-Gordon, & Granger, 2010). This is particularly important in parent-child relationships in SMGs, in which negative parental attitudes against sexual minorities can be a source of significant stress. Third, there is a general societal concern about adolescent girls’ engagement in maladaptive behaviors(Pettit, Keiley, Laird, Bates, & Dodge, 2007), such as substance use, as these behaviors are considered more normative in boys (Mash & Barkley, 2002). Therefore, parents may be more invested in parenting girls to prevent these maladaptive behaviors. Moreover, society views same-sex attraction in girls as a violation of feminine gender norms(Doyle, Rees, & Titus, 2015); thus there may be differences in how parents raise SMGs compared to how parents raise heterosexual girls. Because parents can serve a protective role against adverse health outcomes in all youth, it is important to determine if such differential in parenting behaviors exists, and if such differences can help explain poor health outcomes among SMGs.
Most research on parent-child relationships in sexual minorities focuses on the affective dimension of parenting—behaviors characterized by acceptance versus rejection of a child’s sexual orientation. This appears to be a critical component in parent-child relationships in sexual minority youth.Previous research has demonstrated strained parent-child relationships in this population(Darby-Mullins & Murdock, 2007), with mothers reporting decreased affection for their sexual minority children, sexual minority youth reporting less secure attachment to their parents (Rosario, Reisner, Corliss, Wypij, Frazier, et al., 2014) andperceiving less support from their parents than do heterosexual youth(Needham & Austin, 2010).This decrease in the affective dimension of parenting may have a devastating impact on the health outcomes of sexual minority youth. For example, sexual minority youth whose parents reacted negatively to their same-sex sexual orientation are more likely to report substance use, depressive symptoms, and suicidality (Ryan, Huebner, Diaz, & Sanchez, 2009). Conversely,positive parental reactions to their child’s same-sex sexual orientation are associated with fewer mental health problems (Floyd, Stein, Harter, Allison, & Nye, 1999; Ryan, Russell, Huebner, Diaz, & Sanchez, 2010).
There is little research on the correlation between the instrumental dimensions of parenting—behaviors characterized by controlling versus granting freedom to the child—and the health outcomes of sexual minority youth. Research on thisdimension of parenting, in particular parental monitoring, in sexual minority youthis now emerging.Scholars define parental monitoring as “a set of correlated parenting behaviors involving attention to and tracking of the child’s whereabouts, activities, and adaptations” (Dishion& McMahon 1998, p. 61). Previous research had used parental knowledge of their child’s activity to measure parental monitoring(Racz & McMahon, 2011).Seminal work by Stattin and Kerr in 2000 challenged this approach. They argued that although parental knowledge is correlated with reductions in risky behaviors in adolescents, they also argued that it is the end product of a process that involves bothparents and children. Therefore, it is not a type of parenting behavior. They hypothesized that parental knowledgerequiresthree sources: “adolescent disclosure,” “parental solicitation,” and “parental control.” They defined adolescent disclosure as the degree to which adolescents spontaneously inform their parents about their behavior outside their direct supervision. They defined parental solicitation as a process in which parents seek information about their child’s whereabouts, friends, and behavior. This is distinct from parental control, in which parents set and communicate limits on their child’s behavior.In their study of Swedish teenagers, adolescent disclosure was a stronger predictor of parental knowledge and conduct problems than were parental solicitation and parental control. The investigators concluded that parental monitoring involves a set of three distinct components leading to parental knowledge: adolescent disclosure, parental solicitation, and parental control; thus, this behavior is both a parent and an adolescent activity (Stattin & Kerr, 2000).In this paper, we distinguish parental knowledge separately from behaviors that lead to parental knowledge (i.e.,parental solicitation and adolescent disclosure).
Although Stattin and Kerr (2000) and others have established a strong link between higher levels of parental monitoring and fewerconduct disorders(Racz & McMahon, 2011), this important parenting behavior may also be associated with reductions in other adverse health outcomes. Higher levels of parental monitoring arealso associated with a lower risk of being a victim of adolescent relationship abuse (ARA; East et al., 2010), a lower likelihood of reporting suicidality(King et al., 2001),a lower likelihood of reporting drinking alcohol in the past 12 months (Beck, Boyle, & Boekeloo, 2004), a reduction in anxiety symptoms(Murphy, Marelich, Herbeck, & Payne, 2009), and a reduction in depressive symptoms(Hamza & Willoughby, 2011).Among sexual minority youth, one studyexamined the relationship between parental monitoring and adverse health outcomes in a sample of 257 young men who have sex with men ages 14 to 19 years old. They used a six-item scale that measured the degree of parental knowledge, solicitation, control and disclosure from the perspective of the participant and combined them into a mean score. They found that higher mean scoreswere associated with higher rates of risky sexual behaviors—such as multiple sex partners and sex without a condom—in young men who have sex with men. This was contrary to the authors’ hypothesis that higher levels of parental monitoring would be associated with reductions of these risky behaviors. They thus concluded that parental monitoringmight not be protective for this population(Thoma & Huebner, 2014).As the study sample only had young men who have sex with men, they could not make direct comparisons in these relationships to young heterosexual men. Additionally, because they did not distinguish parental knowledge from its sources (solicitation, control, and disclosure), it was difficult to determine which among these behaviors played a role in the increase in risky sexual behaviors.
The Present Study.
We intend to add to the existing literature on parental monitoring of sexual minority youth by focusing on SMGs, determining if the process of parental monitoring and knowledge is different for SMGs compared to heterosexual girls, and making direct comparisons of the correlation of parental monitoring and adverse health indicators between SMGs and heterosexual girls.Combining a modified interpretation of parental monitoring proposed by Stattin and Kerr (2000) and Minority Stress Theory (2003), this study hasseveral hypotheses. The first hypothesis is that SMGs will disclose their behaviors less accurately to their parents than will heterosexual girls. We also hypothesize that SMGs will perceive less parental solicitation and less parental knowledge than heterosexual girls do. Additionally, because previous research suggests sexual minority youth may experience increased conflict with their parents (Rosario, Reisner, Corliss, Wypij, Calzo, et al., 2014), we hypothesize that same-sex sexual orientation will moderate the relationships between two potential sources of perceived parental knowledge (adolescent disclosure and perceived parental solicitation) and perceived parental knowledge itself. Furthermore, we hypothesize that same-sex sexual orientation will also moderate the relationships among parental monitoring and knowledge and health outcomes disparities often quoted in the literature of sexual minority youth: adolescent relationship abuse (ARA), suicidality, heavy drinking, binge drinking, anxiety symptoms, and depressive symptoms. (Marshal et al. 2013; McCauley et al. 2014; Garofalo et al. 1999; Marshal et al. 2011; Marshal et al. 2012b). Finally, as part of an exploratory analysis, we will test if the above relationships are different between SMGs whose parents know about their sexual orientation (i.e. being out to parents) versus SMGs who are not out to their parents.
Method
Participants
We recruited girls seen by adolescent medicine specialists for routine or consultative medical care at two urban adolescent medicine clinics in Ohio and Pennsylvania.Patients who were 14 – 19 years old, able to read and understand English at the 6th grade level, and came for a clinical visit regardless of health status or presenting problem were eligible to participate.To recruit a large enough sample of SMGs to accomplish the study’s goals, we implemented a purposive sampling strategy using confidential information provided by adolescent medicine patients to identify same-sex attracted youth.This strategy allowed us to have balanced comparison groups across age, race, and sexual minority status. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants greater than 17 years old. We obtained parental consentand youth assent for participants 14 – 17 years old. Institutional Review Boards at the University of Pittsburgh and at Nationwide Children’s Hospital approved this study. Participants completed mental health and psychosocial health questionnaires on a computer in a private room at a research facility.
