Abstract
For the premature newborn, little is known about changes in brain activity during transition to extra-uterine life. We aim to quantify these changes in relation to the longer-term maturation of the developing brain. We analysed EEG for up to 72 hours after birth from 28 infants born <32 weeks of gestation. These infants had favourable neurodevelopment at 2 years of age and were without significant neurological compromise at time of EEG monitoring. Quantitative EEG was generated using features representing EEG power, discontinuity, spectral distribution, and inter-hemispheric connectivity. We found rapid changes in cortical activity over the 3 days distinct from slower changes associated with gestational age: for many features, evolution over 1 day after birth is equivalent to approximately 1 to 2.5 weeks of maturation. Considerable changes in the EEG immediately after birth implies that postnatal adaption significantly influences cerebral activity for early preterm infants. Postnatal age, in addition to gestational age, should be considered when analysing preterm EEG within the first few days after birth.
Introduction
For the newborn preterm infant, many physiological changes occur during the transition at birth from intra- to extra-uterine life. For example, haemodynamic instability during this period can lead to brain injury, with incidence rates as high as 1 in 3 for infants born <32 weeks of gestation1. Monitoring brain function can help the clinician identify infants at risk of injury and inform treatment strategies. Quantitative evaluation of neurological function can maximise the utility of existing monitoring technologies in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU). Renewed interest in this area highlights the potential of a quantitative approach to improve clinical outcomes for these infants2–7.
The most commonly used method of brain monitoring in the NICU is the electroencephalogram (EEG). Between 24 and 32 weeks of gestation EEG activity varies considerably with gestational age (GA)8–17 reflecting the rapid increase in cerebral cortical volume18 and the process of neuronal organisation that begins at approximately five months of gestation19. One of the milestones of this process is the establishment and differentiation of the subplate neurons that provide the site of synaptic contact for thalamocortical and corticortical afferents before the cortical plate is formed and serve as the functional link between “waiting” afferents and cortical connections19,20. Quantitative analysis of the EEG, which involves extracting multiple statistical and signal-processing features from the EEG, provides objective and consistent analysis of this complex EEG. Quantitative EEG (qEEG) is also used to develop fully automated methods of EEG analysis; qEEG measures are used in methods to estimate maturation7,17 and classify sleep states6,21, for example. Such systems can integrate cotside and provide continuous information on cerebral function to the non-expert.
Although preterm EEG is well defined in relation to GA, little is known about how it evolves in the immediate period after birth. Many environmental factors, including the birth process, may have a significant impact on early postnatal adaptation22–24. Existing qEEG studies indicate that some features change during this period, and this change may be specific to GA25–27. The small number of non-specific qEEG features used in these studies, without features of spatial and temporal organisation for example, are not comprehensive enough to capture the complexity of the preterm EEG. In addition, quantification of the change in qEEG over time is lacking.
Quantifying the evolution of preterm EEG after birth in relation to intra-uterine maturation is important for both the visual and quantitative analysis of the EEG, particularly as dysmaturity of the EEG is a prominent diagnostic biomarker5,28,29. Furthermore, the EEG during postnatal adaption may be an important biomarker of neurological function that is complementary to longitudinal EEG monitoring. We hypothesize that the biological and environmental factors associated with preterm birth will alter the typical intra-uterine maturation of the EEG to produce rapid changes in the EEG. To test this hypothesis, we aim to quantify the temporal evolution of qEEG over the first 3 days after birth for preterm infants <32 weeks of gestation and quantify the role GA may play in this evolution.
Results
Twenty-eight infants met the inclusion criteria and were included in this study. Table 1 summarises the clinical characteristics for this cohort. Figure 1 shows the distribution of GA with median age of 28.5 weeks, range 24.0 to 31.9 weeks.
Table 1.
characteristic | n = 28 |
---|---|
female | 20 (71.4%) |
gestational age (weeks) | 28.5 (26.6, 30.1) |
Apgar score at 1 m | 8 (6, 8.25) |
Apgar score at 5 m | 9 (8, 9) |
birth weight (g) | 990 (785, 1250) |
pH | 7.22 (7.15, 7.28) |
cesarean delivery | 13 (46%) |
CRIB† | 8 (7, 10) |
medication‡ | |
morphine | 0 (0%) |
caffeine | 27 (96%) |
surfactant | 18 (64%) |
fentanyl and suxamethonium | 6 (21%) |
Bayleys III | |
cognitive | 95 (95, 103.8) |
motor | 100 (97, 106) |
language | 103 (97, 111.3) |
†Clinical risk index for babies (CRIB) II generated for n = 26. ‡Administered <72 hours after birth.
Median postnatal age (PNA) at the start of EEG recording was 7.8 h (inter-quartile range, IQR: 5.0 to 17.1 h; range: 3.6 to 61.9 h). Median duration of continuous EEG recording was 40.5 h (IQR: 24.2 to 48.0 h; range 4.4 to 71.1 h). The artefact-detection algorithm (O’Toole et al.30) rejected a median of 0.1% (IQR: 0 to 1.1%; range: 0 to 28.7%) of the EEG in each epoch. Median number of channels rejected by this algorithm, per epoch, was 0 (IQR: 0 to 1; range: 0 to 8). If 8 channels were identified as artefact, the epoch was removed from further analysis. As there was only 2 epochs at the 72 h time-point, we removed this time point while keeping the remaining 6, 12, 24, 36, 48, and 60 h time points. After EEG epoch selection, the median number of epochs per infant was 3.5 (IQR: 3 to 5; range: 1 to 6).
Fitting mixed-effects models
Fixed-effects estimates, including 95% confidence intervals, for all 34 features are presented in Tables 2 and 3. Most features are dependent on GA: 27/34 (79.4%) of the models include GA as a fixed effect. Over one-half of the features are dependent on PNA, with 19/34 (55.9%) models including PNA as a fixed effect. For this subset of 19 PNA-dependent features, our modelling approach allows for 3 types of temporal evolution.
Table 2.
