Abstract
Purpose
The purpose of this study was to quantify ipsilesional upper extremity (UE) stand-reaching performance (kinematics and kinetics) among chronic stroke survivors.
Method
Community-dwelling chronic stroke survivors (n=13) and age-similar healthy adults (n=13) performed flexion- and abduction-reaching tasks. Surface EMG and acceleration were sampled using wireless sensors from the prime movers (anterior and middle deltoid) and provided performance-outcome (reaction time, burst duration, movement time, and movement initiation time) and performance-production (peak acceleration) measures and were then evaluated.
Results
Individuals with chronic stroke demonstrated significantly reduced performance outcomes (i.e., longer reaction time, burst duration, movement time, and movement initiation time) and performance production ability (i.e., smaller peak acceleration) compared to their healthy counterparts (p < 0.05) for both flexion- and abduction-reaching movements.
Conclusion
Our results are suggestive of post-stroke deficits in ipsilesional motor execution during a stand-reaching task. Based on these findings, it is essential to integrate ipsilesional UE training into rehabilitation interventions as this might aid functional reaching activities of daily living and could ultimately help community-dwelling chronic stroke survivors maintain their independent living.
1. Introduction
On average, only 5% of chronic stroke survivors regain complete function of their contra-lesioned upper extremity (UE), with 20-80% of survivors having persistent motor deficits [1, 2]. Contra-lesional deficits increase individuals' reliance on their ipsilesional UE for activities of daily living and maintaining functional independence. However, in the past decade, studies have indicated the presence of motor deficits in the ipsilesional UE among individuals with chronic stroke [3–6]. Laboratory-based, kinematic studies have demonstrated ipsilesional upper and lower extremity deficits during the performance of various motor tasks. Such deficits were seen substantially in the finger tapping task (lesser mean number of taps in five 10 second trials), goal-directed aiming task (decreased peak acceleration, final target position error, and increased time to complete the task), and cyclical single-limb movements (cycle durations were higher, and movement amplitudes were lesser) of the ipsilesional upper and/or lower extremity among people with chronic stroke [7, 8].
Another recent large cohort study evaluated the effect of subacute stroke on ipsilesional UE motor deficits during a visually guided seated-reaching task using the KINARM robotic exoskeleton [9]. Based on the study results, the authors suggested that kinematic measurements of ipsilesional UE were similar to those reported in previous studies, such as initial direction error, along with increased reaction time and movement time. Additionally, based on the current research on ipsilesional UE motor deficits, recent studies are exploring and developing rehabilitation methodologies to improve gross motor function. A pilot study exhibited substantial improvements in ipsilesional UE movement kinematics (reduction in distance error, direction error, and aspect ratio) and functional performance (Jebsen-Taylor Hand Function Test, Fugl-Meyer assessment, and Functional Independence Measure) during a seated target reaching task, for a three-week (1.5 hours/session) training paradigm that included practice of real-life ipsilesional UE tasks in a virtual-reality environment [10].
While the current research emphasis is on tasks suitable to understand and rehabilitate ipsilesional UE control and deficits, the majority of studies have only focused on UE target reaching from a seated position. Despite the fact that seated UE reaching is relevant to workplace-related activities, stand-reaching functional tasks are comparatively more challenging, owing to the preparatory demands of the movements, including postural muscle preparation and maintaining the center of mass (CoM) within the base of support (BoS) [11]. Forward and lateral UE functional reaching activities are used abundantly in the real world, and muscles of the shoulder complex such as the anterior and middle deltoid play a principal role in these movements [12]. Additionally, very few studies have reported the use of a stand-reaching task among individuals with chronic stroke [13, 14], and among those that did make use of this task, the majority focused on examining lower extremity postural responses and did not consider activation of the UE muscles.
In addition to UE stand-reaching functional task performance, rapid arm movements facilitate the generation of anticipatory feed forward mechanisms during self-induced perturbations [11, 15]. Such mechanisms promote recovery of equilibrium (postural stability), by regulating the relationship between the body's CoM and the BoS. Studies have also indicated that a rapid muscle force generated at the shoulder joints can facilitate a higher degree of stabilization as reaching with an UE to grasp or touch an object for support rapidly changing the BoS, thus generating “change-in-support” reactions [16]. Given that stand reaching ability is an essential prerequisite to UE functional task performance and is also necessary for independent living and preventing falls via rapid arm movements, it is critical to evaluate ipsilesional UE motor function in standing among community-dwelling individuals with chronic stroke.
Quantification of ipsilesional UE motor deficits in individuals with chronic stroke using functional ability measures (Functional Independence Measure, Chedoke-McMaster Stroke Assessment, and Purdue Pegboard) has found minimal differences compared to healthy controls [9]. Studies have used electromyography (EMG) for assessment of UE dysfunction and have demonstrated the use of both performance-production and performance-outcome measures to understand motor behavior in association with its neural control [17, 18]. Performance production measures may provide more insight into aspects of the nervous system during the performance of a motor task. In other words, the central nervous system (CNS) representation of movement control could be evaluated through performance-production measures (i.e., peak acceleration) [19, 20], and the result of a motor task performance could be evaluated using performance-outcome measures (i.e., reaction time, movement time, burst duration, and movement initiation time). Given the importance of the ipsilesional UE for functional task performance, as well as it being the primary contributor of ‘UE change-in-support' reactions to prevent falls, it is crucial to understand movement deficits of the ipsilesional UE in chronic stroke survivors.