Demographic Data
We collected demographic data from the participants including age, race, and eligibility for free lunch as a proxy for socioeconomic status (SES).We asked participants to self-identify as White, Black or African-American, Asian American, American Indian/AlaskanNative, Native Hawaiian, Other, or Pacific Islander. We categorized any response besides “White” as “Non-White.” Among non-whites, 86% (n=128) identified as Black, African-American, or biracial.
Assessments and Measures
Parental monitoring.
We sought to assess the two components of parental monitoring described by Stattin et al. (2000): adolescent disclosure and parental solicitation. To assess for adolescent disclosure, we asked a one-item question “In the past six months, how often did you lie to your parents about where you were or who you were with?” Responses ranged from 0=never, 1=once, 2=two times 3=three or four times, and 4=five or more times. We reverse-coded the responses to be consistent with the theory that this component of parental monitoring is inversely correlated with externalizing and internalizing behaviors. To assess “parental solicitation,” we used a scale developed by Fletcher et al. (2004) to capture any monitoring efforts that the participants may have perceived. We asked participants if their parents tried to know who their friends were, where they were at night, how they spent their money, what they did during their free time, and where they were most afternoons after school. Responses ranged from 0 = didn’t try, 1 = tried rarely, 2 = tried sometimes 3 = tried most of the time, and 4 = tried all the time, calculated as a mean score of the five items(Cronbach’s α = 0.86).
Parental knowledge.
To assess parental knowledge, we used a scale developed by Brown et al. (1993) asking participants to rate how much their parents really knew who their friends were, where they were at night, how they spent their money, what they did during their free time, and where they were most afternoons after school. Responses ranged from 0 = didn’t know, 1 = rarely knew, 2 = sometimes knew 3 = knew most of the time, and 4 = knew all the time. We took the mean scores of the five items (Cronbach’s α = 0.88).
Same-sex sexual orientation.
As recommended by Badgett and Goldberg(2009), we identifiedSMGs by asking participants about their sexual identity, sexual attractions, and dating and sexual behaviors.For sexual identity, we asked participants to choose a description of what best fits how they feel about themselves. Responses included “100% heterosexual (straight),” “mostly heterosexual,” “bisexual,” “mostly gay” and “100% homosexual (gay).” For sexual attraction, we asked how much participants were attracted to girls in the last 6 months. Responses include “not at all sexually attracted to girls,” “a little bit attracted to girls,” “somewhat attracted to girls,” “very much attracted to girls,” and “extremely attracted to girls.” For sexual behaviors, we asked participants the sex of current and past sexual and romantic partners. We considered any participant who endorsed an identity other than “100% heterosexual,”who endorsed any attraction to the same sex,or whoindicated a current or past same-sex sexual or romantic partner as a SMG.
Disclosure of same-sex orientation to parents.
We asked only SMGs whether they told any of the following people about their same-sex sexual orientation: “mother,” “father,” “other adult relative,” “male sibling,” “female sibling,” “male friend,” “female friend,” “school teacher,” “school counselor,” “health professional,” or “other.” We considered participants as being out if they informed at least one parent of their same-sex sexual orientation.
Adolescent relationship abuse.
We assessed for ARA using modified questions from previous intimate partner violence intervention studies(Miller et al., 2010).We asked participants if,in the last 6 months, they had: a)been hit, pushed, slapped, choked or otherwise physically hurt by someone they were dating or going out with, b) a partner who insisted on sex with them when the participant did not want to, and c) a partner who threatened or forced the participant to have sex with them. Responses ranged from “I’ve never been in a relationship,” “No this has not happened to me,” or “Yes, this has happened to me.” We considered any participant who chose “yes, this has happened to me” to any of these three items as having experienced ARA.
Suicidality.
To assess for suicidality, we asked participants if they ever thought about or tried to commit suicide. Responses were 0 = never,1 = it was a brief passing thought, 2 = I have had a plan at least once to kill myself, but did not try it, 3 = I have had a plan at least once to kill myself and really wanted to die, 4 = I have attempted to kill myself, but did not want to die, and 5 = I have attempted to kill myself, and really hoped to die. We considered any responses other than “never” as evidence forhistory of suicidality.We dichotomized this variable as 1) the variable was highly skewed (1.67) with median score being close to one, and 2) the rate of suicidal ideation in this sample is within the range of rates seen in other studies(Faulkner & Cranston, 1998; Lewinsohn, Rohde, & Seeley, 1996).
Alcohol misuse
We asked two questions to identify those who drink heavily and those who binge drink. To identify heavy drinkers, we asked how often in the past 6 months the participant had gotten drunk or “very high” on alcohol. To identify binge drinkers, we asked how often a participant drank 5 or more alcoholic drinks when they were drinking in the last 6 months. Responses ranged from 0 = not at all, 1 = one to two times, 2 = three to five times, 3 = once a month, 4 = two to three times a month, 5 = once a week, 6 = two to threetimes a week, 7 = four to six times a week, 8 = once a day, 9 = twice a day, and 10 = several times a day. We considered any participant who responded anything besides “none at all” to be a heavy drinker.We considered any participant who responded anything besides “none at all” to be a binge drinker. We dichotomized these responses because they were highly skewed (2.20 for heavy drinking and 2.17 for binge drinking). Furthermore, we believe that any episode of heavy or binge drinking is high-risk for adolescents because such behavior is a major factor in physical and sexual violence, suicide, and unintentional injuries(Faden & Goldman, 2004). Our dichotomization of the substance use outcomes is also consistent with previous studies on substance use in adolescents (Marshal et al., 2013; Marshal, Molina, & Pelham, 2003; Marshal et al., 2012a).
Anxiety and depression
We used the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders (SCARED) Scale, a 38-item self-report questionnaire that screens for anxiety, to assess for anxiety symptoms (Cronbach’s α = 0.93). Per Birmaher et al. (1999), we categorized a cut-off score of 25 and above as high anxiety symptoms and scores below 25 as low anxiety symptoms.We assessed for depressive symptoms in our participants using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (Cronbach’s α = 0.80). Per recommendations from Radloff (1991), we categorized a cut off score of 16 and above as high depressive symptoms and scores below 16 as lowdepressive symptoms in our participants.
Data Analysis
We used a χ2 test (and Fisher’s exact test for cell sizes less than 5) to test differences in demographic data and dichotomous health indicators between SMGs and heterosexual girls. Because the adolescent disclosure score was highly skewed (-2.01), we ran a Wilcoxon signed-rank testto compare the differences in this variable between SMGs and heterosexual girls.Perceived parental solicitation and parental knowledge had a more normal distribution, so we ran a Student’s t-test to compare the differences of these two variables between SMGs and heterosexual girls.
To test if same-sex sexual orientation moderated the relationship between the sources of perceived parental knowledge (adolescent disclosure and perceived parental solicitation) and perceived parental knowledge itself, we ran several linear regression models. In the first set of models, one model used adolescent disclosure as the predictor variable for perceived parental knowledge. Another model used perceived parental solicitation as the predictor variable for perceived parental knowledge. We adjusted for age, race, SES, and site for both models. In the second set of models, we added the terms “adolescent disclosure x SMG” to the model assessing the relationship between adolescent disclosure and parental knowledge and the term “perceived parental solicitation x SMG” to the model assessing the relationship between parental solicitation and parental knowledge, with all models adjusted for age, race, SES, and study site. As the sample size was not large enough to establish power to detect small interaction effects (Aiken, West, & Reno, 1991), we designated a p< 0.10 to identify potentially meaningful interaction terms.
