Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Nutrition. 2018 Nov 24;61:77–83. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2018.11.002

Seafood consumption, toenail mercury and selenium with cognitive function among American adults -- 25 years of follow-up

Xuanxia Mao 1,2,, Cheng Chen 3,, Pengcheng Xun 3, Martha Daviglus 4, Lyn M Steffen 5, David R Jacobs Jr 5, Linda Van Horn 6,7, Stephen Sidney 7, Na Zhu 8, Ka He 3
PMCID: PMC6422693  NIHMSID: NIHMS1514386  PMID: 30703573

Abstract

Objectives:

To examine the longitudinal association between seafood as well as long-chain omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCω−3PUFA) intake and cognitive function and to explore the possible effect modifications by mercury (Hg) and selenium (Se) levels.

Methods:

Participants (n=3,231) from the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults Study underwent baseline examination and were re-examined in 8 follow-ups. Diet was assessed at baseline, exam years 7 and 20. Toenail Hg and Se were measured at exam year 2. Cognitive function was measured at exam year 25 using 3 tests: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), and the Stroop Test. The general linear regression model was used to examine cumulative average intakes of LCω−3PUFA and seafood in relation to the cognitive test scores; and to explore the possible effect modifications by Hg and Se.

Results:

LCω−3PUFA intake was significantly associated with better performance in the DSST test [quintile 5 vs. quintile 1; mean difference = 1.74; 95% CI = (0.19, 3.29); P for trend = 0.048], but not in the RAVLT and Stroop tests. Similar results were observed for intakes of EPA, DHA, and non-fried seafood. The observed associations were more pronounced in participants with body mass index ≥25 kg/m2, but not materially modified by toenail Hg or Se.

Conclusion:

This longitudinal study supports the hypothesis that LCω−3PUFA or non-fried seafood intake is associated with better cognitive performance in psychomotor speed among American adults, especially in overweight and obese individuals.

Keywords: LCω−3PUFA, seafood consumption, Hg, Se, cognitive function

INTRODUCTION

Cognitive dysfunction is a common outcome of aging that may lead to dementia, which is an irreversible disorder. Since cognitive decline begins in middle age and progresses slowly during the decades before a diagnosis of clinical dementia [1], the prevention of cognitive decline at early stages is of great medical and economic importance.

Long-chain omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCω−3PUFA) from seafood may play important roles in maintaining cognitive function through the reduction of inflammation [2]. However, epidemiological data are inconsistent and non-conclusive. Two systematic reviews among randomized clinical trials (RCT) concluded that no sufficient evidence supports the beneficial effects of LCω−3PUFA supplementation on cognitive decline. But, most of the RCTs included in the systematic reviews had small sample sizes (<100 participants) with short intervention durations (<5 months) [3, 4]. On the other hand, observational studies supported benefits of long-term intakes of LCω−3PUFA and seafood on age-related cognitive decline [5, 6]. Of note, most of the previous studies focused on older adults and those of young adults are limited. Since cognition declines with aging and young adults are under the risk development stage, following-up a cohort of young adults to midlife or later will provide insight on the natural history or etiology of cognitive decline.

When examining LCω−3PUFA intake and cognitive decline, mercury (Hg) and selenium (Se) are two elements should be considered since they often coexist with LCω−3PUFA in seafood. Hg has potential neurotoxicity [7], while Se may be neuroprotective due to its anti-oxidative and anti-inflammatory properties [8] or by modulating the neurotoxicity effect of Hg [9]. It has been hypothesized that the benefit of LCω−3PUFA or seafood on cognitive function may be more pronounced in the setting of high Se and low Hg concentrations. However, no study has examined the potential three-way interaction.

We therefore analyzed the data from a cohort of young adults with 25 years of follow-up to prospectively examine the long-term associations between LCω−3PUFA intake and seafood consumption with cognitive performance. We hypothesize that higher intakes of LCω−3PUFA and seafood are associated with better cognitive function, especially in the setting of high Se and low Hg levels.

METHODS

Study population

The Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study is a multicenter, ongoing longitudinal cohort study that recruited 5,115 biracial male and female young adults initially aged 18–30 years in 1985–1986 from 4 study centers. The participants underwent baseline examination and were re-examined in 8 follow-ups at exam years 2, 5, 7, 10, 15, 20, 25, and 30. The detailed design and methods of the CARDIA project are published elsewhere [10].

Among the 3,499 participants who remained at exam year 25, 3,316 had information available on cognitive function measurements. Of them, 85 participants were sequentially excluded because of missing data on baseline LCω−3PUFA or seafood (n=2), report of an extreme energy intake (<600 or >6000 kcal/day for women; <800 or >8000 kcal/day for men) (n=13), and experience of a stroke event during the follow-up (n=70). Thus, the final database included 3,231 participants. Written informed consent was provided by all of the participants. The study design, data collection and analyses were approved by the institutional review boards of the CARDIA participating institutions.

Seafood consumption and LCω−3PUFA intake

Dietary intake information was collected using the interviewer-administered CARDIA Diet History questionnaire at baseline, exam years 7 and 20. The CARDIA questionnaire has been evaluated and discussed elsewhere [11, 12]. In brief, participants were asked about their diet habits in the prior 30 days, including frequency, amount and food preparation methods. Since preparation method, particularly frying, may substantially alter the fatty acids content of a seafood meal [13], seafood consumption was divided into fried and non-fried seafood intake groups. Fried seafood consumption included intakes of fried fish and fried shellfish from commercial and fast food. Non-fried seafood consumption was the sum of fresh fish, smoked fish, lean fish, and shellfish intakes. Because the distribution of fried seafood consumption was extremely skewed and relatively narrow, it was considered as a covariate when examining non-fried seafood, but not as an exposure of interest. Nutrient intake was estimated using an adaptively updated nutrient database from the Nutrition Coordinating Center at the University of Minnesota. Information on supplementation use was also collected. In this study, LCω−3PUFA intake was defined as the sum of eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA), docosapentaenoic acid (DPA), and docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) intakes from diet and supplementation. Because of the relatively small amount and the narrow distribution, DPA was not analyzed as a separate exposure in the analysis. To reduce the measurement errors caused by within-person variation and to best represent the long-term dietary intakes, cumulative average daily intakes of LCω−3PUFA and seafood, which were calculated by averaging the corresponding measurements at baseline and exam years 7 and 20, were used in the analyses. A modified A Priori Diet Quality Score was calculated to measure the quality of diet based on other food groups at baseline and exam years 7 and 20 [14]. The average of the three measurements were used in the analyses.

