Skip to main content
Cambridge Open Access logoLink to Cambridge Open Access
. 2018 Sep 17;49(7):1166–1173. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718002039

Genetic influences on eight psychiatric disorders based on family data of 4 408 646 full and half-siblings, and genetic data of 333 748 cases and controls

E Pettersson 1, P Lichtenstein 1, H Larsson 1,2, J Song 1; Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Working Group of the iPSYCH-Broad-PGC Consortium, Autism Spectrum Disorder Working Group of the iPSYCH-Broad-PGC Consortium, Bipolar Disorder Working Group of the PGC, Eating Disorder Working Group of the PGC, Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the PGC, Obsessive Compulsive Disorders and Tourette Syndrome Working Group of the PGC, Schizophrenia CLOZUK, Substance Use Disorder Working Group of the PGC, A Agrawal 3, A D Børglum 4,5,6, C M Bulik 1,7, M J Daly 8,9,10, L K Davis 11, D Demontis 4,5,6, H J Edenberg 12,13, J Grove 4,5,6,14, J Gelernter 15,16,17, B M Neale 8,9,10, A F Pardiñas 18, E Stahl 19, J T R Walters 18, R Walters 8,10, P F Sullivan 1,20, D Posthuma 21,22, T J C Polderman 21,
PMCID: PMC6421104  NIHMSID: NIHMS1003278  PMID: 30221610

Abstract

Background

Most studies underline the contribution of heritable factors for psychiatric disorders. However, heritability estimates depend on the population under study, diagnostic instruments, and study designs that each has its inherent assumptions, strengths, and biases. We aim to test the homogeneity in heritability estimates between two powerful, and state of the art study designs for eight psychiatric disorders.

Methods

We assessed heritability based on data of Swedish siblings (N = 4 408 646 full and maternal half-siblings), and based on summary data of eight samples with measured genotypes (N = 125 533 cases and 208 215 controls). All data were based on standard diagnostic criteria. Eight psychiatric disorders were studied: (1) alcohol dependence (AD), (2) anorexia nervosa, (3) attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), (4) autism spectrum disorder, (5) bipolar disorder, (6) major depressive disorder, (7) obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), and (8) schizophrenia.

Results

Heritability estimates from sibling data varied from 0.30 for Major Depression to 0.80 for ADHD. The estimates based on the measured genotypes were lower, ranging from 0.10 for AD to 0.28 for OCD, but were significant, and correlated positively (0.19) with national sibling-based estimates. When removing OCD from the data the correlation increased to 0.50.

Conclusions

Given the unique character of each study design, the convergent findings for these eight psychiatric conditions suggest that heritability estimates are robust across different methods. The findings also highlight large differences in genetic and environmental influences between psychiatric disorders, providing future directions for etiological psychiatric research.

Key words: ADHD, alcohol dependence, anorexia nervosa, autism spectrum disorders, bipolar disorder, genes, heritability, major depressive disorder, obsessive compulsive disorder, schizophrenia

Introduction

Psychiatric disorders place an enormous burden on medical resources and society in general (Eaton et al., 2008; Petrou et al., 2010). For most disorders, the causal factors are as yet largely unknown which limits treatment options considerably. A better understanding of the etiology of psychiatric disorders is a crucial step towards advancing treatment and intervention strategies.

Twin studies showed that genetic factors play an important role in the etiology of psychiatric traits. Heritability estimates (h2, i.e. the inherited contribution of genetic variance to trait variance) range from 35% for major depression to over 60% for schizophrenia (SCZ) (Polderman et al., 2015). The remaining variance is explained by non-genetic factors perhaps including non-identifiable environmental factors. Another method to derive estimates of genetic and environmental variance is the use of pedigree data (e.g. parents and children, siblings and half-siblings) from large national registers (Pettersson et al., 2016). Additionally, rapid methodological developments have recently advanced the use of summary data of genome-wide association studies (GWAS) in which heritability is inferred from the linkage disequilibrium scores (LDSC) of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNP) (i.e. the ability of a SNP to tag other genetic variants) (Bulik-Sullivan et al., 2015). Yet, it is recognized that these heritability estimates do not capture all genetic factors contributing to variance in the trait (such as rare genetic effects), and hence can be viewed as lower bound estimates.

While heritability is conceptualized as a single population parameter, estimates depend on the population under study, ascertainment, diagnostic instruments, and study design. Estimates may also change over time due to variations in diagnostic criteria (Zablotsky et al., 2015; Thomas et al., 2015), an increase in awareness and detection of psychiatric disorders (Van Naarden Braun et al., 2015), changes in the exposure to environmental factors (Rokholm et al., 2011), or changes in social situations (Kendler et al., 2000).