To test if same-sex sexual orientation moderated the relationships between the individual components of parental monitoring and several adverse health indicators, we first ran several cross-sectional multivariate logistic regressionmain effects models using adolescent disclosure, perceived parental solicitation, and perceived parental knowledge variables as individual predictor variables and ARA, suicidality, heavy drinking, binge drinking, high anxiety symptoms, and high depressive symptoms as individual outcome variables, with adjustments for age, race, SES, and study site. We then ran several logistic regression models adding several interaction terms. We added the term “adolescent disclosure x SMG” to the models assessing the relationships between adolescent disclosure and the adverse health indicators. We added the term “perceived parental solicitation x SMG” to the models assessing the relationships between perceived parental solicitation and the adverse health indicators. Finally, we added the term “perceived parental knowledge x SMG”to the models assessing the relationships between perceived parental knowledge and adverse health indicators. Again, as the sample size was not large enough to establish power to detect small interaction effects (Aiken et al., 1991), we designated a p< 0.10 to identify potentially meaningful interaction terms. We tested the goodness-of-fit for all logistic regression models using the Hosmer-Lemeshow method (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 2000). If the model with the interaction term was statistically or marginally significant, we calculated the effect of these parental monitoring variables for each comparison group usinga linear combination of estimators. We handled missing data using complete case analysis. We performed all analyses using STATA v13.
Results
Demographics and Adverse Health Indicators
Table 1 summarizes the proportion of SMGs who endorsed same-sex sexual attraction, sexual behavior with same-sex partners, and a sexual identity other than completely heterosexual. Seventy-seven percent of SMGs endorsed more than one measurement of sexual orientation, with almost half of SMGs endorsing all three measurements. A small number endorsed either identity only (n = 7), behavior only (n = 5), or attraction only (n = 2). Table 2 summarizes demographic and dichotomous adverse health indicators between SMGs and heterosexual girls. SMGs comprised 48% of the sample (n=118). No significant differences emerged forrace, education,SES, or study sitecomparing SMGs toheterosexual girls. SMGs were slightly older than heterosexual girls, but this was marginally significant (17.34 ± 1.26 versus 17.04 ± 1.59; t = - 1.67, df = 223, p = 0.05). No statistically significant differences emerged for ARA, heavy drinking, and binge drinking between SMGs and heterosexual girls. There was a higher proportion of SMGs reporting suicidality (Pearson χ2 = 31.29, p< 0.001), high anxiety symptoms (Pearson χ2 = 4.34, p = 0.04), and high depressive symptoms compared toheterosexual girls (Pearson χ2 = 12.38, p< 0.001).
Table 1.
Proportion of SMGs endorsing all three, only two, or only onemeasurement of sexual orientation
Sexual orientation measurement | n(%)a,b |
---|---|
Endorsed identity, attraction, and behavior | 58 (49.2) |
Endorsed identity and attraction only | 26 (22.0) |
Endorsed identity and behavior only | 2 (1.69) |
Endorsed attraction and behavior only | 5 (4.24) |
Endorsed identity only | 7 (5.93) |
Endorsed attraction only | 2 (1.69) |
Endorsed behavior only | 5 (4.24) |
Note: SMG = sexual minority girl.
Total SMG n = 118
Cumulative prevalence will not equal 1 due to missing data
Table 2.
Binary Demographic Variables and Adverse Health Indicators of Girl Participants 14 – 19 Years Old by Sexual Orientation
Variable | Heterosexual (n, %) | SMG (n, %) |
---|---|---|
Total sample (n)a | 128 (52.0) | 118 (48.0) |
Age | 17.04 ± 1.59 | 17.34 ± 1.26 |
Siteb | ||
Pittsburgh | 65 (50.8) | 57 (48.3) |
Columbus | 63 (49.2) | 61 (51.7) |
Raceb | ||
White | 55 (43.0) | 43 (36.4) |
Non-white | 73 (57.0) | 75 (63.6) |
SESb | ||
Higher SES | 42 (36.2) | 32 (30.8) |
Lower SES | 74 (63.8) | 72 (69.2) |
ARAb | ||
No | 32 (82.1) | 35 (79.6) |
Yes | 7 (18.0) | 9 (20.5) |
Suicidalityb | ||
No | 96 (75.6) | 47 (40.5) |
Yes | 31 (24.4) | 69 (59.5)** |
Heavy Drinkingb | ||
No | 19 (36.5) | 23 (33.3) |
Yes | 33 (63.5) | 46 (66.7) |
Binge Drinkingb | ||
No | 32 (61.5) | 37 (53.6) |
Yes | 20 (38.5) | 32 (46.4) |
Anxiety Symptomsb | ||
Low | 85 (66.4) | 63 (53.4) |
High | 43 (33.6) | 55 (46.6)* |
Depression Symptomsb | ||
Low | 96 (75.0) | 62 (53.5) |
High | 32 (25.0) | 54 (56.6)* |
Note.SMG = sexual minority girl; SES = socioeconomic Status; ARA = adolescent relationship abuse.
Row total.
Column total.
p < 0.05.
p< 0.001
Differences in Parental Monitoring between SMGs and Heterosexual Girls
Table 3 summarizes the differences in the components of parental monitoring between SMGs and heterosexual girls. SMGshad lower adolescent disclosure scores than did heterosexual girls(Wilcoxon signed-rank z = 2.08, p = 0.04). SMGs also had lower perceived parental solicitation scores (t = 2.10, df = 232, p = 0.02) and lower perceived parental knowledge scores than did heterosexual girls(t = 5, df = 212, p< 0.001).
Table 3.
Components of Parental Monitoring by Sexual Orientation
Component | Heterosexual | SMG |
---|---|---|
Adolescent Disclosurea | 3.16 (± 0.11) | 2.83 (± 0.13)* |
Perceived Parental Solicitation | 2.62 (± 0.10) | 2.32 (± 0.11)* |
Perceived Parental Knowledge | 3.09 (± 0.07) | 2.48 (± 0.10)** |
Note. SMG = sexual minority girl.
Variable highly skewed, Wilcoxon signed-rank test performed instead
p< 0.05.
p< 0.01
Relationships between Components of Parental Monitoring
There was a statistically significant correlation between adolescent disclosure and perceived parental knowledge and between perceived parental solicitation andperceived parental knowledge (R2 = 0.20, F(5, 211) = 10.45, p< 0.001, 95% CI [0.20, 0.37]and R2 = 0.22, F(7, 209) = 11.43, p< 0.001, 95% CI [0.25, 0.45], respectively); however, the interaction terms added to both models were non-significant (β = 0.10, t = 1.21, p = 0.23, 95% CI [-0.06, 0.27] for “disclosure x SMG” and β = 0.07, t = 0.75,p = 0.45, 95% CI [- 0.12, 0.27] for “solicitation x SMG”).
Relationships between Components of Parental Monitoring and Adverse Health Indicators
Table 4 summarizes the relationships between the components of parental monitoring and adverse health indicators.In the main effects models, there was a statistically significant association between perceived parental knowledge and suicidality (Adjusted Odds Ratio, AOR = 0.57, 95% Confidence Interval, CI, [0.42, 0.79]). There were no statistically significant associations between perceived parental solicitation and ARA, suicidality, heavy drinking, binge drinking, or high depressive symptoms, and there were no statistically significant associations between adolescent disclosure and ARA, heavy drinking, binge drinking, or high depressive symptoms. There were also no statistically significant associations between perceived parental knowledge and ARA, heavy drinking, or binge drinking.
