Skip to main content
Frontiers in Neurology logoLink to Frontiers in Neurology
. 2019 Mar 7;10:63. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00063

Vestibular and Multi-Sensory Influences Upon Self-Motion Perception and the Consequences for Human Behavior

Zelie Britton 1, Qadeer Arshad 1,*
PMCID: PMC6416181  PMID: 30899238

Abstract

In this manuscript, we comprehensively review both the human and animal literature regarding vestibular and multi-sensory contributions to self-motion perception. This covers the anatomical basis and how and where the signals are processed at all levels from the peripheral vestibular system to the brainstem and cerebellum and finally to the cortex. Further, we consider how and where these vestibular signals are integrated with other sensory cues to facilitate self-motion perception. We conclude by demonstrating the wide-ranging influences of the vestibular system and self-motion perception upon behavior, namely eye movement, postural control, and spatial awareness as well as new discoveries that such perception can impact upon numerical cognition, human affect, and bodily self-consciousness.

Keywords: self-motion perception, vestibular system, cerebellum, cortex, behavior

Introduction

Despite the vestibular system being evolutionarily ancient (1), it has long been overlooked as a primary sensory organ, notably by Flourens who, whilst identifying that pigeons with peripheral vestibular lesions suffered from imbalance, concluded that the semi-circular canals were involved in generating motor responses for head and eye movements (2). The inner ear itself was first recorded in the 1,500 s by Andreas Vesalius and Gabriele Fallopio, reviewed by Weist (3). Initial research into the mechanics behind how acceleration can be detected took place in the 1870s by three independent scientists: Josef Breuer, a Viennese doctor, Ernst Mach, a professor of physics and Alexander Crum Brown, who worked as a chemist having received degrees in medicine and chemistry. They identified the semi-circular canals as the organs for motion sensation, suggested relative inertial motion of endolymph to the bony skull as the method of transduction, and observed that the semi-circular canals and otoliths might work in combination to differentiate between linear motion and tilt, and whose work forms the basis of our current understanding, well-reviewed by Weist and Baloh (4) and Weist (3). The vestibular system is found in different forms across the animal kingdom and is reviewed by Lowenstein (5) and Beisel et al. (6).

The paired vestibular organs consist of three semi-circular canals and two otoliths, which together sense rotational and linear accelerations and are responsible for maintaining both stable vision during head movements [via the vestibular-ocular reflex (VOR)] and a stable posture (via vestibular-spinal reflexes). Furthermore, they also contribute to an awareness of our movement in space as demonstrated by the ability of a subject to report passively applied movements whilst seated in a rotating chair in darkness. In everyday life, vestibular stimuli are integrated with visual, somatosensory, auditory, and motor efference inputs to derive estimates of self-motion. Perhaps the reason for the omission of vestibular perception from the traditional human senses is that, compared with the perceptual times for other senses, vestibular perceptual awareness is relatively slow (70–160 ms) and less sensitive (7, 8). Accordingly, during daily life we are often unaware of workings of the vestibular system until it fails. Patients with vestibular disorders suffer not only from difficulties with balance but also report head-movement induced oscillopsia and difficulties during complex behaviors such as self-motion perception and navigation (9). This review will examine the role of the vestibular system in the perception of self-motion and explore how self-motion perception can modulate other behaviors.

Sensing Motion

The vestibular organs are our motion detectors and consist of the otoliths and the semi-circular canals. These detect changes in velocity via stimulation of the hair cells which contain cilia projections from their apical surface. The cilia are named according to their length: the longest being the kinocilium, the others, the stereocilia. Even in the absence of any stimulation, they exhibit a low level of tonic activity (1012). Hair cells depolarise when the stereocilia deflect toward the kinocilium and hyperpolarise when the deflection of the stereocilia is directed away from the kinocilium (1315). Depolarisation leads to release of neurotransmitters onto first-order vestibular neurons. Such deflections occur due to the relative inertia of the endolymph in the semi-circular canals into which the cilia project: when the head accelerates, the lag of this fluid deflects the cilia. The hair cells also receive efferent synapses which can modulate the activity of the hair cells (16). The utricle and saccule are the two otoliths and detect linear accelerations in the axial and coronal planes, respectively. Their hair cells project into a gelatinous layer which is covered with calcium carbonate crystals. The anterior, posterior, and horizontal canals work in pairs to sense rotations in the sagittal (pitch), coronal (roll), and transverse (yaw) planes, with an increase in impulse discharge during ipsilateral rotation and a reduction seen during contralateral rotation (11) [it might be added here that the semi-circular canals have also been shown to respond to tilting and linear acceleration, albeit with a much greater threshold (17)]. Two distinct types of afferent neurons, categorized by the regularity of their resting activity spike pattern, carry signals from the hair cells to the vestibular nuclei (12). For canal afferents, regular fibers, which have smaller axon diameters, are thought to predominantly transmit information about head motion over time whereas the irregular fibers are more sensitive to motion, exhibiting higher gain (18, 19). Both fibers respond similarly to active and passive head motion (20). Otolith afferents are similarly formed of regularly- and irregularly-firing neurons (21).

The vestibulo-ocular Reflex

The vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) serves to stabilize visual input on the retina during short, fast head movements by driving the eyes with a velocity of equal magnitude and in opposite direction to the head movement. It was first described by Andreas Hoegyes who demonstrated that each semi-circular canal was connected to the appropriate extra-ocular muscle (22). The canal afferents, having tonic discharge that is modulated according to the direction of rotation (10, 11), work in pairs, such that stimulation of one side occurs whilst the other side is inhibited (23). Similar mechanisms exist for translational head movements, which result in a linear VOR (24, 25). The VOR is fittingly fast, operating with latencies of 5–6 ms (26), which is in keeping with the short three-neuron pathway involved: the primary afferent neuron of the vestibular nerve, an interneuron, and a motor neuron to the corresponding extra-ocular muscle (2729). The functional importance of which was first recognized by Lorente de No, who discovered that feedback pathways within the neuronal arc are involved in the VOR, a concept extended by Raphan et al. in their description of the velocity storage mechanism (30, 31). Furthermore, the VOR is sensitive, and can respond, to changes in the relationship between vestibular signals and the visual field: wearing magnifying lenses leads to adaptive increases in VOR gain whilst left-right reversing Dove prisms lead to adaptive decreases in the gain of the VOR (3234). The mechanism for these adaptations appears to be via long-term depression in the cerebellar flocculus (35, 36). [Note that removal of the vestibulocerebellum does not abolish the VOR (34)]. Following unilateral vestibular loss, there is an impressive recovery of the VOR, revealing the importance of multimodal input integration, in particular, proprioceptive, and motor efferent inputs (37, 38). Nystagmus arises when there is slow, continuous movement of the head, with the slow, vestibular, component in the opposite direction of the motion, and a fast, “catch-up” saccade in the same direction [note that nystagmus can, of course, arise in other circumstances, namely: physiological nystagmus (optokinetic and end-point); infantile nystagmus and pathologic nystagmus, reviewed by Abadi (39)]. Early work carried out by Lorente de No established the importance of the role of the reticular nuclei in these reflexes: in rabbits with lesions of the raphe nuclei of the pons and the medulla oblongata, thus severing the axons of the reticular nuclei, the fast component of nystagmus disappeared (40, 41).

What Is Self-Motion Perception and How Does the Vestibular System and Associated Central Processing Give Rise to it?

Perception of Angular Motion

Perception of passive self-rotation can be quantified in terms of the minimum (or threshold) rotation required for perceptual awareness and by a subject's estimates of angular velocity and/or displacement. Vestibular perceptual thresholds are dependent upon the axis of rotation, with thresholds for whole-body rotations about the vertical axis (yaw) being significantly lower than those for roll and pitch (42). Additionally, perceptual thresholds improve as the frequency of sinusoidal rotation increases up to 0.2 Hz, and plateau beyond 0.5 Hz, findings that suggest that vestibular signals undergo high-pass filtering (see Figure 1) (43). Vestibular perception thresholds for yaw rotations in young healthy subjects are significantly greater at 1.18 deg/s2 compared with the angular acceleration required for nystagmus (0.51 deg/s2) (44).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

From section Perception of angular motion. This figure, modified from Grabherr et al. (43). Graph showing velocity thresholds as a function of sinusoidal motion frequency, where velocity is the peak velocity achieved in each cycle of sinusoidal acceleration. Black squares represent mean data from Grabherr et al. (43), n = 7. Left and right pointing triangles from Benson et al. (42), n = 6 and n = 8, respectively. Solid black line represents the fitted model for the high-pass filter KTS/(TS + 1).

Eye movements can also be used to indicate perceived rotation by implementing a paradigm in which participants are either asked to hold a given direction of gaze in the dark during angular rotation (which requires both the vestibular-ocular reflex and a compensatory saccade), or are instructed to make a saccade back to a previously seen visual target after having been rotated in the dark (vestibular memory-contingent saccade) (45). Subjects perform marginally better in the latter, possibly secondary to interference between the VOR and the rotation being estimated during the former task (46). As with threshold perceptions, yaw rotations yield the best accuracy (46). Labyrinthine-defective patients are unable to produce any structured response as would be expected of a task designed to test vestibular perception (47).

The ability to estimate and reproduce rotational displacement is another method to probe vestibular perception. Metcalfe and Bronstein examined the ability of patients with labyrinthine disease and healthy controls to re-orientate themselves using a self-controlled motorized Barany chair after passive displacement in the dark in a “go-back-to-start” paradigm (48). Controls demonstrated high accuracy with low degrees of variation (5–15° for 30–180° displacements). Patients with acute unilateral vestibular failure (within a month of symptom onset) demonstrated an inability to accurately perceive rotations in either direction, consistently underestimating magnitude of displacement toward the lesion and exhibiting highly variable responses to rotation in the opposite direction. The study followed the patients up for several months, by which time there was partial restoration of perception and the symmetry of the responses had been restored, suggesting compensatory central mechanisms.

More recently, Panichi et al. sat subjects in a head-fixed rotating chair in darkness and asked them to fixate on the location of a previously seen target (presented straight ahead prior to rotation) (49). The chair rotated in an asymmetric sinusoidal pattern, with a fast component in one direction and a slow, restoring component in the opposite direction, an arrangement previously shown to selectively bias central vestibular perceptual processing (50). They found that patients with acute vestibular neuritis have a large deficit in vestibular perception during conditions in which the slow-phase acceleration was toward the lesioned side and that whilst this improved over the 1-year follow-up period, it did not return to normal. Notably, this asymmetry of self-motion perception correlated with patients' dizziness handicap inventory score. It has been well-documented that clinical outcomes in patients with chronic dizziness correlate poorly with low-level brainstem reflexes (i.e., VOR) (5153) and much better with cortical processes including visual dependence and anxiety and depression (54, 55). These observations provide support for the theory that there exist different central mechanisms for compensation of VOR and vestibulo-perceptual responses, with the latter higher-level processes affording better predication of prognosis following vestibular dysfunction.

Perception of Linear Motion

Linear accelerations are sensed by the otolith organs, and the double integral of their signal can be used to estimate passive linear displacement in the absence of other sensory inputs (56, 57). In their study, Israel et al. found that whilst subjects were unable to spontaneously produce a passive linear displacement of two meters when blindfolded, they were able to reproduce the distance traveled, peak velocity, and velocity profile following passive displacement and that, in this paradigm, reproduction of parameters relating to velocity appeared to have been processed independently of the reproduction of displacement (56). Regarding vestibular perception of linear motion, lateral movements have lower thresholds than anterior-posterior movements: in one study using a sinusoidal stimulus of frequency 1 Hz, thresholds for accelerations were 6.5 cm/s2 and 8.5 cm/2 for lateral and anterior-posterior movements, respectively, whilst thresholds for velocity were 10.4 and 13.5 cm/s (58). Vertical linear movements have a perceptual threshold greater than that for lateral movements but less than that for anterior posterior motion (59). Using single acceleration steps, Gianna et al. found acceleration thresholds of 4.84 cm/ss and velocity thresholds of 7.93 cm/s for lateral movements (60). Other movement profiles with linear and parabolic ramping of the acceleration resulted in higher thresholds, thereby supporting the view that large acceleration gradients facilitate perception (see Figure 2). The study also examined the thresholds of patients with impaired vestibular function. Although their average thresholds were worse than healthy controls, there was overlap between the two groups, suggesting that somatosensory cues were also used in the task. A recent paper examined the effects of a central (vestibular migraine) and peripheral (Menière's disease) vestibular dysfunction on linear motion perception, finding that perceptual thresholds were higher for patients with Menière's disease but not significantly different for vestibular migraine patients compared with controls (59). These findings contradict recent findings of abnormal tilt thresholds in vestibular migraine patients (61).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

From section Perception of linear motion. This figure, modified from Gianna et al. (60). (A) Motion profiles for acceleration steps and corresponding rate of change of acceleration and velocity. (B) Acceleration thresholds for normal subjects (Ns) (mean +/– standard deviation) and individual subjects with vestibular impairment in the different conditions: step accelerations, low linear ramp (SlowR), high linear ramp (FastR), and parabolic acceleration (Par).

Perception of Heading

The ability to estimate one's direction of translation is termed heading perception and the vestibular system plays an important role in this process. For example, in macaques heading discrimination thresholds in the dark increase 10-fold after bilateral labyrinthectomy (62). Regarding the relative contribution of the visual and vestibular systems in heading estimation tasks, it appears that when subjects are asked to point out the heading direction, the visual system enables more precise determination (63), but when asked to perform a discrimination task (forced choice of two), thresholds are similar for the two senses (62). Interestingly, body position relative to gravity can modify vestibular heading perception, but visual heading perception is unaffected by changes in body position (64).

Calculating Self-Motion Relative to the World

Signals generated by the vestibular system create an egocentric reference of self-motion: to be useful for guiding our movements and behavior relative to the external world, a transformation to an earth-referenced frame of self-motion is required. To create an earth-referenced model of self-motion, two difficulties need to be overcome. Firstly, the signal from the semi-circular canals does not vary with the attitude of the head in space, for example, a raw rotation generates the same signals at the level of the hair cells whether the subject is upright or supine. Secondly, the otoliths alone cannot distinguish linear acceleration from head tilt relative to gravity (65). These difficulties can be overcome through integration of vestibular signals with additional inputs including visual and proprioceptive stimuli (reviewed below). However, even when undergoing passive motion in a dark room, a solution can theoretically be computed by combining information from the canals and the otoliths. To resolve these problems, it has long been hypothesized that the brain calculates an estimate of the attitude of the head relative to gravity using multisensory inputs, including canal signals, a value that can then be used to resolve the above issues (66). In monkeys undergoing passive movements, some cerebellar nodulus/uvula Purkinjie cells respond preferentially to translation (or rather, to the vector perpendicular to gravity) (67) whereas others respond to tilt (68). These neurons project to the vestibular nuclei and the fastigial nucleus, and from there to the thalamus, which also demonstrates varying degrees of separation of movement types relative to gravity (69, 70). Modeling work suggests a similar mechanism exists in humans (71).

Differentiating Between Actively-Generated and Passively-Applied Motion

An unaddressed question is how we differentiate active vs. passive motion. In life, we experience a combination of actively-generated and passively-applied motion. Yet the relative movement of the endolymph, and the subsequent deflection of the hair cells, is identical during both active and passive movements of a given profile and acceleration. The question arises as to how changes in sensory signals due to external variables (exafference) vs. those resulting from our own actions (reafference) can be distinguished. An in-built mechanism would be to use a copy of any motor commands against which to compare sensory stimuli: this exists in the form of discharge corollaries, also known as motor efference copies. Subtraction of the actual sensory signal from the predicted sensory result of an action theoretically leaves the signal from any additional passive motion.