Feature Type | Coefficients | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
group | Feature | FB | Cth | Intercept | PNA (days) | GA (weeks) | PNA × GA |
power | SP (μV2) | δ | −177 (−1519, 1142) |
1222 (600, 1821) |
18 (−30, 65) |
−38 (−60, −16) |
|
θ | 30 (−38, 94) |
33 (9, 59) |
−0.36 (−2.62, 2.01) |
−10 (−2, 0) |
|||
α | 4.8 (3.7, 5.8) |
1.1 (0.5, 1.7) |
|||||
β | 1.6 (1.1, 2.1) |
0.59 (0.24, 0.99) |
|||||
rEEG (μV) | 50th | 24 (20, 27) |
5.2 (3.4, 7.1) |
||||
5th | −7.6 (−16.3, 1.3) |
1.2 (0.1, 2.1) |
0.58 (0.27, 0.9) |
||||
95th | 93 (−154, 334) |
175 (54, 298) |
0.98 (−7.55, 9.79) |
−5.9 (−10.2, −1.4) |
|||
discontinuity | r-AS | 1.4 (1.2, 1.7) |
−0.037 (−0.058, −0.016) |
−0.027 (−0.036, −0.018) |
|||
skew | δ | 0.60 (−0.36, 1.55) |
0.72 (0.13, 1.32) |
−0.73e-3 (−35e-3, 33e-3) |
−0.026 (−0.047, −0.005) |
||
θ | 0.60 (0.47, 0.74) |
−0.015 (−0.026, −0.003) |
−0.014 (−0.019, −0.01) |
||||
α | 0.15 (0.13, 0.18) |
−4.2e-3 (−5.0e-3, −3.4e-3) |
|||||
β | 0.89 (0.67, 1.09) |
−0.023 (−0.031, −0.016) |
|||||
kurtosis | δ | 26 (19, 32) |
−0.62 (−0.84, −0.4) |
||||
θ | 78 (61, 95) |
−2.5 (−4, −1.1) |
−1.9 (−2.5, −1.3) |
||||
α | 86 (65, 106) |
−2.3 (−3, −1.6) |
|||||
β | 83 (61, 106) |
−2.2 (−3.1, −1.5) |
|||||
IBI (s) | 95th | 35 (24, 47) |
−0.99 (−1.84, −0.11) |
−0.81 (−1.23, −0.44) |
|||
50th | 6.2 (3.9, 8.5) |
−0.12 (−0.2, −0.04) |
|||||
burst% (%) | 3.3 (−34.9, 39.2) |
5.8 (2.7, 8.7) |
2.3 (1, 3.6) |
||||
burst# | 516 (368, 659) |
−27 (−44, −11) |
−10 (−15, −5) |
Frequency bands (FB): 0.5–3 Hz (δ), 3–8 Hz (θ), 8–15 Hz (α), and 15–30 Hz (β); Cth: centile. rEEG features (median, lower- and upper-margins) are described by corresponding centiles (50th, 5th, and 95th), as is median and maximum inter-burst intervals (50th and 95th centile). SP: spectral power; rEEG: range EEG; r-AS: rEEG asymmetry; IBI: inter-burst interval; burst%: burst ratio; burst#: number of bursts.
Table 3.
Feature type | Coefficients | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
group | Feature | FB | Intercept | PNA (days) | GA (weeks) | PNA × GA |
spectral distribution | RSP (%) | δ | 94 (93, 94) |
|||
θ | 5.1 (4.5, 5.9) |
−0.38 (−0.73, −0.06) |
||||
α | 1.1 (−1.2, 3.4) |
−1.7 (−3.4, 0) |
7.7e-3 (−73.0e-3, 90.4e-3) |
0.058 (−0.002, 0.117) |
||
β | −0.69 (−1.32, −0.05) |
0.041 (0.018, 0.063) |
||||
spectral flatness | δ | 0.27 (0.26, 0.28) |
||||
θ | 0.54 (0.39, 0.69) |
9.5e-3 (3.8e-3, 15.7e-3) |
7.0e-3 (1.6e-3, 12.2e-3) |
|||
α | 0.62 (0.56, 0.69) |
9.5e-3 (2.9e-3, 16.0-e3) |
7.2e-3 (5.0e-3, 9.4e-3) |
|||
β | 0.67 (0.66, 0.68) |
|||||
fractal dimension | 0.98 (0.82, 1.12) |
0.012 (0.007, 0.018) |
||||
SEF (Hz) | 6.2 (0.9, 11.9) |
−3.6 (−7.1, −0.1) |
−0.098 (−0.297, 0.089) |
0.12 (0.01, 0.24) |
||
connectivity | coherence | δ | 0.45 (0.31, 0.58) |
−0.011 (−0.015, −0.006) |
||
θ | 0.42 (0.33, 0.52) |
−0.010 (−0.014, −0.007) |
||||
α | 0.38 (0.28, 0.48) |
−0.010 (−0.013, −0.006) |
||||
β | 0.26 (0.18, 0.34) |
−0.006 (−0.009, −0.003) |
Frequency bands (FB): 0.5–3 Hz (δ), 3–8 Hz (θ), 8–15 Hz (α), and 15–30 Hz (β). RSP: relative spectral power; SEF: spectral edge frequency.
First, the model also includes GA, resulting in 2D trajectories over PNA and GA, such as the rEEG lower-margin feature in Fig. 2(a,b). Almost one-half (9/19) of the features are in this group. Second, the model includes the PNA–GA interaction term, resulting in diverging trends for different GAs, such as the upper-margin rEEG feature in Fig. 2(c,d). A smaller proportion (6/19) of the features fit this model type. And third, the model is dependent on PNA but independent of GA. A low number (4/19) of features meet this type, in keeping with the total percentage (20.5%) of features that are independent of GA.
To aid comprehension of the different model types, Fig. 3 presents an alternative visualisation of the temporal trajectories. These plots show the fixed-effect estimates of PNA plotted in context of increasing GA. For the first group (models with both PNA and GA terms), the change (trend) in PNA is the same but the starting point for this change is dependent on GA. Figure 3(a,c,d) show examples of these types of trajectories. For the second group (models including the PNA–GA interaction term), again the starting point of the PNA trend changes with GA, but also the direction of trend itself changes with increasing GA. Figure 3(b) shows an example of this trajectory type. Plots for all 6 models that include the interaction term are in Supplementary Fig. S1.
Figure 3 shows that the relative change in hours after birth (PNA) is comparable to weeks of intra-uterine maturation (GA). To quantify this relative change, we assess the ratio between postnatal adaption (or PNA) and maturation (or GA). We do so for the 9 features that included both PNA and GA fixed effects, but not the PNA–GA interaction term, as this is not a constant ratio, as Fig. 3(b) shows. Median (IQR) for these ratios, plotted in Fig. 4, are 1.37 (1.30 to 2.04) days/weeks with a range of 1.01 to 2.65 days/weeks. All ratios are positive, meaning the direction of the trend over PNA is in the same direction as the trend for maturation. Thus, the 2.25 days PNA included in our modelling, from 6 to 60 hours PNA, equates to a range of 2.27 to 5.96 weeks of intra-uterine maturation. This relatively large change is also evident in the plots in Fig. 3. For the rEEG lower margin in Fig. 3(a), for example, we can see that at 25 weeks of gestation, the increase after the 2.25 days of postnatal adaption is equivalent to maturation for a 30 week GA infant.
We found no effect for surfactant on the averaged qEEG features (P > 0.05, see Supplementary Table S1). Fentanyl–suxamethonium administration also had negligible effect on the qEEG: almost all (33/34) features were not altered by fentanyl, the exception being the δ-band EEG skew feature (see Supplementary Table S2). As almost all infants received caffeine (27/28), we could not examine the influence of caffeine on the qEEG in this study. None of the infants in our study received morphine before or during the EEG.
Example: Algorithm to Estimate EEG Maturational Age
The algorithm’s feature set included the 27 features that are dependent on gestational age (see Tables 1 and 3) in addition to PNA as a feature. The SD of the cross-validation testing error between GA and estimated EEG maturation was 9.1 days, compared to an SD of 16.0 days for the distribution of GA; correlation was 0.83 between known and estimated GA; and 96% (64%) of estimates were within ±2 (±1) weeks. Figure 5 plots the relation between known and estimated maturation, showing similar results to another method developed on a similar cohort17. The mean rank of relative influence of PNA was 5 (SD 1, range 2–9) from the total of 28 features. The reduction in performance by random permutation of PNA was 1.8% (SD 0.3%, range 1.1–2.9%).
Discussion
From a comprehensive set of qEEG features, we find rapid maturation for a subset of features over the first 60 hours after birth that is equivalent to between 2.3–6.0 weeks of intra-uterine maturation. Most (79%) qEEG measures are dependent on GA, which is in keeping with results from other qEEG studies12–17. What we show, however, is that the majority (56%) of the features change significantly over the first 3 days after birth. This temporal evolution for many features is consistent with increasing intra-uterine maturation, as indicated by the positive PNA-to-GA ratio for all features in Fig. 4.
This remarkable change in qEEG appears to represent an increase in continuity of the EEG, as continuity increases with maturation9–11. Also, of the features from the discontinuous group that included PNA as a dependent variable (7/13), almost all (6/7) showed a progression towards decreasing discontinuous activity. For example, decreasing maximum inter-burst interval in Table 1 indicates decreasing discontinuity of the EEG11. The only exception was the EEG-skew feature in the δ-band which show increasing trends from GAs <28 weeks and then decreasing trends thereafter (see Supplementary Fig. S1). An increase in continuous activity suggests an increase in active, or rapid-eye movement (REM), sleep31,32. Yet without polysomnography to define sleep states, we lack certainty on whether these changes are related to the evolution of the sleep-wake cycle over the first days after birth.
The increase in EEG continuity may reflect a recovery from a suppressed EEG during birth. The birth process is a major stressor for the newborn infant and possibly programmes the response of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis for life22. A certain degree of birth stress is necessary to provide adequate postnatal adaptation for term infants33–35, but the same might be disruptive for the cortical pathway development in the immature preterm brain resulting in altered EEG immediately after birth36.
Another explanation is that the significant temporal evolution is a response to medication. No infant received either anticonvulsants or morphine before or during the EEG recording. We found no effect for surfactant on the qEEG; for fentanyl–suxamethonium administration only 1 feature, from the set of 34, was significantly altered and therefore would not change the main findings presented here. We were unable to test the effect of caffeine therapy as almost all (27/28) babies received caffeine. Infants received a loading dose of 20 mg/kg (caffeine per kg of birth weight) followed by a maintenance dose of 10 mg/kg per day as required. Caffeine may influence the EEG: some studies have shown an increase in aEEG continuity in response to caffeine37,38, although a recent study found no changes in EEG39. A prospective study, with long-term continuous EEG monitoring and a control arm of infants not administered caffeine, is needed to determine the role caffeine may play in this.
Increased cerebral perfusion may be the driving mechanism for the evolving qEEG25,26. Different studies–using different measurement techniques–have shown an increase in cerebral blood flow volume over the first 2 to 3 days. These techniques include xenon-clearance methods40, ultrasound flowmetry41, and changes in oxygenated haemoglobin as measured by near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) in response to changes of the fraction of inspired oxygen for ventilated infants42,43. Other studies, using measures such as NIRS-derived cerebral oxygenation44–47 and superior vena cava flow48, also point to increased cerebral perfusion. For most of these studies the increase in cerebral blood flow (or oxygenation) is most prominent over days 1 and 240–43,45,47, with a smaller increase, or even decrease, on day 340,41,45,47. Although animal studies have shown that the local blood flow is not associated with neural activity in the developing brain49, it seems reasonable that the global nature and magnitude of increased blood flow during transition to the extra-uterine environment may induce increased neuronal activity. These changes in cerebral perfusion may represent normal cardiovascular adaption to postnatal life42,50.
These findings have implications for future studies: the time-varying nature of the EEG should be integrated into analysis of the EEG during this transitional period. For example, when using qEEG to compare between difference groups, PNA should be uniform between groups. If reporting qEEG values, PNA should also be reported. Similar consideration should also be applied to automated methods. For example in the maturation estimator developed here, we find that PNA is an important feature in the algorithm, ranked on average as the 5th most important feature from the set of 28.
This evolving EEG may also affect the visual interpretation of preterm EEG. For example, our results show that PNA alters the duration of maximum inter-burst intervals to the extent of almost 1 s/day, which is equivalent to 1.23 weeks of maturation. These changes may, in part, explain the large variability in reported maximum inter-burst intervals in the literature10. An evolving EEG may also alter grading of the EEG for abnormalities5,51. For example, dysmature patterns, defined as ≥2 weeks delayed maturity, are associated with chronic stage abnormalities and used during visual grading of the EEG to better define the prognosis. Many features, over the 2.25 days of recordings, change more than 2 weeks in maturation; see Fig. 4. Thus, it is plausible that the influence of PNA at EEG recording could falsely indicate dysmaturity for a normal EEG.
Although many studies have analysed extra-uterine maturation with serial EEG, for example Niemarkt et al. and Shany et al.52,53, only a few studies have reported changes in a limited qEEG feature set25–27; some of these findings overlap with ours. We find a similar direction of evolution for δ-power and relative θ- and α-power in studies by Victor et al.26 and Schumacher et al.27, but not in other absolute and relative frequency bands. Similar direction of evolution also for spectral edge frequency in studies by West et al.25 and Victor et al.26, although our modelling includes a PNA–GA interaction term (see Supplementary Fig. S1). And similar again for percentage of inter-burst intervals (inverse of our burst ratio measure) and maximum inter-burst interval (90th centile)26; and median amplitude (similar to our median rEEG measure)25. It is difficult to be more specific in our comparisons for various reasons. First, we selected a cohort without brain injuries and normal neurodevelopment at 2 years of age; other studies did not include neurodevelopmental outcome25–27. Second, methods for generating qEEG will differ; for example Victor et al.26 identified bursts and inter-bursts by visual interpretation on the EEG, whereas we used an automated method54 (although the automated method correlates well with the human interpretation). Also, there may be differences in how qEEG measures are defined. For example, West et al.25 used a medium amplitude measure from the aEEG, but we use rEEG as aEEG is not clearly defined30. The advantage of using the NEURAL feature set30, as we did here, is that all features are clearly defined with full implementation details, including open-source code55 to enable reproducible research and fair comparisons between different studies. Third, our modelling approach allows us to quantify the linear trend of the feature over time, beyond just testing for an increase or decrease as previous studies have done25–27. For example, median rEEG increases by 5.2 μV/day (95% CI: 3.4 to 7.1 μV/day). Including GA in the model, when appropriate, relates this trend in the context of the GA of the infant. For example, the maximum IBI decreases by 0.99 s/day (95% CI: 1.84 to 0.11 s/day) with PNA, but also decreases by 0.81 s/week (95% CI: 1.23 to 0.44 s/week) with GA.
Other factors strengthen our study over previous work. First, although the 1-h epochs were selected as being predominately artefact free, we include an automated method to remove further artefacts, if present30. This was identified by Schumacher et al.27 and Victor et al.26 as a missing component of their analysis, and a potential source of error when estimating spectral-edge frequency by West et al.25. Second, we include multiple time points (6 in total) over the first 3 days after birth, whereas previous studies included only 3, 1 per day. Third, we present a comprehensive set of qEEG features, including automated estimation of burst and inter-burst features (for example maximum IBI), measures of inter-hemispheric connectivity (coherence), higher-order moments of the EEG to represent discontinuity (skew and kurtosis), and measures of spectral shape (spectral entropy and fractal dimension); in addition to spectral power (absolute and relative) and voltage peak-to-peak measures (rEEG) as presented elsewhere25–27.
Although different sleep states will affect the qEEG, we did not include state annotations in our analysis for 2 reasons. First, the duration of the epoch (1 h) is sufficient to incorporate different states25,32. Second, we consider that the sampling of the EEG into epochs to be representative of the continuous recording. We determined sampling times (at 6, 12, 24, 36, 48, and 60 h PNA) before reviewing the EEG and selected epochs as close as possible to these time points, without regard to sleep states. Therefore, we think it unlikely that any sampling error will introduce bias in our results.
A limitation of our study is the moderate numbers of infants (n = 28) included in the analysis. The larger the cohort the stronger the inferences drawn; however, generating a large data set with continuous, long-duration EEG recordings from hours after birth with 2-year neurodevelopment assessment in a cohort of preterm infants <32 weeks of gestation requires significant effort and resources. Yet even with the moderate sample size, the mixed-effects models do find statistical significant (P < 0.05) linear trends for PNA and GA. Another limitation is that we analyse only the immediate postnatal period, up to 2.5 days. We expect that this rapid period of adaption will taper into the more moderate evolution of extra-uterine maturation52,53 or may even return to a baseline level. For this we would need serial EEG recordings to model a longer duration after birth, within the first week for example, including non-linear mixed effect models to account for the expected slowing of this rapid adaption period.
In conclusion, we find rapid evolution in the normal trajectory of qEEG over the first days of life for the preterm infant. The rate of change of the qEEG is significantly higher than intra-uterine changes over the same period. Post-natal adaption of the EEG must be considered when interpreting EEG recordings. The trajectories of EEG within this period can, potentially, be used as an additional biomarker for the identification of infants with disrupted neurological function, often caused by haemodynamic instability during postnatal transition, and complement longitudinal studies of the EEG during the preterm period for the prediction of neurodevelopmental outcome.
Methods
Patients
EEG data was retrospectively selected from a cohort of preterm infants recruited between 2009 and 2011 in the NICU of the Cork University Maternity Hospital, Ireland. Preterm infants <32 weeks of gestation had continuous video and EEG monitoring over the first 3 days after birth. Data collection was approved by the Cork Research Ethics Committee of Cork Teaching Hospitals, Ireland. Informed and written parental consent was obtained before EEG monitoring. Data collection and all methods in this study were conducted in accordance with the principals of the International Conference on Harmonisation of Technical Requirements for Registration of Pharmaceuticals for Human Use of Good Clinical Practice (ICH-GCP).
Preterm infants were included in this study if they met the following criteria:
GA <32 weeks;
Apgar ≥6 at 5 minutes after birth;
absence of congenital brain malformations, chromosomal or genetic aberrations, or inborn errors of metabolism;
absence of grade II or higher intra-ventricular haemorrhage (IVH) and absence of periventricular leukomalacia (PVL);
absence of major comorbidities (such as sepsis, necrotising enterocolitis, cardiac dysfunction, bronchopulmonary dysplasia) at time of EEG recording;
no anti-epileptic drugs before or during EEG recording;
EEG monitoring within 72 h from birth;
favourable outcome at 2 years of age or adverse outcome ascribable to later morbidities with respect to timing of EEG recording.
Outcome was assessed by the Bayley Scales of Infant Development-III at 2 years corrected age. We define favourable outcome as all three Bayley subscales (motor, cognitive, and language) within one standard deviation (SD) of the standardised mean score. IVH and PVL are assessed by cranial ultrasound as part of routine clinical care within the first few days after birth.
EEG recording
EEG was recorded using the NicoletOne device (Natus Medical Inc., Pleasanton, CA, USA) with 11 electrodes placed according to the international 10–20 system of electrode configuration. Electrodes covered the frontal, central, temporal, and occipital regions, with a reference electrode at Fz and a ground electrode behind the left ear56. Recording started after birth when clinical staff determined the infant was stable and continued for up to 72 h after birth.
EEG features
One-hour epochs were pruned from the EEG at 6, 12, 24, 36, 48, 60, and 72 h after birth. Epochs that were significantly corrupted by artefact, as determined by a neonatal EEG expert (EP), were eliminated from further analysis. An automated method removed periods of remaining artefacts, if present, in the EEG30. We applied these rule-based methods, using default settings, to identify and eliminate (1) improper electrode placement, (2) electrode coupling, (3) impedance testing, (4) high-amplitude artefacts, and (5) impulse-like activity30. After artefact removal, the EEG was low-pass filtered to 30 Hz and then down-sampled from 256 Hz to 64 Hz. Quantitative features were then computed on the bipolar montage of F4-C4, C4-O2, F3-C3, C3-O1, T4-C4, C4-Cz, Cz-C3, and C3-T3.
Features were computed using the NEURAL toolbox30 and grouped into measures of EEG power, discontinuity, spectral distribution, and inter-hemispheric connectivity, as shown in Fig. 6. The power-feature group contains spectral power and range-EEG (rEEG)14,16 features. The rEEG is a measure of peak-to-peak voltage developed as an alternative to amplitude-integrated EEG (aEEG)14,16. Median rEEG and the lower- and upper-margin (5th and 95th centiles) of the rEEG were used as features in this group. The discontinuity-feature group contains higher-order moments of the EEG signal (skew and kurtosis), asymmetry of the rEEG14, and features of the burst annotation17. The spectral-feature distribution group contains fractal dimension, spectral-edge frequency, spectral flatness (spectral Wiener entropy), and relative spectral power. And the inter-hemispheric connectivity group includes coherence of channels across hemispheres. Figure 6 outlines some details of the implementation with full details elsewhere30.
The power spectral density estimate (PSD), used to generate the frequency-domain features (see Fig. 6), is implemented with an 8 s Hamming window at 75% overlap. Features generated using the PSD, expect for the spectral edge frequency, were estimated within four frequency bands, namely 0.5–3 Hz (δ-band), 3–8 Hz (θ-band), 8–15 Hz (α-band), and 15–30 Hz (β-band)30,54,57. EEG kurtosis and skewness are also generated for these 4 frequency bands. The rEEG is generated within a 1–20 Hz bandwidth. For both the rEEG, and the EEG skewness and kurtosis features, band-pass filtering is implemented using a 5th-order infinite-impulse response (IIR) filter with a forward–backwards procedure to ensure zero phase.
Connectivity was assessed by quantifying coherence between similar regions on both hemispheres, for example between F4-C4 and F3-C3. Zero coherence was estimated using an analytic expression of the upper 95% CI under the hypothesis of independence58. The auto- and cross-PSD were generated for the coherence function using an 8 s rectangular window with no overlap58. And lastly, fractal dimension was computed using the Higuchi method with a maximum scale value of 659.
Bursts and inter-bursts features are derived from an annotation of the bursts for each channel of EEG. An automated method generates the burst annotation for the 1-h epochs54. Four features are then computed from this annotation: number of bursts, percentage-time of bursts (burst ratio), and median and maximum (95th centile) duration of inter-bursts (inter-burst interval, IBI). The method’s broad definition of a burst includes any EEG waveform not defined as an inter-burst54. This differs to the terminology commonly used in electrophysiology60 and therefore the method will classify continuous activity as long-duration bursts. Thus, we expect the burst ratio to be greater (and number of bursts to be less) in continuous, compared to discontinuous, activity.
All time-domain and frequency-domain (PSD-based) features are estimated within a 32 s segment with 50% overlap (short-time analysis in Fig. 6). The median is used to summarise the segments over the 1-h epoch, in each channel. rEEG and burst features are estimated on the full 1-h epoch. The median is used again to summaries the features, excluding coherence, over all channels. The total set consists of 34 features, 24 of which are estimated on the 4 frequency bands (δ, θ, α, and β bands).
Modelling temporal evolution
We fit a linear mixed-effects model for each feature to assess the temporal evolution over the first 72 h after birth. Fixed effects for the models include intercept, postnatal age (PNA), GA, and PNA–GA interaction terms. Random effects include intercept and PNA terms. PNA is measured in hours after birth over the first 3 days of life. GA, a measure of maturation, is the duration from the first day of a woman’s last menstrual period to birth. We include GA because many qEEG measures are dependent on maturation14,15,17,61–64.
We use a backwards selection procedure for model selection. Starting with the most complex model which includes all fixed and random effects, we systematically remove random and fixed effects if the more complex model is not a significant improvement over the simpler model4,65. We use the log-likelihood, with the χ2-distribution at P < 0.05, to assess the potential improvement in model fit. Backward selection starts with the random effects, first random PNA and then intercept. Next, the fixed effects: PNA–GA interaction, then GA, then PNA. If the PNA–GA interaction term is included in the model, both PNA and GA fixed effects are also included. Confidence intervals for the fixed effects are estimated using a bootstrap with 1,000 iterations. Each feature has a different model.
The PNA coefficient captures the immediate evolution over first few days of transition to extra-uterine life as a linear increase or decrease (trend). The GA coefficient quantifies the linear trend for intra-uterine maturation. When both PNA and GA coefficients are included the overall trend is represented as a linear plane in 2D space. And the PNA–GA interaction term quantifies the influence of GA on this 2D plane, warping it to a nonlinear plane.
To determine if medications influence the temporal evolution of the qEEG, we examine the effect of early medications–specifically morphine, fentanyl, caffeine, and surfactant replacement therapy–on the qEEG66. Fentanyl, combined with the muscle relaxant suxamethonium, is administered before endotracheal intubation and caffeine is administered in response to apneic events or before extubation. As our data set is small, we do not include these potential confounders in the mixed-effects models to avoid over-fitting the models. Instead we employ multiple linear regression to test the effects of these potential confounders on the qEEG. GA and medication usage, as a binary variable (yes/no), are included as the independent variables with the qEEG feature, averaged over the PNA epochs, as the dependent variable. The medication coefficient is determined significant using the log-likelihood ratio test comparing it to a model without the coefficient. Confidence intervals (95%) are generated for the potential confounder.
Automated analysis of EEG maturational age
To further test the hypothesis that short-term postnatal changes in cerebral activity is significant in terms of the long-term intra-uterine maturation, we evaluate the importance of PNA in an algorithm that estimates the maturation of the EEG. Automated methods, using qEEG and machine learning, have recently been developed to estimate EEG maturational age7,17. These methods are trained and tested on the EEG using GA as a marker of EEG maturational age. Here we develop a new model combining qEEG and PNA as features and determine if PNA is central to the model’s performance.
The method uses the proposed qEEG set, providing the feature is dependent on GA. Dependence is determined from the mixed-effect models described in the previous section. These features, including PNA, are then combined in a regression algorithm to relate to maturation, as measured by GA7,17. (PNA was used as a feature in O’Toole et al.17 but its importance was not assessed.) We use a gradient-boosting algorithm, using the -norm loss function with decision trees as the base learner67. Parameter values are set following guidelines by Hastie et al.68: numerous iterations (5,000) and a correspondingly small shrinkage value (0.01) to control over-fitting; tree size of 6 and 1/2 of the data was randomly selected at each iteration. We set these parameters before training and testing the model because in the development stage we found little to no improvement by selecting different parameters. The maturation estimate for each infant is the mean value over the multiple epochs. Training and testing uses a leave-one-out cross-validation scheme and performance reported with the SD of error, Pearson’s correlation coefficient, and percentage of correct classifications of GA within 1 and 2 weeks17.
Two measures are used to assess the importance of PNA in the algorithm: relative influence and performance loss caused by random permutation of the feature. Relative influence ranks features based on their contribution to reducing squared error when building the decision trees67. Because we set the parameters in the gradient boosting’s algorithm to randomly select one-half of the feature set at each boosting iteration, no 2 models are exactly the same. Thus, we iterate the entire process 1,000 times and estimate the relative importance measures at each iteration.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by Science Foundation Ireland (INFANT-12/RC/2272). N.J.S. acknowledges the support of the EU Horizon 2020 programme (H2020-MCSA-IF-656131). J.M.O.T. was supported by Science Foundation Ireland (15/SIRG/3580). The authors thank Farah Abu Dhais and Andreea Mona Pavel for reviewing medical records.
Author Contributions
E.P. designed the inclusion criteria, selected the cohort, and reviewed all EEG epochs. N.J.S. conceived the original idea. J.M.O.T. designed the methods, with revisions from E.P. and N.J.S., implemented the data analysis, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors discussed the results and revised and edited the manuscript.
Data Availability
The feature set was generated using Matlab (release R2013b, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, United States) with the NEURAL toolbox (version 0.4.055) and the burst detector algorithm (version 0.1.269). Multiple linear regression, the linear mixed-models, and the gradient boosting algorithm were computed using R (version 3.3.2, The R Foundation of Statistical Computing, www.r-project.org). The mixed models used the lme4 (version 1.1.10) package and gradient boosting algorithm used the gbm (version 2.1.3) package. The R code to generate the mixed-effects models and the maturation estimator, in addition to the qEEG feature set and parameter file used in the NEURAL toolbox, is available at https://github.com/otoolej/postnatal_EEG_evolution (version 1.0.0).
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-019-41227-9.
References
- 1.Kidokoro H, et al. Brain Injury and Altered Brain Growth in Preterm Infants: Predictors and Prognosis. Pediatrics. 2014;134:e444–e453. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-2336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Pichler G, Schmölzer GM, Urlesberger B. Cerebral Tissue Oxygenation during Immediate Neonatal Transition and Resuscitation. Front. Pediatr. 2017;5:1–5. doi: 10.3389/fped.2017.00029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Plomgaard AM, et al. Early biomarkers of brain injury and cerebral hypo- and hyperoxia in the SafeBoosC II trial. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0173440. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0173440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kenosi M, et al. Monitoring cerebral oxygenation of preterm infants using a neonatal specific sensor. J. Perinatol. 2017 doi: 10.1038/s41372-017-0007-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pavlidis E, Lloyd RO, Boylan GB. EEG–A Valuable Biomarker of Brain Injury in Preterm Infants. Dev. Neurosci. 2017;39:23–35. doi: 10.1159/000456659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Koolen N, et al. Automated classification of neonatal sleep states using EEG. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017;128:1100–1108. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.02.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Stevenson NJ, et al. Functional maturation in preterm infants measured by serial recording of cortical activity. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:12969. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13537-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Tharp BR, Scher MS, Clancy RR. Serial EEGs in normal and abnormal infants with birth weights less than 1200 grams - A prospective study with long term follow-up. Neuropediatrics. 1989;20:64–72. doi: 10.1055/s-2008-1071267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Scher MS. Normal electrographic-polysomnographic patterns in preterm and fullterm infants. Semin. Pediatr. Neurol. 1996;3:2–12. doi: 10.1016/S1071-9091(96)80023-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.André M, et al. Electroencephalography in premature and full-term infants. Developmental features and glossary. Neurophysiol. Clin. 2010;40:59–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2010.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pavlidis E, Lloyd RO, Mathieson S, Boylan GB. A review of important EEG features for the assessment of brain maturation in premature infants. Acta Paediatr. 2017;38:42–49. doi: 10.1111/apa.13956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Holthausen K, Breidbach O, Scheidt B, Frenzel J. Brain dysmaturity index for automatic detection of high-risk infants. Pediatr. Neurol. 2000;22:187–91. doi: 10.1016/S0887-8994(99)00154-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Niemarkt HJ, et al. Maturational changes in automated EEG spectral power analysis in preterm infants. Pediatr. Res. 2011;70:529–534. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31822d748b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.O’Reilly D, Navakatikyan MA, Filip M, Greene D, Van Marter LJ. Peak-to-peak amplitude in neonatal brain monitoring of premature infants. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2012;123:2139–53. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.02.087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Murphy K, et al. Automated analysis of multi-channel EEG in preterm infants. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2015;126:1692–1702. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Navakatikyan MA, O’Reilly D, Van Marter LJ. Automatic measurement of interburst interval in premature neonates using range EEG. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2016;127:1233–1246. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.O’Toole JM, Boylan GB, Vanhatalo S, Stevenson NJ. Estimating functional brain maturity in very and extremely preterm neonates using automated analysis of the electroencephalogram. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2016;127:2910–2918. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.02.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kapellou O, et al. Abnormal cortical development after premature birth shown by altered allometric scaling of brain growth. PLoS Med. 2006;3:1382–1390. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Volpe, J. J. Human Brain Development. In Volpe, J. (ed.) Neurology of the newborn, 3–118, 5th edition (Elsevier, Philadelphia, 2008).
- 20.Fransson P, Åden U, Blennow M, Lagercrantz H. The functional architecture of the infant brain as revealed by resting-state fMRI. Cereb. Cortex. 2011;21:145–154. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Pillay K, et al. Automated EEG sleep staging in the term-age baby using a generative modelling approach. J. Neural Eng. 2018;15:1–13. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aaab73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Meaney MJ, et al. Molecular basis for the development of individual differences in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal stress response. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 1993;13:321–347. doi: 10.1007/BF00711576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Boksa P, Krishnamurthy A, Sharma S. Hippocampal and Hypothalamic Type I Corticosteroid Receptor Affinities Are Reduced in Adult Rats Born by a Caesarean Procedure with or without an Added Period of Anoxia. Neuroendocrinology. 1996;64:25–34. doi: 10.1159/000127094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Taylor A, Fisk NM, Glover V. Mode of delivery and subsequent stress response. Lancet. 2000;355:120. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)02549-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.West CR, Harding JE, Williams CE, Gunning MI, Battin MR. Quantitative electroencephalographic patterns in normal preterm infants over the first week after birth. Early Hum. Dev. 2006;82:43–51. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2005.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Victor S, Appleton RE, Beirne M, Marson AG, Weindling AM. Spectral analysis of electroencephalography in premature newborn infants: Normal ranges. Pediatr. Res. 2005;57:336–341. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR.0000153868.77623.43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Schumacher EM, et al. Feasibility of long-term continuous EEG monitoring during the first days of life in preterm infants: an automated quantification of the EEG activity. Pediatr. Res. 2011;69:413–417. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31821267d2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Scher MS. Neurophysiological assessment of brain function and maturation II. A measure of brain dysmaturity in healthy preterm neonates. Pediatr. Neurol. 1997;16:287–295. doi: 10.1016/S0887-8994(96)00009-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hayashi-Kurahashi N, et al. EEG for predicting early neurodevelopment in preterm infants: an observational cohort study. Pediatrics 130. 2012;7:e891. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.O’Toole, J. M. & Boylan, G. B. NEURAL: quantitative features for newborn EEG using Matlab. ArXiv E-Prints, https://arxiv.org/abs/1704.05694 (2017).
- 31.Vanhatalo S, Kaila K. Development of neonatal EEG activity: from phenomenology to physiology. Semin. Fetal. Neonatal Med. 2006;11:471–8. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2006.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Dereymaeker A, et al. Review of sleep-EEG in preterm and term neonates. Early Hum. Dev. 2017;113:87–103. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gitau R, et al. Umbilical cortisol levels as an indicator of the fetal stress response to assisted vaginal delivery. Eur J. Obstet. Gynecol. Reprod. Biol. 2001;98:14–17. doi: 10.1016/S0301-2115(01)00298-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ramin SM, Porter JC, Gilstrap LC, Rosenfeld CR. Stress Hormones and Acid-Base Status of Human Fetuses at Delivery. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 1991;73:182–186. doi: 10.1210/jcem-73-1-182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.van den Berg A, van Elburg RM, van Geijn HP, Fetter WP. Neonatal respiratory morbidity following elective caesarean section in term infants. A 5-year retrospective study and a review of the literature. Eur J. Obstet. Gynecol. Reprod. Biol. 2001;98:9–13. doi: 10.1016/S0301-2115(01)00292-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tolner EA, Sheikh A, Yukin AY, Kaila K, Kanold PO. Subplate Neurons Promote Spindle Bursts and Thalamocortical Patterning in the Neonatal Rat Somatosensory Cortex. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:692–702. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1538-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Supcun S, Kutz P, Pielemeier W, Roll C. Caffeine Increases Cerebral Cortical Activity in Preterm Infants. J. Pediatr. 2010;156:490–491. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.10.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hassanein SMA, Gad GI, Ismail RIH, Diab M. Effect of caffeine on preterm infants’ cerebral cortical activity: an observational study. J. Matern.-Fetal Neonatal Med. 2015;28:2090–2095. doi: 10.3109/14767058.2014.978757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Dix LM, van Bel F, Baerts W, Lemmers PM. Effects of caffeine on the preterm brain: An observational study. Early Hum. Dev. 2018;120:17–20. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2018.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Pryds O, Greisen G. Effect of PaCO2 and haemoglobin concentration on day to day variation of CBF in preterm neonates. Acta Paediatr. Scand. Supplement. 1989;360:33–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1989.tb11279.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kehrer M, et al. Development of cerebral blood flow volume in preterm neonates during the first two weeks of life. Pediatr. Res. 2005;58:927–930. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR.0000182579.52820.C3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Meek JH, Tyszczuk L, Elwell CE, Wyatt JS. Cerebral blood flow increases over the first three days of life in extremely preterm neonates. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1998;78:F33–F37. doi: 10.1136/fn.78.1.F33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Pellicer A, Valverde E, Gayá F, Quero J, Cabañas F. Postnatal adaptation of brain circulation in preterm infants. Pediatr. Neurol. 2001;24:103–109. doi: 10.1016/S0887-8994(00)00239-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Naulaers G, Morren G, Van Huffel S, Casaer P, Devlieger H. Cerebral tissue oxygenation index in very premature infants. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2002;87:F189–F192. doi: 10.1136/fn.87.3.F189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Lemmers PMA, Toet M, van Schelven LJ, van Bel F. Cerebral oxygenation and cerebral oxygen extraction in the preterm infant: the impact of respiratory distress syndrome. Exp. Brain Res. 2006;173:458–467. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0388-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Sirc J, Dempsey EM, Miletin J. Cerebral tissue oxygenation index, cardiac output and superior vena cava flow in infants with birth weight less than 1250 grams in the first 48 hours of life. Early Hum. Dev. 2013;89:449–452. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Alderliesten T, et al. Reference Values of Regional Cerebral Oxygen Saturation during the First 3 Days of Life in Preterm Neonates. Pediatr. Res. 2016;79:55–64. doi: 10.1038/pr.2015.186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kluckow M, Evans N. Superior vena cava flow in newborn infants: a novel marker of systemic blood flow. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2000;82:182–187. doi: 10.1136/fn.82.3.F182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kozberg MG, Ma Y, Shaik MA, Kim SH, Hillman EMC. Rapid Postnatal Expansion of Neural Networks Occurs in an Environment of Altered Neurovascular and Neurometabolic Coupling. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:6704–6717. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2363-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Gill, A. W. Postnatal cardiovascular adaptation. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 104, F220–F224 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 51.Fogtmann EP, Plomgaard AM, Greisen G, Gluud C. Prognostic Accuracy of Electroencephalograms in Preterm Infants: A Systematic Review. Pediatrics. 2017;139:e20161951. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Niemarkt HJ, et al. Quantitative analysis of maturational changes in EEG background activity in very preterm infants with a normal neurodevelopment at 1year of age. Early Hum. Dev. 2010;86:219–224. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Shany E, et al. In and ex utero maturation of premature infants electroencephalographic indices. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2014;125:270–276. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.06.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.O’Toole JM, et al. Detecting bursts in the EEG of very and extremely premature infants using a multi-feature approach. Med. Eng. Phys. 2017;45:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2017.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.O’Toole JM. NEURAL: quantitative features for newborn EEG in Matlab [Software] (version 0.4.0) Zenodo. 2019 doi: 10.5281/zenodo.2536888. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Lloyd RO, et al. Predicting 2-y outcome in preterm infants using early multimodal physiological monitoring. Pediatr. Res. 2016;80:382–388. doi: 10.1038/pr.2016.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Vanhatalo S, et al. Slow endogenous activity transients and developmental expression of K+-Cl- cotransporter 2 in the immature human cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005;22:2799–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04459.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Halliday DM, et al. A framework for the analysis of mixed time series/point process data–theory and application to the study of physiological tremor, single motor unit discharges and electromyograms. Prog. Biophys. Mol. Biol. 1995;64:237–78. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6107(96)00009-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Higuchi T. Approach to an irregular time series on the basis of the fractal theory. Physica D. 1988;31:277–283. doi: 10.1016/0167-2789(88)90081-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Kane, N. et al. A revised glossary of terms most commonly used by clinical electroencephalographers and updated proposal for the report format of the EEG findings. Clin. Neurophysiol. Practice 21–41 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 61.Hayakawa M, et al. Background electroencephalographic (EEG) activities of very preterm infants born at less than 27 weeks gestation: a study on the degree of continuity. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2001;84:F163–7. doi: 10.1136/fn.84.3.F163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Palmu K, et al. Detection of ‘EEG bursts’ in the early preterm EEG: Visual vs. automated detection. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2010;121:1015–1022. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Le Bihannic A, Beauvais K, Busnel A, de Barace C, Furby A. Prognostic value of EEG in very premature newborns. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2012;97:F106–109. doi: 10.1136/adc.2010.204735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Palmu K, Kirjavainen T, Stjerna S, Salokivi T, Vanhatalo S. Sleep wake cycling in early preterm infants: Comparison of polysomnographic recordings with a novel EEG-based index. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2013;124:1807–1814. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Van Laere D, et al. Decreased Variability and Low Values of Perfusion Index on Day One Are Associated with Adverse Outcome in Extremely Preterm Infants. J. Pediatr. 2016;178:119–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Malk K, Metsäranta M, Vanhatalo S. Drug effects on endogenous brain activity in preterm babies. Brain Dev. 2014;36:116–23. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2013.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Friedman BJH. Greedy function aproximation: A gradient boosting machine. Ann. Stat. 2001;29:1189–1232. doi: 10.1214/aos/1013203451. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Hastie, T., Tibshirani, R. & Friedman, J. The Elements of Statistical Learning: Data Mining, Inference, and Prediction. Springer Series in Statistics 2nd edition (Springer New York, New York, NY, 2017).
- 69.O’Toole JM. Burst detector for preterm EEG [Software] (version 0.1.2) Zenodo. 2017 doi: 10.5281/zenodo.1052139. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The feature set was generated using Matlab (release R2013b, The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, United States) with the NEURAL toolbox (version 0.4.055) and the burst detector algorithm (version 0.1.269). Multiple linear regression, the linear mixed-models, and the gradient boosting algorithm were computed using R (version 3.3.2, The R Foundation of Statistical Computing, www.r-project.org). The mixed models used the lme4 (version 1.1.10) package and gradient boosting algorithm used the gbm (version 2.1.3) package. The R code to generate the mixed-effects models and the maturation estimator, in addition to the qEEG feature set and parameter file used in the NEURAL toolbox, is available at https://github.com/otoolej/postnatal_EEG_evolution (version 1.0.0).