Table 4.
Logistic Regression Main Effects Models and Conditional Logistic Regression Models by Sexual Orientation
Variable | Main Effects AOR [95% CI]a | Heterosexual AOR [95% CI]a | SMG AOR [95% CI]a |
---|---|---|---|
Adolescent Disclosure | |||
ARA | 0.75 [0.48, 1.18] | — | — |
Suicidalityb | — | 0.70 [0.50, 0.99]* | 1.06 [0.80, 1.41] |
Heavy Drinking | 0.99 [0.74, 1.31] | — | — |
Binge Drinking | 0.89 [0.67, 1.17] | — | — |
Anxiety Symptomsb | — | 0.67 [0.49, 0.92]* | 1.06 [0.80, 1.40] |
Depressive Symptoms | 0.85 [0.69, 1.04] | — | — |
Parental Solicitation | |||
ARA | 0.77 [0.45, 1.31] | — | — |
Suicidality | 0.87 [0.68, 1.12] | — | — |
Heavy Drinking | 0.98 [0.68, 1.41] | — | — |
Binge Drinking | 0.99 [0.69, 1.41] | — | — |
Anxiety Symptomsb | — | 0.82 [0.56, 1.20] | 1.37 [0.97, 1.95] |
Depressive Symptoms | 0.80 [0.62, 1.03] | — | — |
Parental Knowledge | |||
ARA | 0.63 [0.36, 1.11] | — | — |
Suicidality | 0.57 [0.42, 0.79]** | — | — |
Heavy Drinking | 0.88 [0.58, 1.35] | — | — |
Binge Drinking | 0.78 [0.51, 1.18] | — | — |
Anxiety Symptomsb | — | 0.53 [0.31, 0.90]* | 1.07 [0.72, 1.59] |
DepressiveSymptomsb | — | 0.26 [0.13, 0.50]** | 0.56 [0.36, 0.88]* |
Note. AOR = adjusted odds ratio; SMG = sexual minority girl; CI = confidence interval; ARA = adolescent relationship abuse;
Adjusted for age, race, SES, and site.
Interactionp≤0.10—conditional model by sexual orientation reported instead.
p < 0.05.
p< 0.001
Our interaction analysis showed the following: the relationships between adolescent disclosure and suicidality (p = 0.07), between adolescent disclosure and high anxiety symptoms (p = 0.04), between perceived parental solicitation and high anxiety symptoms (p = 0.05), between perceived parental knowledge and high anxiety symptoms (p = 0.04), and between perceived parental knowledge and high depressive symptoms (p = 0.05) in heterosexual girls were different than they were forSMGs.To calculate the AOR for each comparison group, we ran post-estimation calculations using a linear combination of estimators. There was a statistically significant association between adolescent disclosure and suicidality in heterosexual girls only (AOR = 0.70 95% CI [0.50, 0.99] versus AOR = 1.06 95% CI [0.80, 1.41]), and there was a statistically significant association between adolescent disclosure and high anxiety symptoms for heterosexual girls only (AOR = 0.67 95% CI [0.49, 0.92] versus AOR = 1.06 95% [0.80, 1.40]). There was an association between perceived parental solicitation and a lower AOR of high anxiety symptoms in heterosexual girls compared to SMGs, but this did not approach statistical significance (AOR = 0.82 95% CI [0.56, 1.20] and AOR = 1.37 95% [0.97, 1.95]). There was also a statistically significant association between perceived parental knowledge and high anxiety symptoms for heterosexual girls only (AOR = 0.53 95% CI [0.31, 0.90] versus AOR = 1.07 95% CI [0.72, 1.59]), andthere was a statistically significant association between perceived parental knowledge and high depressive symptoms in both heterosexual girls and SMGs, with both relationships associated with lower likelihood of reporting high depressive symptoms (AOR = 0.26 95% CI [0.13. 0.50] for heterosexual girls and AOR = 0.56 95% CI [0.36, 0.88] for SMGs).
We graphically represented these interactions by plotting the predicted probabilities of suicidality, high anxiety symptoms, and high depressive symptoms calculated from the logistic regression equations(Vittinghoff, 2012) from the foursignificant interaction models against the adolescent disclosure and perceived parental knowledge variables. For every one unit increase inthe adolescent disclosure score, there was an increase in the proportion of SMGs who reported suicidality (figure 1A) and high anxiety symptoms (figure 1B)while there was a decrease inthe proportion of heterosexual girlswho reported those two health indicators. For every increase of mean perceived parental knowledge score, there was an increasein the proportion of SMGs who reported high anxiety symptoms versus a decrease in proportion of heterosexual girls who reportedhigh anxiety symptoms (figure 1C). For every one unit increase in mean perceived parental knowledge score, the proportion of SMGs who reported high depressive symptoms did not decrease as steeply as the proportion of heterosexual girls who reported high depressive symptoms (1D).
Figure 1.
Interactions between parental monitoring variables (x – axis) by sexual orientation on the predicted proportion of participants who reported these adverse health indicators (y – axis): A) suicidality, B) and C) high anxiety symptoms, and D) high depression symptoms. Models adjusted for age, race, education, socioeconomic status, and study site. Note. SMG = sexual minority girl.*p< 0.10. **p< 0.05
We exploredwhether other factors may play a role in the relationship between components of parental monitoring and between parental monitoring and adverse health outcomes. We first wanted to explorewhether being out to parents impacted these relationships. Among our sample of SMGs (n = 118), 34 % (n = 40) were out to their parents. We then reran our analysesamong SMGs only using “out to parents” as a moderator. The relationships between the components of parental monitoring and between parental monitoring and adverse health outcomes were no different between SMGs who were out and SMGs who were not out to their parents (results available upon request).
We also exploredwhetherassessing the domains of sexual orientation separately may impact the relationships between the components of parental monitoring and between parental monitoring and adverse health indicators. We ran separate interaction models using same-sex sexual identity, same-sex sexual attraction, and same-sex sexual behavior as moderating variables, respectively. These analyses resulted in findings similar to the ones we found using our composite measurement of sexual minority status. Specifically, these distinct measurements of sexual minority status did not moderate the relationships between the components of parental monitoring. Additionally, in our analysis of the relationship between the components of parental monitoring and adverse health indicators, we found the following differences: 1) same-sex behavior did not moderate the relationship between adolescent disclosure and anxiety symptoms; 2) same-sex behavior did moderate the relationship between parental solicitation and suicidality; 3) sexual minority identity did moderate the relationship between parental knowledge and ARA and suicidality; and 4) same-sex attraction and same-sex behavior did moderate the relationship between parental knowledge and suicidality (results available upon request).
Discussion
The first aim of our study was to determine whether the relationship between the sources of parental knowledge (i.e. adolescent disclosure and parental solicitation) and parental knowledge itself is different in SMGs compared to heterosexual girls. The second aim of our study was to determine whether the association between parental monitoring and knowledge andARA, suicidality, heavy drinking, binge drinking, anxiety, and depressive symptoms were different among SMGs compared to heterosexual girls. Our findings showedthat the relationship between the sources of parental knowledge and parental knowledge itself is no different in SMGs compared to heterosexual girls. However, parental monitoring and knowledgeappear to be less protective against adverse health indicators among SMGs compared to heterosexual girls. The general adolescent health literatureindicates that parental monitoring may be protective against many adverse health indicators, but our study suggests that parental monitoring may not be as protective for SMGsas it is for heterosexual girls.