As noted previously, in alert primates, semi-circular canal afferents respond identically to actively- and passively-generated head movements (20). In contrast, vestibular nuclei neurons show differential activation to passive and active head movements, with reduced responsiveness to vestibular afferents during actively-generated movements (7274). In an experiment designed to probe the mechanism for such modulation of vestibular neurons responsiveness, Roy et al. compared the activity of medial vestibular nuclei neurons during a range of tasks including: passive whole-body rotations; active head movements; passive body rotations, controlled by the monkey using a steering wheel to drive a turntable, with an earth-fixed head (to activate neck proprioceptors); and, head restrained monkeys actively trying to turn their heads (motor commands but no corresponding proprioceptive signals) (75). Only during the actively generated head movements did the authors observe a reduction in vestibular nuclei neuron responsiveness to vestibular afferent signals. Furthermore, in the paradigm where the monkeys were attempting but unable to move their heads (i.e., there was muscular activation but not a corresponding change in muscle length and joint movement), there was minimal modulation of vestibular nuclei neuron responsiveness. Taken together, these observations suggest that motor efference copies and not proprioceptive signals nor prior knowledge of the movement that lead to suppression of vestibular neuron responses during actively generated movements, and that only when the motor efference copy matches the proprioceptive input does reafference occur. As a neural correlate of this, during active self-motion, neurons in the fastigial nucleus continually compare predicted and actual sensory stimuli (76) and respond only to unexpected self-motion (77). Neurons in the posterior parietal cortex also exhibit a differential response to active and passive movements, although the responses of individual neurons to different types of movement here is more complicated than that observed for the vestibular nuclei neurons, perhaps reflective of more complicated cortical processing (78).

Prolonging Self-Motion Perception: the Velocity Storage Mechanism

The use of the relative motion of the endolymph to the bony canals as an indicator of head motion works well for short, fast head movements. However, with prolonged head movements, friction reduces the relative motion of the endolymph, leading to a decay in the signal generated. When the head is rotating at constant velocity, the signal from the semi-circular canals falls to 1/e of its maximum after 3–7 s: i.e., the time constant of the canals lies between 3 and 7 s (79, 80). However, it is conceivable that it might be physiologically disadvantageous for vestibular reflexes and perception to exhibit a similar decay curve, and indeed measured time constants for the VOR and vestibular perception are on the order of three to four times greater than that of the canals (30, 81). The network responsible for prolonging the time constant of the VOR and perception and thus sustaining behavioral responses beyond the time when the endolymph has ceased to move relative to the head is called the velocity storage mechanism. It can be modeled as a leaky integrator with reversal of the sign of the signal and works as a form of imperfect positive feedback on the canal signal to the nuclei (30). The integrator is leaky to prevent inappropriate propagation of noise. The velocity storage network is thought to reside in the cerebellum. Whilst there has been some debate as to whether the VOR and vestibular perception use the same velocity storage network, most work now supports the theory that they share the mechanism. In healthy subjects, there were no differences in perceived rotational velocity and the slow-phase response of the VOR after suddenly stopping yaw and pitch rotations in the dark (82); time constants of the VOR and perceived rotation co-varied in patients with chronic vestibulo-cerebellar degeneration and healthy controls (83); and, when measuring post-rotational nystagmus and perceived rotation using a hand-driven wheel connected to a tachometer, intra-subject group decay time constants for the two variables were the same for healthy controls (16 s), and patients with congenital nystagmus (7 s) (see Figure 3) (84). It is worth noting at this point that the velocity storage network is not the only mechanism responsible for prolonged self-motion perception: the visual system also plays an important role. In general, initial, short-latency responses to self-motion are generated by the vestibular system, whilst responses of greater duration and latency are produced predominantly from visual flow inputs (85).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

From section Prolonging self-motion perception: the velocity storage mechanism. This figure, from Okada et al. (84). (A) Eye velocity and vestibular sensation averaged across subjects (normal, n = 31, and congenital nystagmus, n = 14) after suddenly stopping whole-body passive rotations in the dark. (B) Mean duration, time constant and area under the curve of turning sensation in subjects with congenital nystagmus and healthy controls.

Cognitive Cueing

Cognitive, top-down influences are important for many neural processes: self-motion perception is no different. When subjects were asked to imagine themselves rotating in a chair prior to actual rotation, when the imagined and real rotations were in the same direction, vestibular perceptual thresholds were lower and, interestingly, so were thresholds for the VOR (86). Conversely, the ability to generate and manipulate mental images itself relies upon an intact vestibular system: subjects with vestibular impairment performed worse than healthy controls in object-based mental transformations (87). Furthermore, vestibular stimulation can facilitate mental transformations (88), with improved performance during congruent inertial motion (89). Such cognitive cueing is also evident during traditional passive linear self-motion tasks: with sufficient acceleration, one might expect subjects to experience tilt due to the somatogravic illusion. However, this is generally not reported by participants. Wertheim et al found that when subjects have prior knowledge that they will be accelerated from rest during an experiment, they do not report tilt, but that up to 50% of participants report tilt when they have no prior knowledge, suggesting that the sensation of tilt is suppressed in the former group (90).

So Where Is Self-Motion Perception Processed?

Traditionally, perception was thought to be the preserve of the cortex, with sensory inputs passing first through the thalamus and then to a unimodal area of primary cortex before reaching higher association areas to be combined with other sensory inputs. However, this view is changing: more recent findings suggest that multisensory processes occur in primary sensory cortices and recognize the role of non-cortical areas (91). No specific unimodal vestibular cortical area has been identified; rather, cortical neurons that are modulated by vestibular stimuli also respond to visual, proprioceptive, and motor efference inputs. Therefore, the perception of self-motion is believed to be processed by a network of different structures and regions, centered on the lateral fissure and the parieto-insular “vestibular” cortex and including the vestibular nuclei, cerebellum and other cortical areas, a theory that is supported by the multiple areas found to be involved in self-motion perception in animal and human studies. Having a distributed network is of evolutionary benefit as it reduces the risk that a focal brain lesion leads to a significant defect in self-motion perception. Figure 4 summarizes the main components of this vestibular network.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

From section So where is self-motion perception processed? Diagram summarizing the main vestibular projections and brain regions contributing to self-motion perception. Dashed arrows indicate integration with extra-vestibular inputs.

The Vestibular Neurons and Their Projections

The vestibular fibers, whose cell bodies are found in Scarpa's ganglion, run to the four principal vestibular nuclei in the dorsolateral pons and medulla and directly innervate the posterior cerebellum, as well as projecting to other central structures (92). These nuclei are also interconnected. Many second order vestibular neurons receive convergent inputs from otolith and canal afferents, thus providing a mechanism for early integration of the two signals (9395). For vestibular-only neurons, so-called as they respond only to change in head attitude and not to eye movement, this appears to occur physiologically in the form of sub-additive integration, with canal afferents more heavily weighted at lower frequencies and otoliths at higher frequencies (72). As an aside, this may be the basis for a correlate seen in human psychophysical experiments, in which perception of combined passive linear and rotational motion cannot be predicted as the simple sum of the two components (96). Vestibular neurons at this level are also modulated by visual and proprioceptive stimuli and from central, top-down inputs (97). The nuclei project to the spinal cord via the lateral vestibulospinal tract and descending medial longitudinal fasciculus; to the autonomic nervous system; to the extra-ocular nuclei via the ascending medial longitudinal fasciculus; to the cerebellum; and, to the thalamus from where there are connections to the cortex (92). A study of patients with acute posterolateral thalamus lesions using positron emission tomography during caloric vestibular stimulation (CVS), demonstrated reduced vestibular temporo-parietal cortex activation on the side ipsilateral to the lesion but did not find any significant effect on motion perception (98).

Cerebellar Contributions to Self-Motion Perception

Recently, the role of the cerebellum has been recognized as extending beyond the traditional confines of motor control of the eye movements and posture to include sensory discrimination and self-motion perception (99, 100). Anatomically, the nodulus/ uvula and fastigial nucleus of the cerebellum receive significant input from the vestibular system: a smaller contribution is of primary afferent fibers projecting to the ipsilateral uvula and nodulus and a larger proportion of secondary fibers from the vestibular nuclei (101). The vestibular nuclei are reciprocally innervated by the cerebellum. As described above, in monkeys the nodulus/ uvula appear to be important in generation of a world-referenced frame of self-motion (67, 68), whereas the fastigial nucleus generates signals of unexpected self-motion by comparing motor efference signals and actual proprioceptive feedback from movement (76, 77).

In humans, psychophysical studies on subjects with cerebellar degeneration have yielded informative observations: patients with midline cerebellar lesions, when rotated in the dark, showed impairment in multiple parameters compared with healthy controls, including the duration of self-motion perception and the perceptual time-constant (see Figure 5) (102), findings replicated in a subsequent study of patients with chronic degeneration of the vestibulo-cerebellum (83). A further study investigated the vestibular perceptual thresholds of two patients with cerebellar agenesis, finding them to be globally elevated, particularly for movements which only activated the otoliths (103). These observations support the view that the cerebellum has a role in extracting information about self-motion from multiple signals generated by both self and passive movements, and from background noise.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

From section Cerebellar contributions to self-motion perception. This Figure, from Bronstein et al. (102). (A) Graphs of representative individuals for perceived angular velocity after suddenly stopping rotations in the dark. (B) Median duration, time constant and area under the curve for sensation and eye velocity in patients with midline cerebellar degeneration (n = 8) and healthy controls (n = 8).

Cortical Processing of Self-Motion Perception

From the brainstem and cerebellum, vestibular inputs pass through the thalamus, predominantly via the main somatosensory nucleus, the ventroposterior complex, to the cortex. At the thalamus, it has been proposed that information flows in two channels, one encoding head motion, the other body motion (104). Many vestibular-sensitive neurons are already multi-sensory, being modulated by visual, proprioceptive and motor efference signals (105). There are two major cortical areas implicated in the processing of vestibular information for the perception of self-motion: the ventral intraparietal area (VIP) and the parieto-insular vestibular cortex (PIVC). A third, the medial superior temporal area (MST), is critical for visual motion perception but also receives vestibular inputs. The supplementary eye fields appear to be important in the control of eye movements during self-motion: the neurons here are modulated by vestibular stimuli (106); and, patients with lesions of the supplementary eye field exhibited worse accuracy during a vestibular memory-contingent saccade (107, 108).

Neurons in the MST respond predominantly to visual stimuli, and in particular to visual motion stimuli. Several studies have confirmed the importance of this area in visual heading perception (109112). It has also been shown that a subset of MST neurons are modulated by passive whole-body translations [a response that is not seen following bilateral labyrinthectomy (62)] albeit with smaller and less directionally selective responses than for optic flow stimuli. Combining vestibular stimulation with congruent and incongruent optic flows varied the amplitude and direction-selectivity of these neurons (113). These findings were since extended to show that the response of MST neurons to inertial motion cues was correlated with heading discrimination (62) and that visual and vestibular cues were summed in neurons with congruent heading preferences (114). However, it remains likely that vestibular cues only have a minor influence over the MST area, a theory that is supported by the finding that inactivating MST using muscimol, a GABAA agonist, had little effect on vestibular heading thresholds, but did impair visual thresholds (111). Humans with MST lesions display great difficulty in navigation and have impaired visual motion perception (115).

VIP neurons respond reliably to vestibular, visual and somatosensory inputs (116, 117) and receive inputs from MST (118, 119). Compared with MST, VIP neurons are modulated to a greater extent by vestibular stimuli and show greater correlation with perceptual decisions (120). Subgroups of neurons show preferential response to different types of inertial motion, a characteristic that is invariant with respect to head attitude and gaze direction. It is worth noting that responses to active motion are generally smaller than those to passive motion (116), thus raising the question as to the role the VIP cortex plays in distinguishing active vs. passive motions and coordinating appropriate behavioral responses.

Around two-thirds of PIVC neurons respond to vestibular stimulation (121). Vestibular-responsive neurons are more strongly activated by semi-circular canal inputs compared with those from the otoliths. Of those neurons modulated by canal signals, there were subgroups which preferentially encoded rotations in a specific plane. As noted above, the PIVC is multisensory, and neurons there also respond to somatosensory and (particularly large-field) visual stimuli (122, 123). In primates, lesions of the PIVC led to impaired heading perception (124). In humans, whilst direct stimulation of this cortical area during craniotomies led to a range of vestibular sensations including movement of the world and vertigo (125), cerebral infarctions affecting the PIVC are reported to cause an impairment in subjective vertical (126). Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have revealed activation of the PIVC and posterior insular cortex during caloric vestibular and direct galvanic stimulation (127129). During this series of experiments, some conditions required subjects to keep their eyes closed, whilst in others they viewed random movement or a fixation cross. The posterior insular cortex responded strongly to visual motion, whereas in the PIVC there was a trend for visual motion to reduce activity (130). The role of the posterior parietal cortex in self-motion perception is demonstrated in a series of psychophysical experiments. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to this region impairs performance on a whole-body displacement task that required angular path integration based only on vestibular inputs (131). The same stimulation applied to the right posterior parietal cortex also worsened a motion-reproduction task (i.e., one not requiring path integration) when applied during the motion reproduction, although it had no effect when applied during the initial rotation (encoding phase) (132).

The role of the posterior parietal cortex in top-down vestibular perception (as defined by vestibular perceptual thresholds) has also been established: transcranial direct current over the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) alters vestibulo-perceptual and VOR thresholds (133, 134). Nigmatullina et al. found that in ballerinas, who are typically trained to perform multiple pirouettes, display remarkable vestibular adaptation, compared with rowers, who are physically active but not trained to tolerate numerous rotations, there is differential white matter volume in the TPJ bilaterally (135). Lesion studies demonstrate a split in processing dependent upon the nature of the task: subjects with parieto-occipital lesions perform well in “low level” tasks such as discriminating the direction of moving stimuli, but poorly when asked to judge heading direction; the reverse is true for patients with occipital lesions (136). Further studies support this theory of split cortical processing for parallel channels carrying different information about motion (137, 138).

Lateralisation of Vestibular Cortical Processing

Vestibular stimulation activates both cerebral cortices, but it is recognized that there is a right hemisphere dominance in right-handed individuals, and an even stronger left-sided bias in left-handed subjects (139). Although this asymmetry has been shown to lead to differential effects on both vestibular low-level reflex behaviors including the VOR (140) and on vestibular-sensitive cognitive processes [for example, as recently proposed, anxiety (141)], the effects, if any, on self-motion perception remained to be fully explored. As discussed above, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to the posterior parietal cortex can differentially impair perceived whole-body angular displacement, with worse performance when the right hemisphere is stimulated (affecting leftward rotations) compared with left hemisphere rotations (132). Future work could explore whether control of self-motion perception is more commonly the result of asymmetric or symmetric cortical activity.

Integration of Vestibular and Non-vestibular Cues in Self-Motion Perception

Whilst the vestibular system clearly plays an important role in self-motion perception, it is far from the only system that can provide such information. This is important because:

  1. The human body is not rigid and can move with several degrees of freedom, as explored in a novel paradigm establishing relationships in movement between different body parts (142). Thus, the vestibular system alone is insufficient to provide a complete representation of self-motion.

  2. Using one sensory input alone to perceive self-motion would leave one vulnerable to illusions and false interpretations (for example, the somatogravic illusion in which aviators, deprived of adequate visual stimuli, sense linear forward acceleration as a backwards tilt of the head, potentially leading to the dangerous situation of pitching the nose of their aircraft) and to loss of information (constant velocity is not encoded by vestibular system).

  3. When using one input, accuracy is worse than can be achieved by integrating multiple inputs.

  4. Comparison of sensory inputs with motor efference copies enables discrimination between self-generated and passive motion, as described above.

The vestibular system is unusual in that it receives early input from multiple other systems including visual, somatosensory and motor efference signals [reviewed here (97)]. These multisensory inputs enable refinement of self-motion estimates and thereby attune behavioral responses (143). In general, multisensory processing is a skill which improves over time, and self-motion is no different. Supporting this view are recent findings that demonstrate older adults are able to improve their performance in a driving task by a greater margin than younger adults when additional vestibular cues were added to the visual stimulus (144).