Cognitive function assessments

Cognitive function was measured at exam year 25 using 3 tests, including Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), and the Stroop Test. All tests were administered by trained and certified CARDIA research technicians following a standardized protocol. RAVLT examined verbal learning and memory by assessing the ability to correctly memorize and recall 15 words after a 10-minute delay. A greater number of words recalled (corresponds to a higher score; possible range: 0–15) indicated better cognitive performance. DSST from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–III measured psychomotor speed, sustained attention, and working memory [15]. The possible scores ranged from 0 to 133, with a higher score indicating better cognitive performance. The Stroop Test evaluated executive function by assessing the ability to view a complex visual stimulus and to respond while suppressing the responses to another dimension [16, 17]. The test was scored by the time to complete the trials plus the number of errors, thus a higher score indicated worse cognitive performance.

Other Variables

Toenail clippings from all 10 toes were collected at exam year 2 and shipped to a central laboratory at the University of Missouri Research Reactor [18]. Toenail Hg and Se were measured by using instrumental neutron-activation analysis [19]. Plasma HDL-cholesterol, and triglyceride concentrations were measured through baseline to exam year 30 by enzymatic methods at Northwest Lipids Research Laboratory (Seattle, WA). Plasma LDL-cholesterol concentration was estimated by the Friedewald equation. Fasting glucose was measured at baseline and exam years 7, 10, 15, 20, 25, and 30 by using hexokinase coupled to glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase (Millipore, Inc, Bellerica, MA; later at the University of Minnesota) [10]. The cumulative average of repeated measurements across all available examinations of HDL-cholesterol, LDL-cholesterol, triglyceride and fasting glucose by exam year 25 when cognitive function was examined were used in the analyses. The cumulative average daily intakes of total energy, B6, B12, and folate were calculated by using the repeated measurements at baseline and exam years 7 and 20 through CARDIA questionnaire. In addition, some other important covariates were collected at each follow-up. Demographic and lifestyle information, including age, sex (female or male), race (black or white), study center, education levels, smoking status, alcohol consumption, and physical activity were collected through a self-administered questionnaire and were verified in clinic examinations. Education attained through exam year 25 has 3 levels (<12.0, 12.0–15.9, or ≥16.0 years). Smoking status at exam year 25 was classified into 3 groups: never-, former or current smokers. Alcohol consumption was presented as milliliters of alcohol per day. Physical activity was assessed using the CARDIA Physical Activity History Questionnaire. Body weight and height were measured to calculate body mass index (BMI). Blood pressure was measured by using a random zero sphygmanometer from baseline to exam year 15 and the Omron HEM907XL sphygmomanometer (Omron Corporation, Kyota, Japan) at exam years 20, 25, and 30 [20]. A calibration study was performed at exam year 20 in a subgroup of participants. Cumulative average alcohol intake, physical activity, BMI, and blood pressure by exam year 25 were calculated by averaging the repeated measurements.

Statistical analysis

Characteristics of participants were summarized using mean values with standard deviations or medians with inter-quartile ranges for continuous variables and proportions for categorical variables. Analysis of variance (ANOVA), the Kruskal-Wallis test, or the chi-squared test, as appropriate, were used to test for the differences across quintiles of LCω−3PUFA intake. Covariates related to cognitive function and associated with LCω−3PUFA intake were considered in the main analyses.

The general linear regression model was used to examine the associations between intake of LCω−3PUFA, DHA, EPA or non-fried seafood and cognitive test scores. The mean differences of each cognitive test score with the corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs) using the lowest quintile of each exposure as the referent were estimated with adjustment for potential confounders (model 1 and 2). Linear trends were tested by using the continuous variable of the exposure with values over 99th percentile excluded.

Several sensitivity analyses were conducted to test the robustness of our findings. First, only participants with all repeated measurements of LCω−3PUFA, DHA, EPA, and non-fried seafood were included in the analysis. Second, shellfish was excluded from seafood consumption due to its relatively low levels of LCω−3PUFA. Third, model 2 was further adjusted for toenail Hg and Se levels as well as the dietary intakes of total energy, B6, B12, and folate to explore possible confounding effects from other nutrients and contaminants in seafood. Fourth, model 2 was additionally adjusted for the modified A Priori Diet Quality Score to reduce the influence of the diet quality based on other food groups.

In stratified analyses, the associations between intakes of LCω−3PUFA and cognitive test scores using tertiles or the continuous variable for LCω−3PUFA were estimated by stratifying age at exam year 25 (<median 51 years vs. ≥median 51 years), sex (female vs. male), race (black vs. white), BMI (<25 vs. ≥25 kg/m2), smoking status (never- vs. ever-smokers), toenail Se levels (<median 0.85 ppm vs. ≥median 0.85 ppm), and toenail Hg levels (<median 0.22 ppm vs. ≥median 0.22 ppm) with adjustment for all covariates in model 2 except the potential effect modifier. To further explore the possible joint modification by Hg and Se, we examined the association of interest in 4 subgroups defined by the median levels of Hg (0.22 ppm) and Se (0.85 ppm). Interaction was tested using the continuous variable of LCω−3PUFA with values over 99th percentile excluded.