In our analysis,SMGs had lower adolescent disclosure scores than did heterosexual girls. This suggests that SMGs are less likely than heterosexual girls to disclose accurately theirbehaviors to their parents. Although our measurement of adolescent disclosure did not capture the exact information the participant did or did not disclose to their parents, we speculate that SMGs may have compelling reasons to disclose less accurate information about their behaviors to their parents.For example, theymay avoid disclosure of any location and activity that is suggestive of their same-sex sexual orientation, such as joining a gay-straight alliance at school. This is a significant concern for many sexual minorities, because many fear a negative reaction from their parents (Wisniewski, Robinson, & Deluty, 2010)or have experienced verbal and physical abuse from their parents oncetheir parents became aware of their same-sex sexual orientation(Jadwin-Cakmak, Pingel, Harper, & Bauermeister, 2015).
Additionally, our analysis showed that SMGs had lower perceived parental solicitation and knowledge scores than did heterosexual girls. This suggests that SMGs are less likely than are heterosexual girls to believe that their parents are seeking information about their behavior, and itsuggests that SMGs are less likely to believe that their parents actually know their behavior outside their immediate supervision.We hypothesize that these beliefs may be reflective of the quality of parent-child relationship inSMGs.There is a positive association between these two components of parental monitoring and a supportive and positive parent-child relationship(Fletcher et al. 2004). Nevertheless, many sexual minority youth perceive lower support from their parents (Needham & Austin, 2010) and, therefore, may perceive lower parental solicitation and knowledge.
In contrast to the differences in parental monitoring components between SMGs and heterosexual girls, sexual orientation did not moderate the relationship between adolescent disclosure and perceived parental knowledge. This suggests that for both SMGs and heterosexual girls, the more they disclose to their parents, the more likely they believe their parents actually know about their behavior. Research suggests that adolescents are selective of what and how much they tell their parents to maintain privacy (Kerr, Stattin, & Engels, 2008). This may be highly relevant for SMGs, who may not want to disclose anything that may be suggestive of their same-sex sexual orientation. Furthermore, there is consistent evidence that adolescent disclosure is positively correlated with parental knowledge(Willoughby & Hamza, 2011). Other factors, however, play a role inaltering this correlation, such as a supportive parent-child relationship (Laird, Pettit, Bates, & Dodge, 2003). As previously mentioned, sexual minority youthmay perceive lower parental support than do heterosexual youth(Needham & Austin, 2010); therefore, we propose that the quality of the parent-child relationship inSMGs is a more plausible moderator of the relationship between adolescent disclosure and perceived parental knowledge.
Sexual orientation also did not moderate the relationship between parental solicitation and parental knowledge. This suggests that for both SMGs and heterosexual girls, the more they perceive that their parents are seeking information about their behavior, the more likely they believe that their parents will actually know about their behavior.It appears that sexual orientation does not change the relationship between parental solicitation and parental knowledge. Therefore, we hypothesize another factor—such as parental acceptance of a child’s sexual orientation—may be moderating this relationship.Many sexual minority youth may view certain parenting behaviors as rejecting of their same-sex sexual orientation (Ryan et al., 2009). These behaviors may be subtle, suchas blaming the child for anti-gay victimization. We suspect that many SMGs may view parental solicitation as a subtle form of rejection because they may perceive that their parents are tracking their behaviors to prevent them from engaging in same-sex relationships.Nevertheless, in the context of a supportive parent-child relationship, this may not be the case, especially if parents are accepting and supportive of their child’s same-sex sexual orientation.In other words, SMGs who have good relationships with their parents may perceive parental solicitation as less invasive and rejecting than do SMGs who have poor relationships with their parents. This positive perception of parental solicitation may then be correlated with higher perceptions of parental knowledge, but this requires further investigation to confirm our hypothesis.
Despite the fact that sexual orientation did not moderate relationships between the sources of perceived parental knowledge and perceived parental knowledge itself,we have evidence to support the hypothesis that the relationships between the components of parental monitoring and some adverse health indicators were different between SMGs and heterosexual girls. For heterosexual girls, higher levels of adolescent disclosure were associated with reduced oddsof suicidality and anxiety symptoms, but not for SMGs. Furthermore, unlike heterosexual girls, higher levels of perceived parental knowledge were not associated with reduced oddsof anxiety and depressive symptomsamongSMGs. Several studies in the general youth population have demonstrated that higher levels of parental monitoring are associated with lower suicidality (King et al., 2001),anxiety symptoms,(Bacchini, Miranda, & Affuso, 2011), and depressive symptoms (Hamza & Willoughby, 2011). Disclosure to parents and subsequent parental knowledge may provide opportunities for parents to support or counsel their children (Hamza & Willoughby, 2011), which may then reducesuicidality, anxiety symptoms, and depressive symptoms.There may be several possibilities for why this is not the case for SMGs. One possibility is that parents may choose to dismiss or ignore their child’s mental health problems if they arerejectingof their child’s sexual orientation. Alternatively, SMGs may view any attempt by their parents to counsel them as irrelevant, because they may perceive that their parents are approaching their mental health problems from a heteronormative point of view. Finally, there may be other confounding factors, such as bullying at school, which may contribute to mental health problems in SMGs despite higher levels of parental monitoring and knowledge.
Although our analysis showed differences in parental monitoring and knowledge between SMGs and heterosexual girls, our exploratory analysis showed that there were no differences in parental monitoring components between SMGs who were out to their parents versus SMGs who were not.Furthermore, parental awareness of a child’s same-sex sexual orientation did not moderate the relationships between the sources of perceived parental knowledge and perceived parental knowledge itself. Parental awareness of a child’s same-sex sexual orientation also did not moderate the relationships between the individual components of parental monitoring and many adverse health indicators of interest. This suggests that parental knowledge of their child’s sexual orientation does not impact the relationship between the components of parental monitoring and between parental monitoring and adverse health indicators. Thoma and Huebner’s (2014) study also suggested that being out to parents had little impact on parental monitoring. In their study, sexual minority boys who were out to at least one of their parents engaged in riskysexual behaviors despite higher levels of parental monitoring.Nevertheless, bias towards the null hypothesesin these exploratoryanalyses may exist partly due to low statistical power.
The findings from this study bring us closer to understanding how parental monitoring influences the health of sexual minority youth. To date, there is only one other study that examined the role of parental monitoring in the health of this high-risk population. Thoma and Huebner(2014) found that among young men who have sex with men (YMSM),there was an association between higher parental monitoring and parental awareness of a participant’s sexual orientation andhigher rates of unprotectedsexual intercourse. This informative study speculated that parental monitoring may not be as protective for YMSM compared to their heterosexual peers. Our study builds uponthis seminal work in three ways. First, our study shows analogous associations between components of parental monitoring and some health indicators in SMGs, with higher levels of parental monitoring associated with a higher proportion of SMGs reporting some adverse health indicators. This has not been done previously in SMGs. Second, there appear to be discrepancies between SMGs and heterosexual girlsin their perceptions ofthe components ofparental monitoring. Finally, we found differential associationsbetweenthe components of parental monitoringandseveral adverse health indicators comparingSMGs toheterosexual girls.