Thus, the purpose of this study was to quantify the reaching performance of chronic stroke survivors' ipsilesional UE using both, outcome-based and production-based measures for a stand-reaching task, and then compare their performance to age-similar healthy adults. We hypothesized that, on both flexion- and abduction-reaching tasks, the ipsilesional UE of stroke survivors would show measurable deficits in movement control marked by significant deterioration in both performance- and production-based variables compared with age-similar healthy adults.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
Twenty-six individuals participated in this study (N = 26). The study consisted of two groups: individuals with chronic hemiparetic stroke (62.75 ± 6.12, n = 13) and age-similar healthy adults (63.82 ± 6.34, n = 13). The ipsilesional arm was non-dominant for n = 8 and dominant for n = 5 participants. The Institutional Review Board for the University of Illinois at Chicago approved the study, and informed consent was obtained from all the participants.
2.1.1. Subject Eligibility
Inclusion criteria for stroke survivors consisted of the physician's confirmation of chronic hemiparetic stroke. Participants with a history of a single cortical stroke, confirmed by their physician, were also recruited for this study. Individuals were required to be able to stand independently for at least five minutes without the use of an assistive device so that they would be able to complete the functional arm reach task. Participants' mean ± SD disability status was quantified using the Modified Rankin Scale, and the participants ranged from mild to moderate disability (2.72 ± 0.49) [21]. Both stroke and healthy adult participants answered a general health questionnaire, filled out a Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE) [22], and demonstrated a comfortable and full range of motion for the shoulder. Participants were excluded upon reporting recent surgeries (< 6 months). Participants with active signs or symptoms of any discomfort of the shoulder and any other neurological (e.g., Parkinson's disease, vascular (multi-infarct) Parkinsonism, vestibular deficits, peripheral neuropathy, or unstable epilepsy), musculoskeletal, and cardiovascular disorders and cognitive deficits (score > 10 on Short-Orientation-Memory-Concentration (SOMC) test of Cognitive Impairment) were also excluded [23]. Individuals with cardiovascular diseases as assessed by resting heart rate (> 85% of age-predicted maximal) and resting oxygen saturation (< 95) were also excluded.
2.2. Apparatus Design
Set-up for the functional arm reach task included a custom-made arm reaching apparatus, which consisted of a long metal pole attached to a wooden board on the ground. A large load-bearing clamp was fixed to the metal pole, with the long-shaft of the clamp perpendicular to the length of the pole. The large load-bearing clamp was adjusted to participants' shoulder height. Another movable, smaller clamp was used to attach a 36 inch wooden ruler perpendicular to the shaft of the larger clamp. Circular foam of 4 inches in diameter with a 1 inch smaller target marked centrally was attached to one end of the wooden ruler. The target was adjusted in such a way that, it was kept at 90% of the participant's arm length. A passive marker, in line with the target, was taped to the top-end of the screw of the small clamp at a fixed distance of 3.5 inches to serve as gaze fixation. Delsys® Trigno™ wireless electromyography sensors were used to measure the electrical activity of the muscle. These sensors also had tri-axial accelerometers embedded in them that sampled the rate of change of velocity. Reliability analyses for the response variables used in this study have been comprehensively detailed previously [19]. The surface EMG and acceleration data sampled from the wireless sensors in the above study to assess flexion- and abduction-stand-reaching task performance yielded intertrial and test-retest reliability for both performance-outcome and performance-production measures. Such sensitivity and reliability are critical for measuring ipsilesional UE function among individuals with chronic stroke and healthy older adults. A schematic representation of the reaching set-up used in the study is shown in Figure 1.
2.3. Protocol and Instrumentation
Participant's footprints were marked on a paper mat while they were standing with a shoulder-width base of support. For the duration of testing, they were asked to stand within the markings of their feet, to maintain a constant base of support. For flexion reaching, participants stood to face the target and, for abduction reaching, the participants stood to face sideways with their arms at their sides. The target was set by adjusting the ruler to 90% of the participant's maximum arm length, defined as the distance from the acromion to the tip of the middle finger. Following three familiarization trials, participants were provided with three trials each of verbally cued forward reaching through shoulder flexion and sideways reaching through shoulder abduction. Once ready, Delsys EMGworks® Software generated automated verbal cues consisting of a first cue (preparatory), “Get Ready,” given at 2 seconds, where participants had to focus their attention at the passive marker visually, followed with a second cue, “Go,” given at 4 seconds, at which participants were required to reach out and touch the target “as quickly and as accurately” as possible and return to the starting position. Participants were instructed to keep their back supported against the wall between trials. If participants missed the target or started to do the reaching task before the second verbal cue was provided, that trial was repeated to ensure the precise performance of the task. All participants received a mandatory break of 2-3 minutes after a set of three trials to avoid fatigue. EMG signals from a total of six trials per participant were collected from the involved anterior and middle deltoid (i.e., the anterior deltoid for the three flexion-reaching trials and the middle deltoid for the three abduction-reaching trials) and then the variables of interest were extracted and analyzed, using a customized MATLAB code.
Delsys® Trigno™ surface electromyography (EMG) sensors were used to record EMG activity in the ipsilesional UE (anterior deltoid for shoulder flexion and middle deltoid for shoulder abduction) of individuals with chronic stroke and age-similar healthy adults. The sensors were placed in line with the muscle belly using hypodermal tape [24]. EMG signals were sampled at 2,000 Hz and were then hardware band-pass filtered over a bandwidth of 20–450 Hz, applying a standard mode rejection ratio of > 80 db. The sensors enclosed tri-axial accelerometers that sampled signals at 148.1 Hz over a bandwidth of 50 Hz and amplitude range of ±1.5 g. The EMG data was smoothed via digital high-pass filtering using a fourth-order zero-lag Butterworth filter with a cut-off frequency of 20 Hz. This was followed by full-wave rectification and a low-pass filter with a cut-off frequency of 50 Hz. This EMG signal was then used to obtain onset and offset latencies for the muscles (defined below). The acceleration signals were smoothed using a fourth-order low-pass Butterworth filter with a cut-off frequency of 80 Hz, and this signal was used for computing onset and offset to calculate movement initiation time and movement time. EMG signals from a total of six trials per participant were analyzed from the involved prime mover (i.e., the anterior deltoid for the three flexion-reaching trials and the middle deltoid for the three abduction-reaching trials) using a customized MATLAB code to extract the variables of interest.
2.4. Outcome Measures
2.4.1. Primary Measures
Performance-Outcome Measures. Reaction time is the time needed to initiate a movement response following a visual, auditory, or other sensory signal and reflects the speed of transmission of the central nervous system. In this study, reaction time was the time interval calculated between the final auditory cue (“GO” at 4 s) and the initiation of EMG activity. Onset was identified as the time after four seconds when the EMG signal exceeded two standard deviations (SDs) of the ensemble average of the resting baseline EMG over one-second period (i.e., from 3 s to 4 s). Reaction time was measured in milliseconds (ms). Burst duration was defined as the total duration of EMG activity. It was calculated as the time elapsed between onset and offset latency of the EMG signal and was also measured in milliseconds. The offset was identified as the time point always occurring after the onset time point when the signal fell below 2 SDs of the initial resting baseline. Movement time was defined as the total time interval from the start to the end of upper extremity (UE) movement, taken as the interval between the onset (> 2 + SDs) and offset of acceleration (< -2 S. Ds). Movement initiation time is the time measured between the onset of EMG and the onset of acceleration signals, measured in milliseconds.
Performance-Production Measures. Peak acceleration was defined as the maximum amplitude of the acceleration signal along the X-axis of the sensor's coordinate system. We used the X-axis because the sensor was placed closer to the joint rather than to the endpoint of the shaft. Hence, movements occurred along the sagittal axis of the sensor. The unit of measurement was g, (1g = 9.8m/s/s). Raw and processed EMG signals and filtered acceleration trace from the anterior deltoid muscle during a single flexion-reaching trial demonstrate the performance-outcome measures, and performance-production measure is represented in Figure 2.
2.4.2. Secondary Measures
To assess health status post-stroke, the Stroke Impact Scale (SIS) was administered using an interview that measures changes in eight impairment categories (strength, hand function, activities of daily living, mobility, communication, emotion, memory, and thinking) [25]. Participants were administered a short cognitive ability test composed of six items on the Short Orientation-Memory-Concentration (SOMC) test. The SOMC test has been previously validated as a measure of cognitive impairment, assessing changes in verbal memory. The test is scored on a scale from 0 to 28, with scores above 20 being representative of ‘normal cognition', and scores below 10 being indicative of ‘normal-minimal impairment' [23]. Participants were also evaluated on the falls-efficacy scale, which uses a 10-item questionnaire to assess one's confidence in their ability to perform ten daily tasks without falling as an indicator of their fear of falling impacting physical performance. Lower scores indicate more confidence and higher scores entail a lack of confidence and greater fear of falling.
2.5. Statistical Analysis
From the 26 participants, one stroke survivor who volunteered to participate in the study was excluded from analyses because of technical problems during data collection and another was excluded from the analyses due to signal contamination (noise), probably resulting from loose sensor contact. All remaining data (78 trials-39 trials of flexion and 39 trials of abduction), was analyzed. A mixed model ANOVA was conducted with independent variables being, group (age-matched and stroke), task (flexion and abduction) and trials (1-3), and dependent variables being reaction time, burst duration, movement time, movement initiation time, and peak acceleration. Significant interactions were resolved with post hoc paired t-tests for within-subject factors and independent t-tests for between-subject factors. Independent t-tests were performed to compare SOMC test scores between the groups. Age, weight, and height were compared between the stroke survivors and age-similar healthy adults using independent t-tests. A chi-square test was used to compare the sex distributions of the groups. Spearman's correlation was run to assess the relationship between UE functional reaching reaction time (ms) and falls-efficacy scale score in chronic stroke survivors. To assess if limb dominance confounded differences in burst duration between the stroke and the healthy controls; an independent t-test was used to compare burst duration between participants with concordant (dominant UE is stroke affected) and discordant (non-dominant UE is affected) strokes. A significance level (α) of 0.05 was chosen for all statistical comparisons. Analyses were performed using version 17.0 of the commercially available Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS).