Integration of Visual and Vestibular Inputs

The visual system contributes to self-motion perception, with optic flow-induced perception being highly accurate and precise. Studies in primates reveal that they rely predominantly on the visual system for navigation in three-dimensional space (145). Visual-vestibular interactions occur as early as the vestibular nuclei, although this is mostly seen in neurons involved in the VOR, not in vestibular-only neurons, and thus such interactions are unlikely to be involved in self-motion perception (146, 147). Indeed, Bryan and Angelaki show that VOR neurons in the vestibular and deep cerebellar nuclei cease to respond to optic flow once the OKN was suppressed (by requiring the animals to fixate on a head-fixed target) (147). At higher levels, visual-vestibular input is integrated in cortical areas traditionally associated with visual processing, including the MST and VIP areas. In these regions, there are neurons that respond both to motion in darkness and to optic flow, and the former response is abolished following bilateral labyrinthectomy (62, 148, 149). This may be the neural substrate to explain how combined visual-vestibular stimuli improves self-motion perception compared with either stimulus alone.

When examining the relative contributions of the visual and vestibular systems to self-motion perception, it has become clear that they vary depending upon the experimental conditions rather than having some pre-defined weighting. In a study in which subjects experienced linear acceleration, visual cues enabled more precise determination of heading than vestibular cues (150), whereas in a separate experiment, in subjects undergoing roll rotations, vestibular perception was better at frequencies of sinusoidal motion >2 Hz and visual perception better at frequencies < 1 Hz (151). Kolev et al. rotated supine subjects about the earth-vertical axis, i.e., they underwent roll without otolith stimulation (152). Whilst it is unsurprising that coherent, simultaneous visual-vestibular signals improve perceptual thresholds, the authors found that even conflicting visual-vestibular signals, generated when the subjects fixated on a visual target that rotated with them, yield lower perceptual thresholds than seen with vestibular-alone stimulation (152). The study also demonstrated a frequency-dependence of perceptual thresholds. However, experiments designed to probe the sensation of self-motion as induced by moving visual fields reported visual dominance despite the presence of conflicting vestibular stimulation. That is, in subjects undergoing yaw rotations whilst watching visual fields that were rotating in the same direction but at different velocities, the reported magnitude of self-motion appeared to relate to that of the incongruent visual stimuli (153). In a similar setup, subjects reported self-motion perception in the opposite direction to actual whole-body rotation during prolonged periods of yaw rotation during which the visual field rotating in phase and in the same direction as the vestibular rotation (154). These apparent discrepancies are likely to reflect dynamic reweighting of visual and vestibular cues under different conditions, perhaps reflecting the unlikelihood that information from the visual system is incorrect in daily life.

There is additional cross-talk between the two systems beyond the mere computing self-motion: perceptual learning, as measured by an improvement in vestibular motion discrimination thresholds performed in the dark, occurs when training rotations occur in the light, but not when subjects are blindfolded during training (155).

Integration of Proprioceptive, Somatosensory, and Vestibular Inputs

Proprioception is another important sensory input used alongside vestibular signals to calculate self-motion. Anatomically, integration of the proprioceptive and vestibular systems occurs directly (dorsal root axons innervate vestibular nuclei) and indirectly (via second order neurons and via the cerebellum) [reviewed in (156)]. Functionally, vestibular-only neurons are modulated by passive neck rotations in squirrel monkeys (157) and in cynomolgus monkeys (158), leading to reduced neuronal activity during head only motion compared with whole-body motion. The latter study found that during passive vestibular and proprioceptive stimulation signals underwent linear summation, but that sub-additive integration occurred during active head movements and during gaze shifts. Such differential processing under different experimental conditions might explain the apparently conflicting finding that during passive movements of the head of the rhesus monkey no modulation of vestibular-only neurons was seen (74). The authors hypothesized that this might be a reflection of the arboreal habitats of squirrel and cynomolgus monkeys compared with the predominantly ground-dwelling rhesus monkey. Proprioceptive-vestibular interactions are also documented in the thalamus, primary somatosensory cortex and ventral intraparietal region (159, 160).

The role of the proprioceptive system in self-motion perception in humans is well-established. During a remembered-target task, subjects performed better when there were combined vestibular and neck proprioceptive inputs compared with the vestibular-alone condition (161). In a similar setup, reducing the stimulus amplitude reduced gain in the vestibular-only condition, but not in proprioceptive conditions (47); the study also found that detection of head turns was predominantly determined by somatosensory inputs (47); and that proprioceptive afferents can reliably encode head on body rotations even when there is no vestibular stimulation (162).

This relationship between the vestibular and somatosensory systems is to some degree reciprocal. Vestibular activation improves sensitivity to tactile stimuli (153, 163167), possibly via a non-linear mechanism that is only in effect once a certain threshold of self-motion perception has been achieved, and this occurs independently to changes in attention (168). Furthermore, vestibular activation can transiently reverse hemianesthesia secondary to brain lesions, possibly due to altered neuronal dynamics in the putamen, insula and secondary somatosensory cortex (169, 170).

Optimal Integration

As discussed above, the precision of self-motion perception is greater when more than one sensory input is used. Recently there has been interest into the way in which multi-sensory cues deriving from a common cause are integrated. Across sensory systems, including the vestibular system, data from experiments appear to suggest that inputs are integrated in a Bayesian optimal way, i.e., the weight of each cue is proportional to its reliability (1/variance) (171). Regarding self-motion perception, visual and vestibular cues appear to be optimally integrated during heading discrimination and rotational movements (151, 172174). Furthermore, the brain can dynamically change the relative weights of cues to reflect changing conditions (173) and can even integrate conflicting sensory cues in a statistically optimal way to minimize variance (172, 175). Recordings from multi-sensory neurons in the dorsal medial superior temporal area point to its role in visuo-vestibular cue integration, with evidence of near-optimal processing (114, 176).

Aftereffects: Evidence for Shared Hardware to Process Different Stimuli

A method to probe to what degree the same neuronal networks are used to process information from different sensory inputs is to examine cross-modal aftereffects. Aftereffects are the sensations that occur following cessation of the initial stimulus. In the case of motion perception, they typically occur in the opposite direction, thereby shifting perception of subsequent stimuli. For example, in the waterfall illusion, after watching the water drop down toward earth for some time, stationary rocks and trees appear to drift upwards (177, 178). Cross-modal aftereffects refer to sensations that occur in a different modality to the initial stimulus and are thought to represent recalibration rather than a fatigue-induced process as is evidenced by the lack of aftereffects to visual stimuli when they are presented with an appropriate vestibular stimulus (179). When the lights are extinguished following prolonged exposure to a rotating drum, subjects experience self-motion in the opposite direction, an effect which is accompanied by an “after-nystagmus” (180, 181). If the stimulus is not sufficiently long, no aftereffects are experienced: whilst exposure to optic flow inducing a sensation of linear self-motion for 15 s resulted in a shift in perception, shorter durations of up to 7 s had no such effect, even though the onset of vection had occurred by this time (182, 183).

Vection: An Illusion of Self-Motion

First described in the nineteenth century by Mach (184) and Wood (185), vection is the false perception of self-motion induced by sufficiently large stimuli moving across the retina in the absence of any true acceleration as signaled by the vestibular system. Today, vection is perhaps most recognizably experienced whilst sitting as a rail passenger, looking out of the window and believing that one's own carriage is leaving early, only to realize that it is rather a train on an adjacent platform that is pulling away. The illusion is widely exploited in virtual reality, theme park rides and I-Max cinemas, but it also remains of interest in neuroscience as it reveals details of the relationship of different sensory inputs in the generation of self-motion perception (vection is not solely generated by visual inputs: proprioceptive and auditory stimuli have also been shown to provoke the illusion (186, 187).

Vection is typically experienced several seconds after the onset of the stimulus. For visually-generated vection, there are some general precepts that have been established, with the following all increasing the credibility of the illusion: greater velocity up to a point, previously suggested as 120 degrees/s for a rotating stimulus (188); larger stimulus size (188190); increased density of moving objects (190); and, circular and curvilinear rather than linear motion (191). Furthermore, Brandt et al. established that it is predominantly the peripheral vision that is responsible for vection: whilst masking the central visual field with black disks, diameters of up to 120° exerted minimal effect on the generation of vection, but when blocking the peripheral vision, central visual stimuli of up to 30° diameter fail to induce self-motion perception; and, when the central and peripheral visual stimuli are of equivalent area, it is the peripheral stimulus that dominates (188). And the perception is remarkably compelling: Brandt et al also demonstrated that subjects still experienced circular vection when the rotating stimulus accelerated at 15°/s2 (188).

During vection there is a conflict between incoming visual, somatosensory and vestibular information, with corresponding deactivations in the PIVC during rotational vection (192, 193). In contrast, during linear vection functional magnetic resonance imaging found only activations in various cortical areas with no PIVC or any other cortical deactivations (194). A further study attempted to correlate the intensity and duration of vection with brain activity in different regions. Whilst no correlation was found with PIVC, enhanced activity of the cerebellar vermis and parieto-occipital areas amongst others was reported (195). The authors concluded that this might represent a “dorsal stream” responsible for the intensity of vection. As might be expected given the hypothesized role of the cerebellar nodulus, there is increased activity during periods of reported self-motion illusion compared with object motion (193). Experimentally, subjects with bilateral impaired vestibular function report vection sooner, for longer and more compellingly than healthy controls (196, 197). Such a process of reciprocal inhibition might be explained physiologically as a consequence of a system of flexible dominant sensory weights given to incoming signals which enables self-motion perception during periods of incongruent information (for example, after prolonged rotation when the relative motion of the endolymph has ceased).

As with many perceptions, visual vection can be modulated by the presence or absence of additional inputs. Whilst proprioceptive stimuli alone do not reliably induce vection in all subjects, they can enhance vection. For example when small vibrations are applied to the subjects' seat at the time of onset of visual stimuli (198) or during auditory self-motion illusions (199). Proprioceptive stimuli can also enhance vection induced by auditory stimuli (200) and even static leaning of the upper body can enhance vection (201). In addition, the role of top-down processing and expectation should not be underestimated. It is common practice to “prime” subjects by demonstrating that actual self-motion is possible, even if it will not occur. Work in children demonstrated that linear vection is felt earlier is when a chair is placed on rollers compared with directly on the ground (202).

How Vestibular Functioning and Self-Motion Perception can Modulate Behavior

Perception of self-motion is critically important for many human behaviors, including heading and navigation and control of body and eye movements. Therefore, it is not surprising that self-motion perception should modulate such behaviors. This section will review the effects of the vestibular system and self-motion perception upon eye movement, postural control and spatial awareness and more abstract behaviors including numerical cognition, human affect and bodily self-consciousness.

The Relationship Between Self-Motion Perception and Visually-Induced Postural Responses

Lee and Lishmann (203) demonstrated that visual information is important for the control of stance, and visual motion stimuli can induce postural sway (visually-evoked postural response, VEPR). The VEPR is known to be influenced by stimulus size and displacement across the retina (204) and it would appear logical that information containing cues regarding self- vs. object-motion would also modulate sway. Using transient movements of a visual scene to induce a postural response, Guerraz et al. (205) showed that sway was reduced when the subject could control some aspect of the stimulus motion compared with the uncontrolled condition. Moreover, in an oscillating room paradigm, when participants are aware that there is object-motion rather than self-motion, not only do they sway less than subjects who are unaware, they also do not show any change in sway as this distance between them and the wall increases (206). The authors also observe that the variability within each subject group was the same, and concluded that the prior information leads to a reweighting of different sensory cues in the control of posture. In further experiments, (static) subjects viewed a horizontally-translating background with either a head-mounted or earth-fixed LED at the center of a luminescent window frame (207). In these scenarios, the direction of postural sway depends upon the nature of the foreground, being in the direction of the background motion for the head-fixed display and transiently reversed in the earth-fixed case, whilst vection only occurred in one direction (opposite to that of the background motion). Subjects experienced vection sooner and for longer in the head-fixed condition. As vection is delayed compared with the VEPR, and as it is unidirectional compared with the bidirectional VEPR, it is likely that the two are processed differently. However, when subjects were experiencing self-motion there was significantly greater sway in both conditions as measured by displacement at C7 level, an effect that preceded vection onset (as indicated by pushing a button) by ~1 s. The authors argued that there may be a dual system at work, similar to that governing the eyes and reviewed here (85), in which as short-latency, brief VEPR (responsive to parallax) is subsequently replaced by a longer-latency visuo-postural response that can be enhanced by vection and might control posture during prolonged body displacements.

Self-Motion and the Detection of Movement

Whilst freely walking, one perceives the world to be stationary despite its projection moving across one's retina, and additionally, moving objects are perceived as moving, the result of subtracting expected inputs from actual inuts, discussed above and reviewed by DeAmgelis and Angelaki (208). The thresholds at which object motion can be detected, as well as the reaction times for such visual perception, are, however, increased during self-motion as compared with when the subject is stationary (209). Conversely, the threshold for vestibular perception are increased when subjects simultaneously view a moving visual pattern (189). Furthermore, when viewing a bistable rotating Necker cube, participants perceived the cube to be rotating in a congruent direction with their own passive whole-body rotation (210), and when viewing a bistable plaid, in which the observer perceives either two gratings moving across each other, or a single percept moving coherently, self-motion modified the dominance of each percept such that when self-motion and the global coherent percept were in opposite directions, the dominant percept was of a coherent image, and when self- and global percept- motion were orthogonal, subjects were more likely to view the image as two gratings moving independently (congruent motion had no effect) (211). The authors of the latter study suggest that this occurs as a result of an interaction between the visual motion and self-motion vectors at the stage of motion integration.

Self-Motion Perception and Spatial Awareness

Spatial representation within the brain has been the focus of much research over the last 70 years, and the vestibular system plays an important role in tracking and updating one's location in space reviewed by Moser et al. (212, 213) and Fyhn et al. (214). It might be noted here that this role is not limited to space as defined by visual inputs: the construct of auditory space is also dependent upon self-motion and it was recognized in 1940 that, despite movement of the head, human subjects can perceive a stable auditory environment and use it to accurately localize sounds (215). More recently, experiments have demonstrated that auditory space can be distorted by passive and active self-motion, with constructed space shrinking during forward acceleration (i.e., subjects indicate that sounds are located as being physically further away from them during periods of forwards acceleration compared with the stationary scenario, a phenomenon that has a dose-dependent relationship) (216, 217). For the purposes of this review, we will focus on the role of the vestibular system in: the perception of vertical, the modulation of visuospatial attention, with particular reference to patients experiencing visuospatial neglect, and visuospatial memory and navigation.

The Perception of Vertical and Vestibular Dysfunction

Verticality can be perceived through via visual, somatosensory and vestibular cues, and it follows that such perception can be affected by vestibular dysfunction. Following peripheral vestibular lesions, humans tilt their head, and shift their center of mass toward the side of the lesion (218, 219). Vestibular lesions have dissociative upon the perception of verticality dependent upon the experimental paradigm: whilst the subjective visual vertical was strongly deviated toward the side of the lesion in patients, the subjective seated postural vertical was not significantly different between the patient and control groups (220) [perception of the static visual vertical typically returns around 1 year after the insult (221, 222)]. The deviation in visual vertical is likely explained by altered inputs from the otoliths, leading to an altered representation of the gravitational vector and disturbance of the subjective visual vertical (223), and indeed, patients with bilateral peripheral vestibular dysfunction have been observed to have normal subjective visual vertical (224). This latter patient group also have a preserved postural vertical, although the sensitivity of this is reduced in patients who have a fluctuating (as opposed to stable) abnormality in vestibular dysfunction (225), suggesting that proprioceptive and somatosensory inputs are important in this perception, with the vestibular system refining the estimate for verticality. Subjective visual vertical can be improved, although not normalized, by the presence of visual cues for horizontal and vertical, for example as are found in an ordinary room (226). In the same experiment, Borel et al. found that the postural tilt toward the size of the lesion was reversed in the condition when visual cues were provided. Estimates for visual vertical also improve when subjects are balancing in a precarious position, for example on a beam, leading to the hypothesis of the “dynamics of balance,” that is, that we have a heightened awareness of our orientation the more unbalanced we are (227). These findings are reviewed by Lopez et al. (228), who propose that the changes seen in relation to patients' perception of verticality following peripheral vestibular lesions are adaptive and might be explained in terms of changing the frame of reference (gravitationally-, egocentrically- or allocentrically-orientated) and of higher postural constraints. The neural substrate underpinning such reference frames is suggested to be a distributed neural network including the premotor cortex, premotor cortex, inferior parietal lobule, posterior parietal cortex, insula, and the temporo-parietal junction (228).