Using two different methods, this study capitalizes on the largest and most powerful data-sets to date, to estimate the heritability of eight psychiatric conditions: (1) alcohol dependence (AD), (2) anorexia nervosa (AN), (3) attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), (4) autism spectrum disorder (ASD), (5) bipolar disorder (BIP), (6) major depressive disorder (MDD), (7) obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), and (8) SCZ. First, we use a large Swedish national cohort (h2-national) that currently includes over 20 million full and maternal half-sibling pairs. Unlike most twin studies, the Swedish sibling sample uses clinical diagnoses derived from medical in- and out-patient treatment registers, instead of surveys. Second, we use summary data of eight large samples of subjects with measured SNPs (h2-SNP). The uniqueness is estimating heritability from very large samples, based on genetic similarities inferred from distantly related people. As in the h2-national design, case status in the h2-SNP design is based on diagnostic criteria.

Although each study design has its own strengths, they also have study-specific biases and assumptions (listed in Table 1). For instance, in the national register data that we use, not all affected individuals seek help or are correctly diagnosed. The sibling method also relies on certain assumptions, such as a 100% shared environment despite age differences between siblings, and despite the fact that half-siblings might live in two families (with the biological mother and with the biological father) and thus spend potentially less time together than full siblings (Moffitt et al., 2010; Pettersson et al., 2016). However, confounding with shared environmental factors is likely excluded in the h2-SNP design. Yet, in contrast to sibling studies that assume to capture all possible genetic effects, the h2-SNP analysis is based on genome-wide variation that is derived from a selection of common genetic variants only (Vinkhuyzen et al., 2013). By including a large number of observations from both study designs, our study is adequately powered to robustly estimate in each design the heritability, despite relying on different sets of assumptions and methodologies.

Table 1.

Strengths, limitations and assumptions of each study design in estimating heritability (h2) of psychiatric disorders

Study design Strengths Limitations Assumptions
National sibling design (h2–national) Implicitly includes effects of common and rare genetic effects No info on actual causal genetic variants Equal shared sibling environment, also for half siblings
Psychiatric status based on clinical diagnosis Needs large samples sizes (tens of thousands of cases) to be able to estimate heritability Random mating
Reflection of general population
No gene × environment interaction
No gene × environment correlation
SNP-based design (h2-SNP) Based on measured genetic variants Includes (a priori) tagged common genetic effects only Random mating
No confounding with shared environmental factors Needs large samples sizes (hundreds of thousands of cases and controls) to be able to extract small genetic effects Reflection of general population
Psychiatric status based on clinical diagnosis No gene × environment interaction
No gene × environment correlation

The aim of this study is to provide a test of the homogeneity in heritability estimates between family-based data (h2-national) and SNP-based data for eight psychiatric conditions. Our hypothesis is that h2-SNP is lower, but correlates positively with the family-based estimates. In addition, our design can illustrate the differences in etiology between the psychiatric conditions, guiding future directions for etiological research in psychiatry.

Method

National sibling cohort (h2-national)

Personal identification numbers unique to each individual in Sweden were used to create a national population-based cohort from which twins were excluded. Information was extracted from the National Patient Register, which includes all public psychiatric inpatient diagnoses in Sweden since 1973 and outpatient diagnoses since 2001, assigned by the attending physician with a non-hierarchical diagnostic structure in accord with ICD version 8 (1969–1986), 9 (1987–1996), or 10 (1997–present). The ICD codes for the eight disorders are presented in Table S1 in the Supplementary. We used the Multi-Generation Register to link individuals to their full and maternal half-siblings registered as living in Sweden since 1961 and born in Sweden since 1932. Only two siblings per family were included, starting with the oldest siblings in each family followed by the next oldest sibling, but only if born within 5 years of the first sibling to maximize the probability that they had experienced a similar rearing (i.e. shared) environment. If the two eldest were born more than 5 years apart, we proceeded to the second oldest sibling pair within the family, and so on. The final sample size of full- and maternal half-siblings varied by diagnosis. To ensure that the younger sibling in each pair had lived long enough to receive a potential diagnosis, pairs in which the younger sibling was younger than 5 (for ADHD and ASD), 10 (for AD, AN, MDD, and OCD), or 15 years old (BIP and SCZ) were excluded.

The sibling design assumes (a) that full siblings share an average of 50% of additive genetic effects and 25% of non-additive genetic effects, (b) that maternal half-siblings share an average of 25% of additive genetic effects and 0% of non-additive genetic effects, and (c) that shared environmental effects are 100% shared between both full and maternal half-siblings, and that (d) non-shared environmental effects are unique to each individual. By comparing the observed tetrachoric correlations between the binary diagnoses for full and maternal half-siblings, we estimated the contribution of genetic variance (h2-national), and shared and non-shared environmental variance. The analyses were carried out in Mplus (Muthén and Muthén, 1998) using the mean- and variance-adjusted unweighted least squares estimator. We regressed out the effects of sex and age from all diagnoses. For ADHD and ASD, we limited the birth year to 1990 and beyond because these diagnoses only existed in ICD 9 and 10.

Genetic data (h2-SNP)

The h2-SNP estimates were based on the most recent available data for all eight disorders in the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC)(Psychiatric GWAS Consortium Coordinating Committee et al., 2009; Psychiatric GWAS Consortium Steering Committee, 2009) (see Study cohort details in the Supplementary). The LDSC approach was used to estimate the h2-SNP (Bulik-Sullivan et al., 2015). In brief, this method is based on the LDSC of a SNP, which reflects its ability to tag other SNPs. The more SNPs are tagged, the higher the probability that this represents a polygenic signal instead. Therefore, by taking LD into account this method is able to distinguish spurious associations due to population stratification from the true polygenic signal. The LDSC method is also robust to confounding due to shared environmental effects, and is very efficient as it can be applied to GWAS summary statistics (Evans et al., 2018). Of note, GWAS usually include millions of common variants, but not rare variants. The heritability (h2-SNP), adjusted for the prevalence of the disorder, is inferred from the slope of the regression.

Analyses

Differences in heritability estimates were tested using d = h12 − h22, Inline graphic. The ratio Z = d/s.e.(d) gives a test of the null hypothesis that the difference d is zero, by comparing the value of Z to the standard normal distribution(Altman and Bland, 2003). A significance threshold of 0.05 was Bonferroni corrected to accommodate multiple testing (i.e. a correction for eight tests).

Results

Estimates of shared environmental effects were non-significant in the sibling analyses. The heritability estimates (Fig. 1) showed significant differences between the h2-national estimates and the h2-SNP estimates. The latter were significantly lower (corrected p < 0.02), except for AN, BIP, and OCD where the h2-SNP estimates did not significantly differ from h2-national (corrected p > 0.30). However, these differences should be interpreted with caution as due to the somewhat smaller samples sizes of these particular disorders, the standard errors (s.e.) of h2-national were relatively wide for AN, BIP, and OCD. Of note, the s.e. is in general sensitive to sample size, and in particular for h2-national because the full and half-sibling groups only differ by 0.25 in genetic relatedness. Additionally, the nature of summary data of large consortium designs implies that included samples have been genotyped on different platforms and chips, potentially increasing the s.e. of h2-SNP.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

h2-national and h2-SNP estimates ordered from low to high based on h2-national. Note: Error bars represent standard errors. AD, alcohol dependence; ADHD, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder; AN, anorexia nervosa; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; BIP, bipolar disorder; MDD, major depressive disorder; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; SCZ, schizophrenia.

Heritability estimates from the two designs correlated positively (r 0.19). However, this correlation was mainly driven by OCD that showed the highest h2-SNP and lowest h2-national; when removing OCD this correlation increased to 0.50. The high h2-SNP is probably due to the fact that the OCD sample is heavily ascertained from highly multiplex families and early age of onset cases, and consists thus of the most severe and genetically loaded cases.

Tables S2 and S3 in the Supplementary show detailed sample characteristics of each study design.

Discussion

We estimated the heritability of eight psychiatric conditions using two different study designs: a national sibling design, and a SNP-based design. Estimates derived from the two study designs consistently show that all disorders are moderate to highly heritable but also showed large differences between disorders. The correlation between the family-based and SNP-based estimates was positive (0.50 when leaving out OCD) suggesting that a higher family-based heritability is associated with a larger (aggregated) effect of SNPs. This supports the hypothesis that, apart from rare variants, common genetic variants play an important role in psychiatric disorders, and thus confirms the polygenic nature of these complex traits (Visscher et al., 2012).

The heritability estimates based on the large national sibling study (h2-national) were remarkably similar to previous twin studies of psychiatric traits (Polderman et al., 2015), despite different assessment strategies, with twin studies being survey-based, and as such based on psychiatric trait measures, and the national sibling study based on clinical diagnoses. This might suggest that heritability estimates are robust across different diagnostic tools and measures. It is also in line with studies that reported high genetic correlations between survey-based psychiatric traits and clinical diagnoses, e.g. for ASD (Colvert et al., 2015), ADHD (Lubke et al., 2009), and psychosis (Zavos et al., 2014), suggesting an overlap in genetic factors between psychiatric traits as measured in the general population and clinical disorders.

Heritability estimates based on SNP data (h2-SNP) were, as expected, lower than the family-based designs (Yang et al., 2017). An obvious explanation for these differences is that h2-SNP is based on measured common (and not rare) genotypes, whereas the other design is based on familial relationships and hence includes estimates of genetic factors shared by relatives that are rare in populations. The largest differences between the family-based and h2-SNP estimates were observed for the neurodevelopmental traits ADHD and ASD, and for SCZ. Indeed, rare variant risk effects have been reported for ADHD, ASD, and SCZ (Williams et al., 2010; Hiroi et al., 2013; Sanders et al., 2015), although a recent well-powered study on SCZ showed that the explained variance due to rare variants was about 20% of the total explained variance (0.85% for rare variants v. 3.4% for common variants) (CNV and Schizophrenia Working Groups of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium & Psychosis Endophenotypes International Consortium, 2017).

Another explanation for the discrepancy between family-based and SNP-based estimates for ADHD, ASD, and SCZ could be the presence of non-additive effects resulting in overestimates of the narrow-sense heritability in the family-based design, when non-additive influences are removed from the statistical models. Non-additive factors contributing to trait variance have indeed been reported for ADHD (Rietveld et al., 2004). Lower h2-SNP might also indicate the presence of disorder heterogeneity, that is, a disorder is viewed as a single disorder but actually being a combination of disorder dimensions that has biologically distinct causal factors. As this affects GWAS, and hence h2-SNP, most substantially, this explains the lower h2-SNP but is also informative about potentially underlying disorder mechanisms (Wray and Maier, 2014). For ADHD and ASD specifically, the inclusion of trio data (i.e. case-pseudo control design) may have underestimated h2-SNP due to an increased polygenic burden on the un-transmitted chromosomes (Peyrot et al., 2016a), although the trio samples were small compared with the much larger case-control samples.

In general, the nature of the large consortium designs on which SNP-based heritability is based likely increases the standard error, all of which will impact on h2-SNP. The h2-SNP of the eight psychiatric traits as observed in the current study should, therefore, be considered as lower-bound estimates of SNP-heritability. Interestingly, the smallest difference in family-based and SNP-based estimates between both designs was for OCD (respectively, 0.38% v. 0.28%). As mentioned previously, the SNP-based estimate of OCD was based on a clinical sample of most severe and therefore probably most genetically loaded cases. Yet, standard errors for both the family-based and SNP-based estimate of OCD were relatively large, so these findings should be interpreted with caution. In a similar vein, one should not stretch the interpretation of the SNP based AD estimate as it derives from one of the smaller genetic samples.

The family-based estimates showed substantial differences in the relative contributions of genes and environment across the eight psychiatric conditions: Heritability estimates for AD, AN, MDD, and OCD were relatively low, ranging from 30 to 41%. However, the prevalence of AN in the family data was low and hence, statistical power was limited, as illustrated by the large standard error in these data. Still, the estimate of 41% for AN in the family data confirms heritability estimates based on twin studies (Polderman et al., 2015), also in clinical samples (Mazzeo et al., 2009). Heritability estimates of ADHD, ASD, BIP, and SCZ showed the highest narrow-sense heritability estimates between 51 and 80%. With the dramatic increase in sample sizes, the recent endeavors to identify genes that could explain the heritability of psychiatric disorders is becoming more successful. For instance, 108 significantly associated genetic loci were identified for SCZ in a sample of almost 37 000 cases and over 113 000 controls (Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2014). Follow-up analyses on biological pathways revealed that some of the associated genes play an important role in the immune system. However, high heritability not necessarily implies that genetic associations are easy to detect. For example, a recent study in 16 539 ASD cases and over 150 000 controls resulted in only one associated genetic locus (Warrier et al., 2017). Similarly, for ADHD only very recently the first 12 associated genetic loci have been published (Demontis et al., 2017), illustrating that gene identification in the psychiatric domain is a long and complex avenue.

The fact that heritability estimates show variation across disorders and is never estimated >90% indicates that, discounting potential measurement error, stochasticity, and non-definable environmental factors, definable environmental factors might play an important role in the etiology of psychiatric disorders. In particular, for AD, AN, MDD, and OCD with heritability estimates <50%, environmental etiological research might elucidate crucial pathways that significantly increase the risk for these traits. In addition, gene by environment interaction will likely play a role in the development of psychiatric disorders (Uher and Zwicker, 2017). For instance, one study showed that the effect of a genetic risk for MDD increased in individuals with childhood trauma (Peyrot et al., 2014). In other words, given a genetic vulnerability, exposure to certain environmental risk factors will increase the risk for disorder development. However, two recent larger studies (Mullins et al., 2016; Peyrot et al., 2017) showed no interaction effect. All in all, the empirical evidence for gene by environment interaction in psychiatric disorders is as yet limited (Wray and Maier, 2014) but increasing sample sizes and careful assessments of environmental risk factors are crucial in future research aiming to elucidate causal routes in psychiatric disorders.

Limitations

Our study has potential limitations. First, the concept of shared environment is not straightforward in the sibling design. Siblings are born at different points in time, do not share the prenatal environment, and are born into different family structures (e.g. the first child born to young parents v. the second child born to older parents who already have one child). We aimed to minimize potential time effects by limiting age differences between siblings to a maximum of 5 years. Moreover, additional analyses comparing siblings born within 1–2 v. 4–5 years apart showed very similar results (data not shown). Second, in the national sibling design, we assume that the shared environment of full and half siblings is the same. This assumption seems correct: A recent study showed that the vast majority of both Swedish full and maternal half-siblings tend to live together throughout childhood (Pettersson et al., 2016). Third, the inclusion age limit for the different disorders in the national sibling design was relatively young (e.g. minimum age of 10 years old for AD, AN, MDD, and OCD) to strike a balance between power on the one hand, and clinical generalizability on the other. However, we cannot rule out that children of that age develop such a disorder later in life. Yet, two additional sets of sensitivity analyses in which only older subjects were included, showed very similar results (online Tables A1 and A2). Fourth, the national sibling cohort lacked information from primary care, which might result in false negatives, in particular, for disorders from the internalizing spectrum and drug abuse (Sundquist et al., 2017). However, this source of bias probably has limited influence on the heritability estimates as it is unlikely to differentially impact full- v. maternal half-siblings. Nevertheless, failure to include information from primary care decreases power, and limits the generalizability of the study results to the more severe forms of mental health problems that warrant attention by outpatient specialists and inpatient services. Fifth, we compare family-based h2 estimates, that were derived from a Swedish sample only, with h2-SNP results that were based on a variety of cohorts. Although these are all of the European descent, there might be heterogeneity in h2-SNP between cohorts, which makes the comparison with Swedish data less precise. We, therefore, examined heterogeneity in estimates of SCZ, as for this disorder a large Swedish sample contributed to the GWAS from which the h2-SNP was derived (Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, 2014). Supplementary Table S7 of this study clearly shows that the h2-SNP based on the Swedish cohort equals h2-SNP estimates from other samples of similar size (i.e. Germany, UK), suggesting that the Swedish data are fairly comparable with other data from European descent.

Lastly, both designs assume random mating but a large-scale study in psychiatric populations (Nordsletten et al., 2016) observed substantial non-random mating within and across disorders. A recent study, however, concluded that non-random mating has only a very modest effect on SNP-based heritability estimates in psychiatric traits (Peyrot et al., 2016b).

Conclusion

In sum, this study presents a converging picture of the etiology of eight psychiatric disorders. SNP-based estimates were as expected lower but correlated with the family-based estimates. Additionally, the findings highlight large differences in genetic and environmental influences between psychiatric disorders. In contrast to ASD, ADHD, BIP, and SCZ, where genetic influences are most important, non-genetic influences play a large role in AD, AN, MDD, and OCD.

Acknowledgements

Dr H Larsson acknowledges financial support through the Swedish Initiative for Research on Microdata in the Social And Medical Sciences (SIMSAM) framework Grant no. 340-2013-5867. Dr E Petterson acknowledges the Swedish Society for Medical Research. Dr CM Bulik acknowledges the Klarman Family Foundation, Wellcome Trust WT 088827/Z/09. The work of Dr Sullivan and the PGC MDD group was supported by NIMH U01 MH109528. Dr Walters and Dr Pardinas are supported by the UK Medical Research Council (Grants MR/L010305/1 and MR/L011794/1). Dr Børglum and the iPSYCH team acknowledge funding from The Lundbeck Foundation (grant no R102-A9118 and R155-2014-1724), the Stanley Medical Research Institute, an Advanced Grant from the European Research Council (project no: 294838), and grants from Aarhus University to the iSEQ and CIRRAU centers. Dr D Posthuma acknowledges funding from The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO VICI 453-14-005).

The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Appendices

Sensitivity analyses

We conducted two sets of sensitivity analyses to examine whether the pre-specified age of the second sibling might have been too young. First, we conducted the analyses in the adult population only by including pairs in which the youngest sibling was 20 years old or older. Results are displayed in Table A1, and were very similar for all disorders except for OCD, for which the heritability estimate decreased from 0.38 to 0.24. However, due to a lack of maternal half-sibling cases, the standard error about this estimate was 0.20, indicating a lack of precision such that the two estimates did not differ significantly.

Second, we plotted the distributions of the age at first diagnosis for each disorder. We then re-ran the analyses but only included pairs where the younger sibling was at least as old as the median age of diagnosis. This way, the youngest siblings were more likely to have lived through the risk period of onset. Results are displayed in Table A2, and show that the estimates are remarkably similar to the original analyses (although the standard errors are larger).

In sum, the heritability estimates remained very similar regardless of whether we relied on our original age cutoffs, or on adults or the median age of onset.

Table A1.

H2 of psychiatric diagnoses based on ICD 8, 9, and 10 in adults of >19 years

Frequencies
Full siblings Maternal half-siblings Tetrachoric correlation
Cases Controls Prevalence (%) Cases Controls Prevalence (%) Full sibs Maternal half-sibs h2
Alcohol dependence 87 773 3 043 417 2.80 10 589 157 287 6.31 0.253 (0.004) 0.142 (0.012) 0.444 (0.052)
Anorexia nervosa 8067 3 123 123 0.26 657 167 219 0.39 0.207 (0.020) 0.120 (0.076) 0.349 (0.316)
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder 5003 209 959 2.33 895 8927 9.11 0.445 (0.014) 0.175 (0.040) 0.852 (0.171)
Autism spectrum disorder 2430 212 532 1.13 237 9585 2.41 0.388 (0.022) 0.055 (0.098) 0.665 (0.402)
Bipolar disorder 18 655 3 112 535 0.60 1619 166 257 0.96 0.303 (0.009) 0.167 (0.038) 0.543 (0.158)
Major depressive disorder 121 015 3 010 175 3.86 12 377 155 499 7.37 0.194 (0.004) 0.117 (0.011) 0.307 (0.048)
Obsessive compulsive disorder 11 553 3 119 637 0.37 1003 166 873 0.60 0.228 (0.014) 0.167 (0.048) 0.244 (0.198)
Schizophrenia 15 178 3 116 012 0.48 1106 166 770 0.66 0.330 (0.010) 0.050 (0.058) 0.569 (0.049)

Note. Standard errors are presented in parentheses.

Sex and age was regressed out from all disorders.

For ADHD and Autism, birth year was limited to 1990 or higher.

Youngest sibling born at age 20 or later.

Table A2.

H2 of psychiatric diagnoses based on ICD 8, 9 , and 10, with the younger sibling being at least as old as the median age of diagnosis

Frequencies
Full siblings Maternal half-siblings Tetrachoric correlation
Cases Controls Prevalence (%) Cases Controls Prevalence (%) Median age of disorder onset Full sibs Maternal half sibs h2
Schizophrenia 14 188 2 335 292 0.60 967 112 193 0.85 34 0.326 (0.010) 0.053 (0.059) 0.564 (0.050)
Bipolar 14 496 2 184 334 0.66 998 103 240 0.96 37 0.297 (0.011) 0.166 (0.048) 0.525 (0.197)
ADHD 10 452 408 760 2.49 2308 21 324 9.77 16 0.455 (0.009) 0.257 (0.024) 0.807 (0.104)
Autism 4900 414 312 1.17 653 22 979 2.76 16 0.410 (0.015) 0.223 (0.049) 0.748 (0.204)
MDD 82 840 2 063 646 3.86 6642 94 294 6.58 38 0.169 (0.004) 0.108 (0.016) 0.242 (0.065)
Anorexia 8706 3 277 732 0.26 700 177 898 0.39 16 0.211 (0.019) 0.115 (0.075) 0.375 (0.309)
OCD 9478 2 751 410 0.34 781 139 567 0.56 26 0.222 (0.016) 0.155 (0.053) 0.267 (0.220)
SUDS & Alcohol 66 035 2 080 451 3.08 6481 94 455 6.42 38 0.265 (0.005) 0 .177 (0.015) 0.351 (0.065)

Note. Standard errors are presented in parentheses.

Sex and age were regressed out from all disorders.

For ADHD and autism, birth year was limited to 1990 or higher.

Youngest sibling being at least as old as disorder median age of onset or later.

Conflict of interest disclosure

Dr H Larsson has served as a speaker for Eli-Lilly and Shire and has received research grants from Shire; all outside the submitted work. Dr P Lichtenstein has served as a speaker for Medice. Dr CM Bulik is grant recipient and consultant to Shire, and Consultant to Ironshore. Dr PF Sullivan is on the Scientific Advisory Board for Pfizer. Dr B Neale is on the Scientific Advisory Board for Deep Genomics.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718002039.

S0033291718002039sup.zip (444.3KB, zip)

click here to view supplementary material

References

  1. Altman DG and Bland JM (2003) Interaction revisited: the difference between two estimates. BMJ (Clinical research ed.) 326, 219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bulik-Sullivan BK, Loh P-R, Finucane HK, Ripke S, Yang J, Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, Patterson N, Daly MJ, Price AL and Neale BM (2015) LD score regression distinguishes confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies. Nature Genetics 47, 291–295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. CNV and Schizophrenia Working Groups of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, Psychosis Endophenotypes International Consortium (2017) Contribution of copy number variants to schizophrenia from a genome-wide study of 41 321 subjects. Nature Genetics 49, 27–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Colvert E, Tick B, McEwen F, Stewart C, Curran SR, Woodhouse E, Gillan N, Hallett V, Lietz S, Garnett T, Ronald A, Plomin R, Rijsdijk F, Happé F and Bolton P (2015) Heritability of autism spectrum disorder in a UK population-based twin sample. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 415–423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Demontis D, Walters RK, Martin J, Mattheisen M, Als TD, Agerbo E, Belliveau R, Bybjerg-Grauholm J, Bækved-Hansen M, Cerrato F, Chambert K, Churchhouse C, Dumont A, Eriksson N, Gandal M, Goldstein J, Grove J, Hansen CS, Hauberg M, Hollegaard M, Howrigan DP, Huang H, Maller J, Martin AR, Moran J, Pallesen J, Palmer DS, Pedersen CB, Pedersen MG, Poterba T, Poulsen JB, Ripke S, Robinson EB, Satterstrom FK, Stevens C, Turley P, Won H, Con -ADHD Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, Lifecourse & -Early, Epidemiology (EAGLE) G, Team −23 and Me Research, Andreassen OA, Burton C, Boomsma D, Cormand B, Dalsgaard S, Franke B, Gelernter J, Geschwind D, Hakonarson H, Haavik J, Kranzler H, Kuntsi J, Langley K, Lesch K-P, Middeldorp C, Reif A, Rohde LA, Roussos P, Schachar R, Sklar P, Sonuga-Barke E, Sullivan PF, Thapar A, Tung J, Waldman I, Nordentoft M, Hougaard DM, Werge T, Mors O, Mortensen PB, Daly MJ, Faraone SV, Børglum AD and Neale BM (2017) Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for ADHD. Submitted for publication, bioRxiv. 2017; 14558:1–43. [Google Scholar]
  6. Eaton WW, Martins SS, Nestadt G, Bienvenu OJ, Clarke D and Alexandre P (2008) The burden of mental disorders. Epidemiologic Reviews 30, 1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Evans LM, Tahmasbi R, Vrieze SI, Abecasis GR, Das S, Gazal S, Bjelland DW, de Candia TR, Haplotype Reference Consortium, Goddard ME, Neale BM, Yang J, Visscher PM and Keller MC (2018) Comparison of methods that use whole genome data to estimate the heritability and genetic architecture of complex traits. Nature Genetics 50, 737–745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Hiroi N, Takahashi T, Hishimoto A, Izumi T, Boku S and Hiramoto T (2013) Copy number variation at 22q11.2: from rare variants to common mechanisms of developmental neuropsychiatric disorders. Molecular Psychiatry 18, 1153–1165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Kendler KS, Thornton LM and Pedersen NL (2000) Tobacco consumption in Swedish twins reared apart and reared together. Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 886–892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Lubke GH, Hudziak JJ, Derks EM, van Bijsterveldt TCEM and Boomsma DI (2009) Maternal ratings of attention problems in ADHD: evidence for the existence of a continuum. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 48, 1085–1093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Mazzeo SE, Mitchell KS, Bulik CM, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Kendler KS and Neale MC (2009) Assessing the heritability of anorexia nervosa symptoms using a marginal maximal likelihood approach. Psychological Medicine 39, 463–473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Kokaua J, Milne BJ, Polanczyk G and Poulton R (2010) How common are common mental disorders? Evidence that lifetime prevalence rates are doubled by prospective versus retrospective ascertainment. Psychological Medicine 40, 899–909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Mullins N, Power RA, Fisher HL, Hanscombe KB, Euesden J, Iniesta R, Levinson DF, Weissman MM, Potash JB, Shi J, Uher R, Cohen-Woods S, Rivera M, Jones L, Jones I, Craddock N, Owen MJ, Korszun A, Craig IW, Farmer AE, McGuffin P, Breen G and Lewis CM (2016) Polygenic interactions with environmental adversity in the aetiology of major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine 46, 759–770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Muthén L.K. and Muthén B.O. (1998) Mplus User's Guide, 7th Edn Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
  15. Nordsletten AE, Larsson H, Crowley JJ, Almqvist C, Lichtenstein P and Mataix-Cols D (2016) Patterns of nonrandom mating within and across 11 major psychiatric disorders. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 354–361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Petrou S, Johnson S, Wolke D, Hollis C, Kochhar P and Marlow N (2010) Economic costs and preference-based health-related quality of life outcomes associated with childhood psychiatric disorders. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science 197, 395–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Pettersson E, Larsson H and Lichtenstein P (2016) Common psychiatric disorders share the same genetic origin: a multivariate sibling study of the Swedish population. Molecular Psychiatry 21, 717–721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Peyrot WJ, Milaneschi Y, Abdellaoui A, Sullivan PF, Hottenga JJ, Boomsma DI and Penninx BWJH (2014) Effect of polygenic risk scores on depression in childhood trauma. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science 205, 113–119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Peyrot WJ, Boomsma DI, Penninx BWJH and Wray NR (2016a). Disease and polygenic architecture: avoid trio design and appropriately account for unscreened control subjects for common disease. American Journal of Human Genetics 98, 382–391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Peyrot WJ, Robinson MR, Penninx BWJH and Wray NR (2016b). Exploring boundaries for the genetic consequences of assortative mating for psychiatric traits. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 1189–1195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Peyrot WJ, Van der Auwera S, Milaneschi Y, Dolan CV, Madden PAF, Sullivan PF, Strohmaier J, Ripke S, Rietschel M, Nivard MG, Mullins N, Montgomery GW, Henders AK, Heat AC, Fisher HL, Dunn EC, Byrne EM, Air TA, Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, Baune BT, Breen G, Levinson DF, Lewis CM, Martin NG, Nelson EN, Boomsma DI, Grabe HJ, Wray NR and Penninx BWJH (2017) Does childhood trauma moderate polygenic risk for depression? A meta-analysis of 5765 subjects from the psychiatric genomics consortium. Biological Psychiatry 84, 138–147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Polderman TJC, Benyamin B, de Leeuw CA, Sullivan PF, van Bochoven A, Visscher PM and Posthuma D (2015) Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics 47, 702–709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Psychiatric GWAS Consortium Coordinating Committee, Cichon S, Craddock N, Daly M, Faraone SV, Gejman PV, Kelsoe J, Lehner T, Levinson DF, Moran A, Sklar P and Sullivan PF (2009) Genomewide association studies: history, rationale, and prospects for psychiatric disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 540–556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Psychiatric GWAS Consortium Steering Committee (2009) A framework for interpreting genome-wide association studies of psychiatric disorders. Molecular Psychiatry 14, 10–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Rietveld MJH, Hudziak JJ, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt CEM and Boomsma DI (2004) Heritability of attention problems in children: longitudinal results from a study of twins, age 3 to 12. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 45, 577–588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Rokholm B, Silventoinen K, Tynelius P, Gamborg M, Sørensen TIA and Rasmussen F (2011) Increasing genetic variance of body mass index during the Swedish obesity epidemic. PloS ONE 6, e27135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Sanders SJ, He X, Willsey AJ, Ercan-Sencicek AG, Samocha KE, Cicek AE, Murtha MT, Bal VH, Bishop SL, Dong S, Goldberg AP, Jinlu C, Keaney JF, Klei L, Mandell JD, Moreno-De-Luca D, Poultney CS, Robinson EB, Smith L, Solli-Nowlan T, Su MY, Teran NA, Walker MF, Werling DM, Beaudet AL, Cantor RM, Fombonne E, Geschwind DH, Grice DE, Lord C, Lowe JK, Mane SM, Martin DM, Morrow EM, Talkowski ME, Sutcliffe JS, Walsh CA, Yu TW, Autism Sequencing Consortium, Ledbetter DH, Martin CL, Cook EH, Buxbaum JD, Daly MJ, Devlin B, Roeder K and State MW (2015) Insights into autism spectrum disorder genomic architecture and biology from 71 risk loci. Neuron 87, 1215–1233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (2014) Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature 511, 421–427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Sundquist J, Ohlsson H, Sundquist K and Kendler KS (2017) Common adult psychiatric disorders in Swedish primary care where most mental health patients are treated. BMC Psychiatry 17, 235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Thomas R, Sanders S, Doust J, Beller E and Glasziou P (2015) Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics 135, e994–e1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Uher R and Zwicker A (2017) Etiology in psychiatry: embracing the reality of poly-gene-environmental causation of mental illness. World Psychiatry: Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA) 16, 121–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Van Naarden Braun K, Christensen D, Doernberg N, Schieve L, Rice C, Wiggins L, Schendel D and Yeargin-Allsopp M (2015) Trends in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder, cerebral palsy, hearing loss, intellectual disability, and vision impairment, metropolitan Atlanta, 1991-2010. PloS ONE 10, e0124120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Vinkhuyzen AAE, Wray NR, Yang J, Goddard ME and Visscher PM (2013) Estimation and partition of heritability in human populations using whole-genome analysis methods. Annual Review of Genetics 47, 75–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Visscher PM, Goddard ME, Derks EM and Wray NR (2012) Evidence-based psychiatric genetics, AKA the false dichotomy between common and rare variant hypotheses. Molecular Psychiatry 17, 474–485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Warrier V, Grasby KL, Uzefovsky F, Toro R, Smith P, Chakrabarti B, Khadake J, Mawbey-Adamson E, Litterman N, Hottenga J-J, Lubke G, Boomsma DI, Martin NG, Hatemi PK, Medland SE, Hinds DA, Bourgeron T and Baron-Cohen S (2017) Genome-wide meta-analysis of cognitive empathy: heritability, and correlates with sex, neuropsychiatric conditions and cognition. Molecular Psychiatry 23, 1402–1409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Williams NM, Zaharieva I, Martin A, Langley K, Mantripragada K, Fossdal R, Stefansson H, Stefansson K, Magnusson P, Gudmundsson OO, Gustafsson O, Holmans P, Owen MJ, O'Donovan M and Thapar A (2010) Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet 376, 1401–1408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Wray NR and Maier R (2014) Genetic basis of Complex genetic disease: the contribution of disease heterogeneity to missing heritability. Current Epidemiology Reports 1, 220–227. [Google Scholar]
  38. Yang J, Zeng J, Goddard ME, Wray NR and Visscher PM (2017) Concepts, estimation and interpretation of SNP-based heritability. Nature Genetics 49, 1304–1310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Zablotsky B, Black LI, Maenner MJ, Schieve LA and Blumberg SJ (2015) Estimated prevalence of autism and other developmental disabilities following questionnaire changes in the 2014 national health interview survey. National Health Statistics Reports 13, 1–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Zavos HMS, Freeman D, Haworth CMA, McGuire P, Plomin R, Cardno AG and Ronald A (2014) Consistent etiology of severe, frequent psychotic experiences and milder, less frequent manifestations: a twin study of specific psychotic experiences in adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 1049–1057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718002039.

S0033291718002039sup.zip (444.3KB, zip)

click here to view supplementary material


Articles from Psychological Medicine are provided here courtesy of Cambridge University Press

RESOURCES