Assessing the protective effectsof specific parental practices such as parental monitoring is criticalto inform potential interventions. There are several interventions thattrain parents in specific skillslike parental monitoringthat can reduce symptoms of anxiety and depression, delinquent behaviors, and substance abuse among participants(Hawkins, Kosterman, Catalano, Hill, & Abbott, 2005; Stanton et al., 2004).However, many of these interventions assume that parental monitoring functions similarly between sexual minority and heterosexual youth. Thoma and Huebner’s study(2014) and our study suggest that parental monitoring alone may not be protective against many adverse health indicators forsexual minority youth; however, one must use caution in concluding from our study that same-sex sexual orientation itself alters the relationship between the components of parental monitoringand adverse health indicators. Many sexual minority youth experience trouble with parent-child relationships(Bouris et al., 2010). We thus propose that future studies on parental monitoring of SMY should focus more on the acceptance and support of the child’s same-sex sexual orientation instead of the child’ssame-sex sexual orientation itself. Such research may guide the development of interventions aiming toaddress the unique challenges parents face when monitoring their sexual minority children.
Limitations and Future Directions
The strength of this study is our purposive sampling strategy in a clinical setting. This allowed us to directlycompare SMGs versus heterosexual girlsin the associations between the components of parental monitoring and several important adverse health indicators.The majority of research on sexual minority youth relies on convenience sampling from LGBT venues(Bouris et al., 2010). Convenience sampling in this population may overestimate maladaptive behaviors (Binson, Blair, Huebner, & Woods, 2007)and adverse health outcomes such as gay-related victimization(Elze, 2007). Furthermore participants recruited via convenience sampling tend to be more open about their sexual orientation(Hershberger, PiLkington, & D'Augelli, 1997), which may lead to biased results. In contrast, we measured sexual orientation using a composite of sexual identity, same-sex sexual attraction, and same-sex sexual behavior. The composite measurement makes the results of this study generalizable.
There are several limitations to keep in mind.First and foremost, parental monitoring experiences may not be universal to subgroups of SMGs.A girl who is attracted to the same sex, but does not endorse same-sex behavior or a sexual minority identity, may have a different experience with parental monitoring than does a girl who identifies as a lesbian or bisexual. In this study, we found significant overlap of SMGs who endorsed more than one domain of sexual orientation (i.e. a girl who was attracted to the same-sex also endorsed same-sex behavior and a lesbian or bisexual identity). Because of this significant overlap, it is difficult to draw conclusions from the interaction analyses using only one of the measurements of sexual orientation. Additionally, there was a small number of SMGs who endorsed only one of the three measurements. Thus, any interaction analysis using only one of the measurements of sexual orientation will have low statistical power. Future research should recruit a large enough sample of girls using these various measures for sexual minority status to examine whether parental monitoring operates differently between these subgroups of SMGs.
Additionally, although we postulated many factors that may explain our findings, this study is a cross-sectional analysis; therefore, we could not make causal inferencesbetween the components of parental monitoring and adverse health indicators in SMGs. Future studiesshould examinethe temporal and bidirectional associations between the components of parental monitoring and adverse health indicators in SMGsusing a longitudinal design. Furthermore, although we do have a higher proportion of SMGs compared to other studies, we were still underpowered to detect interaction effects (i.e. group differences). We thus used a p-value <0.10 to detect meaningful interaction effects in our study.Nevertheless, the vast majority of statistically significant interaction effects hadp values less than 0.05. Future studies that make direct comparisons between sexual minority and heterosexual youth should use larger samples to detect important interaction effects.
Another limitation is our measurement of being out to parents, for which girls only had to indicate disclosing their sexuality to one parent to be coded as being out. One potential explanation for our findings that the relationship between parental monitoring components and adverse health indicators did not vary by being out to parents may be due to only one parent being aware of the adolescent’s sexual orientation. We suspect that it may be difficult for a parent to monitor effectively their sexual minority child and, at the same time, keep the child’s sexual orientation secret from the other parent. Future studies should analyze how being out to both parents or to one parent only can differentially impact the effectiveness of parental monitoring in preventing adverse health outcomes.
Additionally, the survey relied on the participants’ perceptions of parental solicitation and knowledge. Data from parents’ reports of monitoring their sexual minority children may better elucidate potential pathways for protection against adverse health indicators. Future studies in this population should obtain parental reports of their monitoring behavior.Nevertheless, there is some evidence to suggest that adolescent perception of parental solicitation and parental knowledge may be a good proxy for parental perceptions of solicitation and knowledge (Stattin & Kerr, 2000).
Moreover, our cohort consisted of older adolescents (14 – 19 years old). One may argue that parental monitoring may be less relevant for this age group compared to elementary and middle school children, as they have achieved greater independence from their parents than have younger adolescents. We did adjust for age in our analysisand recent studies suggest that the correlation between parental monitoring and lower reports of many high-risk behaviors may extend beyond the high school years(Napper, Hummer, Chithambo, & LaBrie, 2015).
Although we attempted in our analysis of parental monitoring to be as consistent as we could with the interpretation established by Stattin and Kerr (2000), we did not use the same measures. We only had one item asking if our participants lied to their parents about their behavior. We recognize that there are various strategies that adolescents employ to control what or how much parents know about their behaviors or friends (Tilton-Weaver et al., 2010). These strategies include: 1) not telling their parents any information (i.e. nondisclosure), 2) avoiding parents so parents do not have the opportunity to solicit information about their behavior, 3) outright lying, or 4) selective disclosure of information to parents (i.e. secrecy) (Finkenauer, Engels, & Meeus, 2002; Smetana, 2008). Because of the stigma surrounding same-sex attraction and behavior, SMGs may be more likely to use these strategies to prevent their parents from knowing about their same-sex sexual orientation or behaviors. It is therefore important for future studies to examine these various strategies of adolescent disclosure among SMGs. Ultimately such research can identify ways to encourage SMGs to disclose their behaviors to their parents.
Additionally, our items on parental solicitation were non-specific and asked participants on their perception of their parents seeking information about their behavior. Other measures on parental solicitation are more specific on the behaviors parents used to obtain this information and the type of information solicited. Such behaviors include parents talking to the parents of their child’s friends(Guilamo-Ramos, Jaccard, & Dittus, 2010). Additionally, SMGs may perceive parental solicitation in two ways depending on their relationship with their parents. For SMGs whose parents are rejecting of their sexual orientation, parental solicitation may be a form of punishment or an effort by parents to dissuade SMGs from engaging in same-sex behavior. On the other hand, for SMGs whose parents are accepting, parental solicitation may be an over-reaction by parents to protect their children from a homophobic environment. Our measurement of parental solicitation neither captures the specific behaviors parents use to obtain information or parental motivation for monitoring their children’s behaviors. Information on these two factors may be crucial in developing interventions aimed at improving parental monitoring to be relevant to the psychosocial needs of sexual minorities. Nevertheless, despite the non-specificity of our parental solicitation measures, this approach has consistently shown to have a strong correlation between adolescent disclosure and parental knowledge (Guilamo-Ramos et al., 2010) and adverse health outcomes(Fletcher et al., 2004; Hamza & Willoughby, 2011; Willoughby & Hamza, 2011).Our results prompt additional research on the specific behaviors and attitudes that may be useful for parents of sexual minorities.
Finally, the data set did not have any questions about another important component of parental monitoring:parental control (i.e., parents creating rules and environments that would make it difficult for their children to go to locations or to engage in activities without parental knowledge). Future studies should also explore if same-sex sexual orientation moderates the relationship between parental control and knowledge, and if same-sex sexual orientation moderates the relationship between parental control and adverse health indicators.
Conclusions
Our study highlights significant differences inadolescent disclosure and in perceptions of parental solicitation and knowledge between SMGs and heterosexual girls. Furthermore, there is a significant disparity between SMGs and heterosexual girls in the association between adolescent disclosure, parental solicitation, and parental knowledge and adverse health indicators—with increased adolescent disclosure, perceived parental solicitation, and perceived parental knowledge associated with decreased odds of several adverse health indicatorsinheterosexual girls only. This suggests that some components of parental monitoring may not be as protective for SMGs as they are for heterosexual girls. These results add to the emerging literature on disparities in the characteristics of parent-child relationshipsamong sexual minority youth. Ultimately, these findings can inform potential interventions that address the unique challenges parents face in rearing their sexual minority children.
Acknowledgments
We would also like to thank Debra L.Bogen, Jill RadtkeDemirci, Kristin Ray, Elian Rosenfeld, Thomas Radomski, Charity G. Moore, and Kevin L. Kraemer for the preparation of this manuscript. Without their dedicated help, the completion of this manuscript would not be possible.
References
- Aiken LS, West SG, & Reno RR (1991). Multiple regression : testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, Calif.: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Bacchini D, Miranda MC, & Affuso G (2011). Effects of parental monitoring and exposure to community violence on antisocial behavior and anxiety/depression among adolescents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26(2), 269–292. doi: 10.1177/088626051036287920234055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badgett L, & Goldberg N (2009). Best Practices for Asking Questions about Sexual Orientation on Surveys. Williams Institute Sexual Minority Assessment Research Team. 2014 (February 20, 2015). [Google Scholar]
- Beck KH, Boyle JR, & Boekeloo BO (2004). Parental monitoring and adolescent drinking: results of a 12-month follow-up. American Journal of Health Behavior, 28(3), 272–279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Binson D, Blair J, Huebner DM, & Woods WJ (2007). Sampling in Surveys of Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual People In Meyer IH & Northridge ME (Eds.), The Health of Sexual Minorities: Public Health Perspectives on Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual and Transgender Populations (pp. 375–418). Boston, MA: Springer US. [Google Scholar]
- Birmaher B, Brent DA, Chiappetta L, Bridge J, Monga S, & Baugher M (1999). Psychometric properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): a replication study. Journal of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(10), 1230–1236. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199910000-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouris A, Guilamo-Ramos V, Pickard A, Shiu C, Loosier PS, Dittus P, … Michael Waldmiller J (2010). A systematic review of parental influences on the health and well-being of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth: time for a new public health research and practice agenda. Journal of Primary Prevention, 31(5–6), 273–309. doi: 10.1007/s10935-010-0229-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown BB, Mounts N, Lamborn SD, & Steinberg L (1993). Parenting practices and peer group affiliation in adolescence. Child Dev, 64(2), 467–482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darby-Mullins P, & Murdock TB (2007). The Influence of Family Environment Factors on Self-Acceptance and Emotional Adjustment Among Gay, Lesbian, and Bisexual Adolescents. Journal of GLBT Family Studies, 3(1), 75–91. doi: 10.1300/J461v03n01_04 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, & McMahon RJ (1998). Parental monitoring and the prevention of child and adolescent problem behavior: a conceptual and empirical formulation. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1(1), 61–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doyle CM, Rees AM, & Titus TL (2015). Perceptions of Same-Sex Relationships and Marriage as Gender Role Violations: An Examination of Gendered Expectations (Sexism). Journal of Homosexuality, 62(11), 1576–1598. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2015.1073038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- East PL, Chien NC, Adams JA, Hokoda A, & Maier A (2010). Links between sisters' sexual and dating victimization: the roles of neighborhood crime and parental controls. Journal of Family Psychology, 24(6), 698–708. doi: 10.1037/a0021751 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elze DE (2007). Research with Sexual Minority Youths. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social Services, 18(2), 73–99. doi: 10.1300/J041v18n02_05 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Faden VB, & Goldman M (2004). Alcohol and development in youth--a multidisciplinary overview. Introduction. Alcohol Res Health, 28(3), 107–108. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faulkner AH, & Cranston K (1998). Correlates of same-sex sexual behavior in a random sample of Massachusetts high school students. Am J Public Health, 88(2), 262–266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finkenauer C, Engels RCME, & Meeus W (2002). Keeping secrets from parents: Advantages and disadvantages of secrecy in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31(2), 123–136. doi: 10.1023/A:1014069926507 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher AC, Steinberg L, & Williams-Wheeler M (2004). Parental influences on adolescent problem behavior: revisiting Stattin and Kerr. Child Development, 75(3), 781–796. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00706.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floyd FJ, Stein TS, Harter KSM, Allison A, & Nye CL (1999). Gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths: Separation-individuation, parental attitudes, identity consolidation, and well-being. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28(6), 719–739. doi: 10.1023/A:1021691601737 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman MS, Marshal MP, Guadamuz TE, Wei C, Wong CF, Saewyc E, & Stall R (2011). A meta-analysis of disparities in childhood sexual abuse, parental physical abuse, and peer victimization among sexual minority and sexual nonminority individuals. American Journal of Public Health, 101(8), 1481–1494. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.190009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garofalo R, Wolf RC, Wissow LS, Woods ER, & Goodman E (1999). Sexual orientation and risk of suicide attempts among a representative sample of youth. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 153(5), 487–493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guilamo-Ramos V, Jaccard J, & Dittus P (2010). Parental Monitoring of Adolescents: Current Perspectives for Researchers and Practitioners: Columbia: University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hamza CA, & Willoughby T (2011). Perceived parental monitoring, adolescent disclosure, and adolescent depressive symptoms: a longitudinal examination. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(7), 902–915. doi: 10.1007/s10964-010-9604-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins JD, Kosterman R, Catalano RF, Hill KG, & Abbott RD (2005). Promoting positive adult functioning through social development intervention in childhood: long-term effects from the Seattle Social Development Project. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 159(1), 25–31. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.1.25 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hershberger SL, PiLkington NW, & D'Augelli AR (1997). Predictors of Suicide Attempts Among Gay, Lesbian, and Bisexual Youth. Journal of Adolescent Research, 12(4), 477–497. doi: 10.1177/0743554897124004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hosmer DW, & Lemeshow S (2000). Applied logistic regression (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender Health Issues and Research Gaps and Opportunities (2011). The Health of Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender People: Building a Foundation for Better Understanding. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US) Retrieved from http://www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.pitt.idm.oclc.org/books/NBK64806/ [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jadwin-Cakmak LA, Pingel ES, Harper GW, & Bauermeister JA (2015). Coming Out to Dad: Young Gay and Bisexual Men's Experiences Disclosing Same-Sex Attraction to Their Fathers. American Journal of Men's Health, 9(4), 274–288. doi: 10.1177/1557988314539993 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kann L, Olsen EO, McManus T, Kinchen S, Chyen D, Harris WA, … Prevention. (2011). Sexual identity, sex of sexual contacts, and health-risk behaviors among students in grades 9–12--youth risk behavior surveillance, selected sites, United States, 2001–2009. MMWR Surveill Summ, 60(7), 1–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerr M, Stattin H, & Engels RCME (2008). What can parents do? : new insights into the role of parents in adolescent problem behavior. Chichester, West Sussex; Hoboken N.J.: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- King RA, Schwab-Stone M, Flisher AJ, Greenwald S, Kramer RA, Goodman SH, … Gould MS (2001). Psychosocial and risk behavior correlates of youth suicide attempts and suicidal ideation. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(7), 837–846. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200107000-00019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird RD, Pettit GS, Bates JE, & Dodge KA (2003). Parents' monitoring-relevant knowledge and adolescents' delinquent behavior: evidence of correlated developmental changes and reciprocal influences. Child Dev, 74(3), 752–768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewinsohn PM, Rohde P, & Seeley JR (1996). Adolescent Suicidal Ideation and Attempts: Prevalence, Risk Factors, and Clinical Implications. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 3(1), 25–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.1996.tb00056.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Liu RT, & Mustanski B (2012). Suicidal ideation and self-harm in lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender youth. Am J Prev Med, 42(3), 221–228. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2011.10.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshal MP, Dermody SS, Shultz ML, Sucato GS, Stepp SD, Chung T, … Hipwell AE (2013). Mental health and substance use disparities among urban adolescent lesbian and bisexual girls. Journal of the American Psychiatric Nurses Association, 19(5), 271–279. doi: 10.1177/1078390313503552 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshal MP, Dietz LJ, Friedman MS, Stall R, Smith HA, McGinley J, … Brent DA (2011). Suicidality and depression disparities between sexual minority and heterosexual youth: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Adolescent Health, 49(2), 115–123. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshal MP, Friedman MS, Stall R, King KM, Miles J, Gold MA, … Morse JQ (2008). Sexual orientation and adolescent substance use: a meta-analysis and methodological review. Addiction, 103(4), 546–556. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02149.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshal MP, Molina BS, & Pelham WE Jr. (2003). Childhood ADHD and adolescent substance use: an examination of deviant peer group affiliation as a risk factor. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17(4), 293–302. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.17.4.293 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshal MP, Sucato G, Stepp SD, Hipwell A, Smith HA, Friedman MS, … Markovic N (2012a). Substance use and mental health disparities among sexual minority girls: results from the Pittsburgh girls study. Journal of Pediatric & Adolescent Gynecology, 25(1), 15–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jpag.2011.06.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshal MP, Sucato G, Stepp SD, Hipwell A, Smith HA, Friedman MS, … Markovic N (2012b). Substance use and mental health disparities among sexual minority girls: results from the Pittsburgh girls study. J Pediatr Adolesc Gynecol, 25(1), 15–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jpag.2011.06.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mash EJ, & Barkley RA (2002). Child Psychopathology, Second Edition: Guilford Publications. [Google Scholar]
- McCauley HL, Dick RN, Tancredi DJ, Goldstein S, Blackburn S, Silverman JG, … Miller E (2014). Differences by sexual minority status in relationship abuse and sexual and reproductive health among adolescent females. Journal of Adolescent Health, 55(5), 652–658. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.04.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer IH (2003). Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: conceptual issues and research evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 129(5), 674–697. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.674 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller E, Decker MR, McCauley HL, Tancredi DJ, Levenson RR, Waldman J, … Silverman JG (2010). Pregnancy coercion, intimate partner violence and unintended pregnancy. Contraception, 81(4), 316–322. doi: 10.1016/j.contraception.2009.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy DA, Marelich WD, Herbeck DM, & Payne DL (2009). Family routines and parental monitoring as protective factors among early and middle adolescents affected by maternal HIV/AIDS. Child Development, 80(6), 1676–1691. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01361.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Napper LE, Hummer JF, Chithambo TP, & LaBrie JW (2015). Perceived parent and peer marijuana norms: the moderating effect of parental monitoring during college. Prevention Science, 16(3), 364–373. doi: 10.1007/s11121-014-0493-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Needham BL, & Austin EL (2010). Sexual orientation, parental support, and health during the transition to young adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(10), 1189–1198. doi: 10.1007/s10964-010-9533-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pettit GS, Keiley MK, Laird RD, Bates JE, & Dodge KA (2007). Predicting the Developmental Course of Mother-Reported Monitoring Across Childhood and Adolescence From Early Proactive Parenting, Child Temperament, and Parents' Worries. Journal of family psychology : JFP : journal of the Division of Family Psychology of the American Psychological Association (Division 43), 21(2), 206–217. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.2.206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Racz SJ, & McMahon RJ (2011). The relationship between parental knowledge and monitoring and child and adolescent conduct problems: a 10-year update. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14(4), 377–398. doi: 10.1007/s10567-011-0099-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radloff LS (1991). The use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20(2), 149–166. doi: 10.1007/BF01537606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosario M, Reisner SL, Corliss HL, Wypij D, Calzo J, & Austin SB (2014). Sexual-orientation disparities in substance use in emerging adults: a function of stress and attachment paradigms. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(3), 790–804. doi: 10.1037/a0035499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosario M, Reisner SL, Corliss HL, Wypij D, Frazier AL, & Austin SB (2014). Disparities in depressive distress by sexual orientation in emerging adults: the roles of attachment and stress paradigms. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43(5), 901–916. doi: 10.1007/s10508-013-0129-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose AJ, & Rudolph KD (2006). A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychol Bull, 132(1), 98–131. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.98 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudolph KD, Troop-Gordon W, & Granger DA (2010). Peer victimization and aggression: moderation by individual differences in salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase. J Abnorm Child Psychol, 38(6), 843–856. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9412-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan C, Huebner D, Diaz RM, & Sanchez J (2009). Family rejection as a predictor of negative health outcomes in white and Latino lesbian, gay, and bisexual young adults. Pediatrics, 123(1), 346–352. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-3524 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan C, Russell ST, Huebner D, Diaz R, & Sanchez J (2010). Family acceptance in adolescence and the health of LGBT young adults. J Child Adolesc Psychiatr Nurs, 23(4), 205–213. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6171.2010.00246.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smetana JG (2008). "It's 10 o'clock: Do you know where your children are?" Recent advances in understanding parental monitoring and adolescents' information management. Child Development Perspectives, 2(1), 19–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00036.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stanton B, Cole M, Galbraith J, Li X, Pendleton S, Cottrel L, … Kaljee L (2004). Randomized trial of a parent intervention: parents can make a difference in long-term adolescent risk behaviors, perceptions, and knowledge. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 158(10), 947–955. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.158.10.947 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stattin H, & Kerr M (2000). Parental monitoring: a reinterpretation. Child Development, 71(4), 1072–1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talley AE, Hughes TL, Aranda F, Birkett M, & Marshal MP (2014). Exploring alcohol-use behaviors among heterosexual and sexual minority adolescents: Intersections with sex, age, and race/ethnicity. American Journal of Public Health, 104(2), 295–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thoma BC, & Huebner DM (2014). Parental monitoring, parent-adolescent communication about sex, and sexual risk among young men who have sex with men. AIDS and Behavior, 18(8), 1604–1614. doi: 10.1007/s10461-014-0717-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tilton-Weaver L, Kerr M, Pakalniskeine V, Tokic A, Salihovic S, & Stattin H (2010). Open up or close down: how do parental reactions affect youth information management? Journal of Adolescence, 33(2), 333–346. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.07.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tolan P, Gorman-Smith D, & Henry D (2004). Supporting families in a high-risk setting: proximal effects of the SAFEChildren preventive intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(5), 855–869. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.855 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vittinghoff E (2012). Regression methods in biostatistics : linear, logistic, survival, and repeated measures models (2nd ed.). New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Willoughby T, & Hamza CA (2011). A longitudinal examination of the bidirectional associations among perceived parenting behaviors, adolescent disclosure and problem behavior across the high school years. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(4), 463–478. doi: 10.1007/s10964-010-9567-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wisniewski TJ, Robinson TN Jr., & Deluty RH (2010). An evolutionary psychological investigation of parental distress and reproductive coercion during the "coming out" of gay sons. Journal of Homosexuality, 57(1), 163–190. doi: 10.1080/00918360903446077 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]