All analyses were performed by using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). Two-sided P values ≤0.05 (main effect) and P values ≤0.10 (interaction) were considered statistically significant.

RESULTS

In the study population (n=3,231), 56% of participants were female and 45% were blacks with an average age of 50 years at exam year 25. Table 1 shows the characteristics of the study population across quintiles of LCω−3PUFA intake. Participants with higher LCω−3PUFA intake were more likely to be older, males, blacks, have higher total energy intake, education level, have higher alcohol consumption, be physically active, and have higher levels of fasting glucose and toenail Hg but lower levels of toenail Se. They were less likely to be current smokers.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the study population by quintiles (Q) of cumulative average daily intake of LCω−3PUFA: the CARDIA study (n=3,231), 1985–2010.a,b

Quintiles of LCω−3PUFA intake Total
(n=3,231)
P value
Q1 (n=630) Q2 (n=650) Q3 (n=663) Q4 (n=637) Q5 (n=651)
LCω−3PUFA (g/day) 0.04±0.02 0.08±0.01 0.13±0.02 0.19±0.03 0.41±0.22 0.17±0.17 NA
DHA (g/day) 0.02±0.01 0.04 ±0.01 0.06±0.02 0.09±0.02 0.20±0.11 0.08±0.08 NA
EPA (g/day) 0.01±0.01 0.03±0.01 0.05±0.01 0.07 ±0.02 0.16±0.11 0.06±0.07 NA
Non-fried seafood (servings/day) 0.24±0.43 0.50±0.29 0.80±0.50 1.19±0.62 2.04±1.25 0.96±0.94 NA
Fried seafood (servings/day) 0.04±0.18 0.05±0.17 0.06±0.19 0.10±0.35 0.08±0.32 0.07 ±0.26 NA
Total energy (kcal/day) 2348.7±1074.6 2501.1±1126.7 2683.6±1237.2 2768.7±1098.8 3227.4±1280.8 2707.9±1204.4 <0.01
Age at exam Y25 (year) 49.6±3.8 50.0±3.6 50.1 ±3.7 50.6±3.5 50.5 ±3.6 50.1±3.6 <0.01
Female (%) 64.0 60.2 56.6 54.6 46.7 56.4 <0.01
Blacks (%) 38.9 42.2 47.4 44.3 52.4 45.1 <0.01
Education attained through exam Y25 (year) 14.8±2.7 15.1±2.7 15.1 ±2.7 15.4±2.5 15.3±2.6 15.1±2.7 <0.01
Smoking status (%) 0.02
 Never smoker 63.7 63.6 62.6 61.5 57.8 61.8
 Former smoker 18.6 18.8 22.5 23.8 26.4 22.1
 Current smoker 17.7 17.6 14.9 14.7 15.8 16.1
Alcohol consumption (ml/day) <0.01
 Median 3.7 4.1 4.8 7.5 7.8 5.5
 IQR 0.3–11.5 0.4–12.3 0.9–13.8 1.8–16.3 2.1–21.3 0.9–15.0
Physical activity (exercise unit) <0.01
 Median 265.1 305.8 321.3 334.8 386.3 323.8
 IQR 161.1–414.2 191.9–458.5 198.8–476.9 213.1–481.1 254.0–573.9 200.4–479.1
BMI(%) 0.12
 <18.5 kg/m2 0.5 1.1 0.9 0.5 0.3 0.7
 18.5–24.9 kg/m2 40.2 43.1 36.1 44.0 39.9 40.6
 25.0–29.9 kg/m2 33.7 28.8 39.4 35.6 36.6 34.8
 ≥30.0 kg/m2 25.7 27.1 23.7 20.0 23.2 23.9
HDL-cholesterol (mg/dL) 53.1±12.5 53.0±13.0 53.2±11.9 53.9±12.7 54.6±13.6 53.6±12.8 0.08
LDL-cholesterol (mg/dL) 110.7±25.8 109.2±26.3 110.3±26.1 110.5±25.7 113.0±26.3 110.7±26.0 0.12
Triglycerides (mg/dL) 89.0±40.7 93.3±62.1 89.7±51.2 89.7 ±51.1 94.6±63.2 91.3±54.4 0.24
Glucose (mg/dL) 89.3±12.8 90.5±14.4 89.7±12.9 89.7±10.5 91.3±15.2 90.1±13.3 0.048
SBP (mmHg) 110.5±9.9 111.2±9.5 111.6±9.3 111.0±9.9 111.8±10.1 111.2±9.7 0.14
DBP (mmHg) 70.3±7.3 71.0±7.3 71.1 ±7.0 70.7 ±7.6 71.1 ±7.7 70.8±7.4 0.22
Toenail selenium at exam Y2 (ppm) 0.88±0.15 0.87 ±0.16 0.86±0.16 0.85 ±0.13 0.84±0.15 0.86±0.15 <0.01
Toenail mercury at exam Y2 (ppm) 0.22±0.25 0.26±0.26 0.30±0.32 0.37 ±0.41 0.43±0.50 0.32±0.37 <0.01

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; CARDIA, Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults; DBP, diastolic blood pressure; DHA, docosahexaenoic acid; EPA, eicosapentaenoic acid; HDL, high-density lipoprotein; IQR, inter-quartile range; LCω−3PUFA, long chain ω−3 polyunsaturated fatty acids; LDL, low-density lipoprotein; ml, milliliters; NA, not applicable; SBP, systolic blood pressure; Y, year.

a

All variables are cumulative averages of all available observations of the variable in question, except where noted. Results are presented by means ± standard deviations, medians (inter-quartile ranges) or proportions.

b

P values are for any differences across quintiles of LCω−3PUFA intake (Analysis of variance, Kruskal-Wallis test, or chi-squared test, as appropriate).