The Effect of Self-Motion Perception on Gaze Direction and Optokinetic Nystagmus

During self-motion perception, as compared with visual field motion without self-motion perception, there is a shift of the mean gaze direction toward the incoming visual stimulus, which reverts when the perceptual state reverts to object motion (229, 230). The change in mean gaze direction may be viewed as a shift in visuospatial attention during times of perceived self-motion (230). It is worth noting that this shift in gaze toward the incoming visual stimulus is seen when subjects are instructed to passively stare at the rotating stripes [in contrast, subjects actively pursuing the visual stimulus undergo a shift of mean gaze in the direction the stimulus is moving toward (231)] and thus the subtleties of human behavior modulation, reflecting underlying perceptual strategies, are revealed.

Thilo et al. (230) also found that shifts in perceptual state were also linked to changes in the slow-phase gain of optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), with self-motion associated with reduced gain, possibly as a result of conflict between the need to accurately pursue the visual stimulus (moving in one direction) and the drive of the eye toward the incoming visual field (in the other direction). Additionally, slow-phase gains were generally decreased when the subjects were supine compared with upright (all subjects viewing the same stimulus rotating about their longitudinal axis). This is the opposite finding to earlier work that showed enhanced slow-phase gains in the supine position compared with the upright position when the optokinetic stimulus was rotating about the subjects' naso-occipital axis, generating a torsional OKN (232). The authors postulate that this differential response is a result of the presence or absence of conflict between information arising from the otoliths and the visual system: in times of conflict, the otoliths may exert an inhibitory influence on the OKN.

Visuospatial Neglect and the Vestibular System

Neglect is the clinical phenomenon whereby patients fail to respond to, report or orient toward stimuli on the contralesional side (233). It can be multimodal and includes visuospatial, auditory, and somatosensory neglect. In one study, Bisiach and Luzzatti found the neglect can even affect internal visualization: in patients asked to recall the Piazza del Duomo in Milan, when imagining the scene with their backs to the cathedral, they were observed to omit places on the left side of the scene, places that they subsequently named without prompt when asked to imagine the same scene from the other side of the piazza, facing the cathedral (234). Neuroanatomically, neglect is particularly associated with lesions of the right posterior parietal cortex, including the TPJ (235). Perhaps it is not so surprising then that stimulation of the vestibular system, which is intricately linked with the TPJ, can modulate neglect. First reported in 1941, left-cold and right-warm CVS temporarily alleviates left visuospatial neglect, an effect which appears to be related to shift of spatial attention to the left and facilitation of left lateral gaze (236238), and functional MRI during left-cold stimulation does demonstrate activation of the right hemisphere (239). Galvanic vestibular stimulation appears to have a similar effect, with right-cathodal stimulation most effectively improving line bisection error in patients with neglect (240). These findings have been extended by work demonstrating that optokinetic stimuli can also improve performance in behavioral tests of neglect (241243), improvements that have been reported to last for up to 2–4 weeks after treatment (244, 245). However, whilst performance on behavioral tasks might improve following optokinetic stimulation in patients with neglect, there is evidence to suggest that such stimulation does not correct the suspected underlying asymmetry of spatial representation in the brain. Leftwards optokinetic stimulation improved performance in line bisection, but accentuated the leftward bias that patients had when asked to construct a line of known length on the basis of a given “midpoint” (246).

Vestibular Dysfunction, Visuospatial Memory and Navigation

That the vestibular system might play a role in spatial memory is suggested by neuro-anatomical studies which demonstrate connections between various vestibular centers and the hippocampus, where so-called place cells are found, (9, 247) and supported by functional MR imaging during CVS (248). And whilst it is evident that the vestibular system is responsible for simple navigation tasks in the absence of other cues (for example, estimation of passive rotational and linear displacements in silence and in dark), it has only been more recently demonstrated that such impairments extend to more complex navigational tasks. Peruch et al. allowed subjects to explore a path using either proprioceptive-vestibular, visual-vestibular, or visual-alone inputs and then asked them to reproduce or reverse it or to take a “shortcut” back to the start in the same environment (249). Patients with unilateral vestibular impairment did much worse in the visual-alone and visual-vestibular conditions, the deficit being more marked the more complex the task. The fact that performance is impaired even in the visual-alone paradigm is perhaps surprising: patients with vestibular impairment might have been expected to perform better than their healthy peers in view of upregulated visual pathways to compensate for their vestibular loss. Yet the findings have been replicated in an experiment using a virtual Morris water task to test spatial memory and navigation, and furthermore that patients with bilateral vestibular loss have significant specific hippocampal atrophy compared with healthy controls (9) and also in animals (250, 251), lending further evidence that the vestibular system is important in spatial memory and navigation. Adding to this evidence is the observation that left-cold CVS significantly improved performance in an object-location-recall task (252).

The Vestibular System and a Sense of Self

Bodily self-consciousness, which comprises of self-location, self-identification and first-person perspective, is one of the higher-order functions influenced by the vestibular system. Bodily self-consciousness is thought to be the summation of different sensory and motor efference inputs, including that of the vestibular system, that allows for the construct of personal space (i.e., the space occupied by the body and the space immediately surrounding the body) and of extrapersonal space, reviewed by Blanke (253). Evidence for the role of the vestibular system in the construct of bodily self-consciousness also comes from patients with vestibular impairment: it is well-recognized that patients with vestibular dysfunction can experience a range of abnormal sensations, from distorted body image and schema to depersonalization, derealisation and out-of-body experiences, observations that were first recorded a century ago by Bonnier (254) [republished (255)] and Schilder (256). The role that the vestibular system plays in each of these symptoms is reviewed by Lopez and by Pfeiffer and for a fuller account the reader is directed to (257, 258).

Altered bodily self-consciousness has been linked with changed perception of body parts: patients with vestibular impairment reported changes in how various body parts feel during episodes of dizziness (254, 256) and caloric and galvanic vestibular stimulation has been observed to modify healthy subjects' perception of hand size (259, 260). Additionally, body integrity image disorder, which describes a syndrome in which patients complain of a mismatch between how they feel and how they physically are, with the result that they often request limb amputation, and somatoparaphrenia, in which patients, following a right parietal stroke, reject their left arm as being alien, can be improved temporarily by CVS (261, 262).

Depersonalization, the sensation of being detached from oneself, and derealisation, that of being detached from one's surroundings, are also thought to be a consequence of disturbed bodily self-consciousness. Not only have these symptoms been documented in patients with vestibular dysfunction, but Jáuregi-Renaud et al. found that the depersonalization/derealisation scores of patients as measured by Cox and Swinson's questionnaire were correlated with their error in estimation of passive whole-body rotation (263, 264). Symptoms of depersonalization and derealisation can also be induced in healthy controls by CVS (265). Although the neural correlate of such symptoms has yet to be conclusively identified, the superior temporal gyrus and TPJ seem to be the strongest candidates: electrical stimulation of this area caused subjects to report that they felt strange [for review, see (266)]; patients with depersonalization/derealisation symptoms had altered metabolism here on positron emission tomography (267); and, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right TPJ has been reported to alleviate these symptoms (268).

Out-of-body experiences are also associated with disturbed bodily self-consciousness. Such experiences typically have three characteristics: the person feels that they are in an illusory body that is removed from their physical body and that they have a first-person perspective of looking back at their physical body. They have been linked with a TPJ dysfunction, most often affecting the right side (269, 270). Sufficiently strong electrical stimulation of that region of the cortex also induces the illusion, with lower levels of stimulation inducing a sensation of falling or sinking (271). Out-of-body experiences have been observed to occur most typically when subjects are in a non-upright position (272), which is proposed to be due to visual-vestibular conflict, the otoliths, by signaling the direction of gravity, normally being important for forming a strong world-referenced image of oneself (273). Out-of-body experiences can also be induced by combined visual-vestibular-somatosensory conflict, for example, in healthy subjects watching the back of a virtual body being stroked whilst feeling a synchronous stroking on their own back (274). In such cases, interfering with TPJ functioning using transcranial magnetic stimulation abolished the illusion, yet the ability to imagine transformations of external objects was unaffected, suggesting that the TPJ performs a specific role in the processing of self in space and of bodily self-consciousness (275).

Interestingly, patients with schizophrenia have been observed to have a degree of vestibular dysfunction and reduced functional connectivity of the vestibular system (276278). Schizophrenia can be thought of as a disease of impaired multisensory processing with symptoms of depersonalization, derealisation, distorted first person perception, and loss of agency. The onset of psychosis is often preceded by a period of social withdrawal and sub-delusional detachment from reality. The psychotic period of the illness was defined historically by the presence of Schneider's first rank symptoms of delusional perception, auditory hallucinations, and delusions of thought interference and passivity. The TPJ has been implicated in auditory hallucinations (279, 280), with reported symptom improvement following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (281) and reduced TPJ-hippocampal connectivity has been associated with poorer social performance and negative symptoms (282, 283), reviewed by Wible (284). CVS has been recorded as improving insight into illness in schizophrenia (285) and reducing delusions in schizoaffective disorder (286).

The Vestibular System and Human Affect

Patients with vestibular impairment have been observed to suffer from a high burden of psychiatric disease, particularly affective disorders such as anxiety and depression (287290). Moreover, patients with psychiatric disease and no diagnosis of vestibular impairment have been found to have abnormal behavioral responses in tests known to rely upon intact vestibular functioning, including postural control (291) and vestibulo-oculo-motor tasks (292), and even in healthy subjects, mood state modifies balance control (293). It is thought, therefore, that the increased burden of psychiatric disease amongst patients with vestibular impairment compared with the general population might be explained by more than just the observation that chronic disease can negatively impact upon mood. Neuro-anatomically, there are cortical areas that are known to process vestibular information and to be involved in the regulation of mood and affect, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (294, 295). CVS has been shown to modify activity in the ACC and left-cold stimulation has been shown to increase risk estimation (and reduce unrealistic optimism) (296), improve anosognosia (a syndrome in which patients with evident disability deny any illness) (296) and modulate affective control and mood (297). Furthermore, positron emission tomography has revealed increased activity in the ACC of patients with mania associated with bipolar disorder and euthymic controls (298), and CVS has been reported to temporarily reduce the symptoms of mania in such patients (286, 299). The role of the ACC and vestibular stimulation in depression has yet to be fully investigated, although one might note that there are reports of abnormal eye movement control in depressed subjects (300). Chronic pain, itself associated with changes in mood and affect, is thought to be partly mediated by C-nociceptor input to the ACC, and in some patients, CVS has been reported to reduce symptoms (301, 302).

Self-Motion Perception and Recovery From Vestibular Dysfunction

After an insult to the vestibular system, it is usual for there to be abnormalities of low-level vestibular functions, such as the VOR, as well as of higher-order functions, such as motion perception. In such patients, and after a certain delay, it has been observed that whilst vestibular perception may have returned to normal or near-normal, the lower-level functions may remain abnormal, so-called perceptuo-reflex uncoupling, suggesting that, under optimal circumstances, higher-order processing can compensate for vestibular dysfunction (48, 135). Such compensation need not be the preserve of recovery from illness: as mentioned above, ballerinas who are accustomed to performing multiple pirouettes demonstrate similar perceptuo-reflex uncoupling (135), an uncoupling that has been proposed to occur via cerebellar sensory gating (77, 135, 303). [The reverse, symptoms of dizziness without abnormal VOR is, of course, a well-known phenomenon seen in brainstem infarction, epileptic seizures and electrical stimulation (126, 304). It is possible that in patients who go on to develop chronic symptoms after an initial vestibular insult, there is an impairment in such central compensatory processing. Such patients typically demonstrate poor correlation between VOR function (which is often unremarkable on clinical testing) and their symptoms (52). Research suggests that factors for good recovery from vestibular lesions include anxiety, visual dependence, autonomic arousal, depression, and fear of bodily sensations (54), and whilst some of these might be viewed as contributing to a psychological component to their symptoms, as summarized above, the co-existence of vestibular disorders and anxiety may point to shared central pathways. A better understanding of these might improve identification of such patients and clinical management of their disease.

The Effect of Self-Motion Perception on Numerical Magnitude Allocation

Other more abstract influences of self-motion on behavior include the relationship between numerical magnitude allocation and perception of self-motion. This is interesting not least because, at first consideration, the two processes might appear to be relatively independent. It is worthwhile acknowledging here that the exact relationship between numerical representation in the brain and visuospatial attention is debated (305, 306). Nevertheless, various experiments investigating the effect of self-motion on magnitude allocation have been carried out. Evidence supporting the hypothesis that numerical magnitude allocation can be influenced by self-motion includes: the bidirectional relationship between numerical magnitude and self-motion perception thresholds observed in subjects undergoing whole-body linear motion (307); modulation of the spontaneous number generator by lateral head turns and galvanic vestibular stimulation (308, 309); and, in stroke patients with visuospatial attentional biases (who have been shown to have concomitant biases in numerical estimations (310) viewing a visual stimulus moving toward the side of the neglect temporarily reversed the numerical bias (311, 312). In a recent study examining the effect of perceptual state of self during motion on a mental number-pair bisection task (estimating the mid-point between two numerical values), it was found that: vestibular-alone stimulation exerted no differences in number-pair bisection compared with baseline; when the subject perceives the world to be moving and themselves stationary, rightwards motion reduced the magnitude of estimates compared with baseline and leftwards motion increased the magnitude; and, during vection, both leftwards and rightwards vection elicited the same increase in magnitude of estimates as leftwards world motion, a finding explained by the inhibition seen in the right vestibular cortex during vection and thus leading to left hemisphere dominance and biasing toward larger numbers (see Figure 6) (306).

Figure 6.

Figure 6

From section, The effect of self-motion perception on numerical magnitude allocation. This Figure, modified from Arshad et al. (306). Graph showing percentage error in the number bisection task (normalized to 0% error by subtracting the baseline) for the four perceptual conditions: world motion right and left and self-motion (vection) right and left. Box-plots represent the median and interquartile range with whiskers denoting 10th and 90th percentile. **Marks significance at p < 0.01.

Self-Motion Perception and Economic Decision Making

Related to the role of the vestibular system in numerical magnitude allocation are the recent findings that implicate vestibular stimulation in economic decision making (297). Purchase decision making describes the motives and considerations involved in buying a product and include the desirability of that product as well as its cost and the maximum the individual is willing to pay (313315). In their experiment, Preuss et al. had subjects choose to buy or not to buy products (listed at 20% of the market price) either during sham or left-cold CVS. In the second half of the experiment, subjects ranked the desirability of products and their own “willingness to pay” for those products when the products were displayed at a range of prices, up to 100% of the market price. During left-cold CVS, subjects were less likely to buy products, and they also rated products as being less desirable. In contrast, the willingness of the subjects to pay for those products was not significantly different during sham and stimulation conditions.

Also probing the effects of the vestibular system on economic and prosocial decision making, Arshad et al. (316) used a modified version of the dictator game and a non-numerical prosocial questionnaire probed the effects of vestibular stimulation and binocular rivalry on participants' strategies. They found that there exists a correlation between inherent number-pair bisection error and the mean amount of money a subject donated to an unknown stranger, and that modulating numerical magnitude perception through combined CVS and binocular rivalry led to congruent changes in the mean amount donated, and that this occurred in a proportional manner. The intervention had no effect on the results of the altruism questionnaire, suggesting that the effect was mediated via numerical magnitude. The neural mechanism for such behavior remains to be determined, although a role for the ACC has been hypothesized (316).

Conclusion

The vestibular system may have developed as an organ to sense movement and coordinate postural and eye reflexes designed to stabilize the body, but its role in the generation of perception of self-motion, though less well-recognized, is equally important. Our understanding of the neurophysiology of self-motion perception has increased over the past few decades through a multitude of electrophysiological studies, psychophysical experiments, and observations from clinical medicine. Vestibular afferents undergo early processing and integration with somatosensory, visual and motor efference inputs in the vestibular nuclei. Such processing is evident throughout the vestibular network including in the cerebellum where it contributes to generation of world-framed motion and unexpected motion and in the PIVC and VIP where heading perception is processed. Future work might focus on the effects of different perceptual states on higher cognitive processes, perhaps examining the role, if any, that vestibular cortical lateralisation plays, and in doing so discover better tests for the monitoring of patients with central and peripheral vestibular disorders which may open up new avenues for the treatment of these diseases.

Author Contributions

ZB conceptualized and wrote the manuscript. QA conceptualized and edited the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the Neuro-otology Unit at Charing Cross Hospital.

References

  • 1.Graf W, Klam F. Le système vestibulaire: anatomie fonctionnelle et comparée, évolution et développement. Comptes Rendus Palevol. (2006) 637–55. 10.1016/j.crpv.2005.12.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Flourens P. Expériences sur le Système Nerveux: Faisant Suite aux Recherches Expérimentales sur les Propriétés et les Fonctions du Système Nerveux Dans les Animaux Vertébrés. Paris: Crevot; (1825). [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Weist G. The origins of vestibular science. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2015) 1343:1–9. 10.1111/nyas.12706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Weist G, Baloh R. The pioneering work of josef breuer on the vestibular system. Arch Neurol. (2002) 1647–53. 10.1001/archneur.59.10.1647 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lowenstein OE. Comparative morphology and physiology. In: Kornhuber HH, editors. Vestibular System Part 1: Basic Mechanisms. Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; (1974). p. 75 10.1007/978-3-642-65942-3_3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Beisel KW, Wang-Lundberg Y, Maklad A, Fritzsch B. Development and evolution of the vestibular sensory apparatus of the mammalian ear. J Vestibul Res. (2005) 15:225–41. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Barnett-Cowan M, Raeder SM, Bülthoff HH. Persistent perceptual delay for head movement onset relative to auditory stimuli of different durations and rise times. Exp Brain Res. (2012) 220:41–50. 10.1007/s00221-012-3112-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Barnett-Cowan M, Harris LR. Perceived timing of vestibular stimulation relative to touch, light and sound. Exp Brain Res. (2009) 198:221–31. 10.1007/s00221-009-1779-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Brandt T, Schautzer F, Hamilton DA, Brüning R, Markowitsch HJ, Kalla R, et al. Vestibular loss causes hippocampal atrophy and impaired spatial memory in humans. Brain. (2005) 2732–41. 10.1093/brain/awh617 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hoagland H. Impulses from sensory nerves of catfish. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (1932) 18:701–5. 10.1073/pnas.18.12.701 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lowenstein O, Sand A. The activity of the horizontal semi-circular canal of the dogfish, Scyllium canicula. J Exp. Biol. (1936) 416–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Goldberg JM, Smith CE, Fernandez C. Relation between discharge regularity and responses to externally applied galvanic currents in vestibular nerve afferents of the squirrel monkey. J Neurophysiol. (1984) 51:1236–56. 10.1152/jn.1984.51.6.1236 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Breuer J. Uber die Funktion der Bogengange des Orhlabyrinthes. Vienna: Wien Med Jarhb; (1874). [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lowenstein O, Wersall J. A functional interpretation of the electron microscopic structure of the sensory hairs in the cristae of the elasmobranch Raja clavata in terms of directional sensitivity. Nature. (1959) 184:1807–8. 10.1038/1841807a0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Augustine GJ, Chikaraishi DM, Ehlers MD, Einstein G, Fitzpatrick D, Hall WC, et al. Neuroscience. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, Inc; (2004). [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lowenstein O. Functional significance of the efferent control mechanisms. Prog Brain Res. (1972) 37:459. 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)63920-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lowenstein O. Electrophysiological experiments on the isolated surviving labyrinth of elasmobranch fish to analyse the responses to linear accelerations. In: J. Stahler, editors. Vestibular Function on Earth and in Space. Pergamon: Pergamon Press; (1970). p. 35–41. 10.1016/B978-0-08-015592-0.50010-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sadeghi SG, Chacron MJ, Taylor MC, Cullen KE. Neural variability, detection thresholds, and information transmission in the vestibular system. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:771–81. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4690-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cullen Multisensory integration and the perception of self-motion. In: Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Neuroscience (2018). 10.1093/acrefore/9780190264086.013.91 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Cullen KE, Minor LB. Semicircular canal afferents similarly encode active and passive head-on-body rotations: implications for the role of vestibular efference. J Neurosci. (2002) 22:RC226. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-j0002.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Jamali M, Carriot J, Chacron MJ, Cullen KE. Strong correlations between sensitivity and variability give rise to constant discrimination thresholds across the otolith afferent population. J Neurosci. (2013) 11302–13. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0459-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hoegyes GM. Uber den Nervenmechanismus der assozierten Augenbewegungen. Vienna: Urban und Schwarzenberg; (1913). [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lowenstein O. The tonic function of the horizontal semicircular canals in fishes. J Exp Biol. (1937) 14:473–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Angelaki DE. Eyes on target: what neurons must do for the vestibuloocular reflex during linear motion. J Neurophysiol. (2004) 92:20–35. 10.1152/jn.00047.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Paige GD, Seidman SH. Characteristics of the VOR in response to linear acceleration. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1999) 871:123–35. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09179.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Huterer M, Cullen KE. Vestibuloocular reflex dynamics during high-frequency and high-acceleration rotations of the head on body in rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol. (2002) 88:13–28. 10.1152/jn.2002.88.1.13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cajal SR. Histologie du syste me nerveux de I'Homme et des Vertebres. Paris: Maloine; (1911). [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lorente de No R. (1931). Ausgewählte kapitel aus der vergleichenden physiologie des labyrinthes. Die Augenmuskelreflexe beim Kaninchen und ihre Grundlagen. Ergebnisse der Physiologie. 32:73–242. 10.1007/BF02321720 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lorente de No LR. Vestibulo-ocular reflex arc. Arch Neurol Psych. (1933) 30:245–91. 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1933.02240140009001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Raphan T, Matsuo V, Cohen B. Velocity storage in the vestibulo-ocular reflex arc (VOR). Exp Brain Res. (1979) 35:229–48. 10.1007/BF00236613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Solomon D, Cohen B. Stimulation of the nodulus and uvula discharges velocity storage in the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Exp Brain Res. (1994) 102:57–68. 10.1007/BF00232438 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gauthier GM, Robinson DA. Adaptation of the human vestibuloocular reflex to magnifying lenses. Brain Res. (1975) 92:331–35. 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90279-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Gonshor A, MelvillJones GM. Extreme vestibuloocular adaptation induced by prolonged optical reversal of vision. J Physiol. (1974) 256:381–414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Robinson A. Adaptive gain control of vestibuloocular reflex by the cerebellum. J Neurophysiol. (1976) 39:954–9. 10.1152/jn.1976.39.5.954 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Li J, Smith SS, McElligott JG. Cerebellar nitric oxide is necessary for vestibulo-ocular reflex adaptation, a sensorimotor model of learning. J Neurophysiol. (1995) 74:489–94. 10.1152/jn.1995.74.1.489 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nagao S, Ito M. Subdural application of hemoglobin to the cerebellum blocks vestibuloocular reflex adaptation. Neuroreport. (1991) 2:193–6. 10.1097/00001756-199104000-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Gonshor A, Jones GM. Extreme vestibulo-ocular adaptation induced by prolonged optical reversal of vision. J Physiol. (1976) 256:381–414. 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011330 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sadeghi SG, Minor LB, Cullen KE. Neural correlates of motor learning in the vestibulo-ocular reflex: dynamic regulation of multimodal integration in the macaque vestibular system. J Neurosci. (2010) 30:10158–68. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1368-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Abadi RV. Mechanisms underlying nystagmus. J R Soc Med. (2002) 95:231–4. 10.1177/014107680209500504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lorente No de. Observations sur les Réflexes Toniques Oculaires. Travaux du Laboratoire de Recherches Biologiques; (1924). [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Lorente de No R. Etudes sur l'anatomie et la phisiologie de labirynthe du l'oreille et du huitiéme nerf. I. Réflexes toniques de l'eoil: Quelques Données sur le Mécanisme des Mouvements Oculaires. Travaux du Laboratoire de Recherches Biologiques; (1925). [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Benson AJ, Hutt EC, Brown SF. Thresholds for the perception of whole body angular movement about a vertical axis. Aviat Space Environ Med. (1989) 60:205–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Grabherr L, Nicoucar K, Mast FW, Merfeld DM. Vestibular thresholds for yaw rotation about an earth-vertical axis as a function of frequency. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 186:677–81. 10.1007/s00221-008-1350-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Seemungal BM, Gunaratne IA, Fleming IO, Gresty MA, Bronstein AM. Perceptual and nystagmic thresholds of vestibular function in yaw. J Vestibul Res. (2004) 14:461–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Bloomberg J, Jones GM, Segal B. Adaptive plasticity in the gaze stabilizing synergy of slow and saccadic eye movements. Exp Brain Res. (1991) 84:35–46. 10.1007/BF00231760 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Israel I, Fetter M, Koenig E. Vestibular perception of passive whole-body rotation about horizontal and vertical axes in humans: goal-directed vestibulo- ocular reflex and vestibular memory-contingent saccades. Exp Brain Res. (1993) 96:335–46. 10.1007/BF00227113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Nakamura T, Bronstein AM. The perception of head and neck angular displacement in normal and labyrinthine-defective subjects: a quantitative study using a ‘remembered saccade'technique. Brain. (1995) 118:1157–68. 10.1093/brain/118.5.1157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Metcalfe T, Gresty M. Self-controlled reorientating movements in response to rotational displacements in normal subjects and patients with labyrinthine disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1992) 22:695–8. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb25246.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Panichi R, Faralli M, Bruni R, Kiriakarely A, Occhigrossi C, Ferraresi A, et al. Asymmetric vestibular stimulation reveals persistent disruption of motion perception in unilateral vestibular lesions. J Neurophysiol. (2017) 118:2819–32. 10.1152/jn.00674.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Pettorossi VE, Panichi R, Botti FM, Kyriakareli A, Ferraresi A, Faralli M, et al. Prolonged asymmetric vestibular stimulation induces opposite, long-term effects on self-motion perception and ocular responses. J Physiol. (2013) 591:1907–20. 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.241182 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Patel M, Arshad Q, Roberts RE, Ahmad H, Bronstein AM. Chronic symptoms after vestibular neuritis and the high-velocity vestibulo-ocular reflex. Otol Neurotol. (2016) 37:179–84. 10.1097/MAO.0000000000000949 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Palla A, Straumann D, Bronstein AM. Vestibular neuritis: vertigo and the high-acceleration vestibulo-ocular reflex. J Neurol. (2008) 255:1479–82. 10.1007/s00415-008-0935-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Shupak A, Issa A, Golz A, Kaminer M, Braverman I. Prednisone treatment for vestibular neuritis. Otol Neurotol. (2008) 29:368–74. 10.1097/MAO.0b013e3181692804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Cousins S, Kaski D, Cutfield N, Arshad Q, Ahmad H, Gresty MA, et al. Predictors of clinical recovery from vestibular neuritis: a prospective study. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. (2017) 4:340–6. 10.1002/acn3.386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Cousins S, Cutfield NJ, Kaski D, Palla A, Seemungal BM, Golding JF, et al. Visual dependency and dizziness after vestibular neuritis. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e105426. 10.1371/journal.pone.0105426 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Israel I, Grasso R, Georges-Francois P, Tsuzuku T, Berthoz A. Spatial memory and path integration studied by self-driven passive linear displacement. I. Basic properties. J Neurophysiol. (1997) 77:3180–92. 10.1152/jn.1997.77.6.3180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Israel I, Berthoz A. Contribution of the otoliths to the calculation of linear displacement. J Neurophysiol. (1989) 62:247–63. 10.1152/jn.1989.62.1.247 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Kingma H. Thresholds for perception of direction of linear acceleration as a possible evaluation of the otolith function. BMC Ear Nose Throat Disord. (2005) 5:5. 10.1186/1472-6815-5-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Bremova T, Caushaj A, Ertl M, Strobl R, Böttcher N, Strupp M, et al. Comparison of linear motion perception thresholds in vestibular migraine and Menière's disease. Eur Arch Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. (2016) 273:2931–9. 10.1007/s00405-015-3835-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Gianna C, Heimbrand S, Gresty M. Thresholds for deteckrtion of motion direction during passive lateral whole-body acceleration in normal subjects and patients with bilateral loss of labyrinthine function. Brain Res Bull. (1996) 40:443–47. 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00140-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Lewis RF, Priesol AJ, Nicoucar K, Lim K, Merfeld DM. Dynamic tilt thresholds are reduced in vestibular migraine. J Vestibul Res. (2011) 21:323–30. 10.3233/VES-2011-0422 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Gu Y, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signals. Nat Neurosci. (2007) 10:1038–47. 10.1038/nn1935 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Telford L, Howard IP, Ohmi M. Heading judgments during active and passive self-motion. Exp Brain Res. (1995) 104:502–10. 10.1007/BF00231984 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Hummel N, Cuturi LF, MacNeilage PR, Flanagin VL. The effect of supine body position on human heading perception. J Vision. (2016) 16:19. 10.1167/16.3.19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Angelaki DE, Dickman JD. Spatiotemporal processing of linear acceleration: primary afferent and central vestibular neuron responses. J Neurophysiol. (2000) 84:2113–32. 10.1152/jn.2000.84.4.2113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Angelaki DE, Shaikh AG, Green AM, Dickman JD. Neurons compute internal models of the physical laws of motion. Nature. (2004) 430:560–4. 10.1038/nature02754 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Yakusheva TA, Shaikh AG, Green AM, Blazquez PM, Dickman JD, Angelaki DE. Purkinje cells in posterior cerebellar vermis encode motion in an inertial reference frame. Neuron. (2007) 54:973–85. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Laurens J, Meng H, Angelaki DE. Neural representation of orientation relative to gravity in the macaque cerebellum. Neuron. (2013) 80:1508–18. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Shaikh AG, Ghasia FF, Dickman JD, Angelaki DE. Properties of cerebellar fastigial neurons during translation, rotation, and eye movements. J Neurophysiol. (2005) 93:853–63. 10.1152/jn.00879.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Meng H, May PJ, Dickman JD, Angelaki DE. Vestibular signals in primate thalamus: properties and origins. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:13590–602. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3931-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Merfeld DM, Zupan L, Peterka RJ. Humans use internal models to estimate gravity and linear acceleration. Nature. (1999) 398:615–8. 10.1038/19303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Carriot J, Jamali M, Brooks JX, Cullen KE. Integration of canal and otolith inputs by central vestibular neurons is sub-additive for both active and passive self-motion: Implication for perception. J Neurosci. (2015) 35:3555–65. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3540-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.McCrea RA, Gdowski GT, Boyle R, Belton T. Firing behavior of vestibular neurons during active and passive head movements: vestibulo-spinal and other non-eye-movement related neurons. J Neurophysiol. (1999) 82:416–28. 10.1152/jn.1999.82.1.416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Roy JE, Cullen KE. Passive activation of neck proprioceptive inputs does not influence the discharge patterns of vestibular nuclei neurons. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2001) 942:486–9. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb03776.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Roy JE, Cullen KE. Selective processing of vestibular reafference during self-generated head motion. J Neurosci. (2001) 21:2131–42. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-06-02131.