3. Results
Both groups were similar in demographics (age, weight, height, and sex) (Table 1). The mean score for SIS hand function was 33.41 (S.D ± 5.56), indicating moderate impairment among chronic stroke survivors [26]. Furthermore, both groups exhibited moderate cognitive ability with a mean score of 5.21 (S.D ± 2.13) on the SOMC test.
Table 1.
Subject | Sex M/F |
Age (year) |
Weight (kg) |
Height (cm) |
I Side (L/R) |
Concordant/ Discordant |
Type (H/I) |
Onset (year) |
SIS (UE) |
SIS (LE) |
SOMC | FES |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n = 13 | 5/8 | 7/6 | 8/5 | 6/7 | ||||||||
| ||||||||||||
Mean | 60.75 | 93.48 | 169.27 | 9.72 | 33.41 | 31.45 | 5.21 | 15.36 | ||||
| ||||||||||||
SD | 5.12 | 41.27 | 8.80 | 3.32 | 5.56 | 7.36 | 2.13 | 2.76 |
I Side = Involved Side, M = Male, F = Female, L = Left; R = Right; H = Hemorrhagic; I = Ischemic; M = Male; F = Female; BMI = Body Mass Index; Concordant stroke = Dominant hand is also stroke-affected hand or Discordant = Dominant hand is not stroke-affected), SIS = Stroke Impact Scale, UE = Upper Extremity, LE = Lower Extremity, SOMC = Short Orientation-Memory-Concentration Test of Cognitive Impairment, and FES = Falls-Efficacy Scale.
For reaction time, there was a significant main effect of task [F (1,48) = 2.003; p < 0.05] and group [F (1,24) = 14.555; p < 0.001], as well as a significant task × group interaction [F (1,48) = 1.492; p < 0.05]. However, there was no significant task × trial interaction [F (2,48) = 0.330; p =0.721]. There was also no significant task × group × trial interaction [F (2,48) = 0.081; p =0.922]. Post hoc analyses showed no significant difference in reaction time between flexion and abduction for the stroke group (p =0.695). However, reaction time for abduction was significantly greater than for flexion for the age-similar healthy adults (p < 0.05). There was also a significant increase in reaction time for the stroke group compared with the age-similar healthy controls for both flexion (p < 0.001) and abduction (p < 0.001) (Figure 3(a)).
For the burst duration, there was a significant main effect of task [F(1,48) = 19.127; (p < 0.001], and group [F(1,24) = 49.379; p < 0.001], as well as a significant task × group interaction [F(1,24) = 12.232; p < 0.001]. However, there was no significant task × trial interaction [F(2,48) = 2.022; p = 0.144]. Similarly, there was no significant task × group × trial interaction [F(2,48) = 1.670; p = 0.199]. Post hoc analyses showed no significant difference in burst duration between flexion and abduction for the stroke group (p = 0.87); however burst duration was significantly higher for abduction than for flexion (p < 0.001) for the age-similar healthy adults. Burst duration was also significantly greater for the stroke group compared to the age-similar healthy adults group for both flexion (p < 0.001) and abduction (p < 0.05) (Figure 3(b)).
For movement time, there was a significant main effect of task [F(1,48) = 159.543; p < 0.001], and there was a significant task × group interaction [F(1,48) =8.110; p < 0.05]. However, there was no significant task × trial interaction [F(2,48) = 1.076; p =.349)] and task × group × trial interaction [F(2,48) = 1.771; p = 0.181)]. Post hoc analyses exhibited no significant difference in movement time between flexion and abduction for the stroke group (p = 0.972) or for the age-similar healthy adults group (p = 0.167). However, the age-similar healthy adults group did demonstrate significantly decreased movement time compared to the stroke group for both flexion (p < 0.05) and abduction (p < 0.05) (Figure 3(c)).
For movement initiation time, there was no significant main effect of task [F(1,48) = 0.208; p = 0.652]; however, there was a main effect of group [F(1,24) = 14.844; p < 0.001]. There was no significant task × group interaction [F(1,48) = 1.426; p = 0.244)]. Similarly, there was no significant task × trial interaction [F(2,48) = 3.067; p = 0.056)] and task × group × trial interaction [F(2,48) = 0.714; p =0.495)]. Post hoc analysis showed significantly decreased movement initiation time in the age-similar healthy adults group compared to the stroke group for both flexion (p < 0.05) and abduction (p < 0.05) (Figure 3(d)).
For peak acceleration, there was no significant main effect of task [F(1,48) = 1.004; p = 0.326]; however, there was a main effect of group [F(1,24) = 1643.784; p < 0.001]. There was additionally no significant task × group interaction [F(1,48) = 0.262; p = 0.613], task × trial interaction [F(2,48) = 0.382; p = 0.685] and task × group × trial interaction [F(2,48) = 0.873; p = 0.424]. The stroke group also exhibited reduced peak acceleration in comparison to age-similar healthy adults for both flexion (p < 0.05) and abduction (p < 0.05) (Figure 4).
Correlation analysis demonstrated a significant positive correlation between UE reaching reactions and Falls-self-efficacy scores (rs = 72, p < 0.001). For flexion, burst duration was longer for participants whose ipsilesional arm was non-dominant (n = 8) than for participants whose ipsilesional UE was dominant (n = 5) for flexion; however, this difference was not statistically significant.
4. Discussion
Recognizing the impact of stroke on the ipsilesional upper extremity is an essential step for implementing effective rehabilitation. The purpose of this study was to quantify ipsilesional upper extremity reaching performance among chronic stroke survivors using a reliable stand-reaching paradigm simulating real-life functional reaching that has associated postural demands. This would then be used to compare them to age-similar healthy adults. Our findings indicate that chronic stroke survivors exhibited more severe deficits in movement control compared to age-similar adults, seen by significantly poorer performance-outcome and performance-production measures during the performance of a stand reaching task.
Individuals with stroke exhibited increased reaction time compared with age-similar healthy adults. Although the functional, multiplanar stand-reaching task has not yet been evaluated, other studies in stroke survivors also showed a disproportional delay in reaction time of the ipsilesional upper extremity in comparison to the control group [27]. Such increased response time in hemiparetic chronic stroke survivors has been indicated to be among the leading predictors for a decline in movement initiation, motor function performance, and functional outcomes [28, 29]. Further, the study found that reaction time was strongly correlated with falls self-efficacy. The significant associations between these variables could suggest that initiation of upper extremity motor response may play an essential role in the initiation and control of quick, protective grasping arm raising responses required to counter destabilization. Previous evidence has demonstrated that delayed choice reaction time on a lower extremity stepping task could significantly predict fall-risk among older adults [30]. More importantly, evidence has suggested that upper extremity finger-press choice reaction time has been used to distinguish between fallers and non-fallers [31].
The longer burst duration seen among stroke survivors in comparison to their counterparts may be attributed to their reduced ability to produce optimal levels of neuromuscular activation [32]. With these longer burst durations, it has been shown that ipsilesional deficits tend to be more profound when present on the nondominant side [10]. In line with previous research, this study also indicated increased burst duration when the ipsilesional side was the nondominant side (ipsilesional side as nondominant n = 8, ipsilesional side as the dominant n = 5). The delay in reaction time and the prolonged burst duration seen in the ipsilesional upper extremity could, therefore, be due to the lack of control mediated through the small percentage of descending cortical tracts that originate from the lesioned site in the dominant hemisphere [33].
Another very interesting finding from the current study was that the age-similar healthy adults presented with increased reaction time and burst duration for abduction, rather than for flexion, reaching tasks. Such differences could have resulted from the comparatively increased use of flexion-reaching rather than abduction-reaching during activities of daily living in age-similar healthy adults [34]. However, the above scenario of increased reaction time and burst duration for abduction compared to flexion-reaching tasks was not seen among the individuals with stroke. It could be postulated that the loss of shoulder joint flexibility contributed to the reduction of both flexion- and abduction-stand reaching task performance after stroke. Based on the current study results it is recommended that rehabilitation of ipsilesional upper extremity should include multidimensional (flexion and abduction) movement control training. However, more in-depth research is needed to understand and compare movement control for both flexion- and abduction- reaching tasks.
Similar to the results seen in the current study, previous work has demonstrated increased movement time of the ipsilesional upper-extremity in adults with unilateral brain damage during tasks such as Fitts' task [35], single target tapping [36], and sequencing hand postures [37]. Studies have suggested that this slowing of tasks could be due to the task requirement of rapid alternating changes in direction, difficulties in sequencing, and prolonged dwell time (i.e., time on target) for adults with chronic stroke [38, 39]. Visuospatial integration has also been shown to be associated with the high endpoint precision required for goal-directed task performance [40]. Furthermore, it has been demonstrated that poststroke individuals exhibited abnormal interactions between sensory systems (i.e., somatosensory, visual, and vestibular afferents), as well as impaired dynamic regulation of the integrated signals (sensory reweighing) of the ipsilesional UE [41]. Thus, this study, in line with others, adds to the research literature showing that the organization and execution of multisegment ipsilesional upper extremity movements are disrupted among chronic stroke survivors, ultimately resulting in slower task performance.
Prior studies using electromyography in stroke survivors have also demonstrated a significant delay in movement initiation time, similar to that seen in this study. Studies have indicated that such delay in simple functional tasks of the ipsilesional upper extremity can be associated with lesions causing specific impairments in motor processing and descending efferent mechanisms [38]. In general, the posterior parietal cortex and premotor areas mediate motor processing, while the primary motor and premotor areas mediate selection of motor strategy and motor execution [42]. Nonetheless, studies have indicated that the eventual motor output among stroke survivors is modulated by changes in descending and propriospinal excitatory and inhibitory inputs into the spinal interneurons and alpha motoneurons [43]. However, the degree to which this difference was due to impairment in motor processing rather than the impairment of efferent mechanisms would need further investigation.
Compared to age-similar nondisabled healthy adults, chronic stroke survivors demonstrated lower peak acceleration for both flexion- and abduction-reaching tasks. These findings are consistent with previous literature supporting the presence of kinematic deficits in movement velocity and acceleration, on the ipsilesional side [3]. These deficits have been postulated to indicate poor anticipatory planning, an essential component of motor skills [44, 45]. Such deficits in movement planning could be attributed to the disruption of ipsilateral contributions from the affected hemisphere. Bilateral corticospinal contributions to each arm are crucial for economizing the speed of control processes [46]. Specifically, ipsilateral corticospinal processing time is slower than crossed contralateral tracts and may be especially important for early movement planning (inhibitory influence of the ipsilateral motor cortex on responses to stimulation of the human cortex and pyramidal tract). Similarly, transcallosal exchanges of information are slower than ipsilateral hemisphere processing alone [47, 48]. Thus, the lack of ipsilateral control after a stroke can have a significant impact on the speed of movement and hence can impact the amplitude of peak acceleration. Alternatively, lower peak accelerations could have been due to impaired regulation of force and firing pattern of single motor units on the ipsilesional upper extremity [49, 50]. Although there is a lack of concrete evidence to support this hypothesis, it serves as a credible avenue for further research.
Several possible mechanisms could account for the ipsilesional motor deficits among chronic stroke survivors. A dominant theory suggests that the ipsilateral uncrossed descending corticospinal pathways may play an important role in the movement control of the ipsilesional upper extremity [46, 48]. A stroke event causes injury to the uncrossed corticospinal system [51] thus impairing such supplementary ipsilateral control. An alternate hypothesis is that ipsilesional movement control depends on complex interhemispheric communication between cortical (i.e., dorsal premotor cortex, supplementary motor area) areas likely mediated through the corpus callosum for the interhemispheric transfer of perceptual, sensory, and motor information underlying complex and integrated behaviors [42].
While it is well known that the ipsilesional upper extremity in people with chronic stroke exhibits significant motor deficits which lead to reduced accuracy and efficiency of movements [5, 36], the majority of evidence in this area is from seated upper extremity movement tasks. In the current study, we have focused on extending this evidence to include reaching while standing. Our findings concur with the previous studies and additionally allow us to conclude that ipsilesional deficits persist even in standing. This is particularly of note because depending upon the stroke severity the ipsilesional upper extremity may play a dominant role in maintaining or restoring stability control during full-body tasks, such as walking [52]. To best understand the role of the upper extremity standing balance and falls, the study adopted a reaching task that imposed similar demands on response programming as would be evident during standing [16, 53]. This is crucial because, as indicated earlier, upper extremity responses a change-in-support reactions during standing appear to be important for maintaining standing balance and recovering from perturbations [54, 55].
The findings from this study add to existing literature by relating ipsilesional motor deficits of upper extremity to falls self-efficacy among stroke survivors. Future research is required to investigate the characteristics of the ipsilesional upper extremity during recovery from a fall. The current research does, however, suggest that the implementation of training paradigms to improve motor control in the ipsilesional UE may be crucial for the rehabilitation of chronic stroke survivors.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank all the researchers and students at the Cognitive, Motor, and Balance Rehabilitation Laboratory (CogMoBal) for their support during the data collection and analysis. They are grateful to Alison Schenone for her work in editing the final draft of this manuscript. They would also like to thank the Society of Neuroscience, October 2015, for accepting and having them present the initial work of this study at their conference.
Abbreviations
- UE:
Upper extremity
- ADL:
Activities of daily living
- PASE:
Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly
- SOMC:
Short-Orientation-Memory-Concentration
- EMG:
Electromyography
- RT:
Reaction time
- MT:
Movement time
- MIT:
Movement initiation time
- BD:
Burst duration
- PA:
Peak acceleration
- N:
Number
- DS:
Dominant side
- AS:
Affected side
- L:
Left
- R:
Right
- H:
Hemorrhagic
- I:
Ischemic
- M:
Male
- F:
Female
- BMI:
Body mass index
- FES:
Falls-Efficacy Scale
- LE:
Lower extremity
- SIS:
Stroke Impact Scale.
Data Availability
The data used to support the findings of this study are included within the article.
Additional Points
Highlights. (i) Ipsilesional UE stand reaching is critical to understand anticipatory feed-forward mechanisms. (ii) Poststroke individuals exhibited deficits in movement control during a stand reaching task. (iii) Interventions should include ipsilesional UE training to improve motor control and prevent falls.
Conflicts of Interest
There are no conflicts of interest.
References
- 1.Morris J. H., van Wijck F., Joice S., Ogston S. A., Cole I., MacWalter R. S. A comparison of bilateral and unilateral upper-limb task training in early poststroke rehabilitation: A randomized controlled trial. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2008;89(7):1237–1245. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.11.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Stinear C. Prediction of recovery of motor function after stroke. The Lancet Neurology. 2010;9(12):1228–1232. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(10)70247-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Schaefer S. Y., Haaland K. Y., Sainburg R. L. Ipsilesional motor deficits following stroke reflect hemispheric specializations for movement control. Brain. 2007;130(8):2146–2158. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kwon Y.-H., Kim C. S., Jang S. H. Ipsi-lesional motor deficits in hemiparetic patients with stroke. NeuroRehabilitation. 2007;22(4):279–286. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Son S. M., Kwon Y. H., Lee N. K., Nam S. H., Kim K. Deficits of movement accuracy and proprioceptive sense in the ipsi-lesional upper limb of patients with hemiparetic stroke. Journal of Physical Therapy Science. 2013;25(5):567–569. doi: 10.1589/jpts.25.5_567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Wetter S., Poole J. L., Haaland K. Y. Functional implications of ipsilesional motor deficits after unilateral stroke. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2005;86(4):776–781. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Carey J. R., Baxter T. L., Di Fabio R. P. Tracking control in the nonparetic hand of subjects with stroke. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 1998;79(4):435–441. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(98)90146-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Debaere F., Van Assche D., Kiekens C., Verschueren S. M. P., Swinnen S. P. Coordination of upper and lower limb segments: Deficits on the ipsilesional side after unilateral stroke. Experimental Brain Research. 2001;141(4):519–529. doi: 10.1007/s002210100891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Semrau J. A., Herter T. M., Kenzie J. M., Findlater S. E., Scott S. H., Dukelow S. P. Robotic Characterization of Ipsilesional Motor Function in Subacute Stroke. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 2017;31(6):571–582. doi: 10.1177/1545968317704903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Sainburg R. L. Handedness: Differential specializations for control of trajectory and position. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews. 2005;33(4):206–213. doi: 10.1097/00003677-200510000-00010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Maki B. E., Holliday P. J., Topper A. K. A prospective study of postural balance and risk of falling in an ambulatory and independent elderly population. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 1994;49(2):M72–M84. doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.2.m72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Culham E., Peat M. Functional anatomy of the shoulder complex. Journal of Orthopaedic & Sports Physical Therapy. 1993;18(1):342–350. doi: 10.2519/jospt.1993.18.1.342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Chern J.-S., Lo C.-Y., Wu C.-Y., Chen C.-L., Yang S., Tang F.-T. Dynamic postural control during trunk bending and reaching in healthy adults and stroke patients. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation. 2010;89(3):186–197. doi: 10.1097/phm.0b013e3181c56287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Chern J.-S., Yang S.-W., Wu C.-Y. Whole-body reaching as a measure of dynamic balance in patients with stroke. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation. 2006;85(3):201–208. doi: 10.1097/01.phm.0000200357.98448.d7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kim S. W., Shim J. K., Zatsiorsky V. M., Latash M. L. Anticipatory adjustments of multi-finger synergies in preparation for self-triggered perturbations. Experimental Brain Research. 2006;174(4):604–612. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0505-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.McIlroy W. E., Maki B. E. Early activation of arm muscles follows external perturbation of upright stance. Neuroscience Letters. 1995;184(3):177–180. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(94)11200-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sherif M. H., Gregor R. J., Lyman J. Comments on "upper extremity limb function discrimination using emg signal analysis". IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering. 1984;BME-31(5):p. 440. doi: 10.1109/TBME.1984.325284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.van Dijk H., Jannink M. J. A., Hermens H. J. Effect of augmented feedback on motor function of the affected upper extremity in rehabilitation patients: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine. 2005;37(4):202–211. doi: 10.1080/16501970510030165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Varghese R., Hui-Chan C. W. Y., Wang E., Bhatt T. Internal consistency and test-retest reliability of an instrumented functional reaching task using wireless electromyographic sensors. Journal of Electromyography & Kinesiology. 2014;24(5):593–600. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2014.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Zachry T., Wulf G., Mercer J., Bezodis N. Increased movement accuracy and reduced EMG activity as the result of adopting an external focus of attention. Brain Research Bulletin. 2005;67(4):304–309. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.06.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Quinn T. J., Dawson J., Walters M. R., Lees K. R. Reliability of the modified rankin scale: A systematic review. Stroke. 2009;40(10):3393–3395. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.557256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Washburn R. A., Smith K. W., Jette A. M., Janney C. A. The physical activity scale for the elderly (PASE): Development and evaluation. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 1993;46(2):153–162. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(93)90053-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wade D. T., Vergis E. The short orientation-memory-concentration test: a study of its reliability and validity. Clinical Rehabilitation. 1999;13(2):164–170. doi: 10.1191/026921599673848768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.De Luca C. J., Gilmore L. D., Kuznetsov M., Roy S. H. Filtering the surface EMG signal: movement artifact and baseline noise contamination. Journal of Biomechanics. 2010;43(8):1573–1579. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2010.01.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Carod-Artal F. J., Coral L. F., Trizotto D. S., Moreira C. M. The stroke impact scale 3.0: evaluation of acceptability, reliability, and validity of the brazilian version. Stroke. 2008;39(9):2477–2484. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.513671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Duncan P., Lai S., Bode R., Perera S., DeRosa J. Stroke Impact Scale-16: A brief assessment of physical function. Neurology. 2003;60(2):291–296. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000041493.65665.D6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kusoffsky A., Apel I., Hirschfeld H. Reaching-lifting-placing task during standing after stroke: Coordination among ground forces, ankle muscle activity, and hand movement. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2001;82(5):650–660. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2001.22611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Heller A., Wade D. T., Wood V. A., Sunderland A., Hewer R. L., Ward E. Arm function after stroke: Measurement and recovery over the first three months. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 1987;50(6):714–719. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.50.6.714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Smutok M. A., Grafman J., Salazar A. M., Sweeney J. K., Jonas B. S., DiRocco P. J. Effects of unilateral brain damage on contralateral and ipsilateral upper extremity function in hemiplegia. Physical Therapy in Sport. 1989;69(3):195–203. doi: 10.1093/ptj/69.3.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Pijnappels M., Delbaere K., Sturnieks D. L., Lord S. R. The association between choice stepping reaction time and falls in older adults—a path analysis model. Age and Ageing. 2010;39(1):99–104. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afp200.afp200 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Woolley S. M., Czaja S. J., Drury C. G. An assessment of falls in elderly men and women. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 1997;52A(2):M80–M87. doi: 10.1093/gerona/52A.2.M80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Klass M., Baudry S., Duchateau J. Voluntary activation during maximal contraction with advancing age: A brief review. European Journal of Applied Physiology. 2007;100(5):543–551. doi: 10.1007/s00421-006-0205-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Dewald J. P., Beer R. F. Abnormal joint torque patterns in the paretic upper limb of subjects with hemiparesis. Muscle & Nerve. 2001;24(2):273–283. doi: 10.1002/1097-4598(200102)24:2<273::AID-MUS130>3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Duncan P. W., Weiner D. K., Chandler J., Studenski S. Functional reach: A new clinical measure of balance. Journal of Gerontology. 1990;45(6):M192–M197. doi: 10.1093/geronj/45.6.M192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Giuliani C., Purser J. L., Light K. E., Genova P. A. Impairments in arm control in subjects with left and right hemisphere stroke. NeuroRehabilitation. 9(1):71–87. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8135(97)00015-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kwon Y. H., Kim C. S., Jang S. H. Ipsi-lesional motor deficits in hemiparetic patients with stroke. Neurorehabilitation. 2007;22(4):279–286. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Carmon A. Sequenced motor performance in patients with unilateral cerebral lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(4):445–449. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90009-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Chae J., Yang G., Park B. K., Labatia I. Delay in initiation and termination of muscle contraction, motor impairment, and physical disability in upper limb hemiparesis. Muscle & Nerve. 2002;25(4):568–575. doi: 10.1002/mus.10061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Pohl P. S., Luchies C. W., Stoker-Yates J., Duncan P. W. Upper extremity control in adults post stroke with mild residual impairment. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 2000;14(1):33–41. doi: 10.1177/154596830001400104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Jeannerod M. Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage. 2001;14(1):103–109. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Lima N. M., Menegatti K. C., Yu É., et al. Sensory deficits in ipsilesional upper-extremity in chronic stroke patients. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria. 2015;73(10):834–839. doi: 10.1590/0004-282X20150128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Beaule V., Tremblay S., Théoret H. Interhemispheric control of unilateral movement. Neural Plasticity. 2012;2012:11. doi: 10.1155/2012/627816.627816 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Noskin O., Krakauer J. W., Lazar R. M., et al. Ipsilateral motor dysfunction from unilateral stroke: Implications for the functional neuroanatomy of hemiparesis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 2008;79(4):401–406. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.118463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Stewart J. C., Gordon J., Winstein C. J. Planning and adjustments for the control of reach extent in a virtual environment. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation. 2013;10(1):p. 27. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-10-27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Tan C., Tretriluxana J., Pitsch E., Runnarong N., Winstein C. J. Anticipatory planning of functional reach-to-grasp: A pilot study. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 2012;26(8):957–967. doi: 10.1177/1545968312437938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ziemann U., Ishii K., Borgheresi A., et al. Dissociation of the pathways mediating ipsilateral and contralateral motor-evoked potentials in human hand and arm muscles. The Journal of Physiology. 1999;518(3):895–906. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.0895p.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Grefkes C., Fink G. R. Reorganization of cerebral networks after stroke: New insights from neuroimaging with connectivity approaches. Brain. 2011;134(5):1264–1276. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ward N. S. Mechanisms underlying recovery of motor function after stroke. Postgraduate Medical Journal. 2005;81(958):510–514. doi: 10.1136/pgmj.2004.030809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Haaland K. Y., Schaefer S. Y., Knight R. T., et al. Ipsilesional trajectory control is related to contralesional arm paralysis after left hemisphere damage. Experimental Brain Research. 2009;196(2):195–204. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1836-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Nowak D. A., Grefkes C., Dafotakis M., Küst J., Karbe H., Fink G. R. Dexterity is impaired at both hands following unilateral subcortical middle cerebral artery stroke. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;25(10):3173–3184. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05551.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Stinear C. M., Barber P. A., Smale P. R., Coxon J. P., Fleming M. K., Byblow W. D. Functional potential in chronic stroke patients depends on corticospinal tract integrity. Brain. 2007;130(1):170–180. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Bourke T. C., Coderre A. M., Bagg S. D., Dukelow S. P., Norman K. E., Scott S. H. Impaired corrective responses to postural perturbations of the arm in individuals with subacute stroke. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation. 2015;12(1):p. 7. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-12-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hyndman D., Ashburn A., Stack E. Fall events among people with stroke living in the community: circumstances of falls and characteristics of fallers. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2002;83(2):165–170. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2002.28030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Pijnappels M., Kingma I., Wezenberg D., Reurink G., Van Dieën J. H. Armed against falls: The contribution of arm movements to balance recovery after tripping. Experimental Brain Research. 2010;201(4):689–699. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2088-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Roos P. E., McGuigan M. P., Kerwin D. G., Trewartha G. The role of arm movement in early trip recovery in younger and older adults. Gait & Posture. 2008;27(2):352–356. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data used to support the findings of this study are included within the article.