Higher LCω−3PUFA intake was significantly associated with a better cognitive performance measured by DSST (Table 2). Compared with participants in the lowest quintile (Q1) of LCω−3PUFA intake, those in the highest quintile (Q5) had 2.79 more points in DSST [95% CI: 1.19, 4.40; P for trend<0.01; model 1], and 1.42 less points in the Stroop Test [95% CI: −2.53, −0.31; P for trend = 0.013; model 1]. The observed associations were attenuated to some extent after further adjustment for other confounding variables in model 2, but remained statistically significant for DSST [Q5 vs. Q1; mean difference: 1.74; 95% CI: 0.19, 3.29; P for trend = 0.048]. No statistically significant association was found between LCω−3PUFA intake and RAVLT score. The findings persisted when examining DHA and EPA separately (Table 2).

Table 2.

Multivariable-adjusted mean differences (95% CI) in cognitive test scores according to quintiles (Q) of cumulative average daily intakes of LCω3PUFA, DHA, EPA, and non-fried seafood: the CARDIA study (n=3,231), 1985–2010.ad

RAVLT words
Mean=8.7 (SD=2.6)
DSST symbols
Mean=70.4 (SD=16.0)
Stroop test points
Mean=22.7(SD=10.6)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2
LCω−3PUFA (g/day)
 Ql (≤0.06) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.)
 Q2 (0.07–0.10) 0.15 (−0.10,0.40) 0.12 (−0.13,0.36) 0.21 (−1.35,1.78) −0.12 (−1.60, 1.36) −0.92 (−2.00,0.16) −0.77 (−1.84,0.30)
 Q3 (0.11–0.15) 0.09 (−0.16,0.34) 0.02 (−0.23,0.27) 1.06 (−0.51,2.62) 0.28 (−1.21, 1.77) 0.04 (−1.04,1.12) 0.46 (−0.61,1.53)
 Q4 (0.16–0.25) 0.09 (−0.17,0.35) −0.03 (−0.28, 0.22) 1.35 (−0.24,2.94) 0.02 (−1.50, 1.54) −1.10 (−2.20,−0.003) −0.39 (−1.49,0.70)
 Q5 (>0.25) 0.17 (−0.09,0.43) 0.08 (−0.17,0.34) 2.79 (1.19,4.40) 1.74 (0.19,3.29) −1.42 (−2.53,−0.31) −0.76 (−1.88,0.36)
P for trend 0.38 0.84 <0.01 0.048 0.013 0.21
DHA (g/day)
 Ql (≤0.03) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.)
 Q2 (0.04–0.05) 0.26 (0.005,0.51) 0.26 (0.02,0.50) −0.54 (−2.10,1.02) −0.55 (−2.02,0.93) −0.39 (−1.47,0.70) −0.29 (−1.35,0.78)
 Q3 (0.06–0.07) 0.13 (−0.12,0.38) 0.07 (−0.18,0.31) 1.26 (−0.30,2.81) 0.59 (−0.89,2.06) −0.25 (−1.33,0.82) 0.10 (−0.97,1.17)
 Q4 (0.08–0.12) 0.18 (−0.08,0.43) 0.04 (−0.21,0.29) 1.55 (−0.02,3.13) 0.21 (−1.29,1.71) −1.00 (−2.09,0.09) −0.26 (−1.35,0.82)
 Q5 (>0.12) 0.24 (−0.01,0.50) 0.15 (−0.11,0.40) 2.80 (1.21,4.38) 1.75 (0.22,3.28) −1.50 (−2.60,−0.40) −0.82 (−1.93,0.28)
P for trend 0.09 0.41 <0.01 0.051 <0.01 0.16
EPA (g/day)
 Ql (≤0.02) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.)
 Q2 (0.03–0.04) 0.04 (−0.21,0.29) −0.06 (−0.30, 0.19) 1.23 (−0.33,2.78) 0.43 (−1.05, 1.91) −1.49 (−2.56,−0.41) −1.12 (−2.19,−0.06)
 Q3 (0.05–0.06) 0.16 (−0.10,0.41) 0.09 (−0.16,0.33) 0.99 (−0.59,2.57) 0.18 (−1.32, 1.68) −0.72 (−1.81,0.37) −0.29 (−1.37,0.79)
 Q4 (0.07–0.09) 0.07 (−0.19,0.32) −0.07 (−0.32, 0.18) 2.12 (0.53,3.71) 0.69 (−0.83,2.22) −1.58 (−2.69,−0.48) −0.84 (−1.94,0.26)
 Q5 (>0.09) 0.07 (−0.19,0.33) −0.01 (−0.27,0.24) 2.51 (0.90,4.11) 1.50 (−0.06,3.05) −1.63 (−2.74,−0.52) −0.99 (−2.11,0.13)
P for trend 0.35 0.68 <0.01 0.02 0.015 0.15
Non-fried seafood (servings/day)
 Ql (≤0.29) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.) 0 (Ref.)
 Q2 (0.30–0.55) 0.34 (0.09,0.59) 0.21 (−0.03,0.46) 1.68 (0.12,3.24) 0.65 (−0.83,2.13) −1.44 (−2.52,−0.36) −0.96 (−2.02,0.11)
 Q3 (0.56–0.88) 0.21 (−0.04,0.47) 0.09 (−0.16,0.33) −0.04 (−1.61,1.52) −1.45 (−2.94,0.04) −0.38 (−1.47,0.70) 0.24 (−0.83,1.32)
 Q4 (0.89–1.43) 0.28(0.03,0.54) 0.13 (−0.12,0.38) 1.28 (−0.30,2.85) −0.25 (−1.75,1.26) −1.36 (−2.45,−0.27) −0.56 (−1.65,0.52)
 Q5 (>1.43) 0.11 (−0.14,0.37) −0.01 (−0.27,0.24) 2.87 (1.29,4.45) 1.48 (−0.05, 3.01) −1.72 (−2.81,−0.62) −0.97 (−2.08,0.14)
P for trend 0.58 0.96 <0.01 0.04 <0.01 0.13

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; CARDIA, Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults; CI, confidence interval; DHA, docosahexaenoic acid; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test; EPA, eicosapentaenoic acid; LCω−3PUFA, long chain ω−3 polyunsaturated fatty acids; RAVLT, The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; Ref., reference; SD, standard deviation.

a

All models were constructed using general linear model analysis. P for trend was examined by using the continuous variable of exposure with values over 99th percentile excluded.

b

Model 1 was adjusted for age, sex (female or male), race (white or black), and study center.

c

Model 2 was additionally adjusted for educational attainment through exam year 25 (<12.0, 12.0–15.9, or ≥16.0 years), cumulative average BMI (<18.5, 18.5–24.9, 25.0–29.9, or ≥30.0 kg/m2), smoking status at exam year 25 (never, former, or current smokers), cumulative average alcohol consumption (0, 0.1–11.9, 12.0–23.9 or ≥24 ml/day), cumulative average physical activity (quintiles), and cumulative average glucose level (continuous).

d

Fried seafood intake (yes or no) was adjusted for in both model 1 and 2 when studying non-fried seafood consumption.

Similar results were observed for non-fried seafood consumption (Table 2). Higher non-fried seafood consumption was associated with better performances in DSST [Q5 vs. Q1; mean difference: 2.87; 95% CI: 1.29, 4.45; P for trend <0.01; model 1] and the Stroop test [Q5 vs. Q1; mean difference: −1.72; 95% CI: −2.81, −0.62; P for trend <0.01; model 1]. After adjusting for other confounding variables in model 2, the significant findings remained for DSST [Q5 vs. Q1; mean difference: 1.48; 95% CI: −0.05, 3.01; P for trend = 0.04]. Non-fried seafood consumption was not associated with performance measured by RAVLT.

In a sensitivity analysis, when only including participants who have data in all repeated measurements of LCω−3PUFA, DHA, EPA, and non-fried seafood, the results were not materially changed. In addition, the results were generally consistent when excluding shellfish from seafood consumption or further adjusting for toenail Hg and Se levels. The observed associations were not materially changed when additionally adjusting for total energy intake and other nutrients in seafood. The associations were attenuated when adjusting for the modified A Priori Diet Quality Score (Data not shown).

Because intakes of LCω−3PUFA and non-fried seafood were consistently associated with cognitive performance measured by DSST, we conducted stratified analyses based on DSST by a few pre-specified factors (Table 3). A significant interaction was observed between LCω−3PUFA intake and BMI (P for interaction <0.01); the association of interest was more pronounced in participants with BMI ≥25 kg/m2 [tertile 3 vs. tertile 1; mean difference: 1.50; 95% CI: −0.08, 3.09; P for trend <0.01].

Table 3.

Associations [adjusted mean differences (95% CI)] between cumulative average daily intake of LCω−3PUFA and DSST score by pre-specified factors, the CARDIA study (n=3,231), 1985–2010.a

Levels of LCω−3PUFA
[mean (SD)]
Tertiles of LCω−3PUFA intake P for trend
T1
(≤0.09 g/day
T2
(0.09–0.18 g/day)
T3
(≥0.18 g/day)
All participants 0.17 (0.17) 0 (Ref.) 0.37 (−0.78, 1.52) 1.12 (−0.08, 2.32) 0.048
Age at exam Y25
 <Median 51 years 0.16 (0.16) 0 (Ref.) 0.13 (−1.40, 1.66) 1.32 (−0.30, 2.94) 0.11
 ≥Median 51 years 0.18 (0.17) 0 (Ref.) 0.51 (−1.25,2.27) 0.19 (−1.61, 2.00) 0.59
P for interaction −- 0.68
Sex
 Female 0.15 (0.14) 0 (Ref.) 0.35 (−1.17, 1.86) 0.66 (−0.97, 2.29) 0.26
 Male 0.19 (0.19) 0 (Ref.) 0.34 (−1.43, 2.11) 1.57 (−0.22, 3.36) 0.12
P for interaction -- 0.65
Race
 Blacks 0.19 (0.19) 0 (Ref.) 1.24 (−0.53, 3.00) 2.57(0.77,4.37) 0.03
 Whites 0.16 (0.14) 0 (Ref.) −0.20 (−1.72, 1.32) −0.19 (−1.81, 1.42) 0.66
P for interaction -- 0.50
BMI
 <25 kg/m2 0.17 (0.18) 0 (Ref.) 1.38 (−0.41, 3.16) 0.73 (−1.11, 2.57) 0.97
 ≥25 kg/m2 0.17 (0.16) 0 (Ref.) −0.27 (−1.78, 1.24) 1.50 (−0.08, 3.09) <0.01
P for interaction -- <0.01
Smoking status
 Never 0.16 (0.15) 0 (Ref.) −0.11 (−1.53,1.32) 0.68 (−0.83, 2.19) 0.17
 Former 0.19 (0.19) 0 (Ref.) 0.39 (−2.12, 2.90) 1.45 (−1.10, 4.01) 0.15
 Current 0.17 (0.17) 0 (Ref.) 2.84 (−0.36, 6.03) 2.32 (−1.00, 5.65) 0.63
P for interaction -- 0.82
Toenail selenium levels
 <Median 0.85 ppm 0.18 (0.16) 0 (Ref.) −0.39 (−2.18, 1.41) 0.25 (−1.59, 2.09) 0.43
 ≥Median 0.85 ppm 0.16 (0.17) 0 (Ref.) 1.18 (−0.47, 2.83) 2.06(0.30,3.82) 0.04
P for interaction -- 0.25
Toenail mercury levels
 <Median 0.22 ppm 0.14 (0.14) 0 (Ref.) 0.27 (−1.40, 1.94) 1.45 (−0.46, 3.35) 0.12
 ≥Median 0.22 ppm 0.20 (0.19) 0 (Ref.) 0.79 (−1.00, 2.58) 0.88 (−0.90, 2.67) 0.21
P for interaction -- 0.54
Joint classification of selenium-mercury levels
 Low Se / high Hg 0.21 (0.18) 0 (Ref.) −0.12 (−2.76, 2.52) −0.30 (−2.90, 2.30) 0.76
 Low Se / high Hg 0.19 (0.20) 0 (Ref.) 1.46 (−1.03, 3.95) 2.01 (−0.52, 4.53) 0.16
 Low Se / low Hg 0.15 (0.15) 0 (Ref.) −0.72 (−3.26, 1.81) 0.75 (−2.04, 3.54) 0.42
 High Se / low Hg 0.13 (0.13) 0 (Ref.) 1.13 (−1.15, 3.41) 1.91 (−0.76, 4.58) 0.19
P for interaction -- 0.60

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; CARDIA, Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults; CI, confidence interval; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test; Hg, mercury; LCω−3PUFA, long chain ω−3 polyunsaturated fatty acids; Ref., reference; SD, standard deviation; Se, selenium; Y, year.

a

All models were constructed using general linear model analysis with adjustment for covariates in model 2, Table 2, except the potential modifier. P for trend and P for interaction were examined by using the continuous variable of LCω−3PUFA intake with values over 99th percentile excluded.

We also examine the main effects of toenail Se and Hg on cognitive function tests, but no significant associations were found after adjustment for potential confounders (Supplemental Table 1). Although the interaction was not statistically significant (P for interaction = 0.25), the association between LCω−3PUFA intake and DSST score was more profound in participants with higher toenail Se levels [tertile 3 vs. tertile 1; Se ≥median 0.85 ppm: mean difference: 2.06; 95% CI: 0.30, 3.82; P for trend = 0.04; Se <median 0.85 ppm: mean difference: 0.25; 95% CI: −1.59, 2.09; P for trend = 0.43]. However, Hg levels did not appreciably modify the association (P for interaction = 0.54). We further explored the joint modification of Se and Hg on LCω−3PUFA intake, a three-way interaction. When participants were jointly classified according to median levels of Se (0.85 ppm) and Hg (0.22 ppm), the association between LCω−3PUFA intake and DSST score was not materially modified (P for interaction = 0.60).

DISCUSSION

Over 25 years of follow-up, we found that higher LCω−3PUFA intake and non-fried seafood consumption were associated with better cognitive performance particularly measured by DSST, but not RAVLT and the Stroop test. The observed associations were more pronounced in overweight and/or obese individuals. While DSST score mainly reflects psychomotor speed indicating the overall efficiency of brain operations [21], RAVLT and the Stroop test measure verbal memory and executive functions, respectively. Since slow psychomotor speed appears far prior to the onset of mild cognitive impairment [22, 23], DSST score is considered a more sensitive index reflecting cognitive decline [21, 24].

Although the beneficial effects of LCω−3PUFA on cognitive function is supported by evidence from laboratory studies [2527], epidemiological studies yielded inconsistent findings. Two recent systematic reviews of RCTs did not find a significant improvement of cognitive function with respect to LCω−3PUFA supplementation. However, most of the RCTs included in the reviews were conducted in less than 100 participants with an intervention period shorter than 5 months [3, 4]. Of note, some RCTs suggested a potential beneficial effect with a relatively long-term supplementation of LCω−3PUFA [28, 29]. Observational studies also suggested that long-term intakes of LCω−3PUFA and seafood were associated with a slower age-related cognitive decline [5, 6].

One important contribution of this study is that it recruited young adults and followed them up for more than 25 years, while most of the previous studies focused on older adults. Middle-aged or older men and women are more likely to already have onset of diseases, so their lifestyle choice and health conditions may be affected by perceived ill health or treatment for existing disease. Follow-up of a cohort of young adults for 25 years provides insight on the natural history or early etiology of cognitive decline. Since long-term randomized clinical trials may not be feasible, a longitudinal study such as the present one certainly provides important data to the literature. In addition, we used cumulative average intake to reflect the usual diet as well as took cooking methods into account, which has not been considered in previous studies due to lack of data.

When examining LCω−3PUFA and seafood, Se and Hg are two elements warrant consideration because they often coexist with LCω−3PUFA in seafood. Se is an element with antioxidant capacity and may protect against cognitive decline alone or by interacting with LCω−3PUFA [30]. Se can also modulate the neurotoxicity effect of Hg [9, 31, 32]. In the present study, the findings are consistent with a recent report of US adults that failed to observe a significant interaction between LCω−3PUFA and Hg with neurobehavioral outcomes [33]. The association appeared to be more pronounced at higher toenail Se levels, though the interaction was not statistically significant.

In the present study, overweight or obese individuals seemed to have a better response to intake of LCω−3PUFA in terms of cognitive decline. Since obesity is characterized by chronic inflammation [34], it is possible that the influence of LCω−3PUFA on inflammation, and consequently cognitive function, is more evident in overweight and obese individuals who have a higher inflammation level [35]. This hypothesis is supported by previous studies that found associations between LCω−3PUFA and inflammatory biomarkers [36, 37]. It is also possible that having more adipose tissues protects against fat soluble pollutants, which are suggested to be associated with the risk of cognitive impairment [38].