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Brooks JX, Carriot J, Cullen KE. Learning to expect the unexpected: rapid updating in primate cerebellum during voluntary self-motion. Nature neuroscience. (2015) 18:1310–7. 10.1038/nn.4077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Brooks JX, Cullen KE. The primate cerebellum selectively encodes unexpected self-motion. Curr Biol. (2013) 23:947–55. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Klam F, Graf W. Vestibular signals of posterior parietal cortex neurons during active and passive head movements in macaque monkeys. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2003) 1004:271–82. 10.1196/annals.1303.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Blanks RHL, Estes MS, Markham CH. Physiologic characteristics of vestibular first-order canal neurons in the cat. II. Response to constant angular acceleration. J Neurophysiol. (1975) 38:1250–68. 10.1152/jn.1975.38.5.1250 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.St George RJ, Day BL, Fitzpatrick RC. Adaptation of vestibular signals for self-motion perception. J Physiol. (2011) 589:843–53. 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.197053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Leigh RJ, Zee DS. The Neurology of Eye Movements. Oxford: Oxford University Press; (2015). [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Bertolini G, Ramat S, Laurens J, Bockisch CJ, Marti S, Straumann D, et al. Velocity storage contribution to vestibular self-motion perception in healthy human subjects. J Neurophysiol. (2010) 105:209–23. 10.1152/jn.00154.2010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Bertolini G, Ramat S, Bockisch CJ, Marti S, Straumann D, Palla A. Is vestibular self-motion perception controlled by the velocity storage? insights from patients with chronic degeneration of the vestibulo-cerebellum. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e36763. 10.1371/journal.pone.0036763 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Okada T, Grunfeld E, Shallo-Hoffmann J, Bronstein AM. Vestibular perception of angular velocity in normal subjects and in patients with congenital nystagmus. Brain. (1999) 122:1293–303. 10.1093/brain/122.7.1293 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Miles FA. (1998). The neural processing of 3D visual information: evidence from eye movements. Eur J Neurosci. 10:811–22. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00112.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Nigmatullina Y, Arshad Q, Wu K, Seemungal BM, Bronstein AM, Soto D. How imagery changes self-motion perception. Neuroscience. (2015) 291:46–52. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.01.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Péruch P, Lopez C, Redon-Zouiteni C, Escoffier G, Zeitoun A, Sanjuan M, et al. Vestibular information is necessary for maintaining metric properties of representational space: evidence from mental imagery. Neuropsychologica. (2011) 49:3136–44. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Falconer CJ, Mast FW. Balancing the mind: vestibular induced facilitation of egocentric mental transformations. Exp Psychol. (2012) 59:332–9. 10.1027/1618-3169/a000161 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.van Elk M, Blanke O. Imagined own-body transformations during passive self-motion. Psychol Res. (2014) 78:18–27. 10.1007/s00426-013-0486-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Wertheim AH, Mesland BS, Bles W. Cognitive suppression of tilt sensations during linear horizontal self-motion in the dark. Perception. (2001) 2001:733–41. 10.1068/p3092 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Kayser C, Logothetis NK. Do early sensory cortices integrate cross-modal information? Brain Struct Func. (2007) 212:121–32. 10.1007/s00429-007-0154-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Barmack NH. Central vestibular system: vestibular nuclei and posterior cerebellum. Brain Res Bull. (2003) 60:511–41. 10.1016/S0361-9230(03)00055-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Zhang X, Zakir M, Meng H, Sato H, Uchino Y. Convergence of the horizontal semicircular canal and otolith afferents on cat single vestibular neurons. Exp Brain Res. (2001) 140:1–11. 10.1007/s002210100764 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Ono S, Kushiro K, Zakir M, Meng H, Sato H, Uchino Y. Properties of utricular and saccular nerve-activated vestibulocerebellar neurons in cats. Exp Brain Res. (2000) 134:1–8. 10.1007/s002210000424 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Kushiro K, Zakir M, Sato H, Ono S, Ogawa Y, Meng H, et al. Saccular and utricular inputs to single vestibular neurons in cats. Exp Brain Res. (2000) 131:406–15. 10.1007/s002219900312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Ivanenko YP, Grasso R, Israël I, Berthoz A. The contribution of otoliths and semicircular canals to the perception of two-dimensional passive whole-body motion in humans. J Physiol. (1997) 502:223–33. 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1997.223bl.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Cullen KE. The vestibular system: multimodal integration and encoding of self-motion for motor control. Trends Neurosci. (2012) 185–96. 10.1016/j.tins.2011.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Dieterich M, Bartenstein P, Spiegel S, Bense S, Schwaiger M, Brandt T. Thalamic infarctions cause side-specific suppression of vestibular cortex activations. Brain. (2005) 128:2052–67. 10.1093/brain/awh551 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Baumann O, Borra RJ, Bower JM, Cullen KE, Habas C, Ivry RB, et al. Consensus paper: the role of the cerebellum in perceptual processes. Cerebellum. (2015) 14:197–220. 10.1007/s12311-014-0627-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Gao JH, Parsons LM, Bower JM, Xiong J, Li J, Fox PT. Cerebellum implicated in sensory acquisition and discrimination rather than motor control. Science. (1996) 272:545–7. 10.1126/science.272.5261.545 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Angelaki DE, Yakusheva TA. How vestibular neurons solve the tilt/translation ambiguity. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2009) 1164:19–28. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03939.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Bronstein AM, Grunfeld EA, Faldon M, Okada T. Reduced self-motion perception in patients with midline cerebellar lesions. Neuroreport. (2008) 19:691–3. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282fbf9f6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Dahlem K, Valko Y, Schmahmann JD, Lewis RF. Cerebellar contributions to self-motion perception: evidence from patients with congenital cerebellar agenesis. J Neurophysiol. (2016) 115:2280–5. 10.1152/jn.00763.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Marlinski V, McCrea RA. Coding of self-motion signals in ventro-posterior thalamus neurons in the alert squirrel monkey. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 189:463. 10.1007/s00221-008-1442-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Meng H, Angelaki DE. Responses of ventral posterior thalamus neurons to three-dimensional vestibular and optic flow stimulation. J Neurophysiol. (2009) 103:817–26. 10.1152/jn.00729.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Fukushima J, Akao T, Takeichi N, Kurkin S, Kaneko CR, Fukushima K. Pursuit-related neurons in the supplementary eye fields: discharge during pursuit and passive whole body rotation. J Neurophysiol. (2004) 91:2809–25. 10.1152/jn.01128.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Israël I, Rivaud S, Gaymard B, Berthoz A, Pierrot-Deseilligny C. Cortical control of vestibular-guided saccades in man. Brain. (1995) 118:1169–83. 10.1093/brain/118.5.1169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Israel I, Berthoz A, Rivaud S, Gaymard B. Role of the different frontal lobe areas in the control of the horizontal component of memory-guided saccades in man. Exp Brain Res. (1993) 95:166–71. 10.1007/BF00229665 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Bremmer F, Kubischik M, Pekel M, Hoffmann KP, Lappe M. Visual selectivity for heading in monkey area MST. Exp Brain Res. (2010) 200:51. 10.1007/s00221-009-1990-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Britten KH, van Wezel RJ. Electrical microstimulation of cortical area MST biases heading perception in monkeys. Nat Neurosci. (1998) 1:59–63. 10.1038/259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Gu Y, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Causal links between dorsal medial superior temporal area neurons and multisensory heading perception. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:2299–313. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5154-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Perrone JA, Stone LS. Emulating the visual receptive-field properties of MST neurons with a template model of heading estimation. J Neurosci. (1998) 18:5985–75. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-15-05958.1998 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Duffy CJ. MST neurons respond to optic flow and translational movement. J Neurophysiol. (1998) 80:1816–27. 10.1152/jn.1998.80.4.1816 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Gu Y, Angelaki DE, DeAngelis GC. Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd. Nat Neurosci. (2008) 11:1201–10. 10.1038/nn.2191 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Vaina LM. Complex motion perception and its deficits. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (1998) 8:494–502. 10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80037-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Klam F, Graf W. Discrimination between active and passive head movements by macaque ventral and medial intraparietal cortex neurons. J Physiol. (2006) 574(Pt. 2):367–86. 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.103697 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Duhamel JR, Colby CL, Goldberg ME. Ventral intraparietal area of the macaque: congruent visual and somatic response properties. J Neurophysiol. (1998) 79:126–36. 10.1152/jn.1998.79.1.126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Baizer JS, Ungerleider LG, Desimone R. Organization of visual inputs to the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in macaques. J Neurosci. (1991) 11:168–90. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-01-00168.1991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Boussaoud D, Ungerleider LG, Desimone R. Pathways for motion analysis: cortical connections of the medial superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas in the macaque. J Compar Neurol. (1990) 296:462–95. 10.1002/cne.902960311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Chen A, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Functional specializations of the ventral intraparietal area for multisensory heading discrimination. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:3567–81. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4522-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Grüsser OJ, Pause M, Schreiter U. Localization and responses of neurones in the parieto-insular vestibular cortex of awake monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). J Physiol. (1990) 430:537–57. 10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018306 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Fasold O, Heinau J, Trenner MU, Villringer A, Wenzel R. Proprioceptive head posture-related processing in human polysensory cortical areas. Neuroimage. (2008) 40:1232–42. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Grüsser OJ, Pause M, Schreiter U. Vestibular neurones in the parieto-insular cortex of monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): visual and neck receptor responses. J Physiol. (1990) 430:559–83. 10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018307 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Chen A, Gu Y, Liu S, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Evidence for a causal contribution of macaque vestibular, but not intraparietal, cortex to heading perception. J Neurosci. (2016) 36:3789–8. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2485-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Penfield W. LI vestibular sensation and the cerebral cortex. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngoll. (1957) 66:691–8. 10.1177/000348945706600307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Brandt T, Dieterich M, Danek A. Vestibular cortex lesions affect the perception of verticality. Ann Neurol. (1994) 403–12. 10.1002/ana.410350406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Billington J, Smith AT. Neural mechanisms for discounting head-roll-induced retinal motion. J Neurosci. (2015) 35:708–12. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3640-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Smith AT, Wall MB, Thilo KV. Vestibular inputs to human motion-sensitive visual cortex. Cereb Cortex. (2012) 22:1068–77. 10.1093/cercor/bhr179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Frank SM, Wirth AM, Greenlee MW. Visualvestibular processing in the human Sylvian fissure. J Neurophysiol. (2016) 116:263–71. 10.1152/jn.00009.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Frank SM, Baumann O, Mattingley JB, Greenlee MW. Vestibular and visual responses in human posterior insular cortex. J Neurophysiol. (2014) 112:2481–91. 10.1152/jn.00078.2014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Seemungal BM, Rizzo V, Gresty MA, Rothwell JC, Bronstein AM. Posterior parietal rTMS disrupts human path integration during a vestibular navigation task. Neurosci Lett. (2008) 437:88–92. 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.03.067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Seemungal BM, Rizzo V, Gresty MA, Rothwell JC, Bronstein AM. Perceptual encoding of self-motion duration in human posterior parietal cortex. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2009) 1164:236–8. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03772.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Bednarczuk NF, Ortega MC, Fluri AS, Bronstein AM, Arshad Q. Inter-hemispheric control of vestibular thresholds. Brain Stimul. (2017) 10:988–91. 10.1016/j.brs.2017.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Kyriakareli A, Cousins S, Pettorossi VE, Bronstein AM. Effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on vestibular-ocular and vestibulo-perceptual thresholds. Neuroreport. (2013) 24:808–812. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283646e65 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Nigmatullina Y, Hellyer PJ, Nachev P, Sharp DJ, Seemungal BM. The neuroanatomical correlates of training-related perceptuo-reflex uncoupling in dancers. Cereb Cortex. (2013) 25:554–62. 10.1093/cercor/bht266 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Vaina LM. Selective impairment of visual motion interpretation following lesions of the right occipito-parietal area in humans. Biol Cybermetr. (1989) 61:347–59. 10.1007/BF00200800 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Vaina L, Makris N, Kennedy D, Cowey A. The selective impairment of the perception of first-order motion by unilateral cortical brain damage. Visual Neurosci. (1998) 15:333–48. 10.1017/S0952523898152082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Vaina LM, Rushton SK. What neurological patients tell us about the use of optic flow. Int Rev Neurobiol. (2000) 44:293–314. 10.1016/S0074-7742(08)60747-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Nigmatullina Y, Siddiqui S, Khan S, Sander K, Lobo R, Bronstein AM, et al. Lateralisation of the vestibular cortex is more pronounced in left-handers. Brain Stimul. (2016) 9:942–44. 10.1016/j.brs.2016.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Arshad Q, Nigmatullina Y, Bronstein AM. Handedness-related cortical modulation of the vestibular-ocular reflex. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:3221–7. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2054-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Bednarczuk NF, Casanovas Ortega M, Fluri AS, Arshad Q. Vestibulo-cortical hemispheric dominance: the link between anxiety and the vestibular system? Ear J Neurosci. (2018) 47:1517–24. 10.1111/ejn.13948 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Hollands MA, Ziavra NV, Bronstein AM. A new paradigm to investigate the roles of head and eye movements in the coordination of whole-body movements. Exp Brain Res. (2004) 154:261–6. 10.1007/s00221-003-1718-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Dokka K, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Multisensory integration of visual and vestibular signals improves heading discrimination in the presence of a moving object. J Neurosci. (2015) 35:13599–607. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2267-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Ramkhalawansingh R, Keshavarz B, Haycock B, Shahab S, Campos JL. Examining the effect of age on visual–vestibular self-motion perception using a driving paradigm. Perception. (2016) 46:566–85. 10.1177/0301006616675883 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Britten KH. Mechanisms of self-motion perception. Ann Rev Neurosci. (2008) 31:389–410. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112953 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Beraneck M, Cullen KE. Activity of vestibular nuclei neurons during vestibular and optokinetic stimulation in the alert mouse. J Neurophysiol. (2007) 98:1549–65. 10.1152/jn.00590.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Bryan AS, Angelaki DE. Optokinetic and vestibular responsiveness in the macaque rostral vestibular and fastigial nuclei. J Neurophysiol. (2009) 101:714–20. 10.1152/jn.90612.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Angelaki DE, Gu Y, DeAngelis GC. Visual and vestibular cue integration for heading perception in extrastriate visual cortex. J Physiol. (2011) 589:825–33. 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.194720 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Takahashi K, Gu Y, May PJ, Newlands SD, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Multimodal coding of three-dimensional rotation and translation in area MSTd: comparison of visual and vestibular selectivity. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:9742–56. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0817-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.MacNeilage PR, Banks MS, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Vestibular heading discrimination and sensitivity to linear acceleration in head and world coordinates. J Neurosci. (2010) 30:9084–94. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1304-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Karmali F, Lim K, Merfeld DM. Visual and vestibular perceptual thresholds each demonstrate better precision at specific frequencies and also exhibit optimal integration. J Neurophysiol. (2013) 111:2393–403. 10.1152/jn.00332.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Kolev OI. Thresholds for self-motion perception in roll without and with visual fixation target - the visual-vestibular interaction effect. Func Neurol. (2015) 30:99–104. 10.11138/fneur/2015.30.2.099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Kaliuzhna M, Ferrè ER, Herbelin B, Blanke O, Haggard P. Multisensory effects on somatosensation: a trimodal visuo-vestibular-tactile interaction. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:26301. 10.1038/srep26301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Ishida M, Fushiki H, Nishida H, Watanabe Y. Self-motion perception during conflicting visual-vestibular acceleration. J Vestibul Res. (2008) 18:267–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Hartmann M, Furrer S, Herzog MH, Merfeld DM, Mast FW. Self-motion perception training: thresholds improve in the light but not in the dark. Exp Brain Res. (2013) 226:231–40. 10.1007/s00221-013-3428-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Cullen KE, Roy JE. Signal processing in the vestibular system during active versus passive head movements. J Neurophysiol. (2004) 91:1919–33. 10.1152/jn.00988.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Gdowski GT, McCrea RA. Neck proprioceptive inputs to primate vestibular nucleus neurons. Exp Brain Res. (2000) 135:511–26. 10.1007/s002210000542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Sadeghi SG, Mitchell DE, Cullen KE. Different neural strategies for multimodal integration: comparison of two macaque monkey species. Exp Brain Res. (2009) 195:45–57. 10.1007/s00221-009-1751-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Sans A, Raymond J, Marty R. Thalamic and cortical responses to electric stimulation of the vestibular nerve in the cat. Exp Brain Res. (1970) 10:265–75. 10.1007/BF00235050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Bremmer F, Klam F, Duhamel JR, Hamed SB, Graf W. Visual-vestibular interactive responses in the macaque ventral intraparietal area (VIP). Ear J Neurosci. (2002) 16:1569–86. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02206.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Blouin J, Okada T, Wolsley C, Bronstein A. Encoding target-trunk relative position: cervical versus vestibular contribution. Exp Brain Res. (1998) 122:101–7. 10.1007/s002210050496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Schweigart G, Chien RD, Mergner T. Neck proprioception compensates for age-related deterioration of vestibular self-motion perception. Exp Brain Res. (2002) 147:89–97. 10.1007/s00221-002-1218-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Wright WG, DiZio P, Lackner JR. Vertical linear self-motion perception during visual and inertial motion: more than weighted summation of sensory inputs. J Vestibul Res. (2005) 15:185–95. Available online at: https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/c082/5ac7ff947d7b755b42b486281b66125efbbc.pdf [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Zacharias GL, Young LR. Influence of combined visual and vestibular cues on human perception and control of horizontal rotation. Exp Brain Res. (1981) 41:159–71. 10.1007/BF00236605 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Ferrè ER, Walther LE, Haggard P. Multisensory Interactions between vestibular, visual and somatosensory signals. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0124573. 10.1371/journal.pone.0124573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Ferrè ER, Sedda A, Gandola M, Bottini G. How the vestibular system modulates tactile perception in normal subjects: a behavioural and physiological study. Exp Brain Res. (2011) 208:29–38. 10.1007/s00221-010-2450-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Ferrè ER, Day BL, Bottini G, Haggard P. How the vestibular system interacts with somatosensory perception: a sham-controlled study with galvanic vestibular stimulation. Neurosci Lett. (2013) 550:35–40. 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.06.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Rorden C, Karnath HO, Driver J. Do neck-proprioceptive and caloric-vestibular stimulation influence covert visual attention in normals, as they influence visual neglect? Neuropsychologica. (2001) 39:364–75. 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00126-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Bottini G, Paulesu E, Gandola M, Loffredo S, Scarpa P, Sterzi R, et al. Left caloric vestibular stimulation ameliorates right hemianesthesia. Neurology. (2005) 65:1278–83. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000182398.14088.e8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Bottini G, Paulesu E, Sterzi R, Warburton E, Wise RJS, Vallar G, et al. Modulation of conscious experience by peripheral sensory stimuli. Nature. (1995) 376:778–81. 10.1038/376778a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 171.Fetsch CR, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Visual-vestibular cue integration for heading perception: Applications of optimal cue integration theory. Eur J Neurosci. (2010) 31:1721–9. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07207.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Butler JS, Smith ST, Campos JL, Bülthoff HH. Bayesian integration of visual and vestibular signals for heading. J Vision. (2010) 10:23. 10.1167/10.11.23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Fetsch CR, Turner AH, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Dynamic reweighting of visual and vestibular cues during self-motion perception. J Neurosci. (2009) 29:15601–12. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2574-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Prsa M, Gale S, Blanke O. Self-motion leads to mandatory cue fusion across sensory modalities. J Neurophysiol. (2012) 108:2282–91. 10.1152/jn.00439.2012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Butler JS, Campos JL, Bülthoff HH. Optimal visual-vestibular integration under conditions of conflicting intersensory motion profiles. Exp Brain Res. (2015) 233:587–97. 10.1007/s00221-014-4136-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Fetsch CR, Pouget A, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Neural correlates of reliability-based cue weighting during multisensory integration. Nature neuroscience. (2012) 15:146–54. 10.1038/nn.2983 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Adams R. An account of a peculiar optical phenomenon seen after having looked at a moving body, etc. In: D. Taylor R, Phillips, R. Brewster, editors. The London and Edinburgh Philosophical Magazine and Journal of Science. London: University of London; (1834). p. 373–374. [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Snowden RJ, Milne AB. Phantom motion aftereffects–evidence of detectors for the analysis of optic flow. Curr Biol. (1997) 7:717–22. 10.1016/S0960-9822(06)00329-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Harris LR, Morgan MJ, Still AW. Moving and the motion after-effect. Nature. (1981) 293:139–41. 10.1038/293139a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Brandt T, Dichgans J, Buchle W. Motion habituation: inverted self-motion perception and optokinetic after-nystagmus. Exp Brain Res. (1974) 21:337–52. 10.1007/BF00237897 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.Kommerell G, Thiele H. Der optokinetische kurzreiznystagmus [Optokinetic short-stimulation nystagmus]. Graefes Arch. Klin. Exp. Ophthalmol. (1970) 179:220–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 182.Cuturi LF, MacNeilage PR. Optic ?ow induces nonvisual self-motion aftereffects. Curr Biol. (2014) 24:2817–21. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.10.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Crane BT. Limited interaction between translation and visual motion aftereffects in humans. Exp Brain Res. (2013) 224:165–78. 10.1007/s00221-012-3299-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Mach E. Grundlinien der Lehre von der Bewegungsempfindung. Leipzig: Engelmann; (1875). [Google Scholar]
  • 185.Wood R. The 'Haunted Swing' illusion. Psychol Rev. (1895) 2:277–8. 10.1037/h0073333 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Väljamäe A. Auditorily-induced illusory self-motion: a review. Brain Res Rev. (2009) 61:240–55. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Nordahl R, Nilsson NC, Turchet L, Serafin S. Vertical illusory self-motion through haptic stimulation of the feet. IEEE VR Workshop on Perceptual Illusions in Virtual Environments. (2012) 21–26. 10.1109/PIVE.2012.6229796 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 188.Brandt T, Dichgans J, Koenig E. (1973). Differential effects of central versus peripheral vision on egocentric and exocentric motion perception. Experimental brain research 476–491. 10.1007/BF00234474 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Berthoz A, Pavard B, Young LR. Perception of linear horizontal self-motion induced by peripheral vision (linearvection) basic characteristics and visual-vestibular interactions. Exp Brain Res. (1975) 23:471–89. 10.1007/BF00234916 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Dichgans J, Brandt T. Visual-vestibular interaction: effects on self-motion perception and postural control. Perception. (1978) 8:755–804. 10.1007/978-3-642-46354-9_25 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Trutoiu LC, Mohler BJ, Schulte-Pelkum J, Bülthoff HH. Circular, linear, and curvilinear vection in a large-screen virtual environment with floor projection. Comput Grap. (2009) 33:47–58. 10.1016/j.cag.2008.11.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Brandt T, Bartenstein P, Janek A, Dieterich M. Reciprocal inhibitory visual-vestibular interaction. Visual motion stimulation deactivates the parieto-insular vestibular cortex. Brain. (1998) 121:1749–58. 10.1093/brain/121.9.1749 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 193.Kleinschmidt A, Thilo KV, Büchel C, Gresty MA, Bronstein AM, Frackowiak RS. Neural correlates of visual-motion perception as object-or self-motion. Neuroimage. (2002) 16:873–82. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 194.Kovács G, Raabe M, Greenlee MW. Neural correlates of visually induced self-motion illusion in depth. Cereb Cortex. (2007) 18:1779–87. 10.1093/cercor/bhm203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 195.Becker-Bense S, Buchholz HG, Eulenburg P, Best C, Bartenstein P, Schreckenberger M, et al. Ventral and dorsal streams processing visual motion perception (FDG-PET study). BMC Neurosci. (2012) 13:81. 10.1186/1471-2202-13-81 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 196.Cheung BSK, Howard IP, Nedzelski JM, Landolt JP. Circularvection about earth-horizontal axes in bilateral labyrinthine-defective subjects. Acta Oto-Laryngol. (1989) 108:336–44. 10.3109/00016488909125537 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 197.Johnson WH, Sunahara FA, Landolt JP. Importance of the vestibular system in visually induced nausea and self-vection. J Vestib Res. (1999) 9:83–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 198.Riecke BE, Schulte-Pelkum J, Caniard F, Bülthoff HH. Spatialized auditory cues enhance the visually-induced self-motion illusion (circular vection) in virtual reality. Rapport Tech. (2005) 138 Available online at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/216055709_Spatialized_auditory_cues_enhance_the_visually-induced_self-motion_illusion_circular_vection_in_Virtual_Reality
  • 199.Riecke BE, Feuereissen D, Rieser JJ. Auditory self-motion illusions (circular vection) can be facilitated by vibrations and the potential for actual motion. In: Proceedings of the 5th Symposium on Applied Perception in Graphics and Visualisation. New York, NY: (2008). [Google Scholar]
  • 200.Riecke BE, Väljamäe A, Schulte-Pelkum J. Moving sounds enhance the visually-induced self-motion illusion (circular vection) in virtual reality. ACM Trans Appl Percept. (2009) 6:7 10.1145/1498700.1498701 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 201.Kruijff E, Riecke B, Trekowski C, Kitson A. Upper body leaning can affect forward self-motion perception in virtual environments. In: Proceedings of the 3rd ACM Symposium on Spatial User Interaction. New York, NY (2015). p. 103–12. 10.1145/2788940.2788943 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 202.Lepecq JC, Giannopulu I, Baudonniere PM. Cognitive effects on visually induced body motion in children. Perception. (1995) 435–49. 10.1068/p240435 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 203.Lee DN, Lishman JR. Visual Proprioceptive Control of Stance. J Human Movement Stud. (1975) [Google Scholar]
  • 204.Paulus W, Straube A, Krafczyk S, Brandt T. Differential effects of retinal target displacement, changing size and changing disparity in the control of anterior/posterior and lateral body sway. Exp Brain Res. (1989) 78:243–52. 10.1007/BF00228896 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 205.Guerraz M, Thilo KV, Bronstein AM, Gresty MA. Influence of action and expectation on visual control of posture. Cogn Brain Res. (2001) 11:259–66. 10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00080-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 206.Júnior PBF, Barela JA. Postural control as a function of self- and object-motion perception. Neurosci Lett. (2004) 64–8. 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.07.075 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 207.Guerraz M, Bronstein AM. Mechanisms underlying visually induced body sway. Neurosci Lett. (2008) 443:12–6. 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 208.DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Visual–Vestibular Integration for Self-Motion Perception. In: Wallace MT, Murray MM, editors. The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor and Francis; (2012). p. 629–52. [Google Scholar]
  • 209.Probst T, Brandt T, Degner D. Object-motion detection affected by concurrent self-motion perception: psychophysics of a new phenomenon. Behav Brain Res. (1986) 22:1–11. 10.1016/0166-4328(86)90076-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 210.van Elk M, Blanke O. Balancing bistable perception during self-motion. Exp Brain Res. (2012) 222:219–28. 10.1007/s00221-012-3209-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 211.Hogendoorn H, Verstraten FA, MacDougall H, Alais D. Vestibular signals of self-motion modulate global motion perception. Vision Res. (2017) 130:22–30. 10.1016/j.visres.2016.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 212.Moser EI, Kropff E, Moser MB. Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system. Ann Rev Neurosci. (2008) 31:69–89. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.061307.090723 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 213.Moser EI, Moser MB, McNaughton BL. Spatial representation in the hippocampal formation: a history. Nature. (2017) 20:1448–64. 10.1038/nn.4653 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 214.Fyhn M, Molden S, Witter MP, Moser EI, Moser MB. Spatial representation in the entorhinal cortex. Science. (2004) 1258–64. 10.1126/science.1099901 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 215.Wallach H. The role of head movements and vestibular and visual cues in sound localization. J Exp Psychol. (1940) 27:339–68. 10.1037/h0054629 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 216.Sakamoto S, Teramoto W, Terashima H, Gyoba J. Effect of active self-motion on auditory space perception. Interdiscipl Inform Sci. (2015) 21:167–72. 10.4036/iis.2015.A.08 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 217.Teramoto W, Sakamoto S, Furune F, Gyoba J, Suzuki Y. Compression of auditory space during forward self-motion. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e39402. 10.1371/journal.pone.0039402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 218.Halmagyi GM, Gresty MA, Gibson WPR. Ocular tilt reaction with peripheral vestibular lesion. Ann Neurol. (1979) 6:80–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 219.Takemori S, Ida M, Umezu H. Vestibular training after sudden loss of vestibular functions. ORL. (1985) 47:76–83. 10.1159/000275748 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 220.Anastasopoulos D, Haslwanter T, Bronstein A, Fetter M, Dichgans J. Dissociation between the perception of body verticality and the visual vertical in acute peripheral vestibular disorder in humans. Neurosci Lett. (1997) 233:151–53. 10.1016/S0304-3940(97)00639-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 221.Lopez C, Lacour M, Ahmadi AE, Magnan J. Borel L. Changes of visual vertical perception: a long-term sign of unilateral and bilateral vestibular loss. Neuropsychologia. (2007) 45:2025–37. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 222.Vilbert D, Hausler R. Long-term evolution of subjective visual vertical after vestibular neurectomy and labyrinthectomy. Acta Oto-Laryngol. (2000) 120:620–2. 10.1080/000164800750000432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 223.Bohmer A, Mast F. Assessing otolith function by the subjective visual vertical. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2006) 871:221–31. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09187.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 224.Bronstein AM, Yardley L, Moore AP, Cleeves L. Visually and posturally mediated tilt illusion in Parkinson's disease and in labyrinthine defective subjects. Neurology. (1996) 47:651–6. 10.1212/WNL.47.3.651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 225.Bisdorff AR, Wolsley CJ, Anastasopoulos D, Bronstein AM, Gresty MA. The Perception of body verticality (subjective postural vertical) in peripheral and central vestibular disorders. Brain. (1996) 119:1523–34. 10.1093/brain/119.5.1523 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 226.Borel L, Harlay F, Magnan J, Lacour M. How changes in vestibular and visual reference frames combine to modify body orientation in space. Neuroreport. (2001) 12:3137–41. 10.1097/00001756-200110080-00031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 227.Bray A, Subanandan A, Isableu B, Ohlmann T, Golding JF, Gresty MA. We are most aware of our place in the world when about to fall. CurrBiol. (2004) 14:R609–10. 10.1016/j.cub.2004.07.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 228.Lopez C, Lacour M, Borel L. Perception de la verticalité et représentations spatiales dans les aires corticales vestibulaires. Bipédie, contrôle postural et représentation corticale. (2005) 35–86. Available online at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/251586348_Perception_de_la_verticalite_et_representations_spatiales_dans_les_aires_corticales_vestibulaires
  • 229.Thilo KV, Guerraz M, Bronstein AM, Gresty MA. Changes in horizontal oculomotor behaviour coincide with a shift in visual motion perception. Neuroreport. (2000) 11:1987–90. 10.1097/00001756-200006260-00036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 230.Thilo KV, Guerraz M, Bronstein AM, Gresty MA. Percept-related changes in horizontal optokinetic nystagmus at different body orientations in space. Exp Brain Res. (2002) 145:215–21. 10.1007/s00221-002-1114-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 231.Hood JD, Leech J. The significance of peripheral vision in the perception of movement. Acta oto-laryngol. (1974) 77:72–9. 10.3109/00016487409124600 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 232.Thilo KV, Probst T, Bronstein AM, Ito Y, Gresty MA. Torsional eye movements are facilitated during perception of self-motion. Exp Brain Res. (1999) 126:495–500. 10.1007/s002210050757 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 233.Heilman KM, Bowers D, Coslett B, Whelan H, Watson RT. Directional hypokinesia: prolonged reaction times for leftward movements in patients with right hemisphere lesions and neglect. Neurology. (1985) 35:855–9. 10.1212/WNL.35.6.855 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 234.Bisiach E, Luzzatti C. Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex. (1978) 14:129–33. 10.1016/S0010-9452(78)80016-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 235.Vallar G, Perani D. The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man. Neuropsychologia. (1986) 24:609–22. 10.1016/0028-3932(86)90001-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 236.Cappa S, Sterzi R, Vallar G, Bisiach E. Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia during vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia. (1987) 25:775–82. 10.1016/0028-3932(87)90115-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 237.Rubens AB. Caloric stimulation and unilateral visual neglect. Neurology. (1985) 35:1019–24. 10.1212/WNL.35.7.1019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 238.Silberfenning J. Contribution to the problem of eye movements. Disturbances of ocular movements with pseudo hemianopsia in frontal tumors. Stereotactic Func Neurosurg. (1941) 4:1–13. 10.1159/000106147 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 239.Fasold O, von Brevern M, Kuhberg M, Ploner CJ, Villringer A, Lempert T, et al. Human vestibular cortex as identified with caloric stimulation in functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage. (2002) 17:1384–93. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1241 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 240.Utz KS, Keller I, Kardinal M, Kerkhoff G. Galvanic vestibular stimulation reduces the pathological rightward line bisection error in neglect—A sham stimulation-controlled study. Neuropsychologia. (2011) 49:1219–25. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 241.Mattingley JB, Bradshaw JL, Bradshaw JA. Horizontal visual motion modulates focal attention in left unilateral spatial neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr. (1994) 57:1228–35. 10.1136/jnnp.57.10.1228 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 242.Pizzamiglio L, Frasca R, Guariglia C, Incoccia C, Antonucci G. Effect of optokinetic stimulation in patients with visual neglect. Cortex. (1990) 26:535–40. 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80303-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 243.Vallar G, Papagno C, Rusconi ML, Bisiach E. Vestibular stimulation, spatial hemineglect and dysphasia, selective effects. Cortex. (1995) 31:589–93. 10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80070-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 244.Kerkhoff G, Keller I, Ritter V, Marquardt C. Repetitive optokinetic stimulation induces lasting recovery from visual neglect. Restor Neurol Neurosci. (2006) 24:357–69. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 245.Thimm M, Fink GR, Küst J, Karbe H, Willmes K, Sturm W. Recovery from hemineglect: differential neurobiological effects of optokinetic stimulation and alertness training. Cortex. (2009) 45:850–62. 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 246.Bisiach E, Pizzamiglio L, Nico D, Antonucci G. Beyond unilateral neglect. Brain. (1996) 119:851–7. 10.1093/brain/119.3.851 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 247.Smith PF. Vestibular-hippocampal interactions. Hippocampus. (1997) 7:465–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 248.Vitte E, Derosier C, Caritu Y, Berthoz A, Hasboun D, Soulie D. Activation of the hippocampal formation by vestibular stimulation: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Exp Brain Res. (1996) 11:523–6. 10.1007/BF00227958 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 249.Péruch P, Borel L, Magnan J, Lacour M. Direction and distance deficits in path integration after unilateral vestibular loss depend on task complexity. Cogn Brain Res. (2005) 25:862–72. 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 250.Stackman RW, Herbert AM. Rats with lesions of the vestibular system require a visual landmark for spatial navigation. Behav Brain Res. (2002) 128:27–40. 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00270-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 251.Stackman RW, Clark AS, Taube JS. Hippocampal spatial representations require vestibular input. Hippocampus. (2002) 12:291–303. 10.1002/hipo.1112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 252.Bächtold D, Baumann T, Sandor PS, Kritos M, Regard M, Brugger P. Spatial- and verbal-memory improvement by cold-water caloric stimulation in healthy subjects. Exp Brain Res. (2001) 136:128–32. 10.1007/s002210000588 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 253.Blanke O. Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2012) 13:556–71. 10.1038/nrn3292 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 254.Bonnier P. L'Aschématie. Revue Neurol. (1906) 16:605–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 255.Bonnier P. Asomatognosia P. Bonnier. L'aschématie. Revue Neurol. 1905;13:605-9. Epil Behav. (2009) 16:401–3. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.09.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 256.Schilder Selbstbewusstsein und Persönlichkeitsbewusstsein: Eine Psychopathologische Studie. Berlin;Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag; (1914). [Google Scholar]
  • 257.Lopez C. A neuroscientific account of how vestibular disorders impair bodily self-consciousness. Front Integrat Neurosci. (2013) 7:91. 10.3389/fnint.2013.00091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 258.Pfeiffer C, Serino A, Blanke O. The vestibular system: a spatial reference for bodily self-consciousness. Front Integr Neurosci. (2014) 8:31. 10.3389/fnint.2014.00031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 259.Ferrè ER, Vagnoni E, Haggard P. Vestibular contributions to bodily awareness. Neuropsychologia. (2013) 51:1445–52. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 260.Lopez C, Schreyer HM, Preuss N, Mast FW. Vestibular stimulation modifies the body schema. Neuropsychologia. (2012) 1830–7. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 261.Ramachandran VS, McGeoch P. Can vestibular caloric stimulation be used to treat apotemnophilia? Med Hypotheses. (2007) 69:250–52. 10.1016/j.mehy.2006.12.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 262.Spitoni GF, Pireddu G, Galati G, Sulpizio V, Paolucci S, Pizzamiglio L. Caloric vestibular stimulation reduces pain and somatoparaphrenia in a severe chronic central post-stroke pain patient: a case study. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0151213. 10.1371/journal.pone.0151213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 263.Cox BJ, Swinson RP. Instrument to assess depersonalization-derealization in panic disorder. Dep Anxiety. (2002) 15:172–5. 10.1002/da.10051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 264.Jáuregui-Renaud K, Sang FYP, Gresty MA, Green DA, Bronstein AM. Depersonalisation/derealisation symptoms and updating orientation in patients with vestibular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr. (2008) 79:276–83. 10.1136/jnnp.2007.122119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 265.Yen Pik Sang F, Jauregui-Renaud K, Green DA, Bronstein AM, Gresty MA. Depersonalisation/derealisation symptoms in vestibular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr. (2006) 77:760–6. 10.1136/jnnp.2005.075473 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 266.Penfield WG. Ferrier lecture - some observations on the cerebral cortex of man. Proc R Soc Biol Sci. (1947) 134:329–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 267.Simeon D, Guralnik O, Hazlett EA, Spiegel-Cohen J, Hollander E, Buchsbaum MS. Feeling unreal: a PET study of depersonalization disorder. Am J Psychiatr. (2000) 157:1782–8. 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1782 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 268.Rachid F. Treatment of a patient with depersonalization disorder with low frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right temporo-parietal junction in a Private practice setting. J Psychiatr Prac. (2017) 23:145–7. 10.1097/PRA.0000000000000214 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 269.Blanke O, Landis T, Spinelli L, Seeck M. Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin. Brain. (2004) 127:243–58. 10.1093/brain/awh040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 270.Ionta S, Heydrich L, Lenggenhager B, Mouthon M, Fornari E, Chapuis D, et al. Multisensory mechanisms in temporo-parietal cortex support self-location and first-person perspective. Neuron. (2011) 70:363–74. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 271.Blanke O, Ortigue S, Landis T, Seeck M. Neuropsychology: stimulating illusory own-body perceptions. Nature. (2002) 419:269–70. 10.1038/419269a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 272.Green CE. Out-Of-Body Experiences. London: Hamish Hamilton; (1968) [Google Scholar]
  • 273.Lopez C, Halje P, Blanke O. Body ownership and embodiment: vestibular and multisensory mechanisms. Neurophysiol Clin Neurophysiol. (2008) 38:149–61. 10.1016/j.neucli.2007.12.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 274.Lenggenhager B, Tadi T, Metzinger T, Blanke O. Video ergo sum: manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science. (2007) 317:1096–9. 10.1126/science.1143439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 275.Blanke O, Mohr C, Michel CM, Pascual-Leone A, Brugger P, Seeck M, et al. Linking out-of-body experience and self processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction. J Neurosci. (2005) 25:550–7. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2612-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 276.Levy DL, Holzman PS, Proctor LR. Vestibular dysfunction and psychopathology. Schizophr Bull. (1983) 9:383–438. 10.1093/schbul/9.3.383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 277.Orntiz E. Vestibular dysfunction in schizophrenia and childhood autism. Compr Pychiatry. (1970) 11:159–73. 10.1016/0010-440X(70)90157-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 278.Schilder P. The vestibular apparatus in neurosis and psychosis. J Nervous Ment Dis. (1933) 78:137–64. 10.1097/00005053-193308000-00005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 279.Mondino M, Jardri R, Suaud-Chagny MF, Saoud M, Poulet E, Brunelin J. Effects of fronto-temporal transcranial direct current stimulation on auditory verbal hallucinations and resting-state functional connectivity of the left temporo-parietal junction in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2015) 42:318–26. 10.1093/schbul/sbv114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 280.Vercammen A, Knegtering H, den Boer JA, Liemburg EJ, Aleman A. Auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia are associated with reduced functional connectivity of the temporo-parietal area. Biol Psychiatr. (2010) 67:912–8. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 281.Vercammen A, Knegtering H, Liemburg EJ, den Boer JA, Aleman A. Functional connectivity of the temporo-parietal region in schizophrenia: effects of rTMS treatment of auditory hallucinations. J Psychiatr Res. (2010) 44:725–31. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.12.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 282.Bitsch F, Berger P, Nagels A, Falkenberg I, Straube B. Impaired right temporoparietal junction-hippocampus connectivity in schizophrenia and its relevance for generating representations of other minds. Schizophr Bull. (2018) 2018:sby132 10.1093/schbul/sby132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 283.Bodnar M, Hovington CL, Buchy L, Malla AK, Joober R, Lepage M. Cortical thinning in temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) in non-affective first-episode of psychosis patients with persistent negative symptoms. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e101372. 10.1371/journal.pone.0101372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 284.Wible CG. Hippocampal temporal-parietal junction interaction in the production of psychotic symptoms: a framework for understanding the schizophrenic syndrome. Front Human Neurosci. (2012) 6:180. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 285.Gerretsen P, Pothier DD, Falls C, Armstrong M, Balakumar T, Uchida H, et al. Vestibular stimulation improves insight into illness in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Psychiatry Res. (2017) 251:333–41. 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.02.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 286.Levine J, Toder D, Geller V, Kraus M, Gauchman T, Puterman M, et al. Beneficial effects of caloric vestibular stimulation on denial of illness and manic delusions in schizoaffective disorder: a case report. Brain Stimul. (2012) 5:267–73. 10.1016/j.brs.2011.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 287.Carmeli E. Anxiety in the elderly can be a vestibular problem. Front Public Health. (2015) 3:216. 10.3389/fpubh.2015.00216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 288.Monzani D, Casolari L, Guidetti G, Rigatelli M. Psychological distress and disability in patients with vertigo. J Psychosomatic Res. (2001) 50:319–23. 10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00208-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 289.Persoons P, Luyckx K, Desloovere C, Vandenberghe J, Fischler B. Anxiety and mood disorders in otorhinolaryngology outpatients presenting with dizziness: validation of the self-administered PRIME-MD Patient Health Questionnaire and epidemiology. Gen Hospital Psychiatry. (2003) 25:316–23. 10.1016/S0163-8343(03)00072-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 290.Yuan Q, Yu L, Shi D, Ke X, Zhang H. Anxiety and depression among patients with different types of vestibular peripheral vertigo. Medicine. (2015) 94:e453. 10.1097/MD.0000000000000453 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 291.Redfern MS, Furman JM, Jacob RG. Visually induced postural sway in anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disor. (2007) 21:704–16. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.09.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 292.Sklare DA, Stein MB, Pikus AM, Uhde TW. Dysequilibrium and audiovestibular function in panic disorder: symptom profiles and test findings. Am J Otol. (1990) 11:338–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 293.Bolmont B, Gangloff P, Vouriot A, Perrin PP. Mood states and anxiety influence abilities to maintain balance control in healthy human subjects. Neurosci Lett. (2002) 329:96–100. 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00578-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 294.Bush G, Luu P, Posner M. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. (2000) 4:215–22. 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 295.Dieterich M, Brandt T. Functional brain imaging of peripheral and central vestibular disorders. Brain. (2008) 131:2538–52. 10.1093/brain/awn042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 296.McKay R, Tamagni C, Palla A, Krummenacher P, Hegemann SC, Straumann D, et al. Vestibular stimulation attenuates unrealistic optimism. Cortex. (2013) 49:2272–5. 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 297.Preuss N, Hasler G, Mast F. Caloric vestibular stimulation modulates affective control and mood. Brain Stimul. (2014) 7:133–40. 10.1016/j.brs.2013.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 298.Blumberg HP, Stern E, Martinez D, Ricketts S, De Asis J, White T, et al. Increased anterior cingulate and caudate activity in bipolar mania. Biol Psychiatry. (2000) 48:1045–52. 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00962-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 299.Dodson M. Vestibular stimulation in mania: a case report. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2004) 168–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 300.Soza Ried AM, Aviles M. Asymmetries of vestibular dysfunction in major depression. Neuroscience. (2007) 144:128–34. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.09.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 301.Ramachandran VS, McGeoch PD, Williams L. Can vestibular caloric stimulation be used to treat dejerine–roussy syndrome? Med Hypotheses. (2007) 69:486–8. 10.1016/j.mehy.2006.12.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 302.McGeoch PD, Williams LE, Lee RR, Ramachandran VS. Behavioural evidence for vestibular stimulation as a treatment for central post-stroke pain. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2008) 79:1298–301. 10.1136/jnnp.2008.146738 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 303.Shaikh AG, Palla A, Marti S, Olasagasti I, Optican LM, Zee DS, et al. Role of cerebellum in motion perception and vestibulo-ocular reflex-similarities and disparities. Cerebellum. (2013) 12:97–107. 10.1007/s12311-012-0401-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 304.Blanke O, Perrig S, Thut G, Landis T, Seeck M. Simple and complex vestibular responses induced by electrical cortical stimulation of the parietal cortex in humans. J Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. (2000) 553–6. 10.1136/jnnp.69.4.553 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 305.Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S. Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2005) 6:435–48. 10.1038/nrn1684 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 306.Arshad Q, Nigmatullina Y, Roberts RE, Goga U, Pikovsky M, Khan S, et al. Perceived state of self during motion can differentially modulate numerical magnitude allocation. Eur J Neurosci. (2016) 44:2369–74. 10.1111/ejn.13335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 307.Hartmann M, Farkas R, Mast FW. Self-motion perception influences number processing: evidence from a parity task. Cogn Process. (2012) 13:189–92. 10.1007/s10339-012-0484-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 308.Ferrè ER, Vagnoni E, Haggard P. Galvanic vestibular stimulation influences randomness of number generation. Exp Brain Res. (2013) 224:233–41. 10.1007/s00221-012-3302-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 309.Loetscher T, Schwarz U, Schubiger M, Brugger P. Head turns bias the brain's internal random generator. Curr Biol. (2008) 18:R60–2. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 310.Zorzi M, Priftis K, Umiltà C. Brain damage: neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature. (2002) 417: 138–9. 10.1038/417138a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 311.Priftis K, Pitteri M, Meneghello F, Umiltà C, Zorzi M. Optokinetic stimulation modulates neglect for the number space: evidence from mental number interval bisection. Front Human Neurosci. (2012) 6:23. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 312.Salillas E, Granà A, Juncadella M, Rico I, Semenza C. Leftward motion restores number space in neglect. Cortex. (2009) 45:730–7. 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 313.Knutson B, Rick S, Wimmer GE, Prelec D, Loewenstein G. Neural predictors of purchases. Neuron. (2007) 147–56. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 314.Paulus MP, Frank LR. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation is critical for preference judgments. Neuroreport. (2003) 14:1311–5. 10.1097/01.wnr.0000078543.07662.02 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 315.Plassmann H, O'Doherty J, Rangel A. Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:9984–8. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2131-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 316.Arshad Q, Nigmatullina Y, Siddiqui S, Franka M, Mediratta S, Ramachandaran S, et al. Influence of biases in numerical magnitude allocation on human prosocial decision making. J Neurophysiol. (2017) 118:3007–13. 10.1152/jn.00372.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Neurology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES