Abstract
Background
Premature infants are at risk for abnormal sensory development due to brain immaturity at birth and atypical early sensory experiences in the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit. This altered sensory development can have downstream effects on other more complex developmental processes. There are currently no interventions that address rehabilitation of sensory function in the neonatal period.
Methods
This study is a randomized controlled trial of preterm infants enrolled at 32–36 weeks postmenstrual age to either standard care or standard care plus multisensory intervention in order to study the effect of multisensory intervention as compared to standard care alone. The study population will consist of 100 preterm infants in each group (total n = 200). Both groups will receive standard care, consisting of non-contingent recorded parent’s voice and skin-to-skin by parent. The multisensory group will also receive contemporaneous holding and light pressure containment for tactile stimulation, playing of the mother’s voice contingent on the infant’s pacifier sucking for auditory stimulation, exposure to a parent-scented cloth for olfactory stimulation, and exposure to carefully regulated therapist breathing that is mindful and responsive to the child’s condition for vestibular stimulation. The primary outcome is a brain-based measure of multisensory processing, measured using time locked-EEG. Secondary outcomes include sensory adaptation, tactile processing, speech sound differentiation, motor and language function, measured at one and two years corrected gestational age.
Discussion
This is the first randomized controlled trial of a multisensory intervention using brain-based measurements in order to explain the causal effects of the multisensory intervention on neural processing changes to mediate neurodevelopmental outcomes in former preterm infants. In addition to contributing a critical link in our understanding of these processes, the protocolized multisensory intervention in this study is therapist administered, parent supported and leverages simple technology. Thus, this multisensory intervention has the potential to be widely implemented in various NICU settings, with the opportunity to potentially improve neurodevelopment of premature infants.
Trial registration
NIH Clinical Trials (clinicaltrials.gov): NCT03232931. Registered July 2017.
Keywords: Multisensory, Intervention, Preterm, Neurodevelopment, Event-related potential, Sensory function
Background
Every year, half a million infants are born prematurely in the United States and 15 million worldwide [1, 2]. The vast majority of preterm infants will have only moderate to mild impairments or delays in early childhood, with intellectual and behavioral consequences of prematurity only apparent at school age and beyond [3–7]. Almost all preterm infants suffer from atypical brain maturation and its developmental consequences resulting from interactions between brain immaturity and premature extra-uterine sensory experience [8–15]. Brain development in the neonatal period is experience-dependent, yet the neonatal intensive care experience is largely comprised of atypical sensory stimuli [11, 13, 15–22]. The critical importance of establishing functional sensory systems in infancy as the basis for all higher order processes (cognition, communication, behavioral adaptation) has been demonstrated in both animal models and humans [13, 15, 23–25]. Preterm infants at discharge to home often have altered sensory reactivity and modulation in response to their environment, which are associated with negative neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood [20, 22, 26].
Parents are essential in scaffolding early learning and development, especially with regards to early sensory exposures and responses [27–29]. In particular, parental linguistic input is a key concept in learning language [9, 30–36]. Importantly, this input is more effective when it is contingent and immediate (i.e., language is presented immediately after and only upon infant action) [9, 30–36]. This precept holds true even in early pre-linguistic phases, when infants differentiate among speech sounds, which is necessary for later development of higher-order language milestones [9, 30–36]. Another type of scaffolding provided by parents is the multisensory support of skin-to-skin care (STS) contact, which helps maintain quiet and well-regulated states in immature infants who have frequent autonomic instability [37–40]. Unfortunately, providing STS is often challenging for parents who often have to travel long distances to see their infant, while balancing responsibilities of other children and jobs, in addition to potential unreliable transportation and/or lack of social support [41–45].
Multisensory processes (MSPs) are rarely studied in neonates, yet in children and adults MSPs are essential to building a coherent and unified perception of the world, a foundation for learning and social interactions [46]. There are currently no mechanistically proven interventions that address rehabilitation of sensory function in the neonatal period, when brain-plasticity is at its greatest and when improvements can have an exponential downstream effect on later neurodevelopment [47–50]. The few associative studies of sensory processing, while critical, have not examined causal effects of interventions on neural processing changes to mediate neurodevelopmental outcomes. More infant-directed speech is associated with better language outcomes in the first 18 months and increased STS care with improved autonomic system stability and muscle tone in the hospital, and improved behavioral and motor outcomes in infancy [30–33, 37, 51–53]. The current study is the first to use brain-based measures to test predictions regarding how the brain changes in response to multisensory treatment, which in turn affects functional outcomes.
To accomplish our goals, we designed a prospective, interventional Randomized Controlled Trial (RCT) in preterm infants aimed at restoring more typical multisensory, rather than unisensory, processing. Our multisensory intervention using parents’ voice and nurturing touch can be administered regularly in the NICU (Neonatal Intensive Care Unit) during sensitive periods of sensory development, even when parents cannot always be present. Our test of this multisensory intervention will involve sessions of standardized, therapist-administered, multisensory stimulation. This treatment will combine contingent presentation of the recorded mother’s voice delivered using a suck-activated system during holding with supportive vestibular stimulation and tactile containment against a cloth scented by parent contact on the therapist’s chest. This treatment will be tested in an internally-valid RCT. In addition to testing the efficacy of the treatment on gold-standard measures of language and motor functioning, we will also test whether the treatment effect is due to intermediate treatment effects on multisensory and unisensory processing. Understanding the mechanism by which the treatment works is important for laying the foundations of future improvements and potential recommendations for treatment that might be useful for widespread use even in lower resource settings with the ultimate goal of improving neurodevelopmental outcomes for these premature infants.
Our research aims are as follows:
To demonstrate that preterm infants receiving a standardized, parent-supported, auditory-tactile-olfactory-vestibular intervention in addition to standard of care in the NICU will have more typical cortical multisensory processing at discharge and better sensory adaptation and motor and language outcomes than infants receiving only standard of care.
To test the role of multisensory responses at discharge in mediating intervention effects on later sensory adaptation, and motor and language outcomes.
To explore the role of unisensory responses in mediating intervention effects on later motor and language outcomes.
Methods
Study design
In order to test these aims, we will conduct an RCT of a multisensory (MS) intervention with 200 hospitalized preterm infants in our Level IV NICU (Fig. 1). Enrollment is planned from October 2018–October 2020. For infants who meet inclusion criteria at 31 weeks, parental consent will be obtained, parent’s voice recorded and parent-scented clothes collected. Infants will then be randomized to the control or intervention group. Both control and intervention groups will receive standard care, which includes STS care on parent, when present, and playing of parents’ voice non-contingent on infant suck. Infants assigned to the MS group will also receive 20 sessions of the standardized MS intervention over 2–3 weeks, starting at 32 0/7 weeks postmenstrual age (PMA) or at enrollment prior to 36 weeks. ERP (Event-related potential) testing will be performed prior to the intervention and at discharge, which occurs at 36 weeks PMA on average but later in the most preterm infants. All infants will be seen at the NICU Follow-Up Program clinics at 9–12 months PMA (Year 1) and 22–24 months PMA (Year 2), when Bayley III and Hammersmith Infant Neurological Exam are performed per standard care. At the Year 1 visit, the research coordinator administers the Infant Toddler Sensory Profile (ITSP), and at the Year 2 time-point, the research team administers the PLS-5 (Preschool Language Scales-5) [54].
Study participants
The study population will comprise 200 preterm infants. All races and ethnicities will be included. All materials will be translated by an interpreter for non-English speaking participants.
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
Inclusion criteria are PMA 32 0/7 weeks gestation - 36 0/7 weeks gestation. Exclusion criteria are ventilation using an endotracheal tube, major congenital malformations, family history of genetic hearing loss, and use of sedatives or seizure medications. Major congenital malformations may prevent accurate ERP measurements, and the medications above may mask sensory processing as assessed by ERPs.
Randomization and RCT design elements
Assignment to groups will be carried out using unified reproducible methods with a permuted block randomization scheme with random block size. Allocation will be concealed from all study personnel who could influence scores [55]. Except for parent reports, examiners will be blind to treatment assignment. Recordings of parents’ voices will be obtained from both groups with the intent of masking parents to assignment.
Adherence to treatment protocol
A highly manualized protocol for the MS treatment is monitored and 10% of videos are reviewed by an independent reviewer. Standard care is also monitored using nursing logs in both groups. These measures should help ensure uniform, high-quality implementation of the MS treatment. Sufficiently and randomly sampled fidelity of treatment (FOT) measures will be collected [56]. Treatment is provided in the NICU by experienced therapists.
Total attrition
Attrition is expected to be less than 10% [25, 57–59]. Should motion artifacts occur, we will retest patients with insufficient data within 24 h. For the Bayley III, should children prove unable to complete follow-up testing during scheduled visits, they will be rescheduled within two weeks of the scheduled visit. Testers are trained to perform the Bayley III in the home environment, if necessary. The ITSP can be performed over the phone or through the mail if necessary and will be performed within one week of a missed visit. Analysis will follow an intent-to-treat protocol [60]. As such, all randomized participants will be analyzed and missing data will be handled using multiple imputation [61].
Differential attrition
Differential attrition is unlikely because children must be in the NICU during the treatment phase due to health concerns. There are no non-NICU treatments during the treatment phase. Thus, compensatory responses due to parental displeasure of group assignment are unlikely.
Covariates
Biological variables are factored into our research design as potential covariates. These include gestational age at birth, sex, presence of severe white neural injury on neuroimaging (intraventricular hemorrhage grade III or IV, periventricular leukomalacia, cerebellar hemorrhage, ischemic or thrombotic injury), presence of systemic inflammatory conditions (history of necrotizing enterocolitis Bells stage IIA or above, culture documented sepsis or meningitis, moderate or severe bronchopulmonary dysplasia per modified Shennan definition), cumulative caffeine exposure post intervention, total parental STS time during the study period, and pretreatment status on the General Movements Assessment exam. Several pretreatment covariates will be statistically controlled if needed. These variables will be quantified at the pretreatment period, as past research or theory suggests they may be associated with neurodevelopmental outcomes or sensory processing [58, 62–65]. If preliminary tests of between-group differences of these pretreatment variables and the pretreatment variables showing between-group differences are associated with putative mediators or neurodevelopmental outcomes, these will be statistically controlled. Their statistical interaction with treatment group must be tested as part of the process of determining whether they should be statistically controlled. Thus, the possibility that treatment varies as a function of these biological variables will also be explored.
Intervention design
Standard care
The standard care of infants currently follows two medical protocols; one for STS holding and one for exposure to the parent’s voice. The protocols will be monitored as follows:
Exposure to recorded parent’s voice
Preterm infants in the NICU currently receive non-contingent recordings of the parent’s voice during two to three 20-min sessions daily. Recordings are standardized [66]. Recordings are then played through a sterilizable music player with a median volume of 45 dB and a maximum volume of 55 dB [67]. Monitoring of compliance with standard care will be accomplished through daily review of the medical record to determine the number of times the recording is played and to ensure that the recorded voice is never played at the same time as STS.
Skin-to-skin holding
Per standard care, STS care will be carried out by parents in both groups. Parents in the NICU currently use either their hands or positioners to facilitate prolonged STS. Infants are placed in a prone position with head positioned over the sternum, allowing transmission of breath and heart sounds to the developing ear. Deeper pressure is applied to offer support and feedback to the child’s bottom. When primary caregivers are not comfortable with direct STS contact (e.g. are not a direct relation to the child, or parent or child skin problem), a thin single-use hospital gown that is not previously imprinted with the parent’s scent is used to facilitate the experience without hindering sound transmission. In order to ensure safety during STS holding, vital signs including heart rate, breathing patterns and rate, oxygen saturation and temperature are continuously and automatically monitored with preset alarms per unit protocol. If any negative deviation from the infant’s daily vital sign patterns occurs, the nurses examine the infant and decide whether to stop the STS. To monitor STS, we will review the medical record for daily start time, duration, and caregiver during STS. We will also record any deviations from autonomic stability during STS (tachy/bradycardic events, tachypnea, or apnea).
Multisensory intervention
The MS intervention will be carried out in addition to standard care and will include the following components: holding and light pressure containment of the infant against the hospital-gown covered chest of the therapist for tactile and non-specific auditory stimulation simultaneous with playing of the mother’s voice contingent on infant pacifier sucking for the first 20 min of holding [68]. Additionally, a cotton square from a T-shirt scented with parent’s skin will be placed under the infant’s face on the therapists’ chest, to provide olfactory stimulation without risk of suffocation. The final component of the MS intervention is the carefully regulated, mindful breathing of the therapist for infant vestibular stimulation. The 20 intervention sessions will be dispersed across 2–3 weeks. On the occasion that the parent is present, parental STS will always take precedence over the intervention and the intervention session will be separated from previous parental holding by a minimum of 2 nursing care intervals (6 h) in one 24-h period. Infant autonomic stability and negative deviations during the MS intervention with the therapist will be recorded as above for parent STS care.
Contingent parent’s voice exposure
We will use the Pacifier Activated Lullaby® (PAL®) device, a 510 k FDA approved digital music delivery system that integrates a sensor, a pacifier routinely used in the NICU, and a receiver [68]. It delivers a predetermined 10 s of recorded parent’s voice singing lullabies upon detection of a suck that meets a preset pressure threshold. The original systems were modified for research use by decreasing the lower limit of activation thresholds for delivering the recording [69]. Minimal effort is required to trigger the device. However, the settings ensure that regular attempts are needed to continue to receive continual presentation of the recording of mother’s voice by requiring another suck after 10 s. The auditory simulation with PAL will be provided when the infants are still awake (i.e., during the first 20 min of holding).
STS holding
The therapist will wear a clean single use cotton hospital gown and wrap the “kangaroo” positioner securely over the top. The positioner will allow containment and even deep pressure and PAL operations. Should assistance be required, personnel will use the unit standard personal voice-activated call system to request a team member and minimize disruptions to the infant. The scented cotton placed next to the infant’s nose will be obtained by having the parent wear for a timed interval, cutting it with gloved hands and storing it in a sealed bag immediately upon removal. Cloth will be replaced if contaminated with infant’s bodily fluids.
Relaxation training of the MS therapy team
Because effects of STS holding are thought to be partly mediated by the holder’s heart and respiratory rate and because therapists are providing vestibular stimulation to infants, it is essential to maintain calm throughout the PAL administration in the way that parents would while holding their infant without additional activities. To promote this, therapists will attend a workshop on mindfulness techniques and practice this prior to start of the intervention [70]. Data on therapist heart and respiratory regulation in simulated stressful sessions will be obtained before, after, and one month after consistent training to ensure proficiency and fidelity before start of treatment. During intervention MS sessions, therapists will wear Spire Stone (Spire Inc., San Francisco CA) breathing rate monitors which provides a gentle reminder should they need return to a calmer state.
Ensuring high intervention fidelity
A script for the therapy session detailing the essential steps and sequence of the procedure is produced in a video with a checklist of critical elements [71]. All study therapists and an independent observer in the laboratory study the videos prior to beginning implementation. During a common training phase, the observer scores all therapists on all steps using a Likert-scaled rating system. The therapists also score themselves on the fidelity rating scale in order to immediately compare their self-assessments with those of the observer. The training phase is concluded when there is 90% adherence to the protocol and concordance between therapists and observer scoring. A random sample of 10% of all MS treatment sessions for each patient and intervention therapist will be assessed for fidelity with the checklist rating scale by the trained observer.
Outcomes assessment methods
Our primary outcome is multisensory response and our secondary outcome is neurodevelopmental outcomes, including sensory adaptation, motor, language, tactile processing, and speech sound differentiation (Table 1).
Table 1.
Construct | Procedure(s) | Timing |
---|---|---|
Multisensory response (Auditory-Tactile processing) | ERP to simultaneous puff + speech sound | Pre- and post-intervention (near discharge) |
Sensory functioning | ITSP | 9–12 months |
Language | Bayley III PLS-5 |
22–26 months |
Motor | Bayley III | 9–12 months 22–26 months |
Tactile processing Functional tactile connectivity |
ERP to calibrated air puff ERP to calibrated air puff |
Pre/Post intervention Pre/Post intervention |
Speech Sound differentiation | ERP to 6 speech sounds | Pre/Post intervention |
This table shows the experimental constructs, procedures, and timing of variables for analysis. (ERP Event-Related Potential, ITSP Infant Toddler Sensory Profile, PLS-5 Preschool Language Scales-5)
Sensory processing measurement by ERP
ERP recording
A high-density array of 128 electrodes embedded in soft sponges (Hydrocel Sensor Net, EGI, Inc., Eugene, OR) will be used to record ERPs with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz, filters set to 0.1–400 Hz. Recording of brainwaves will be controlled by Net Station (v. 4.3; EGI, Inc., Eugene, OR). E-Prime (v. 4.0, PST, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA) software will control stimulus delivery.
Stimulus-presentation paradigms
The ERP procedure involves blocks of trials from four conditions: multisensory (simultaneous speech sound-puff), puff alone, speech sound alone, and sham puff in a randomly generated sequence. To prevent habituation, no more than 2 repetitions of a condition occur in a row, with inter-trial intervals varying randomly between 2000 and 2500 ms. The “light touch” stimulus is an air puff emanating from a nozzle positioned above the skin of the palmar surface of the right hand secured in a mold holder. Another mold holder connected to the second nozzle is placed 15 cm away at midline (sham condition). The entire test session generates 60 trials per condition and lasts 8–10 min [72, 73]. The speech sound condition is a computer-generated woman’s voicing of one of six syllables (i.e. /ba/, /da/, /ga/, /bu./, /du/, /gu/) delivered in a free field setting using a microphone placed at midline 15 cm. The speech stimuli are computer-synthesized consonant-vowel syllables as previously published [74]. The syllables are presented at 65 dB SPL(A) (sound pressure level). More than the 2 key stimuli (/bu./ and /gu/) are presented to prevent habituation [59].
Preparation and analysis of ERP data
The recorded data will be filtered using a 0.3–40 Hz bandpass filter and segmented on stimulus onset to include a 200-ms pre-stimulus baseline and a 500-ms post-stimulus interval. Electrodes will be referred to Cz and re-referenced offline to an average reference. Resulting segments will be screened for motor/ocular artifacts using standard algorithms in NetStation, followed by a manual review. We will utilize previously published time windows and electrode clusters.
Index of multisensory processing (IMP)
We have already identified template maps over the cumulative 500 ms post-stimulus time window using a topographic cluster analysis (i.e., Topographic Atomize & Agglomerate Hierarchical Clustering approach) for full-term (FT) and preterm (PT) infants, which account for 96.3% of the global variance in ERP response to the MS stimulus [75]. Using a within-participant spatial correlation process (i.e., cases are electrodes and the two variables are (a) participant’s ERP and (b) template map value), we can identify which template map (FT or PT) best fits each time-samples’ observed topographical data for each participant. From this procedure, we calculate the percentage of time the topographical pattern of the participant’s ERP to MS stimuli is most like the FT template map. We call this index the IMP. This index is expressed as a percentage from 100% (all time samples show nearly-typical activation) to 0% (no time samples show nearly-typical activation).
Infant Toddler Sensory Profile (ITSP) [76–78]
The ITSP is the most validated test for the behavioral evaluation of sensory processing. This parent-rated questionnaire has 48 questions, addressing five sensory processing sections and a General Measure. One variable from this instrument is the infant’s neurological threshold (tendency to respond to sensory stimuli). Raw individual section scores are also provided. The ITSP has been used in large studies of preterm infants.
Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley III) — 3rd Edition [79]
The Bayley III is the gold standard for the evaluation of former NICU graduates, especially preterm infants. We will use the language and motor composite standard scores for corrected age. The Bayley is currently administered in the Follow-Up Clinic by trained examiners who undergo yearly retraining by a Gold-standard examiner.
Analysis plan
Power/sample size
A total sample size of 200 (100 in each group) will be recruited. Although attrition in past similar work has been much lower, we estimate power under an assumption of 10% attrition (i.e., 180). Because pilot studies afford effect size estimates with wide confidence intervals due to small sample sizes, power analysis results were conducted for the effect size available from pilot data and lowest effect size that the after-attrition sample size affords. Feasibility of the expected effect size is then evaluated. Power analyses are based on primary variables (multi- and uni- sensory processing). If between-group differences occur on the putative covariates (i.e., pretreatment variables related to the outcomes) and they are associated with putative mediators or outcomes, the pretreatment variables will be statistically controlled after ensuring that the homogeneity of slopes assumption has been met. Pretreatment ERP variables will be statistically controlled regardless of the effect size of between-group differences to improve effect size estimates. Missing data will be handled using multiple imputation [61].
Statistical analysis
Our first aim is to demonstrate that preterm infants receiving a standardized multisensory auditory-tactile-olfactory-vestibular intervention in addition to standard of care in the NICU will have more typical cortical multisensory processing at discharge and better sensory adaptation and motor and language outcomes than infants receiving only standard of care. For Aim 1, the between-group differences will be tested on ERP-measured multisensory processing at discharge from the NICU, sensory reactivity and adaption at Year 1, and motor and language ability at Year 2. When the dependent variable is continuous, we will use ANCOVAs when controlling for pretreatment variables and independent sample t-tests when there is no need for a covariate. When testing the nominal form of the sensory reactivity and adaptation outcome, we will use logistic regression.
Statistical power
Using PASS software, the estimated power using a 1-tailed test for an effect size based on pilot data with a sample size of 180 is over 99%. Even if the actual effect size is g = .35, we will have over 80% power to detect the effect under the proposed conditions.
Our second aim is to test the role of multisensory responses at discharge in mediating intervention effects on later sensory adaptation and motor and language outcomes.
Mediation models are presented in Fig. 2. The indirect effect (the product of the a-path coefficient * the b path coefficient) will be tested for significance using the bias-corrected bootstrap method [80]. The a-path is the main effect of treatment on the ERP measure of multisensory processing. The b-path is the association of the ERP measure of multisensory processing with the outcome controlling for the treatment group.
Statistical power
Using simulation data, an indirect effect produced by paths each with at least the mid-point between small and medium effect sizes (0.26; i.e., at least 6.8% of the variance of the criterion variable accounted for in each path) will be detected with over 80% power when using a sample size of 180 [81].
Our third aim is to explore the role of unisensory responses in mediating intervention effects on later motor and language outcomes. The putative mediators are the two unisensory processing (i.e., speech processing or touch) variables. The putative mediators are speech processing for the language outcome and touch processing for the motor outcome.
Statistical power
With regards to the a-path, the sample-size-adjusted standardized mean difference on speech processing effect size from the contingent parent-voice versus control (a treatment that is a subset of the proposed treatment) contrast was large. The proposed treatment will provide more nurturing touch than infants in control group and nurturing touch has been associated with better touch perception experience in our preliminary studies.
Data management
All data will be stored using Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap), a secure, web-based application designed to support data capture for research studies by building and managing online surveys and databases [82]. Support for REDCap is available through Nationwide Children’s Hospital.
Ethics
This study is based on our previous work and the NIH call for applications regarding multisensory processing and interventions [83]. This NIH-funded study was approved by the Nationwide Children’s Hospital IRB. It was assigned a risk level 1 (no greater than minimal risk). Informed consent will be obtained from the parent/guardian in accordance with the IRB protocol. A data safety monitoring committee consisting of the principal investigator, study coordinator, NICU nursing clinical leader, clinical program manager of developmental therapists in the NICU, and a parent of a NICU graduate on the parent advisory committee will further oversee the ongoing study.
Discussion
Previous studies prove that early sensory experiences shape brain development in former preterm infants [13, 15, 23, 24]. A few associative studies have demonstrated improved neurodevelopmental outcomes with supportive, targeted sensory input (ie STS or infant directed speech) [19, 30–33, 51–53]. A few other studies have examined associations between protocolized multisensory interventions, such as Auditory, Tactile, Visual and Vestibular Stimulus (ATVV) with short term NICU outcomes, such as feeding, behavioral states, and neuromotor assessments [84–87]. Only one study of ATVV with 37 preterm patients examined neurodevelopment at one year [88], and no studies have examined neurodevelopment after one year. To our knowledge, ours is the first RCT to design and test a protocolized MS intervention using brain-based measurements in order to elucidate the causal effects of the MS intervention on neural processing changes to mediate neurodevelopmental outcomes. This study provides a critical link in further understanding interactions between brain development, plasticity, environmental input, and subsequent neurodevelopment in this particularly vulnerable population of infants, and offers the possibility of an intervention that could be implemented in a variety of NICU settings.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Funding
This trial is funded through a five-year grant from the NICHD/NIH R01 HD093706 awarded to NL Maitre. Funding received from this grant supports direct research costs and research assistant salaries. The NICHD had no direct role in the design of this trial and will not in the collection, analysis or interpreting of data, or manuscript writing.
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- ERP
Event-related potential
- FOT
Fidelity of treatment
- FT
Full-term
- IMP
Index of multisensory processing
- IRB
Institutional Review Board
- ITSP
Infant Toddler Sensory Profile
- MS
Multisensory
- MSP
Multisensory processes
- NICU
Neonatal Intensive Care Unit
- PAL®
Pacifier Activated Lullaby®
- PLS-5
Preschool Language Scales-5
- PMA
Postmenstrual age
- PT
Preterm
- RCT
Randomized Controlled Trial
- REDCap
Research Electronic Data Capture
- SPL
Sound pressure level
- STS
Skin-to-skin care
Authors’ contributions
NLM: is the project leader, developed the original concept of the trial and drafted the original trial protocol and methodology. PY contributed to trial design and designed all statistical plans. MLN wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. SB, AM, JP, KH, LE, CP, ML: are participating in patient recruitment and data collection. PM and MM: are performing the ERP analysis. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
This study has been approved by the Institutional Review Board at Nationwide Children’s Hospital (#18–00056). Parents or guardians provide written consent for their child to participate.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
None of the authors have any competing interests to disclose.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Mary Lauren Neel, Phone: 614-355-6656, Email: marylauren.neel@nationwidechildrens.org.
Paul Yoder, Email: paul.yoder@vanderbilt.edu.
Pawel J. Matusz, Email: pawel.matusz@gmail.com
Micah M. Murray, Email: Micah.Murray@chuv.ch
Ashley Miller, Email: Ashley.Miller2@nationwidechildrens.org.
Stephanie Burkhardt, Email: Stephanie.Burkhardt@nationwidechildrens.org.
Lelia Emery, Email: Lelia.Emery@nationwidechildrens.org.
Kaleigh Hague, Email: Kaleigh.Hague@nationwidechildrens.org.
Caitlin Pennington, Email: Caitlin.Pennington@nationwidechildrens.org.
Jessica Purnell, Email: Jessica.Purnell@nationwidechildrens.org.
Megan Lightfoot, Email: Megan.Lightfoot@nationwidechildrens.org.
Nathalie L. Maitre, Email: Nathalie.Maitre@nationwidechildrens.org
References
- 1.Lawn J. Born too soon: the global action report on preterm birth. In: World Health Organization. 2012. http://www.who.int/pmnch/media/news/2012/20120522_joylawn_presentation.pdf?ua=1. Accessed 24 Sept 2018.
- 2.The global problem of premature birth. In: March of Dines. http://www.marchofdimes.org/mission/the-global-problem-of-premature-birth.aspx. Accessed 24 Sept 2018.
- 3.Kirkby S, Greenspan JS, Kornhauser M, Schneiderman R. Clinical outcomes and cost of the moderately preterm infant. Adv Neonat Care. 2007;7:80–87. doi: 10.1097/01.ANC.0000267913.58726.f3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cradock MM, Anand KJ. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis. J Am Med Assoc. 2002;288:728–737. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.6.728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.van Baar AL, Vermaas J, Knots E, de Kleine MJ, Soons P. Functioning at school age of moderately preterm children born at 32 to 36 weeks’ gestational age. Pediatrics. 2009;124:251–257. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kerstjens JM, de Winter AF, Bocca-Tjeertes IF, ten Vergert EM, Reijneveld SA, Bos AF. Developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children at school entry. J Pediatr. 2011;159:92–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.12.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kerstjens JM, Bocca-Tjeertes IF, de Winter AF, Reijneveld SA, Bos AF. Neonatal morbidities and developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children. Pediatrics. 2012;130:e265–e272. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-0079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kapellou O, Counsell S, Kennea N, et al. Abnormal cortical development after premature birth shown by altered allometric scaling of brain growth. PLoS Med. 2006;3:e265. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.McMahon E, Wintermark P, Lahav A. Auditory brain development in premature infants: the importance of early experience. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1252(1):17–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06445.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ment L, Kesler S, Vohr B, et al. Longitudinal brain volume changes in preterm and term control subjects during late childhood and adolescence. Pediatrics. 2009;123:503–511. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pineda R, Neil J, Dierker D, et al. Alterations in brain structure and neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm infants hospitalized in different neonatal intensive care unit environments. J Pediatr. 2014;164:52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.08.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Pineda R, Guth R, Herring A, Reynolds L, Oberle S, Smith J. Enhancing sensory experiences for very preterm infants in the NICU: an integrative review. J Perinatol. 2017;37:323–332. doi: 10.1038/jp.2016.179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tierney AL, Nelson CA., III Brain development and the role of experience in the early years. Zero Three. 2009;30:9–13. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Thompson RA, Nelson CA. Developmental science and the media: early brain development. Am Psychol. 2001;56:5–15. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Nelson CA, Furtado EA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH. The deprived human brain: developmental deficits among institutionalized Romanian children—and later improvements—strengthen the case for individualized care. Am Sci. 2009;97:222–229. doi: 10.1511/2009.78.222. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ranger M, Grunau RE. Early repetitive pain in preterm infants in relation to the developing brain. Pain Manag. 2014;4:57–67. doi: 10.2217/pmt.13.61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Vinall J, Miller SP, Chau V, Brummelte S, Synnes AR, Grunau RE. Neonatal pain in relation to postnatal growth in infants born very preterm. Pain. 2012;153:1374–1381. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Brummelte S, Grunau R, Chau V, et al. Procedural pain and brain development in premature newborns. Ann Neurol. 2012;71:385–396. doi: 10.1002/ana.22267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Maitre NL, Key AP, Chorna O, et al. The dual nature of early-life experience on somatosensory processing in the human infant brain. Curr Biol. 2017;27:1048–1054. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.02.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Chorna O, Solomon JE, Slaughter JC, Stark AR, Maitre NL. Abnormal sensory reactivity in preterm infants during the first year correlates with adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes at 2 years of age. Arch Dis Child-Fetal. 2014;99:F475–F479. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2014-306486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Brauer J, Xiao Y, Poulain T, Friederici AD, Schirmer A. Frequency of maternal touch predicts resting activity and connectivity of the developing social brain. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26:3544–3552. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Eeles A, Anderson P, Brown N, et al. Sensory profiles obtained from parental reports correlate with independent assessments of development in very preterm children at 2 years of age. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89:1075–1080. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Baroncelli L, Braschi C, Spolidoro M, Begenisic T, Sale A, Maffei L. Nurturing brain plasticity: impact of environmental enrichment. Cell Death Differ. 2009;17:1092–1103. doi: 10.1038/cdd.2009.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lewkowicz D, Ghazanfar A. The emergence of multisensory systems through perceptual narrowing. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13:470–478. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Maitre NL, Lambert WE, Aschner JL, Key AP. Cortical speech sound differentiation in the neonatal intensive care unit predicts cognitive and language development in the first 2 years of life. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55:834–839. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Eeles A, Anderson P, Brown N, et al. Sensory profiles of children born <30 weeks' gestation at 2years of age and their environmental and biological predictors. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89:727–732. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Botvinick M, Niv Y, Barto A. Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: a reinforcement learning perspective. Cognition. 2009;113:262–280. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Fonagy P, Gergely G, Target M. The parent-infant dyad and the construction of the subjective self. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2007;48:288–328. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01727.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Barrett L, Bar M. See it with feeling: affective predictions during object perception. Philos T Roy Soc B. 2009;364:1325–1334. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Caskey M, Stephens B, Tucker R, Vohr B. Adult talk in the NICU with preterm infants and developmental outcomes. Pediatrics. 2014;133:e578–e584. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-0104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Caskey M, Stephens B, Tucker R, Vohr B. Importance of parent talk on the development of preterm infant vocalizations. Pediatrics. 2011;128:910–916. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-0609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lester B, Hawes K, Abar B, et al. Single-family room care and neurobehavioral and medical outcomes in preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2014;134:754–760. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-4252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Filippa M, Devouche E, Arioni C, Imberty M, Gratier M. Live maternal speech and singing have beneficial effects on hospitalized preterm infants. Acta Paediatr. 2013;102:1017–1020. doi: 10.1111/apa.12356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Naoi N, Minagawa-Kawai Y, Kobayashi A, et al. Cerebral responses to infant-directed speech and the effect of talker familiarity. Neuroimage. 2012;59:1735–1744. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Moon C, Fifer W. Evidence of Transnatal auditory learning. J Perinatol. 2000;20:S37–S44. doi: 10.1038/sj.jp.7200448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.DeCasper A, Carstens A. Contingencies of stimulation: effects on learning and emotion in neonates. Infant Behav Dev. 1981;4:19–35. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(81)80004-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Feldman R, Eidelman A. Skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo care) accelerates autonomic and neurobehavioural maturation in preterm infants. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2003;45:274–281. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2003.tb00343.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Ludington-Hoe S, Anderson GC, Swinth J, Thompson C, Hadeed A. Randomized controlled trial of kangaroo care: cardiorespiratory and thermal effects on healthy preterm infants. Neonatal Netw. 2008;23:39–48. doi: 10.1891/0730-0832.23.3.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bauer K, Uhrig C, Sperling P, Pasel K, Wieland C, Versmold HT. Body temperatures and oxygen consumption during skin-to-skin (kangaroo) care in stable preterm infants weighing less than 1500 grams. J Pediatr. 1997;130:240–244. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(97)70349-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Feldman R, Rosenthal Z, Eidelman A. Maternal-preterm skin-to-skin contact enhances child physiologic organization and cognitive control across the first 10 years of life. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75:56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Reynolds L, Duncan M, Smith G, et al. Parental presence and holding in the neonatal intensive care unit and associations with early neurobehavior. J Perinatol. 2013;33:636–641. doi: 10.1038/jp.2013.4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Busse M, Stromgren K, Thorngate L, Thomas KA. Parents' responses to stress in the neonatal intensive care unit. Crit Care Nurse. 2013;33:52–59. doi: 10.4037/ccn2013715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Feeley N, Waitzer E, Sherrard K, Boisvert L, Zelkowitz P. Fathers' perceptions of the barriers and facilitators to their involvement with their newborn hospitalised in the neonatal intensive care unit. J Clin Nurs. 2013;22:521–530. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2012.04231.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Garten L, Maass E, Schmalisch G, Bührer C. O father, where art thou?: parental NICU visiting patterns during the first 28 days of life of very low-birth-weight infants. J Perinat Neonat Nur. 2011;25:342–348. doi: 10.1097/JPN.0b013e318233b8c3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Chan GJ, Labar AS, Wall S, Atun R. Kangaroo mother care: a systematic review of barriers and enablers. B World Health Organ. 2015;94:130–41J. doi: 10.2471/BLT.15.157818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Murray M, Wallace M. The neural bases of multisensory processes. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2011. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Maitre N. Neurorehabilitation after neonatal intensive care: evidence and challenges. Arch Dis Child-Fetal. 2015;100:F534–F540. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2013-305920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Johnston M. Plasticity in the developing brain: implications for rehabilitation. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2009;15:94–101. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kolb B, Gibb R. Brain plasticity and recovery from early cortical injury. Dev Psychobiol. 2007;49:107–118. doi: 10.1002/dev.20199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Bremner A, Lewkowicz D, Spence C. Multisensory development. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012. The multisensory approach to development; pp. 1–26. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Altimier L, Phillips R. The neonatal integrative developmental care model: advanced clinical applications of the seven Core measures for neuroprotective family-centered developmental care. Newborn Infant Nurs Rev. 2016;16:230–244. doi: 10.1053/j.nainr.2016.09.030. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Campbell-Yeo ML, Disher TC, Benoit BL, Johnston CC. Understanding kangaroo care and its benefits to preterm infants. Pediatric Health Med Ther. 2015;6:15–32. doi: 10.2147/PHMT.S51869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Vesel L, Bergh A, Kerber K, et al. Kangaroo mother care: a multi-country analysis of health system bottlenecks and potential solutions. BMC Pregnancy Childb. 2015;15:S5. doi: 10.1186/1471-2393-15-S2-S5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Zimmerman I, Steiner V, Pond R. The preschool language scale- 5. Pearson: San Antonio; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Pannucci C, Wilkins E. Identifying and avoiding Bias in research. Plast Reconstr Surgery. 2010;126:619–625. doi: 10.1097/PRS.0b013e3181de24bc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Borrelli B. The assessment, monitoring, and enhancement of treatment fidelity in public health clinical trials. J Public Health Dent. 2011;71:S52–S63. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-7325.2011.00233.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Maitre N, Henderson G, Gogliotti S, et al. Feasibility of event-related potential methodology to evaluate changes in cortical processing after rehabilitation in children with cerebral palsy: a pilot study. J Clin Exp Neuropsyc. 2014;36:669–679. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.925094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Maitre NL, Chan J, Stark AR, Lambert WE, Aschner JL, Key AP. Effects of caffeine treatment for apnea of prematurity on cortical speech-sound differentiation in preterm infants. J Child Neurol. 2014;30:307–313. doi: 10.1177/0883073814538500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Key AP, Lambert EW, Aschner JL, Maitre NL. Influence of gestational age and postnatal age on speech sound processing in NICU infants. Psychophysiology. 2012;49:720–731. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01353.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.West S, Sagarin B. Participant selection and loss in randomized experiments. In: Bickman L, editor. Research design: Donald Cambell’s legacy. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publishing; 2000. pp. 117–154. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Hayes A. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Volpe J. The encephalopathy of prematurity--brain injury and impaired brain development inextricably intertwined. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2009;16:167–178. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2009.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Malaeb S, Dammann O. Fetal inflammatory response and brain injury in the preterm newborn. J Child Neurol. 2009;24:1119–1126. doi: 10.1177/0883073809338066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Maitre N, Ballard R, Ellenberg J, et al. Respiratory consequences of prematurity: evolution of a diagnosis and development of a comprehensive approach. J Perinatol. 2015;35:313–321. doi: 10.1038/jp.2015.19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Darsaklis V, Snider LM, Majnemer A, Mazer B. Predictive validity of Prechtl’s method on the qualitative assessment of general movements: a systematic review of the evidence. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2011;53:896–906. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04017.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Sounds of Love. http://www.nationwidechildrens.org/nicu-sounds-of-love. Accessed September 25, 2018.
- 67.Center for Perinatal Research, Nationwide Childrens Hospital . FDA Patent application: Dino Egg (Sanitizable and safe audio device for developmentally-appropriate delivery of parents voice) 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Standley JM. FDA Patent US5830235 A. Pacifier system and method of therapeutically treating infant sucking response 1998
- 69.Chorna OD, Slaughter JC, Wang L, Stark AR, Maitre NL. A pacifier-activated music player with mother’s voice improves oral feeding in preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2014;133:462–468. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-2547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Tang Y, Ma Y, Fan Y, et al. Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short-term meditation. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:8865–8870. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904031106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Chorna O, Heathcock J, Key A, et al. Early childhood constraint therapy for sensory/motor impairment in cerebral palsy: a randomised clinical trial protocol. BMJ Open. 2015;5:e010212. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Maitre NL, Key AP. Quantitative assessment of cortical auditory-tactile processing in children with disabilities. JOVE-J Vis Exp. 2014;83:e51054. doi: 10.3791/51054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Maitre NL, Barnett Z, Key AP. Novel assessment of cortical response to somatosensory stimuli in children with hemiparetic cerebral palsy. J Child Neurol. 2012;27:1276–1283. doi: 10.1177/0883073811435682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Stevens K, Blumstein S. Invariant cues for place of articulation in stop consonants. J Acoust Soc Am. 1978;64:1358–1368. doi: 10.1121/1.382102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Murray M, Brunet D, Michel CM. Topographic ERP analyses: a step-by-step tutorial review. Brain Topogr. 2008;20:249–264. doi: 10.1007/s10548-008-0054-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Daniels D, Dunn W. Development of the infant-toddler sensory profile. Occup Ther J Res. 2000;20:86S–90S. doi: 10.1177/15394492000200S107. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Eeles A, Olsen J, Walsh J, et al. Reliability of neurobehavioral assessments from birth to term equivalent age in preterm and term born infants. Phys Occup Ther Pedi. 2017;37:108–119. doi: 10.3109/01942638.2015.1135845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Eeles A, Spittle A, Anderson P, et al. Assessments of sensory processing in infants: a systematic review. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55:314–326. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04434.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 80.Preacher K, Hayes A. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav Res Meth Ins C. 2004;36:717–731. doi: 10.3758/BF03206553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Fritz M, MacKinnon D. Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychol Sci. 2007;18:233–239. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01882.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Electronic data capture (REDCap) - a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009;42:377–381. doi: 10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.National Institutes of Health. Funding opportunity Research on the Mechanisms and/or Behavioral Outcomes of Multisensory Processing (R01). https://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/pa-files/PA-15-347.html. Accessed September 25, 2018.
- 84.Medoff-Cooper B, Rankin K, Li Z, Liu L, White-Traut R. Multi-sensory intervention for preterm infants improves sucking organization. Adv Neonatal Care. 2015;15:142–149. doi: 10.1097/ANC.0000000000000166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.White-Traut R, Rankin KM, Pham T, Li Z, Liu L. Preterm infants’ orally directed behaviors and behavioral state responses to the integrated H-HOPE intervention. Infant Behav Dev. 2014;37:583–596. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.White-Traut RC, Rankin KM, Yoder JC, et al. Influence of H-HOPE intervention for premature infants on growth, feeding progression and length of stay during initial hospitalization. J Perinatol. 2015;35:636–641. doi: 10.1038/jp.2015.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Kanagasabai PS, Mohan D, Lewis LE, Kamath A, Rao BK. Effect of multisensory stimulation on neuromotor development in preterm infants. Indian J Pediatr. 2013;80:460–464. doi: 10.1007/s12098-012-0945-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Nelson MN, White-Traut RC, Vasan U, et al. One-year outcome of auditory-tactile-visual-vestibular intervention in the neonatal intensive care unit: effects of severe prematurity and central nervous system injury. J Child Neurol. 2001;16:493–498. doi: 10.1177/088307380101600706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.