Some limitations of the present study need to be acknowledged. First, cognitive function was assessed only once and not at baseline. Thus, we were not able to evaluate the association with changes in cognitive function. The inclusion of cognitive impaired individuals at baseline is unlikely given the young age of the participants at study enrollment and the fact that they remained in the study for 25 years. Second, other nutrients in seafood may confound the association between LCω−3PUFA and cognitive function. We adjusted for some nutrients in a sensitivity analysis and found the results were not materially changed, but the concern still remains. The fact that the associations were attenuated after adjustment for the diet quality based on other food groups suggests that seafood intake may be a part of healthy diet [39]. Third, objective measurements of LCω−3PUFA were not available. However, the diet history used in the present study has been validated [11, 12]. The use of food frequency questionnaire to reflect the levels of LCω−3PUFA exposure is also supported in other studies [40, 41]. Fourth, similar to other observational studies, the possibility of residual confounding from dietary and non-dietary factors cannot be completely ruled out. But the consistent results from the main and sensitivity analyses provide reassurance about the validity of our findings.

CONCLUSIONS

Findings from this longitudinal study support that intakes of LCω−3PUFA and non-fried seafood are associated with better cognitive performance in psychomotor speed. This study adds additional scientific evidence supporting the recommendation of seafood consumption for preventing or slowing down the process of cognitive decline.

Supplementary Material

1

Highlights.

  • Intake of long-chain omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids was significantly associated with better cognitive performance in psychomotor speed.

  • Similar results were observed for intakes of EPA, DHA, and non-fried seafood.

  • The observed associations were more pronounced in overweight and obese participants.

  • Levels of mercury or selenium did not materially modified the observed associations.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults Study is supported by grants from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) in collaboration with the University of Alabama at Birmingham (HHSN268201300025C and HHSN268201300026C), Northwestern University (HHSN268201300027C), University of Minnesota (HHSN268201300028C), Kaiser Foundation Research Institute (HHSN268201300029C), and Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (HHSN268200900041C). CARDIA is also partially supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute on Aging (NIA) and an intra-agency agreement between NIA and NHLBI (grant AG0005).

The authors thank the other investigators, the staff, and the participants of the CARDIA study for their valuable contributions. Representatives of the funding agency have been involved in the review of the manuscript but not directly involved in the collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data.

Abbreviations

95% CI

95% confidence interval

ANOVA

analysis of variance

BMI

body mass index

CARDIA

Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults Study

DBP

diastolic blood pressure

DHA

docosahexaenoic acid

DPA

docosapentaenoic acid

DSST

Digit Symbol Substitution Test

EPA

eicosapentaenoic acid

HDL

high-density lipoprotein

Hg

mercury

IQR

inter-quartile range

LCω−3PUFA

long-chain omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids

LDL

low-density lipoprotein

NA

not applicable

RAVLT

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test

RCT

randomized clinical trials

Ref.

reference

SBP

systolic blood pressure

SD

standard deviation

Se

selenium

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

REFERENCES

  • [1].Singh-Manoux A, Kivimaki M, Glymour MM, Elbaz A, Berr C, Ebmeier KP, et al. Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ 2012;344:d7622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [2].Weiser MJ, Butt CM, Mohajeri MH. Docosahexaenoic acid and cognition throughout the lifespan. Nutrients 2016;8(2):99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [3].Rangel-Huerta OD, Gil A. Effect of omega-3 fatty acids on cognition: an updated systematic review of randomized clinical trials. Nutr Rev 2018;76(1):1–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [4].Teo L, Crawford C, Yehuda R, Jaghab D, Bingham JJ, Chittum HK, et al. Omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids to optimize cognitive function for military mission-readiness: a systematic review and recommendations for the field. Nutr Rev 2017;75(suppl_2):36–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [5].Zhang Y, Chen J, Qiu J, Li Y, Wang J, Jiao J. Intakes of fish and polyunsaturated fatty acids and mild-to-severe cognitive impairment risks: a dose-response meta-analysis of 21 cohort studies. Am J Clin Nutr 2016;103(2):330–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [6].Samieri C, Morris MC, Bennett DA, Berr C, Amouyel P, Dartigues JF, et al. Fish Intake, genetic predisposition to Alzheimer disease, and decline in global cognition and memory in 5 cohorts of older persons. Am J Epidemiol 2018;187(5):933–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [7].Aschner M, Aschner JL. Mercury neurotoxicity: mechanisms of blood-brain barrier transport. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 1990;14(2):169–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [8].Zhang ZH, Wen L, Wu QY, Chen C, Zheng R, Liu Q, et al. Long-term dietary supplementation with selenium-enriched yeast improves cognitive impairment, reverses synaptic deficits, and mitigates tau pathology in a triple transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of agricultural and food chemistry 2017;65(24):4970–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [9].Sumino K, Yamamoto R, Kitamura S. A role of selenium against methylmercury toxicity. Nature 1977;268(5615):73–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [10].Friedman GD, Cutter GR, Donahue RP, Hughes GH, Hulley SB, Jacobs DR Jr., et al. CARDIA: study design, recruitment, and some characteristics of the examined subjects. Journal of clinical epidemiology 1988;41(11):1105–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [11].McDonald A, Van Horn L, Slattery M, Hilner J, Bragg C, Caan B, et al. The CARDIA dietary history: development, implementation, and evaluation. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 1991;91(9):1104–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [12].Liu K, Slattery M, Jacobs D Jr., Cutter G, McDonald A, Van Horn L, et al. A study of the reliability and comparative validity of the CARDIA dietary history. Ethn Dis 1994;4(1):15–27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [13].Candela MAI, Bello J. Deep-fat frying modifies high-fat fish lipid fraction. Journal of Agricultural & Food Chemistry 1998;46(7):2783–6. [Google Scholar]
  • [14].Zhu N, Jacobs DR, Meyer KA, He K, Launer L, Reis JP, et al. Cognitive function in a middle aged cohort is related to higher quality dietary pattern 5 and 25 years earlier: the CARDIA study. J Nutr Health Aging 2015;19(1):33–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [15].Wechsler D WAIS-III: administration and scoring manual: Wechsler adult intelligence scale: Psychological Corporation San Antonio (TX) 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • [16].MacLeod CM. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull 1991;109(2):163–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [17].Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935;18(6):643–62. [Google Scholar]
  • [18].Xun P, Liu K, Morris JS, Daviglus ML, Stevens J, Jacobs DR, Jr., et al. Associations of toenail selenium levels with inflammatory biomarkers of fibrinogen, high-sensitivity c-reactive protein, and interleukin-6: The CARDIA Trace Element Study. American journal of epidemiology 2010;171(7):793–800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [19].Steven Morris J, Stampfer MJ, Willett W. Dietary selenium in humans toenails as an indicator. Biological trace element research 1983;5(6):529–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [20].Kramer H, Colangelo L, Lewis CE, Jacobs DR, Jr., Pletcher M, Bibbins-Domingo K, et al. Cumulative exposure to systolic blood pressure during young adulthood through midlife and the urine albumin-to-creatinine ratio at midlife. Am J Hypertens 2017;30(5):502–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [21].Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological review 1996;103(3):403–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [22].Backman L, Jones S, Berger AK, Laukka EJ, Small BJ. Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 2005;19(4):520–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [23].Saxton J, Lopez OL, Ratcliff G, Dulberg C, Fried LP, Carlson MC, et al. Preclinical Alzheimer disease: neuropsychological test performance 1.5 to 8 years prior to onset. Neurology 2004;63(12):2341–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [24].Proust-Lima C, Amieva H, Dartigues JF, Jacqmin-Gadda H. Sensitivity of four psychometric tests to measure cognitive changes in brain aging-population-based studies. American journal of epidemiology 2007;165(3):344–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [25].Dyall SC, Michael-Titus AT. Neurological benefits of omega-3 fatty acids. Neuromolecular Med 2008;10(4):219–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [26].Serhan CN, Chiang N, Van Dyke TE. Resolving inflammation: dual anti-inflammatory and pro-resolution lipid mediators. Nature reviews Immunology 2008;8(5):349–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [27].Fraga VG, Carvalho MDG, Caramelli P, de Sousa LP, Gomes KB. Resolution of inflammation, n-3 fatty acid supplementation and Alzheimer disease: A narrative review. J Neuroimmunol 2017;310:111–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [28].Witte AV, Kerti L, Hermannstadter HM, Fiebach JB, Schreiber SJ, Schuchardt JP, et al. Long-chain omega-3 fatty acids improve brain function and structure in older adults. Cereb Cortex 2014;24(11):3059–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [29].Stonehouse W, Conlon CA, Podd J, Hill SR, Minihane AM, Haskell C, et al. DHA supplementation improved both memory and reaction time in healthy young adults: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 2013;97(5):1134–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [30].Gao S, Jin Y, Hall KS, Liang C, Unverzagt FW, Ji R, et al. Selenium level and cognitive function in rural elderly Chinese. American journal of epidemiology 2007;165(8):955–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [31].Ganther HE, Goudie C, Sunde ML, Kopecky MJ, Wagner P. Selenium: relation to decreased toxicity of methylmercury added to diets containing tuna. Science 1972;175(4026):1122–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [32].Seppanen K, Kantola M, Laatikainen R, Nyyssonen K, Valkonen VP, Kaarlopp V, et al. Effect of supplementation with organic selenium on mercury status as measured by mercury in pubic hair. J Trace Elem Med Biol 2000;14(2):84–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [33].Vacchi-Suzzi C, Karimi R, Kruse D, Silbernagel SM, Levine KE, Rohlman DS, et al. Low-level mercury, omega-3 index and neurobehavioral outcomes in an adult US coastal population. European journal of nutrition 2016;55(2):699–711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [34].Ridker PM, Buring JE, Cook NR, Rifai N. C-reactive protein, the metabolic syndrome, and risk of incident cardiovascular events: an 8-year follow-up of 14 719 initially healthy American women. Circulation 2003;107(3):391–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [35].Donath MY, Dalmas E, Sauter NS, Boni-Schnetzler M. Inflammation in obesity and diabetes: islet dysfunction and therapeutic opportunity. Cell metabolism 2013;17(6):860–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [36].Rodriguez-Cruz M, Cruz-Guzman ODR, Almeida-Becerril T, Solis-Serna AD, Atilano-Miguel S, Sanchez-Gonzalez JR, et al. Potential therapeutic impact of omega-3 long chain-polyunsaturated fatty acids on inflammation markers in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: A double-blind, controlled randomized trial. Clin Nutr 2017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [37].Yan Y, Jiang W, Spinetti T, Tardivel A, Castillo R, Bourquin C, et al. Omega-3 fatty acids prevent inflammation and metabolic disorder through inhibition of NLRP3 inflammasome activation. Immunity 2013;38(6):1154–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [38].Lee DH, Lind PM, Jacobs DR, Salihovic S, van Bavel B, Lind L. Association between background exposure to organochlorine pesticides and the risk of cognitive impairment: A prospective study that accounts for weight change. Environ Int 2016;89–90: 179–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [39].Pistollato F, Iglesias RC, Ruiz R, Aparicio S, Crespo J, Lopez LD, et al. Nutritional patterns associated with the maintenance of neurocognitive functions and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: A focus on human studies. Pharmacol Res 2018;131:32–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [40].Lucas M, Asselin G, Merette C, Poulin MJ, Dodin S. Validation of an FFQ for evaluation of EPA and DHA intake. Public health nutrition 2009;12(10):1783–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [41].Tjonneland A, Overvad K, Thorling E, Ewertz M. Adipose tissue fatty acids as biomarkers of dietary exposure in Danish men and women. The American journal of clinical nutrition 1993;57(5):629–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES