Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 Mar 7;8:e45013. doi: 10.7554/eLife.45013

Ventrolateral periaqueductal gray neurons prioritize threat probability over fear output

Kristina M Wright 1,, Michael A McDannald 1,
Editors: Geoffrey Schoenbaum2, Laura Colgin3
PMCID: PMC6435320  PMID: 30843787

Abstract

Faced with potential harm, individuals must estimate the probability of threat and initiate an appropriate fear response. In the prevailing view, threat probability estimates are relayed to the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray (vlPAG) to organize fear output. A straightforward prediction is that vlPAG single-unit activity reflects fear output, invariant of threat probability. We recorded vlPAG single-unit activity in male, Long Evans rats undergoing fear discrimination. Three 10 s auditory cues predicted unique foot shock probabilities: danger (p=1.00), uncertainty (p=0.375) and safety (p=0.00). Fear output was measured by suppression of reward seeking over the entire cue and in one-second cue intervals. Cued fear non-linearly scaled to threat probability and cue-responsive vlPAG single-units scaled their firing on one of two timescales: at onset or ramping toward shock delivery. VlPAG onset activity reflected threat probability, invariant of fear output, while ramping activity reflected both signals with threat probability prioritized.

Research organism: Rat

eLife digest

The brain is hard-wired to detect and respond to threats. Catching sight of a spider or a snake, or hearing an unfamiliar sound in the house at night, can make you freeze momentarily. Multiple regions of the brain contribute to this process. According to the textbook view of threat detection, the amygdala and prefrontal cortex estimate the size of the threat. They then send this information to an area called the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray (vlPAG). The vlPAG responds by triggering a fear response such as freezing.

But some studies suggest that the vlPAG may do more than just trigger a fear response. To test this idea, Wright and McDannald trained rats to associate three different 10-second tones with different probabilities of receiving a mild electric shock to the foot. One of the tones was followed by a foot shock 100% of the time. Another tone was followed by a foot shock 37.5% of the time, while the third was never followed by a foot shock. The first tone thus signaled danger, the second uncertainty, and the third safety.

Wright and McDannald recorded vlPAG activity as the rats heard each of the tones. The recordings revealed two distinct groups of vlPAG neurons. Both groups responded most to the tone that signaled danger, less to the tone that signaled uncertainty, and least to the tone that signaled safety. However, they responded at different times. One group of neurons was most active at the start of the tone. The activity of this group depended upon the degree of threat, and not upon whether the rat showed a fear response. The second group of neurons increased its activity over the course of each tone. The activity of this group mainly reflected the degree of threat, but also represented the rat's fear response to a lesser extent.

The vlPAG thus helps to signal the size of a threat, rather than simply generating a fear response. This distinction is important because people with anxiety disorders tend to overestimate threats, and many treatments for anxiety target the brain regions involved in threat estimation. Future studies should examine how the vlPAG works together with other areas, including the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex, to evaluate threats. Understanding this circuit in full could ultimately lead to better treatments for phobias and anxiety.

Introduction

When confronted with potential harm, an estimate of threat probability must be made and followed by an appropriate fear response. The ventrolateral periaqueductal gray (vlPAG) has long been implicated in this fear response (Fanselow, 1993; Kim et al., 1993; Liebman et al., 1970; Carrive et al., 1997; Bandler et al., 1985; Vianna et al., 2001; Arico et al., 2017; Assareh et al., 2017). In the canonical fear circuit, threat probability estimates (the stored associative strength of cue and foot shock, for example a danger cue in Pavlovian fear conditioning) originate in amygdalar nuclei (Duvarci and Pare, 2014; Fanselow and LeDoux, 1999; Davis, 2006; Maren et al., 2013). Amygdalar threat estimates are sent to the vlPAG, which in turn organizes behavioral components of fear output, most notably freezing (Perusini and Fanselow, 2015; Tovote et al., 2015; Dejean et al., 2015; Koutsikou et al., 2014; Walker et al., 1997). While the vlPAG contributes to diverse fear and pain-related processes (Assareh et al., 2017; Bellgowan and Helmstetter, 1998; Parsons et al., 2010; McNally et al., 2011; Johansen et al., 2010), the view of the vlPAG as a node for fear output is so prevalent, it is commonly presented in introductory neuroscience textbooks (Bear et al., 2016; Carlson and Birkett, 2017).

A series of studies have uncovered a vlPAG population showing short-latency increases in firing to a danger cue. This characteristic would be expected of neurons organizing fear output. Yet in these studies, robust relationships between vlPAG single-unit activity and freezing were observed in a minority of neurons (Tovote et al., 2016), weakly observed at danger cue onset (Watson et al., 2016), or mixed with activity that purely reflected the danger cue (Ozawa et al., 2017). At best, freezing only partially accounted for vlPAG activity. If not freezing, then what aspect of fear do vlPAG neurons signal? Here we test the hypothesis that vlPAG neurons signal threat probability.

We drew from learning theory (Rescorla, 1968) to devise a fear discrimination procedure in which three cues predict unique foot shock probabilities: danger (p=1.00), uncertainty (p=0.375) and safety (p=0.00). This behavioral approach was essential as previous studies utilized procedures in which a single cue predicted foot shock with certainty (Tovote et al., 2016; Watson et al., 2016; Ozawa et al., 2017), precluding the ability to observe neural activity reflecting threat probability. We measured fear output using conditioned suppression of reward seeking (Rescorla, 1968; Bouton and Bolles, 1980) over the entire cue and in one-second cue intervals. Using this procedure, we have found that suppression non-linearly scales to shock probability. So while suppression is strong to danger, intermediate to uncertainty and weak to safety; uncertainty produces more suppression than would be expected given its shock probability (Walker et al., 2018; Ray et al., 2018; DiLeo et al., 2016; Wright et al., 2015Berg et al., 2014). Concurrent with fear discrimination, we recorded vlPAG single-unit activity. Indeed, the vlPAG could potentially signal fear output via conditioned suppression of reward seeking (Arico et al., 2017), a long-established measure of fear (Estes and Skinner, 1941) that correlates with freezing (Bouton and Bolles, 1980). The non-linear relationship between behavior and shock probability permitted us to determine whether vlPAG single-unit activity was better captured by fear output or threat probability.

Results

Six male, Long Evans rats were trained to nose poke in a central port in order to receive a food pellet from a cup below. During fear discrimination (Figure 1A), three distinct auditory cues predicted unique foot shock probabilities: danger (p=1.00), uncertainty (p=0.375) and safety (p=0.00). Trial order was randomized for each rat, each session. Fear was measured with suppression ratio and was calculated by comparing nose poke rates during baseline and cue periods (see Materials and methods). After eight discrimination sessions, rats were implanted with 16-wire, drivable microelectrode bundles dorsal to the vlPAG (Figure 1B). Following recovery, rats were returned to fear discrimination. Single-units were isolated and held for the duration of each recording session. The electrode bundle was advanced ~40–80 µm between sessions to record from new single-units in subsequent sessions.

Figure 1. Fear discrimination, histology and vlPAG single-unit activity.

(A) Pavlovian fear discrimination consisted of three cues, each predicting a unique probability of foot shock: danger, p=1.00 (red); uncertainty, p=0.375 (purple); and safety, p=0.00 (blue). (B) Microelectrode bundle placements for all rats (n = 6) and all neurons (n = 245) during recording sessions are represented by red bars. (C) Mean + SEM suppression ratio during the entire 10 s cue for danger, uncertainty, and safety trials is shown for all sessions in which single-units were recorded (n = 88). Discrimination was observed for each cue pair (danger vs. uncertainty, t87 = 12.36, p=7.44×10−21, red asterisk; uncertainty vs. safety, t87 = 20.85, p=3.50×10−35, blue asterisk). (D) Mean ± SEM suppression ratio during each 1 s interval of 10 s cue presentation for danger, uncertainty, and safety trials is shown (n = 88). Discrimination was observed during every interval for each cue pair (danger vs. uncertainty, all t87 >3.00, all p<0.005 [Bonferroni correction for 10 tests], red asterisks; uncertainty vs. safety, all t87 >7.00, all p<0.005, blue asterisks). (E) Scatterplot comparing half the duration of the waveform (x axis) to baseline firing rate (y axis) in all recorded neurons (n = 245). Clustering revealed two populations based on baseline firing rate (High Firing Rate (HFR), open circles; Low Firing Rate (LFR), solid gray circles). X symbols indicate cluster centroids. Onset neurons (n = 29, peach), Ramping neurons (n = 14, maroon) and HFR cue-responsive neurons (n = 3, maroon outline) are indicated. (F and G) Representative single-units from the (F) Onset population and (G) Ramping population. Cue onset (On) and offset (Off) indicated by vertical black lines. Shock delivery indicated by yellow bars. Trial-by-trial firing (top four raster plots), as well as mean firing (bottom, line graphs) are shown for each neuron. Each raster tick represents a spike and each row of spikes reflects one trial [danger (n = 6), uncertainty shock (n = 6), uncertainty omission (n = 10), safety (n = 10)]. The bottom row of spikes in each raster plot corresponds to the first cue trial, subsequent trials are above. Line graphs: Mean firing rate (Hz) across all trials for each cue was constructed using 100 ms bins and smoothed, cue boundaries and shock visualization maintained from raster plots above.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Fear discrimination measured by nose poke rate.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Mean + SEM nose poke during baseline (open bars) and cue (filled bars) plotted for each trial type, for all 88 recording sessions. Asterisks indicate a significant difference between baseline and cue (paired samples t-test, p<0.05). Baseline nose poke rates did not differ between trial types and rats showed significant reductions in poking to danger and uncertainty, but not safety. (B) Relationship between suppression ratio and nose poke rate for each cue (3) and session (88) is shown. The two measures were significantly correlated, particularly for danger and uncertainty. (C) Mean ± SEM nose poke rate shown for each trial type and number (danger 6, uncertainty-shock 6, uncertainty-omission 10 and safety 10). Nose poke patterns were stable across the discrimination session. (D) Mean ± SEM nose poke rate shown in 1 s bins across the cue period for each trial type (4). Overall patterns mimicked those in A, but now temporal changes were apparent. Uncertainty and danger showed similar decreases in poking at cue onset, diverged, then converged as shock drew closer. Nose poking returned to base line rates within approximately 4 s of shock delivery.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Imperfectly distributed nose poking inflates interval suppression ratios.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

In this study, and others from our lab, we consistently find that calculating suppression ratios in short intervals inflates suppression ratios. Here we use hypothetical nose poke data from four trial types to account for this finding. (A) For all trial types, the baseline nose poke rate is 10 (pokes/unit time). (B) Nose poke rate during each 1 s interval of the 10 s cue is shown for the four trial types. In trial type 1 (blue), nose poking is perfectly distributed across all ten intervals (five pokes/unit time). In trial type 2 (green), nose poking alternates between 0 pokes/unit time and 10 pokes/unit time every 1 s interval. In trial type 3 (orange), nose poking spikes to 25 pokes/unit time in two intervals, but is zero in all other intervals. In trial type 4 (pink), nose poking is confined to a single interval (50 pokes/unit time). (C) These four trial types produce equivalent levels of nose poking when rate over the total cue is considered (five pokes/unit time). (D) As expected, calculation of a suppression ratio for the total cue produces identical ratios for each trial type (suppression ratio = 0.333). (E) Calculation of a suppression ratio for each interval, for each trial type, mirrors the pattern of nose poking. The suppression ratio calculation is designed for maximal sensitivity in detecting decreases in poking. A suppression ratio of 1.0 is found [(10-0) / (10 + 0) = 1.0] when no poke occurs in an interval. Extreme increases in poking (Trial Type 4, First Interval) are not weighted equivalently to extreme decreases, yielding suppression ratios that fall well short of −1.0 [(10-50) / (10 + 50) = −0.667]. (F) Calculating a suppression ratio for each interval, then averaging the 10 intervals, only produces a ratio equivalent to the total cue if nose poke rates are perfectly distributed across the entire 10 s cue (as in Trial Type 1, suppression ratio = 0.333). Of course, the natural nose poking behavior of a rat will never match this fixed schedule. As nose poking becomes imperfectly distributed over the 10 s cue, the mean suppression ratio across all intervals increases [TT4 (0.83) > TT3 (0.71) > TT2 (0.50)]. For this reason, suppression ratios calculated in short time intervals (F) produce inflated suppression ratios compared to the total cue (D). However, inflation is uniform for danger, uncertainty and safety trials, making suppression ratios in short intervals valuable for detected relative differences in suppression ratios.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Firing and behavior for every recorded neuron.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(A) A heat plot for normalized firing rate in 1 s intervals is shown for each neuron (y axis, n = 245) prior to and following cue onset for each trial type (danger, uncertainty shock, uncertainty omission and safety). Color scale for normalized firing rate is shown to the right; yellow indicates high firing and blue low firing. Firing rate during foot shock period could have been contaminated by noise artifact and is not shown. (B) An identical heat plot is made for suppression ratio; yellow indicates high suppression ratio and high fear, whereas dark blue indicates low suppression ratio and low fear. (C) Heat plot for nose poke rate; yellow indicates low nose poke rates and dark blue indicates high nose poke rates. Order of neurons 1–245 maintained for A, B and C. Note the virtually identical patterns of behavior obtained with suppression ratio and nose poke rate.

Figure 1—figure supplement 4. Waveform and firing characteristics of Onset neurons.

Figure 1—figure supplement 4.

Mean waveform (mV x time) is shown for each Onset neuron (n = 29, waveform color is peach as in Figure 1E). For each neuron, the baseline firing rate (b), half the duration (d) and amplitude ratio (a) are provided. The waveform properties used to determine half the duration (D) and the properties contributing to the amplitude ratio (N and P) are demonstrated in the top left. The individual the neuron was obtained from is shown in the upper left, animal identity is color coded. The spike number, indicating the channel on which the neuron was recorded, and the behavior session # are shown in the top right. We performed a correlation analysis to determine if the waveform shape of each neuron was more correlated with waveform from the same individual, vs. the other individuals. R2 values were determined for each waveform pair (n = 28), then a two-tailed t-test (p<0.05, uncorrected) was performed comparing the R2 values from the same individual or all others. Only three neurons from one individual, RF03 (SPK05/Session 23, SPK07/Session 34 and SPK07/Session 35), were found to have waveforms that were more similar to neurons from that individual than others. However, any correction on this p value (28 tests were performed for each waveform) resulted in no neuron achieving significance. Further, only two of these neurons (SPK07/Session 34 and SPK07/Session 35) showed cue activity patterns that resembled one another. Removal of one or both of these waveforms did not impact any statistical result.

Figure 1—figure supplement 5. High firing cue-responsive units.

Figure 1—figure supplement 5.

K-means cluster analysis revealed three neurons in the High Firing Rate cluster that were responsive to at least one cue during the last 1 s interval. Trial-by-trial firing (top four raster plots) and mean firing (bottom, line graphs) are shown for each neuron (A, B and C) and each trial type (danger, red; uncertainty shock/omission (dashed), purple; and safety, blue). Each raster tick represents a spike and each row of spikes reflects one trial [danger (n = 6), uncertainty shock (n = 6), uncertainty omission (n = 10) and safety (n = 10)]. The bottom row of spikes in each raster plot corresponds to the first cue trial, subsequent trials are above. Bottom: Mean firing rate (Hz) across all trials for each cue was constructed using 100 ms bins and smoothed, cue boundaries and shock visualization maintained from the raster plots above. Each neuron showed maximal firing to danger in the last 1 s cue interval. We visualized single-unit firing of the three HFR units to danger vs. uncertainty (D), uncertainty vs. safety (E) and danger first interval vs. danger last interval (F) as in Figure 4. We also visualized single-unit beta coefficients for total fear vs. probability (G) and interval fear vs. probability (H) as in Figure 5. HFR cue-responsive units signaled threat probability.

Figure 1—figure supplement 6. Waveform and firing characteristics of Ramping neurons.

Figure 1—figure supplement 6.

Mean waveform (mV x time) is shown for each Ramping neuron (n = 14, waveform color maroon as in Figure 1E). For each neuron, the baseline firing rate (b), half the duration (d) and amplitude ratio (a) are provided. The waveform properties used to determine half the duration (D) and the properties contributing to the amplitude ratio (N and P) are demonstrated in the top left inset. The individual the neuron was obtained from is shown in the upper left, with identity color coded. The spike number, indicating the channel on which the neuron was recorded, and the behavior session # are shown in the top right. We performed a correlation analysis to determine if the waveform shape of each Ramping neuron was more correlated with waveform shapes from the same individual, vs. the other individuals. R2 values were determined for each neuron pair (n = 13), then a two-tailed t-test (p<0.05, uncorrected) was performed comparing the R2 values from the same individual or all others. No waveform shape was more similar to those obtained from the same individual compared to those obtained from others.

Across all 88 recording sessions, these six rats showed excellent discriminative fear: high to danger, intermediate to uncertainty, and low to safety (Figure 1C). ANOVA for suppression ratios for the total 10 s cue [factor: cue (danger vs. uncertainty vs. safety)] found a significant effect of cue (F2,174 = 592.00, p=2.32×10−78, ηp2 = 0.87, observed power (op) = 1.00). Paired t-tests confirmed differing ratios for each cue (danger vs. uncertainty, t87 = 12.36, p=7.44×10−21; uncertainty vs. safety, t87 = 20.85, p=3.50×10−35). Visualizing nose poke rates to each cue revealed a discrimination pattern matching that for suppression ratio (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). While the foot shock probability associated with uncertainty was closer to safety: danger (p=1.00) >> uncertainty (p=0.375) > safety (p=0.00); the mean suppression ratio to uncertainty was closer to danger: danger (ratio = 0.80) > uncertainty (ratio = 0.53) >> safety (ratio = 0.03). The non-linear relationship between shock probability and behavior will be critical for regression analyses that seek to determine if single-unit firing is better captured by threat probability or fear output.

When the total cue period was divided into 10, 1 s intervals, discrimination was observed in each interval and its pattern showed subtle change over cue presentation (Figure 1D). ANOVA for suppression ratios [factors: cue (danger vs. uncertainty vs. safety) and interval (1-10)] found a significant effect of cue (F2,174 = 489.40, p=3.59×10−72, ηp2 = 0.85, op = 1.00) and a cue x interval interaction (F18,1566 = 6.21, p=5.90×10−15, ηp2 = 0.07, op = 1.00). Suppression ratios are artificially high when calculated in short intervals, making for a poor measure of absolute fear (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). However, this artificial skewing is observed equally to all cues, making suppression ratios in short intervals a valid, relative measure of fear.

vlPAG neurons are responsive at cue onset or ramp over cue presentation

We recorded the activity of 245 neurons in six rats over 88 fear discrimination sessions. A previous study optogenetically identified vlPAG glutamate neurons (vGluT2+) as having low baseline firing rates, compared to GABA (gamma-aminobutyric acid) neurons (Gad1+) that exhibited high baseline firing rates (Tovote et al., 2016). We performed k-means clustering for all 245 neurons using baseline firing rate and the waveform characteristics: amplitude ratio and half the duration (Roesch et al., 2007). All neurons separated into one of two clusters purely on the basis of baseline firing rate (Figure 1E), with majority of neurons falling into the low firing rate (LFR) cluster (n = 199) and the remaining in the high firing rate (HFR) cluster (n = 46).

Independent of cluster membership, we determined the cue-responsiveness of each neuron (n = 245; cue firing of all neurons shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Previous studies have identified a population of vlPAG neurons showing short-latency firing increases to auditory cues paired with foot shock (Tovote et al., 2016; Watson et al., 2016; Ozawa et al., 2017). We identified 29 neurons (obtained from 5/6 rats, ~12% of all neurons recorded) with phasic increases in firing to danger, uncertainty, or safety (t-test for firing rate, baseline [2 s prior to cue onset] vs. first 1 s cue interval, p<0.017, Bonferroni correction for three tests). All 29 neurons belonged to the LFR cluster and these neurons are referred to as the Onset population (single-unit example, Figure 1F; information for each Onset neuron’s waveform, firing characteristics, subject and session can be found in Figure 1—figure supplement 4). Consistent with the most recent report (Ozawa et al., 2017), 17 neurons increased firing to at least one cue during the last 1 s cue interval (interval > baseline, p<0.017). Three neurons belonged to the high firing cluster and are likely a unique class of neurons (see Figure 1—figure supplement 5 for a full analysis). The remaining 14 neurons (obtained from 4/6 rats, ~6% of all neurons recorded) belonged to the LFR cluster and are referred to as the Ramping population (single-unit example, Figure 1G; information for each Ramping neuron’s waveform, firing characteristics, subject and session can be found in Figure 1—figure supplement 6). All manuscript analyses were performed on these 29 Onset neurons and 14 Ramping neurons.

vlPAG neurons show differential firing that is maximal to danger

Despite identifying neurons without regard for relative firing to the three cues, differential firing was observed in Onset neurons at the single-unit (Figure 1F) and population (Figure 2A) levels. Onset neurons (n = 29) sharply increased activity during the first 1 s cue interval, with greatest firing to danger, lesser firing to uncertainty, and least firing to safety (Figure 2B, Left). The differential firing pattern diminished as cue presentation proceeded and was absent by the last 1 s cue interval (Figure 2B, Right). The temporal firing pattern (onset → offset) was observed for all trials in the session (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A–D). ANOVA for normalized firing rate (Z-score transformation) for the 29 Onset neurons [data from Figure 2A; factors: cue (danger vs. uncertainty vs. safety) and bin (100 ms: 1 s prior to cue onset through 10 s cue)] revealed main effects of cue and bin (Fs > 9, ps<0.01, ηp2 > 0.20, op > 0.95) but most critically, a cue x bin interaction (F218,6104 = 1.94, p<0.01, ηp2 = 0.06, op = 1.00). Consistent with the ANOVA interaction, Onset neurons showed significantly greater firing to danger compared to uncertainty in the first 1 s cue interval (t28 = 4.54, p=9.70×10−5). While numerically greater firing to uncertainty over safety failed to reach significance in the first 1 s interval (t28 = 1.37, p=0.18), ANOVA restricted to uncertainty and safety (100 ms: 1 s prior to cue onset through first 5 s of the cue) revealed a significant cue x bin interaction (F59,1652 = 1.50, p=0.01, ηp2 = 0.051, op = 1.00). Differential firing was not observed to danger vs. uncertainty (t28 = 1.69, p=0.10) or to uncertainty vs. safety (t28 = 0.60, p=0.55) in the last 1 s cue interval.

Figure 2. vlPAG neurons are responsive to cue onset or ramp over cue presentation.

(A) Mean, Z-score normalized firing to danger (red), uncertainty (purple) and safety (blue) is shown for the 1 s pre-cue period and the 10 s cue period for the Onset population (n = 29). Cue onset (On) and offset (Off) are indicated by vertical black lines. (B) Mean + SEM, Z-score normalized firing during the first, 1 s cue interval (left) and the last, 1 s cue interval (right), is shown for each cue. Differential firing was observed for danger vs. uncertainty (t28 = 4.54, p=9.70×10−4, red asterisk) but not for uncertainty vs. safety (t28 = 1.37, p=0.18), in the first interval. No differences were observed for danger vs. uncertainty (t28 = 1.69, p=0.10) or uncertainty vs. safety, (t28 = 0.60, p=0.55) in the last interval. (C) Normalized firing for the Ramping population (n = 14) plotted as in A. (D) First and last interval firing for the Ramping population (n = 14) plotted as in B. Differential firing was not observed for danger vs. uncertainty (t13 = 0.62, p=0.55) or uncertainty vs. safety (t13 = 0.24, p=0.82), in the first interval. By contrast, differential firing was observed for danger vs. uncertainty (t13 = 3.17, p=7.41×10−3, red asterisk) and uncertainty vs. safety (t13 = 8.26, p=2.00×10−6, blue asterisk), in the last interval. (E) A t-test comparing danger (red) and uncertainty (purple) population firing to safety in a 1 s window was slid across the 10 s cue in 100 ms increments. P value of t-test reported on y axis. Dotted line indicates p=0.05. Inset: Mean + SEM change in firing rate from the first window of activity departed from safety to the last interval, is shown for danger (red) and uncertainty (purple). ns = no significance of a paired t-test.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Trial by trial firing for Onset and Ramping populations.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A, left) Raw firing rate (Hz) for the Onset population (n = 29) is shown across cue presentation for each of the six danger trials. Trials are color-coded from dark (first trial) to light (last trial) with ‘On’ indicating cue onset and ‘Off’ indicating cue offset. (A, right) Population mean firing rate is shown for the first 1 s cue interval (dashed box in A), for each trial. Color scheme maintained from (A, left). Population mean baseline firing (2 s prior to cue presentation) is shown for each individual trial (gray circles). Identical plots using unique color schemes (uncertainty, purple and safety, blue) were made for each trial type: (B) uncertainty shock, (C) uncertainty omission and (D) safety. Note that increased cue firing over baseline tended to be highest on trial 1, but was observed for every individual trial for all cues. A nearly identical set of plots were made for the Ramping population (n = 14, E - H), only now population mean firing rate for each trial (right) is shown for the last 1 s interval of the cue (dashed box on left). Increased cue firing over baseline was consistently observed on danger and uncertainty trials, but not safety trials. Firing patterns observed for the mean of all trials (Figure 2A), (C) were observed at the single trial level for Onset and Ramping populations.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Nose poke cessation is insufficient to drive activity of Onset and Ramping neurons.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

We identified ITI periods during which a nose poke was followed by at least a 2 s cessation period. Mean nose poke rate during the 1 s interval of the poke (−1) and two, 1 s no-poke intervals (+1 and+2) are shown for (A) Onset units and (B) Ramping units (cessation period in gray). If phasic firing to danger by Onset neurons was driven by nose poke cessation, then greater activity should have been observed to nose poke suppression. By definition, nose poke cessation is complete (suppression ratio = 1.00), whereas mean suppression ratio to danger was high, but incomplete (0.83). (C) Onset neurons were nearly unresponsive to nose poke cessation. (D) Ramping neurons were only weakly responsive. Comparison of single-unit firing to danger (Onset, first interval; Ramping, last interval) and nose poke suppression (first interval) revealed no correlation and an overwhelming bias towards greater firing to danger for (E) Onset neurons (R2 = 0.01, p=0.32, p(sign)=1.62×10−6) and (F) Ramping neurons (R2 = 0.08, p=0.10, p(sign)=1.22×10−4). Firing of Onset and Ramping neurons was not the result of nose poke suppression, or nose poking itself.

Selective firing was observed in Ramping neurons at the single-unit (Figure 1G) and population (Figure 2C) levels. Ramping neurons (n = 14) did not increase firing to any cue during the first 1 s cue interval (Figure 2D, Left). Instead, activity ramped over cue presentation with greatest firing observed during the last 1 s cue interval (Figure 2D, Right). Ramping activity was most apparent to danger, intermediate to uncertainty, and absent to safety. The temporal firing pattern (onset → offset) was consistent across trials (Figure 2—figure supplement 1E–H). ANOVA for normalized firing rate for the 14 Ramping neurons [data from Figure 2C; factors: cue (danger vs. uncertainty vs. safety) and bin (100 ms: 1 s prior to cue onset through 10 s cue)] revealed main effects of cue and bin (Fs >60, ps <0.01, ηp2 > 0.40, op = 1.00) and a cue x bin interaction (F218,2834 = 4.33, p<0.01, ηp2 = 0.24, op = 1.00). Illustrative of the ANOVA interaction, Ramping neurons showed no significant differences in firing to danger vs. uncertainty (t13 = 0.62, p=0.55) or uncertainty vs. safety (t13 = 0.24, p=0.81) in the first 1 s cue interval. However, differential firing to danger vs. uncertainty (t13 = 3.17, p=7.41×10−3), and uncertainty vs. safety (t13 = 8.26, p=2.00×10−6), was observed in the last 1 s cue interval.

Ramping activity to danger and uncertainty could be the product of the time at which activity began to increase, or the rate of increase. We performed a two-tailed t-test for population firing to danger vs. safety (Figure 2E, red line) and uncertainty vs. safety (Figure 2E, purple line) in a 1 s window, starting with cue onset. We slid the 1 s window across the 10 s cue in 100 ms increments, to reveal the time in which danger and uncertainty population firing departed from safety. We then calculated the rate of firing increase from the departure window to the last 1 s interval for danger and uncertainty. Differential firing was determined by the time of departure from safety, as opposed to the rate of increase. Ramping activity to danger emerged earlier (Figure 2E; 2.8 s following cue onset for p<0.05; 5.8 s for full Bonferroni correction, p<0.00025 [0.05/200]) than ramping activity to uncertainty (5.7 s following cue onset; 6.5 s for full Bonferroni correction). Change in firing rate did not differ between danger and uncertainty (Figure 2E, Inset).

It is possible that while Onset and Ramping neurons are distinct, activity of one population drives the other. We identified four Onset-Ramping pairs recorded in the same session. We then asked if trial-by-trial variations in firing over each 1 s cue interval were negatively correlated for any of the four pairs. We failed to uncover such a relationship (Figure 3), demonstrating that Onset and Ramping populations are distinct and independent.

Figure 3. Onset and Ramping single-unit activity are independent.

Figure 3.

Activity was simultaneously recorded for a Ramping (maroon) and Onset (peach) unit in four separate sessions (four pairs). For Pair 1, activity is shown for each trial type: (A) danger, (B) uncertainty shock, (C) uncertainty omission, and (D) safety. Single-unit regression was performed for each interval. The primary regression output was a beta coefficient (β) and a p value (p) for: (E) pair 1, (F) pair 2, (G) pair three and (H) pair 4. A significant (p<0.05), negative β was only found for one interval, for one neuron pair (pair 1, interval indicated by +).

Population biases are evident in vlPAG single-units

To demonstrate that Onset population activity was the result of a consistent bias across neurons, we directly compared single-unit firing to cue pairs. Danger and uncertainty firing were correlated, and single-units were biased towards greater firing to danger (Figure 4A). Uncertainty and safety firing were also correlated; however, the single-unit bias towards greater firing to uncertainty was not significant (Figure 4B). Underscoring their specificity to cue onset, single-units were biased towards greater firing to danger in the first 1 s cue interval compared to the last interval, and there was no correlation between firing in the two epochs (Figure 4C). Examining relative cue firing for each Onset neuron in the first 1 s interval revealed the most common pattern to be: danger > uncertainty > safety (n = 14). This was the only pattern to contain more units than would be expected than chance (Figure 4D).

Figure 4. Single-unit biases in Onset and Ramping populations.

Figure 4.

(A) Normalized firing to uncertainty (purple) vs. danger (red) during the first, 1 s cue interval is plotted for all Onset neurons (n = 29). Trendline, the square of the Pearson correlation coefficient (R2) with associated p value (p), and sign test p value p(s) are shown for each plot. (B) Normalized firing to safety (blue) vs. uncertainty (purple) during the first, 1 s cue interval is plotted for Onset neurons (n = 29). (C) Normalized firing to danger in the first, 1 s cue interval vs. the last, 1 s cue interval is plotted for Onset neurons (n = 29). (D) Number of observed neurons (closed circle) vs shuffled distribution (median, 25th percentile, 75th percentile, lowest/highest non-outliers) shown for four most common firing patterns: danger > uncertainty > safety, danger > safety > uncertainty, safety > uncertainty > danger, and uncertainty > danger > safety. (E) Normalized firing to uncertainty (purple) vs. danger (red) during the last, 1 s cue interval is plotted for Ramping neurons (n = 14). (F) Normalized firing to safety (blue) vs. uncertainty (purple) during the last, 1 s cue interval is plotted for Ramping neurons (n = 14). (G) Normalized firing to danger in the first, 1 s cue interval vs. the last, 1 s cue interval is plotted for Ramping neurons (n = 14). (H) Number of observed neurons vs shuffled distribution reported as in D.

We performed the same analysis for the Ramping population, only for the last 1 s interval. Ramping neurons showed a selective firing pattern. A significant correlation between firing to danger and uncertainty was observed, along with a single-unit bias towards greater firing to danger (Figure 4E). Only now, there was no correlation between uncertainty and safety firing, but a consistent bias towards greater uncertainty firing (Figure 4F). Ramping single-units were biased towards danger activity in the last 1 s cue interval, and there was no correlation between firing in the two epochs (Figure 4G). The most common firing pattern in the last 1 s interval was: danger > uncertainty > safety (n = 11). This was the only pattern to contain more units than expected by chance (Figure 4H).

VlPAG activity is greatest to danger, the cue most strongly suppressing rewarded nose poking. It is therefore possible that Onset and Ramping neurons are simply responsive to nose poke cessation. To examine this possibility, we identified naturally occurring periods of nose poke cessation in inter-trial intervals, when no cues were presented. This analysis found no meaningful changes in Onset or Ramping activity during periods of nose poke cessation (Figure 2—figure supplement 2), demonstrating activity patterns are specific to cue-induced suppression of nose poking.

At first glance, the firing patterns of Onset and Ramping neurons appear to support the prevailing hypothesis that vlPAG neurons signal fear output. Differential fear (Figure 1C) and differential firing (Figure 2B,D) show the same general pattern: danger > uncertainty > safety. However, closer inspection reveals that relative differences in fear do not match relative differences in firing. Rats showed robust discrimination between uncertainty and safety, regardless of the temporal resolution with which fear was measured (Figure 1C,D). Yet, robust differential firing to uncertainty and safety was modest in the Onset population (Figure 2B, left; Figure 4B). The Ramping population showed stronger differential firing between uncertainty and safety (Figure 2D, right; Figure 4F), but this pattern did not emerge in until the end of the cue. Fear discrimination was reliably detected in the first 1 s interval (Figure 1D), indicating that Ramping neurons cannot organize fear output early in cue presentation. While inconsistencies between fear output and neural activity are evident for the Onset and Ramping populations, the analyses conducted so far cannot conclusively test the relative contribution of threat probability and fear output to vlPAG single-unit activity.

Onset neurons signal threat probability

To formally test the degree to which vlPAG activity is captured by fear output and threat probability, we used simultaneous linear regression for single-unit firing (Figure 5; see Materials and methods for example regression input). For each single-unit, we calculated the normalized firing rate for each trial (32 total: six danger, six uncertainty shock, 10 uncertainty omission, and 10 safety), in 1 s bins over the 10 s cue. For each trial, we calculated fear on two time scales: total fear (suppression ratio for the entire 10 s cue) and interval fear (suppression ratio for the specific 1 s interval). The corresponding shock probability was assigned for each trial (danger = 1.00, uncertainty = 0.375 and safety = 0.00). Total fear, interval fear and threat probability were used as regressors to explain trial-by-trial variance in single-unit firing. Statistical output was a beta coefficient quantifying the strength (|>0| = stronger) and direction (>0 = positive) of the predictive relationship between each regressor and single-unit firing. Beta coefficients for single-units comprising the Onset and Ramping populations were subjected to ANOVA with regressor (total fear vs. interval fear vs. probability) and interval (1 s intervals for 10 s cue) as factors. This allowed us to determine the relative contribution of total fear, interval fear and probability to single-unit firing over the course of cue presentation.

Figure 5. vlPAG neurons prioritize threat probability over fear output.

(A) Mean + SEM beta coefficient is shown for each regressor, during the first, 1 s cue interval, for the Onset population (n = 29): probability (green), total fear (dark gray) and interval fear (light gray). Beta coefficient for probability was significantly greater than either measure of fear (probability vs. total fear, t28 = 3.75, p=8.01×10−4; probability vs. interval fear, t28 = 4.57, p=8.90×10−5). *paired samples t-test, p<0.05. (B) Beta coefficient for total fear (dark gray) vs. probability (green) during the first, 1 s cue interval is plotted for all Onset neurons (n = 29). Trendline, the square of the Pearson correlation coefficient (R2) with associated p value, and sign test p value comparing each regressor to zero is shown. (C) Beta coefficient for interval fear (light gray) vs. probability (green) during the first, 1 s cue interval is plotted for all Onset neurons (n = 29). Sign test result for probability is identical to B. (D) Mean ± SEM beta coefficient is shown for each regressor, during each 1 s cue interval, for the Onset population (n = 29). Dash outlined box indicates interval analyzed in (A). #single-sample t-test comparison to zero, p<0.005 (Bonferroni correction for 10 tests), color indicates regressor compared to zero. (E) Mean beta coefficient for probability is shown for the each of nine uncertainty assignments for the Onset population (n = 29). Dashed line indicates the actual foot shock probability associated with uncertainty (0.375). (F) Mean + SEM beta coefficient is shown for each regressor, during the last, 1 s cue interval, for the Ramping population (n = 14). Beta coefficients for probability and total fear were significantly greater than interval fear (probability vs. interval fear, t13 = 3.61, p=3.20×10−3; total fear vs. interval fear, t13 = 4.68, p=4.34×10−4), but did not differ from one another (t13 = 1.11, p=0.29). *paired samples t-test, p<0.05. (G) Beta coefficient for total fear (dark gray) vs. probability (green) during the last, 1 s cue interval is plotted for Ramping neurons (n = 14). (H) Beta coefficient for interval fear (light gray) vs. probability (green) during the last, 1 s cue interval is plotted for Ramping neurons (n = 14). Sign test result for probability is identical to F. (I) Mean ± SEM beta coefficient is shown for each regressor, during each 1 s cue interval, for the Ramping population (n = 14). Dash outlined box indicates interval analyzed in (E). #single-sample t-test comparison to zero, p<0.005 (Bonferroni correction for 10 tests), color indicates regressor compared to zero. (J) Mean beta coefficient for probability is shown for the each of nine uncertainty assignments for the Ramping population (n = 14). Dashed line indicates the actual foot shock probability associated with uncertainty (0.375).

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Onset and Ramping single-units prioritize threat probability irrespective of regressor combination.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

Linear regression was performed for Onset neurons (n = 29) exactly as described in the manuscript, but using different regressor combinations: (A) probability and total fear, (B) probability and interval fear, (C) interval fear only, (D) total fear only and (E) probability only. Beta coefficients are plotted as mean ± SEM (probability, green; total fear, dark gray; and interval fear, light gray). Linear regression was performed for Ramping neurons (n = 14), using identical regressor combinations (F - J). F statistic and p value shown for main effect of regressor in multiple regressor analyses (A, B, F, G), F and p shown for main effect of interval in single regressor analyses.
Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Onset single-unit activity is better captured by threat probability than a binary output.

Figure 5—figure supplement 2.

We compared (A and B) regression as performed in manuscript to (C and D) regression performed with a binary regressor (danger = 1.000, uncertainty and safety = 0.000). Beta coefficients are plotted as mean ± SEM (probability green; binary, teal; total fear, dark gray; interval fear, light gray). Separate regression is performed to avoid multicollinearity; the probability and binary regressors are highly correlated (R2 = 0.77). ANOVA for beta coefficients using analysis (probability vs. binary), regressor and interval as factors found an analysis x regressor x interval interaction (F18,504 = 2.51, p=5.89×10−4). ANOVA for only the first 1 s interval found an analysis x regressor interaction (F2,56 = 7.80, p=1.03×10−3). The activity of vlPAG single-units was better captured by the regressor that reflected the actual foot shock probability (uncertainty = 0.375) over a regressor that equated uncertainty to safety (uncertainty = 0.000).

The results of primary interest for the Onset population came from the first 1 s cue interval, when activity was highest and differential firing was observed. Linear regression unequivocally revealed that Onset single-unit activity was captured by threat probability (Figure 5A). The beta coefficient for the probability regressor was positive and significant, exceeding the beta coefficient for either measure of fear output – neither of which differed from zero. The population bias was observed across Onset neurons. Single-unit beta coefficients were positively biased for threat probability, but not for either measure of fear output (Figure 5B,C).

Examining the entirety of cue presentation, threat probability signaling was highest in the first interval, persisted several more seconds and diminished (Figure 5D). Total fear or interval fear did not account for variance in single-unit firing at any interval. Consistent with this description, ANOVA for beta coefficient with factors of regressor and interval (10 total) revealed a main effect of regressor (F2,56 = 7.16, p<0.01, ηp2 = 0.20, op = 0.92) and a regressor x interval interaction (F18,504 = 2.38, p<0.01, ηp2 = 0.08, op = 0.99). Identical results were obtained when probability was compared to total fear or interval fear separately. In fact, significance for fear output could only be found if total fear was the only regressor used in the analysis – producing a result very similar to that of a previous study (Watson et al., 2016). Even then, the predictive relationship was weaker than that of probability (Figure 5—figure supplement 1, A–E).

The threat probability regressor in the above analyses utilized the actual shock probability assigned to each cue. Of course, the subjects had no a priori knowledge of shock probability assignments. It is then possible that vlPAG activity is ‘tuned’ to an alternative shock probability. To examine this, we performed single-unit linear regression for normalized firing in the first 1 s cue interval maintaining the probabilities for danger (1.00) and safety (0.00), but incrementing the probability assigned to uncertainty from 0 to 1 in 0.125 steps (0.000, 0.125, 0.250, 0.375, 0.500, 0.625, 0.750, 0.875, and 1.000). The mean beta coefficient for each of the nine increments is plotted as a threat-tuning curve for the Onset population (Figure 5E). The beta coefficient resulting from regression using the actual shock probability (uncertainty = 0.375), was the ‘peak’ of the tuning curve. The probabilities with the next highest beta coefficients were those flanking 0.375. Beta coefficients dropped off rapidly as the uncertainty assignment moved to the extremes.

This result is particularly revealing for the analysis in which the uncertainty assignment was 0.000 (first data point on the curve Figure 5E). Onset neurons showed high firing to danger but lower and more similar firing to uncertainty and safety, leaving open the possibility that Onset neurons signal a more binary output (danger = 1.000) > (uncertainty and safety = 0.000). However, the actual uncertainty assignment (0.375) captured single-unit activity better than the binary assignment (0.000) in the first 1 s interval and across the remainder of cue presentation (Figure 5—figure supplement 2).

Ramping neurons prioritize threat probability over fear output

Linear regression for the Ramping population in the last 1 s cue interval revealed that single-unit activity was captured by a mixture of threat probability and total fear output (Figure 5F). Ramping single-units were biased towards positive beta coefficients for probability and total fear (Figure 5G), but there was no correlation between these regressors. Ramping single-units were not biased towards positive beta coefficients for interval fear (Figure 5H), but signaling of probability and interval fear were negatively correlated. Linear regression for all ten intervals revealed that threat probability signaling was prioritized over total fear and interval fear (Figure 5I). ANOVA for beta coefficients with factors of regressor and interval revealed a main effect of regressor (F2,26 = 8.96, p<0.01, ηp2 = 0.41, op = 0.96) and regressor x interval interaction (F18,234 = 2.16, p<0.01, ηp2 = 0.14, op = 0.99). Mean ± SEM beta coefficients for each regressor over all 10 intervals were: threat probability: 0.58 ± 0.14, total fear: 0.12 ± 0.10 and interval fear: −0.03 ± 0.05. Only the beta coefficient for probability differed significantly from zero (t13 = 4.21, p=1.02×10−3) and was significantly greater than either total fear (t13 = 2.32, p=0.04) or interval fear (t13 = 4.32, p=8.28×10−4). This pattern of results held when probability was separately compared to total fear and interval fear, and when each regressor was considered in isolation (Figure 5—figure supplement 1, F–J).

If Ramping neurons contain information about threat probability, as well as fear output, the tuning curve for ramping neurons ought to be shifted right of 0.375. This is because the relative weighting of uncertainty for threat probability (danger >> uncertainty > safety) and average fear output (danger > uncertainty >> safety) differ. We constructed a population threat-tuning curve for normalized firing in the last 1 s interval (Figure 5J, as in Figure 5E). Tuning was shifted right of the actual probability, with a ‘peak’ at 0.625. This is consistent with mixed signaling of fear output and threat probability by Ramping neurons, rather than a pure threat probability signal.

Ramping neurons signal threat probability through delay until shock receipt

Onset neurons are tuned to initial cue presentation, providing a rapid estimate of threat probability. By contrast, Ramping neurons are initially unresponsive, but gradually increase activity over cue presentation. Ramping neurons may continue to signal threat probability through the delay period, up until foot shock receipt. We first examined population activity during the five seconds following cue offset (2 s delay, 0.5 s shock and 2.5 s post-shock). Activity during the 500 ms shock period was not analyzed because it may have been contaminated by electrical artifacts. Onset neurons showed negligible delay activity and little to no post-shock activity (Figure 6A). Ramping neurons continued firing to danger and uncertainty throughout the delay period, but this firing diminished shortly after shock presentation (Figure 6B). ANOVA for normalized firing rate [factors: trial type (danger vs. uncertainty shock vs. uncertainty omission vs. safety) and bin (100 ms: 5 s post cue)] revealed no main effects or interactions for Onset neurons (Fs <2.3, ps >0.09). By contrast, identical ANOVA revealed main effects of trial and bin (Fs >3, ps <0.01), as well as a trial x bin interaction for Ramping neurons (F132,1716 = 3.14, p<0.01, ηp2 = 0.19, op >0.99). The Onset and Ramping firing patterns differed from one another. ANOVA with neuron type as a factor (Onset vs. Ramping) found significant interactions for trial type x neuron type, bin x neuron type, and trial type x bin x neuron type (Fs >2.50, ps <0.01, ηp2 > 0.19, op >0.94).

Figure 6. Ramping neurons signal threat probability until shock delivery.

Figure 6.

(A) Mean normalized firing to danger (red), uncertainty shock (purple, solid line), uncertainty omission (purple, dashed line) and safety (blue) is shown for the 5 s post-cue period for the Onset population (n = 29). Cue offset (Off) is indicated by the arrow, shock indicated by striped yellow bar. (B) Mean normalized firing is shown for the Ramping population (n = 14), as in A. (C) Mean ± SEM beta coefficient is shown for each regressor, over the 5 s post-cue interval, for the Onset population (n = 29): probability (green) and interval fear (light gray). (D) Mean ± SEM beta coefficient is shown for each regressor, over the 5 s post-cue interval, for the Ramping population (n = 14). #single-sample t-test comparison to zero, p<0.005 (Bonferroni correction for 10 tests), and color indicates regressor compared to zero. (E) A correlation matrix was constructed for the 29 Onset neurons using the probability beta coefficient from the four delay and five post-shock intervals. The p value of the Pearson correlation coefficient is plotted for each interval compared (legend shown next to figure). (F) A correlation matrix was constructed for the 14 Ramping neurons as in E.

Observed differences in neural activity suggest that Ramping neurons maintain threat probability signaling throughout the delay period and this signal abruptly decreases following foot shock presentation. Single-unit regression was used to determine whether trial-by-trial, post-cue firing was best described by interval fear or probability (total fear sampled only the prior 10 s cue period and was omitted). Regression was performed in 500 ms intervals for the 5 s post-cue period minus the shock interval (nine total intervals). Onset neurons did not signal interval fear or probability at any time following cue offset (Figure 6C). ANOVA revealed no main effect of regressor (F1,28 = 1.02, p=0.32, ηp2 = 0.04, op = 0.16) or regressor x interval interaction (F8,224 = 0.53, p=0.83, ηp2 = 0.02, op = 0.24). Illustrative of the lack of information contained in Onset neurons, post hoc comparisons found that no beta coefficient, for any regressor differed from zero. By contrast, Ramping neurons signaled probability throughout the delay period (Figure 6D, first four intervals), which diminished following shock delivery. In support, ANOVA revealed a significant regressor x interval interaction (F8,104 = 5.20, p<0.01, ηp2 = 0.29, op = 0.99).

Threat probability signaling prior to shock delivery was abruptly halted following shock delivery in Ramping neurons, but not Onset neurons. To reveal the degree to which this occurred, we constructed correlation matrices using the probability beta coefficient from each of the nine relevant intervals (4 delay and five post-shock). Of greatest interest were the twenty comparisons between the probability beta coefficient for the four delay and five post-shock intervals (Figure 6E & F; bottom-left quadrant). Significant between-interval correlations for the probability beta coefficient were observed for 13/20 comparisons in Onset neurons (Figure 6E), indicating that threat probability signaled during the delay tended to persist following foot shock. By contrast, significant correlations were found for only 4/20 comparisons in Ramping neurons (Figure 6F); and the proportion of intervals showing a significant correlation differed for the Onset and Ramping neurons (χ2 = 8.08, p=4.5×10−3). Bonferroni correction (0.05/5 = 0.01, five tests per interval) found significant correlations for 6/20 Onset comparisons, 1/20 Ramping comparisons, and these proportions significantly differed (χ2 = 4.22, p=0.039).

Discussion

We recorded vlPAG single-unit activity while rats discriminated between danger, uncertainty and safety. Consistent with previous reports (Tovote et al., 2016; Watson et al., 2016; Ozawa et al., 2017), we found a population of Onset neurons with short-latency excitation to danger. Consistent with the most recent report (Ozawa et al., 2017), we found a Ramping population that increased activity over danger presentation. Onset activity reflected an estimate of threat probability, invariant of fear output. Ramping activity reflected threat probability and fear output, though probability emerged earlier and was stronger overall. While vlPAG signals for fear output could potentially emerge at the ensemble level (Jones et al., 2007; Zhou et al., 2018), these multi-unit codes would be composed of single-units primarily signaling threat probability. Activity reflecting fear output may be found in other vlPAG populations, such as neurons showing inhibition of firing to cues (Tovote et al., 2015), or in non-cue-responsive single-units (Insanally et al., 2019). Yet, this would still mean that signals for threat probability and fear output co-exist in the vlPAG.

It is important to note that these results are correlative and that Onset neuron activity may not play a causal role in fear output. Previous work has found that short-latency, excitatory responses to danger are largely observed in vlPAG VGlut2 +neurons, and that excitation of this population is sufficient to produce freezing (Tovote et al., 2016). Though all Onset neurons fell into the low firing cluster, we cannot conclude these were VGlut2 +neurons. Moreover, inhibition of vlPAG GABA neurons also promotes freezing (Tovote et al., 2016). These neurons have comparatively higher baseline firing rates and respond to danger cues through inhibition of neural activity. A causal, vlPAG signal for fear output may be observed in a GABAergic/cue-inhibited population, in concert with or independent of a glutamatergic/cue-excited population. At the same time, it is clear how the observed Onset signal could play a causal role in fear output. Blocking vlPAG Onset activity to danger and uncertainty would equate neural activity to that for safety, removing the threat impetus for suppression of reward seeking, freezing and related defensive behaviors.

Before further discussing implications, we consider some alternative accounts for the observed firing patterns. Perhaps vlPAG neurons signaling fear output are anatomically distinct from those recorded here. We intentionally recorded from caudal vlPAG, the subregion preferentially activated by fearful contexts in rats (Carrive et al., 1997). VlPAG manipulations that disrupt fear-related behaviors typically include this more caudal region (De Oca et al., 1998), and high-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging reveals caudal vlPAG activation specific to aversive stimuli in humans (Satpute et al., 2013). We observed threat probability signaling in all recording locations (bregma −7.62 → −7.98). The vlPAG stretches ~0.7 mm beyond our most caudal recording site. It is therefore possible that neurons signaling fear output are restricted to the extreme caudal vlPAG.

Maybe the vlPAG signals fear output, but we did not measure the relevant output. Previous studies have failed to find robust relationships between vlPAG activity and cued freezing. Here we used conditioned suppression of rewarded nose poking to provide an objective measure of fear on two timescales and to perhaps better capture vlPAG activity. This measure of fear did not capture Onset neuron firing, and only partially captured Ramping neuron firing at the end of cue presentation. Further, Onset and Ramping activity were not merely driven by nose poke cessation or withdrawal from the port.

If not freezing, conditioned suppression, or nose poke cessation then perhaps another measure of fear? Danger cues elicit active fear responses: escape-like behaviors such as darting (Greiner et al., 2018; Gruene et al., 2015). However, darting is prevalent in females, but less so in males. Further, the males used in this study had extensive experience with fear discrimination, and at no point was escape from the foot shock possible. Danger cues increase arterial blood pressure and reduce heart rate, however, neither of these abilities has been linked to vlPAG function (Helmstetter and Tershner, 1994; LeDoux et al., 1988; Wilson and Kapp, 1994). Danger cues also enhance startle, perhaps more in line with Onset population firing. Yet, dorsal, rather than ventrolateral, PAG subregions have been implicated in startle behavior (Walker et al., 1997; Zhao et al., 2009). Of course, many other fear behaviors are possible: piloerection, hyperventilation, changes in body temperature, vocalization, etc. (Kim et al., 2010; Iwata and LeDoux, 1988; Gallego et al., 2001; Vianna and Carrive, 2005). Problematic is that most fear behaviors are initiated at cue onset and maintained until the aversive event occurs. We did not observe a substantial population of neurons with these temporal firing characteristics, making the vlPAG a poor candidate for sustained fear output. Above all, any potential behavior signaled by Onset neurons must closely match shock probability, confounding this behavior signal with probability itself.

The present findings are best understood through comparison to the account of vlPAG function outlined in the predatory imminence continuum (PIC), a highly influential theory of defensive behavior (Fanselow and Lester, 1988). Organizing features of the PIC are time and degree of threat. As predation becomes more imminent (pre-encounter → post-encounter → circa-strike), the form and intensity of defensive behaviors change. Cued fear is argued to capture post-encounter defenses: immobility elicited when predators are nearby. In the neural instantiation of PIC, the amygdala integrates information about environmental stimuli (auditory cues here), nociceptive information (foot shock) and time to produce a signal for degree of threat (Fanselow and Lester, 1988). This amygdala-derived signal is relayed to the vlPAG to organize fear output (Fanselow, 1991; Fanselow, 1994). Implicit in the PIC model, is that the vlPAG does not contain information about degree of threat – only the resultant fear output. Yet, we find that single vlPAG neurons contain detailed information about time and degree of threat.

These results require more careful consideration of the role of the vlPAG in the fear circuit and the PIC. Rather than signaling fear output, vlPAG Onset neurons signal threat probability. This information could be used to organize a variety of fear responses, but these neurons do not intrinsically signal fear output. For example, vlPAG projections to the central amygdala (CeA) and rostral ventromedial medulla (RVM) could inform fear output via freezing (Vianna et al., 2008), while projections to midline/intralaminar thalamus could rapidly relay threat probability estimates to a larger fear network (basolateral amygdala, prelimibic cortex, infralimbic cortex, insular cortex, etc.) (Vertes et al., 2015; Krout and Loewy, 2000; Buchanan and Thompson, 1994; Sengupta and McNally, 2014), promoting a variety of threat-related processes (Faull et al., 2016). In this way, rapid threat probability estimates generated by vlPAG Onset neurons may be more akin to brainstem-derived signals for rapid detection of visual threats, which are distributed to a wider brain network (Liddell et al., 2005).

Neurons responsive toward the end of cue presentation were more heterogeneous, in terms of their baseline firing rate and their signaling. Ramping neurons prioritized threat probability but also signaled fear output. However, Ramping activity could not drive fear output in full. Differential fear to safety, uncertainty and danger was observed even in the first second of cue presentation, when these neurons were unresponsive. Unlike Onset neurons, Ramping neurons showed selective firing throughout the delay period, only diminishing once foot shock had been delivered. Ramping neurons may provide a threat probability estimate that increases as threat draws nearer and peaks when threat is imminent. Ramping neurons may help sustain threat estimates in the absence of explicit stimuli, such as in trace conditioning (McEchron et al., 1998; Büchel et al., 1999), or estimate more precisely when the noxious event will occur. Shifts toward PAG-centric activity are apparent in humans, as capture becomes imminent (Mobbs et al., 2007) or natural threats draw closer Mobbs et al., 2010; with the caveat that neither of these studies could specify the PAG subregion activated.

Ramping neurons may also provide two forms of feedback: the estimated probability of foot shock and a readout of fear output on that trial. Either type of information could be compared to that trial’s outcome, particularly for uncertainty trials on which shock can be present or absent. This could be used to adjust estimates of threat probability and fear output on future encounters with the cue. This is broadly consistent with a central role for the vlPAG in feedback processes (McNally et al., 2011; Ozawa et al., 2017; Yeh et al., 2018). Even more, this Ramping signal could alter or reduce foot shock processing through descending control of dorsal horn nociceptive inputs via endogenous opioid circuits. This behavioral phenomenon, conditioned analgesia (MacLennan et al., 1980; Fanselow and Bolles, 1979; Chance et al., 1978), may be related to a more general phenomenon of placebo analgesia. VlPAG activation is consistently observed in studies of placebo analgesia (Tracey, 2010; Eippert et al., 2009; Wager and Atlas, 2015; Petrovic et al., 2002) and our observation of increasing neural activity toward presentation of a noxious event may provide a suitable neural substrate for this process. Perhaps most remarkable is that although independent, between the Onset and Ramping populations, the vlPAG contains an estimate of threat probability from the time of first encounter up through the noxious event itself. The vlPAG may not signal fear output per se, but is rich with information that would inform a variety of fear processes and behaviors.

If fear output via conditioned suppression is non-linear, and vlPAG activity scales linearly to threat probability, how does the vlPAG fit into the fear circuit? An ultimate explanation for non-linear fear output may be that threat systems evolved to avoid predation, not to precisely match degree of defensive behavior to threat probability. Erring on the side of greater fear to uncertain threats may promote survival. A proximate explanation may be that fear output is the summed product of multiple threat signals. VlPAG output to the RVM may instruct fear output to match threat probability. If this were the only threat signal, then vlPAG activity would in fact reflect fear output. We speculate that additional threat signals govern fear output. For example, neurons in the retrorubral field (RRF) project to the RVM, as well as the CeA (Zahm and Trimble, 2008; Deutch et al., 1988; Von Krosigk and Smith, 1991). Preliminary data from our laboratory suggest that RRF neurons are primarily responsive to danger and uncertainty, but weight uncertainty similarly to danger (danger = uncertainty > safety). RRF neurons may signal threat probability plus uncertainty-induced stress, or may favor cue-shock contiguity over contingency (Rescorla, 1967). Summation of vlPAG-derived and RRF-derived threat signals by RVM neurons would produce a non-linear fear output like that observed in our discrimination procedure.

The results pose questions about the specific relationship between the vlPAG and the CeA. VlPAG threat probability signals may be trained up by the CeA, but become CeA-independent with sufficient training (Ozawa et al., 2017). Consistent with this interpretation, the CeA is essential to the acquisition of conditioned suppression with limited training, but that more extended training mitigates the effects of CeA lesions (Lee et al., 2005; McDannald, 2010). This is not to say that we would expect the CeA to become inessential following extensive fear discrimination training. Updating threat probability should occur when environmental stimuli become more or less predictive of noxious events. We anticipate the CeA is essential to updating vlPAG threat probability signaling (McNally et al., 2011; Ozawa et al., 2017).

It is near universally accepted that the amygdala is a key node of dysfunction in stress (Rauch et al., 2000) and anxiety disorders (Etkin and Wager, 2007). This may be driven in part by technical considerations: whole-brain fMRI can detect amygdala BOLD signals (Johnstone et al., 2005), while detecting subregion-specific PAG BOLD signals requires a more deliberate approach (Satpute et al., 2013). Perhaps the primary intellectual driver is that the amygdala is theorized to be a privileged cite of integration/learning in the fear circuit (Mahan and Ressler, 2012; Admon et al., 2013). The present findings illustrate that the amygdala is not privileged in this regard, and mark the vlPAG as likely node of dysfunction in psychiatric disorders of stress and anxiety. Appreciation for the vlPAG as a site of integration will hasten mapping of a more complete fear circuit. Deliberate study of vlPAG function (Arico et al., 2017; Assareh et al., 2017Rozeske et al., 2018) and dysfunction in psychiatric disease (Yeh et al., 2018), will be essential to developing effective therapies for disorders characterized by exaggerated threat estimation and aberrant fear.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or
resource
Designation Source or reference Identifier Additional
information
Antibody Anti-Tryptophan Hydroxylase Raised in Sheep Sigma Cat # T8575 [1:1000] in 0.05M PBS
Antibody Biotinylated Anti-Sheep Raised in Rabbit Vector Labs Cat # PK-6106 [1:200] in 0.05M PBS
Chemical compound, drug Normal Rabbit Serum Vector Labs Cat # PK-6106 1% in 0.05M PBS
Chemical
compound, drug
Avidin Vector Labs Cat # PK-6106 [1:200] in0.05M PBS
Chemical compound, drug Biotin Vector Labs Cat # PK-6106 [1:200] in 0.05M PBS
Chemical compound, drug NovaRED Perioxidase (HRP) Substrate Kit Vector Labs Cat # SK-4800 18 drops (1), 12 drops (2), 12 drops (3) and 12 drops H2O2 solution in DI H2O.
Chemical compound, drug Triton Sigma Cat # T8787
Chemical compound, drug Hydrogen Peroxide Sigma Cat # 216763
Chemical compound, drug Paraformaldehyde Sigma Cat # P6148
Chemical compound, drug Sucrose Fisher Scientific Cat # S5
Chemical compound, drug Sodium Chloride Fisher Scientific Cat # S 640
Chemical compound, drug Histoprep100% Reagent Alcohol Fisher Scientific Cat # HC800
Chemical compound, drug Histoprep 95% Reagent Alcohol Fisher Scientific Cat # HC1300
Chemical compound, drug Histoclear II Fisher Scientific Cat # 5089990150
Chemical compound, drug Omnimount Fisher Scientific Cat # 5089990146
Chemical compound, drug 10% Neutral Buffered Formalin Fisher Scientific Cat # 22899402
Chemical compound, drug Potassium Phosphate Monobasic Fisher Scientific Cat # P285
Chemical compound, drug Potassium Phosphate Dibasic Fisher Scientific Cat # P288
Software, algorithm MED PC-IV Med Associates RRID:SCR_012156
Software, algorithm OmniPlex Plexon
Software, algorithm Offline Sorter V6 Plexon RRID:SCR_000012
Software, algorithm NeuroExplorer Plexon RRID:SCR_001818
Software, algorithm MATLAB Mathworks RRID:SCR_001622
Software, algorithm Statistica StatSoft RRID:SCR_014213
Software, algorithm SPSS IBM RRID:SCR_002865
Software, algorithm Adobe
Illustrator
Adobe RRID:SCR_010279
Software,
 algorithm
Adobe
Photoshop
Adobe RRID:SCR_014199
Other Plexon
standard
commutator
Plexon Cat # 50122
Other Plexon head stage cable – Metal Mesh Plexon Cat # 91809–017
Other Plexon head stage Plexon Cat # 40684–020
Other Omnetics connector Omnetics Corporation Cat # A79042-001
Other Green board - Moveable Array San Francisco Circuits Cat # PCB
Other Stainless Steel ground
wire
AM Systems Cat # 791400
Other Formvar Insulated
Nichrome Wire
AM Systems Cat # 761500
Other Dustless Precision Pellets Bio-Serv Cat # F0021

Experimental subjects

Ten adult male Long Evans rats (RRID:RGD_2308852) weighing 241–268 g arrived from Charles River Laboratories, Raleigh, NC on postnatal day 55. All rats were implanted with drivable microelectrode bundles. Data are reported from six rats; three rats did not yield units and one rat had incorrect electrode placement. All rats were single-housed throughout the duration of the experiment on a 12 hr light cycle (lights off at 6:00pm) and maintained at 85% of their free-feeding body weight with standard laboratory chow (18% Protein Rodent Diet #2018, Harlan Teklad Global Diets, Madison, WI) except during an 11 day, post-surgery recovery period where animals had ad libitum access to standard chow. Ad libitum access to water was always available in the home cage. All protocols were approved by the Boston College Animal Care and Use Committee and all experiments were carried out in accordance with the NIH guidelines regarding the care and use of rats for experimental procedures.

Electrode assembly

Microelectrodes consisted of a drivable bundle of sixteen 25.4 µm diameter Formvar-Insulated Nichrome wires (761500, A-M Systems, Carlsborg, WA) within a 27-gauge cannula (B000FN3M7K, Amazon Supply) and two 127 µm diameter PFA-coated, annealed strength stainless-steel ground wires (791400, A-M Systems, Carlsborg, WA). All wires were electrically connected to a nano-strip omnetics connector (A79042-001, Omnetics Connector Corp., Minneapolis, MN) on a custom 24-contact, individually routed and gold immersed circuit board (San Francisco Circuits, San Mateo, CA).

Surgery

Stereotaxic surgery was performed aseptic conditions under isoflurane anesthesia (1–5% in oxygen). Carprofen (i.p., 5 mg/kg) and lactated ringers solution (~2–5 mL) were administered preoperatively. The skull was scoured in a crosshatch pattern with a scalpel blade to increase efficacy of implant adhesion. Five screws were installed in the skull to further stabilize the connection between the skull, electrode assembly and a protective head cap (screw placements: two anterior to bregma, two between bregma and lambda about ~3 mm medial to the lateral ridges of the skull, and one on the midline ~5 mm posterior of lambda). A 1.4 mm diameter craniotomy was performed to remove a circular skull section centered on the implant site and the underlying dura was removed to expose the cortex. Nichrome recording wires were freshly cut with surgical scissors to extend ~2.0 mm beyond the cannula at a ~15° angle. Just before implant, current was delivered to each recording wire in a saline bath, stripping each tip of its formvar insulation. Current was supplied by a 12 V lantern battery and each Omnetics connector contact was stimulated for 2 s using a resistor-equipped lead. Machine grease was placed by the cannula and on the microdrive.

For implantation dorsal to the vlPAG, the electrode assembly was slowly advanced at a 20° angle to the following coordinates from cortex (anterior-posterior: −8.00 mm, medial-lateral: −2.45 mm and dorsal-ventral: −5.52 mm). Once in place, stripped ends of both ground wires were wrapped around a sixth screw inserted previously to ground the electrode (anterior-posterior: −8.00 mm, medial-lateral: +2.45 mm). The microdrive base and a protective head cap surrounding the electrode assembly were cemented in place at the end of the procedure using orthodontic resin (C 22-05-98, Pearson Dental Supply, Sylmar, CA).

Behavior apparatus

The apparatus for Pavlovian fear conditioning consisted of two individual chambers with aluminum front and back walls retrofitted with clear plastic covers, clear acrylic sides and top, and a grid floor. Each grid floor bar was electrically connected to an aversive shock generator (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT) through a grounding device. This permitted the floor to be grounded at all times except during shock delivery. An external food cup and a central nose poke opening, equipped with infrared photocells were present on one wall. Auditory stimuli were presented through two speakers mounted on the ceiling.

Nose poke acquisition

Prior to discrimination sessions, rats were food-deprived to 85% of their free-feeding body weight and were fed specifically to maintain this weight through the behavioral procedure. Starting on P59, rats were shaped to nose poke for pellet delivery in the experimental chamber using a fixed ratio schedule in which one nose poke yielded one pellet. Shaping sessions lasted 30 min or until approximately 50 nose pokes were completed. Over the next 3 days, rats were placed on 5 days of variable interval (VI) schedules in which nose pokes were reinforced on average every 30 s (day 1), or 60 s (days 2 through 5). For the remainder of behavioral testing, nose pokes were reinforced on a VI-60 schedule independent of all Pavlovian contingencies.

Pre-exposure

In two separate sessions, each rat was pre-exposed to the three cues to be used in Pavlovian discrimination. Auditory cues consisted of repeating motifs of broadband click, phaser or trumpet. These 42 min sessions consisted of four presentations of each cue (12 total presentations) with a mean inter-trial interval (ITI) of 3.5 min. The order of trial type presentation was randomly determined by the behavioral program and differed for each rat during each session.

Fear discrimination

Prior to recording, each rat received eight, 93 min sessions of fear discrimination. Each session consisted of 32 trials, with a mean ITI of 3.5 min. Auditory cues were 10 s in duration and consisted of repeating motifs of a broadband click, phaser, or trumpet. Each cue was associated with a unique probability of foot shock (0.5 mA, 0.5 s): danger, p=1.00; uncertainty, p=0.375; and safety, p=0.00. Auditory identity was counterbalanced across rats. Foot shock was administered 2 s following the termination of the auditory cue on danger and uncertainty shock trials. This was done in order to observe possible neural activity during the delay period not driven by an explicit cue. A single session consisted of six danger trials, ten uncertainty no-shock trials, six uncertainty shock trials, and ten safety trials. The order of trial type presentation was randomly determined by the behavioral program, and differed for each rat, each session. After the eighth session, rats were removed from discrimination, given full food and received stereotaxic surgery. Following recovery, discrimination (identical to that described above) resumed with single-unit recording. Animals received discrimination every other day with recording. After each discrimination session with recording, electrodes were advanced either 0.042 mm or 0.084 mm to record from new units during the following session.

Histology

Rats were deeply anesthetized using isoflurane and final electrode coordinates were marked by passing current from a 6 V battery through 4 of the 16 nichrome electrode wires. Rats were perfused with 0.9% biological saline and 4% paraformaldehyde in a 0.2 M Potassium Phosphate Buffered Solution. Brains were extracted and post-fixed in a 10% neutral-buffered formalin solution for 24 hr, stored in 10% sucrose/formalin and sectioned via microtome. All brains processed for light microscopy using anti-tryptophan hydroxylase immunohistochemistry (T8575, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) and a NovaRed chromagen reaction (SK-4800, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) Sections were mounted, imaged using a light microscope and electrode placement was confirmed (Paxinos and Watson, 2007).

Single-unit data acquisition

Sixteen individual recording wires were bundled and soldered to individual channels of an Omnetics connector. The bundle was integrated into a microdrive permitting advancement in ~0.042 mm increments. The microdrive was cemented on top of the skull and the Omnetics connector was affixed to the head cap. During recording sessions, a 1x amplifying head stage connected the Omnetics connector to the commutator via a shielded recording cable (head stage: 40684–020 and Cable: 91809–017, Plexon Inc, Dallas TX). Analog neural activity was digitized and high-pass filtered via amplifier to remove low-frequency artifacts and sent to the Ominplex D acquisition system (Plexon Inc, Dallas TX). Behavioral events (cues, shocks, nose pokes) were controlled and recorded by a computer running Med Associates software. Timestamped events from Med Associates were sent to Ominplex D acquisition system via a dedicated interface module (DIG-716B). The result was a single file (.pl2) containing all time stamps for recording and behavior. Single-units were sorted offline with a template-based spike-sorting algorithm (Offline Sorter V3, Plexon Inc, Dallas TX). Timestamped spikes and events (cues, shocks, nose pokes) were extracted and analyzed with statistical routines in MATLAB (Natick, MA). Neural activity was recorded throughout the 500 ms shock delivery period. However, we cannot be certain that shock artifact did not disrupt spike collection, so we do not present activity from this period.

Statistical analysis

Calculating suppression ratios

Fear was measured by suppression of rewarded nose poking, calculated as a ratio: (baseline poke rate – cue poke rate) / (baseline poke rate +cue poke rate) (Rescorla, 1968; Pickens et al., 2009; Anglada-Figueroa and Quirk, 2005; Arico and McNally, 2014Lee et al., 2005; McDannald and Galarce, 2011). A ratio of ‘1’ indicated high fear, ‘0’ low fear, and gradations between intermediate levels of fear. Use of the suppression ratio permitted the objective measure of relative fear in 1 s intervals across the cue, as well as total fear over the entire 10 s cue presentation (Wright et al., 2015).

K-means clustering

The following characteristics were determined for each neuron: baseline firing rate, half the duration of the mean waveform and amplitude ratio of the mean waveform. Duration was determined by measuring the time (ms) from peak depolarization to the trough of after-hyperpolarization and dividing by two. Amplitude ratio was calculated using (n – p) / (n + p), in which p=initial hyperpolarization (in mV) and n = maximal depolarization (in mV). This approach has been used to successfully separate neuron types in the ventral tegmental area (Roesch et al., 2007). K-means clustering used these three firing characteristics to partition the 245 recorded neurons into two clusters (k = 2). Two clusters were chosen because previous studies have found that two neuron types, glutamatergic vGluT2 neurons and GABAergic Gad1 neurons, comprise the majority of vlPAG neurons, and these neurons can be differentiated by baseline firing rate (Tovote et al., 2016). ANOVA for cluster results found that only baseline firing rate contributed to cluster membership (F1,243 = 829, p<0.001). Neither amplitude ratio nor duration reached significance (Fs <0.2, ps >0.6). All neurons were clustered, with the majority falling in the low firing rate cluster (n = 199) and the remaining in the high firing rate cluster (n = 46).

Identifying cue-responsive vlPAG neurons

Independent of cluster analysis, all 245 neurons were screened for short-latency, excitatory firing to auditory cue onset (Tovote et al., 2016; Watson et al., 2016; Ozawa et al., 2017). This was achieved using a paired, two-tailed t-test comparing raw firing rate (spikes/s) during a 2 s baseline period just prior to cue onset and during the first, 1 s cue interval. A t-test was performed for each of the three cues (danger, uncertainty and safety), corrected for multiple comparisons (p<0.017). The remaining neurons were screened for longer-latency, excitatory firing to the later portion of auditory cues (Ozawa et al., 2017), using an identical t-test, only now comparing firing rate during a 2 s baseline period just prior to cue onset and the last, 1 s cue interval.

Z-score normalization

For each neuron, and for each trial type, firing rate (spikes/s) was calculated in 100 ms bins from 10 s prior to cue onset to 12 s following cue offset, for a total of 320 bins. Mean firing rate over the 320 bins was calculated by averaging all trials for each trial type. Mean differential firing was calculated for each of the 320 bins by subtracting mean baseline firing rate (2 s prior to cue onset), specific to that trial type, from each bin. Mean differential firing was Z-score normalized across all trial types within a single neuron, such that mean firing = 0, and standard deviation in firing = 1. Z-score normalization was applied to firing across the entirety of the recording epoch, as opposed to only the baseline period, in case neurons showed little/no baseline activity. As a result, periods of phasic, excitatory firing contributed to normalized mean firing rate (0). For this reason, Z-score normalized baseline activity is below zero in Figure 2A & C. Z-score normalized firing during cue (Figure 2A,C) and post-cue periods (Figure 6A,B), was analyzed with ANOVA using bin and trial-type as factors. F and p values are reported, as well as partial eta squared and observed power.

For post hoc cue firing analyses (Figure 2B,D), cue correlation analyses (Figure 3 all), and cue regression analyses, it was necessary to calculate normalized firing in 1 s intervals. To do this, differential firing in the interval of interest (for example, first cue 1 s interval) was calculated for each individual of the 32 trials in a single session. Differential firing in this interval was then Z-score transformed. This process was repeated for each interval of interest. This done in order to maximize the distribution of firing within a single interval. Importantly, statistical outcomes were identical if a single Z-score transformation was applied to all intervals at once. An identical approach was used to Z-score normalize firing in 500 ms intervals for post-cue firing (Figure 6).

Determining observed and expected cue firing patterns

The analysis for Onset neurons (n = 29) utilized mean normalized firing to each cue (danger, uncertainty and safety) in the first 1 s interval; analysis for Ramping neurons (n = 14) utilized firing in the last 1 s interval. Relative firing to the three cues was used to categorize each Onset and Ramping neuron: (d > u > s), (d > s > u), (s > u > d) or (u > d > s). Counting the number observed in each category determined the actual number for each population. In order to determine the number in each category expected by chance, each neurons firing pattern was shuffled and the number of neurons in each category counted. Shuffling and counting neuron category was repeated 1000 times for each population. Box and whisker plots for each category/population were constructed showing the median, 25th percentile, 75th percentile and most extreme non-outliers. The actual number and expected statistics are reported plotted together in Figure 4D/H.

Population and single-unit firing analyses

Population firing was analyzed using analysis of variance (ANOVA) with trial type and bin (100 ms) as factors. ANOVA for cue firing contained three trial types (danger, uncertainty and safety). Uncertainty trial types were collapsed because they did not differ for either suppression ratio or firing analysis. This was expected, during cue presentation rats did not know the current uncertainty trial type. ANOVA for post-cue firing contained four trial types (danger, uncertainty shock, uncertainty omission and safety). Uncertainty trial types were split because shock was delivered during the period. F statistic, p value, observed power and partial eta squared are reported for effects and interactions. Interval firing was compared within a population using a two-tailed, dependent samples t-test. Identical firing analyses were performed for the post-cue period, only now four trial types were used (danger, uncertainty shock, uncertainty omission and safety).

The sliding window analysis for the Ramping population (Figure 2E) employed a two-tailed, dependent samples t-test. Ramping population danger firing was compared to safety firing in 1 s intervals, with the window advancing in 100 ms steps across cue presentation. The p value for each comparison was recorded, and the first window with p<0.05 was termed the interval of departure (when danger and safety firing significant differed). The same analysis was performed for uncertainty and safety. Change in firing from interval of departure to the final 1 s window was calculated by subtracting the departure firing rate from the last 1 s window firing rate and dividing by time between these two periods.

Biases in single-unit firing to the three cues (Figure 3) were determined using the sign test comparing z firing to danger vs. uncertainty, and uncertainty vs. safety. The relationship between firing was determined using the Pearson correlation coefficient.

Single-unit, linear regression

Single-unit, linear regression was used to determine the degree to which fear output and threat probability explained trial-by-trial variation in firing of single neurons in a specific time interval. The cue analysis used 1 s intervals, while the post-cue analyses used 500 ms intervals. Shorter intervals were used in the post-cue analysis to accommodate the 500 ms foot shock duration. For each regression, all 32 trials from a single session were ordered by type. Z-firing was specified for the interval of interest. The interval fear regressor was the suppression ratio for that specific interval/trial, while the total fear regressor was the suppression ratio for the entire cue, for that specific trial. The probability regressor was the foot shock probability associated with the specific cue. Regression (using the regress function in MATLAB) required a separate, constant input. To better visualize the organization of the regression input, the complete regression input for first interval firing of an Onset neuron is shown below. Identical regression analysis was performed for the post-cue period, only now 500 ms intervals were examined and the total fear regressor was removed. This interval was used to accommodate the 500 ms shock period and the total fear regressor was removed because it only sampled behavior from the 10 s cue period.

trial # trial type Z firing constant interval fear total fear probability
1 danger 2.32 1 1.00 1.00 1
2 danger 0.51 1 1.00 1.00 1
3 danger 0.81 1 1.00 1.00 1
4 danger 1.56 1 1.00 1.00 1
5 danger 1.41 1 -1.00 0.73 1
6 danger 2.92 1 1.00 1.00 1
7 unc-shock 0.81 1 1.00 1.00 0.375
8 unc-shock -0.40 1 -0.33 0.47 0.375
9 unc-shock 0.21 1 1.00 0.64 0.375
10 unc-shock -0.70 1 1.00 0.67 0.375
11 unc-shock -0.85 1 1.00 1.00 0.375
12 unc-shock -0.70 1 -1.00 0.64 0.375
13 unc-omission -0.70 1 1.00 1.00 0.375
14 unc-omission -0.70 1 -1.00 0.40 0.375
15 unc-omission -0.70 1 0.00 0.23 0.375
16 unc-omission 0.66 1 1.00 1.00 0.375
17 unc-omission 0.21 1 1.00 1.00 0.375
18 unc-omission -0.70 1 1.00 0.71 0.375
19 unc-omission -1.00 1 1.00 0.43 0.375
20 unc-omission 1.11 1 1.00 1.00 0.375
21 unc-omission -0.09 1 -0.60 0.56 0.375
22 unc-omission -0.85 1 1.00 1.00 0.375
23 safety -0.70 1 -0.33 0.06 0
24 safety -0.70 1 0.00 0.27 0
25 safety -0.70 1 1.00 -0.04 0
26 safety -0.40 1 1.00 0.14 0
27 safety -0.70 1 1.00 0.54 0
28 safety -0.70 1 -0.50 0.03 0
29 safety 0.21 1 1.00 0.47 0
30 safety -0.85 1 1.00 1.00 0
31 safety 0.06 1 1.00 0.20 0
32 safety -0.70 1 1.00 -0.14 0
beta coefficient: 0.24 -0.14 2.19

The regression output of greatest interest was the beta coefficient for each regressor (interval fear, total fear and probability), quantifying the strength (greater distance from zero = stronger) and direction (>0 = positive) of the predictive relationship between each regressor and single-unit firing. ANOVA, two-tailed dependent samples t-test, sign test and The Pearson correlation coefficient was used to analyze beta coefficients, exactly as described for normalized firing. An identical approach was used for the pairs analysis (Figure 3) only now firing of the Ramping neuron was used as a regressor to predict firing of the paired Onset neuron. Beta coefficient (β) and significance of the predictive relationship (p) are reported.

Threat probability tuning curve

Single-unit, linear regression was performed using the interval fear, total fear and probability regressor as above. Only now, nine separate regression analyses were performed in which the uncertainty component of the probability regressor was systematically varied from 0 to 1 in 0.125 increments (0.000, 0.125, 0.250, 0.375, 0.500, 0.625, 0.750, 0.875 and 1.000). The result of primary interest was the mean beta coefficient for the probability regressor from each variant of regression, as plotted in Figure 5E/J.

Correlation matrix

A correlation matrix was created for the post-cue period for the Onset population (n = 29) as well as the Ramping population (n = 14). The periods of interest were the four, 500 ms intervals prior to shock presentation and the five, 500 ms intervals following shock. Starting with interval 1, The Pearson correlation coefficient (R2) and associated p value was calculated by comparing the probability beta coefficient for each neuron to those for intervals 2–4 and 6–10. Most critical were the comparison with intervals 6–10, that permitted comparison of beta coefficients between pre-shock and post-shock periods. The same relationships were determined for each of the remaining intervals (2–4, and 6–10). The p value of R2 for each interval comparison is plotted. The proportion of significant intervals forming the pre-shock and post-shock comparison (n = 20) were compared for the Onset and Ramping populations using the chi-square test.

Data and software availability

Full electrophysiology data set will be uploaded to http://crcns.org/ upon acceptance for publication.

Additional resources

Med Associates programs used for behavior and MATLAB programs used for behavioral analyses are made freely available at our lab website: http://mcdannaldlab.org/resources.

Acknowledgements

We thank Dr. Hiram Brownell for statistical advice on linear regression, Dr. Thomas C Jhou for designing and assembling the shock floor grounding device, Dr. Donald B Katz for guidance on electrode construction, Dr. Thomas Stalnaker for guidance on waveform analysis, Blake Zimmerman for guidance on MATLAB scripts, Jasmin Strickland for manuscript feedback and Daniel Pimpinelli for assistance with animal care. This work was supported by NIH DA034010 to MAM.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Kristina M Wright, Email: wrightko@bc.edu.

Michael A McDannald, Email: michael.mcdannald@bc.edu.

Geoffrey Schoenbaum, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institutes of Health, United States.

Laura Colgin, The University of Texas at Austin, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health MH117791 to Michael A McDannald.

  • National Institutes of Health DA034010 to Michael A McDannald.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All of the animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols (#2018-002) of Boston College. All surgery was performed under isofluorane anesthesia, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.45013.019

Data availability

Single-unit data are publicly available on CRCNS (http://crcns.org/data-sets/brainstem/pag-1), under the doi: 10.6080/K0R49P0V. Users must first create a free account (https://crcns.org/register) before they can download the datasets from the site.

The following dataset was generated:

Wright KM, McDannald MA. 2019. Data from: Ventrolateral periaqueductal gray neurons prioritize threat probability over fear output. CRCNS.

References

  1. Admon R, Milad MR, Hendler T. A causal model of post-traumatic stress disorder: disentangling predisposed from acquired neural abnormalities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17:337–347. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anglada-Figueroa D, Quirk GJ. Lesions of the basal amygdala block expression of conditioned fear but not extinction. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:9680–9685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2600-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arico C, Bagley EE, Carrive P, Assareh N, McNally GP. Effects of chemogenetic excitation or inhibition of the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray on the acquisition and extinction of pavlovian fear conditioning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2017;144:186–197. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arico C, McNally GP. Opioid receptors regulate blocking and overexpectation of fear learning in conditioned suppression. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;128:199–206. doi: 10.1037/a0036133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Assareh N, Sarrami M, Carrive P, McNally GP. The organization of defensive behavior elicited by optogenetic excitation of rat lateral or ventrolateral periaqueductal gray. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2016;130:406–414. doi: 10.1037/bne0000151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Assareh N, Bagley EE, Carrive P, McNally GP. Brief optogenetic inhibition of rat lateral or ventrolateral periaqueductal gray augments the acquisition of pavlovian fear conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2017;131:454–459. doi: 10.1037/bne0000217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bandler R, Depaulis A, Vergnes M. Identification of midbrain neurones mediating defensive behaviour in the rat by microinjections of excitatory amino acids. Behavioural Brain Research. 1985;15:107–119. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(85)90058-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bear MF, Connors BW, Paradiso MA. Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bellgowan PS, Helmstetter FJ. The role of mu and kappa opioid receptors within the periaqueductal gray in the expression of conditional hypoalgesia. Brain Research. 1998;791:83–89. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00057-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Berg BA, Schoenbaum G, McDannald MA. The dorsal raphe nucleus is integral to negative prediction errors in pavlovian fear. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;40:3096–3101. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bouton ME, Bolles RC. Conditioned fear assessed by freezing and by the suppression of three different baselines. Animal Learning & Behavior. 1980;8:429–434. doi: 10.3758/BF03199629. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Buchanan SL, Thompson RH. Neuronal activity in the midline thalamic nuclei during pavlovian heart rate conditioning. Brain Research Bulletin. 1994;35:237–240. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(94)90128-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Büchel C, Dolan RJ, Armony JL, Friston KJ. Amygdala-hippocampal involvement in human aversive trace conditioning revealed through event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:10869–10876. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-24-10869.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carlson NR, Birkett MA. Physiology of Behavior. Pearson; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  15. Carrive P, Leung P, Harris J, Paxinos G. Conditioned fear to context is associated with increased fos expression in the caudal ventrolateral region of the midbrain periaqueductal gray. Neuroscience. 1997;78:165–177. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)83047-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chance WT, White AC, Krynock GM, Rosencrans JA. Conditional fear-induced antinociception and decreased binding of [3H]N-leu-enkephalin to rat brain. Brain Research. 1978;141:371–374. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(78)90208-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Davis M. Neural systems involved in fear and anxiety measured with fear-potentiated startle. American Psychologist. 2006;61:741–756. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.8.741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. De Oca BM, DeCola JP, Maren S, Fanselow MS. Distinct regions of the periaqueductal gray are involved in the acquisition and expression of defensive responses. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:3426–3432. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-09-03426.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dejean C, Courtin J, Rozeske RR, Bonnet MC, Dousset V, Michelet T, Herry C. Neuronal circuits for fear expression and recovery: recent advances and potential therapeutic strategies. Biological Psychiatry. 2015;78:298–306. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Deutch AY, Goldstein M, Baldino F, Roth RH. Telencephalic projections of the A8 dopamine cell group. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1988;537:27–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1988.tb42095.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. DiLeo A, Wright KM, McDannald MA. Subsecond fear discrimination in rats: adult impairment in adolescent heavy alcohol drinkers. Learning & Memory. 2016;23:618–622. doi: 10.1101/lm.043257.116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Duvarci S, Pare D. Amygdala microcircuits controlling learned fear. Neuron. 2014;82:966–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell ED, Yacubian J, Klinger R, Lorenz J, Büchel C. Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia. Neuron. 2009;63:533–543. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Estes WK, Skinner BF. Some quantitative properties of anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1941;29:390–400. doi: 10.1037/h0062283. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Etkin A, Wager TD. Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164:1476–1488. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fanselow MS, Lester LS. A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. In: Bolles R. C, Beecher M. D, editors. In Evolution and Learning. New Jersey: Erlbaum; 1988. pp. 185–212. [Google Scholar]
  27. Fanselow MS. The midbrain periaqueductal gray as a coordinator of action in response to fear and anxiety. Midbrain Periaqueductal Gray Matter. 1991;213:151–173. [Google Scholar]
  28. Fanselow MS. The periaqueductal gray and the organization of defensive behavior. Aggressive Behavior. 1993;19:18–19. [Google Scholar]
  29. Fanselow MS. Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 1994;1:429–438. doi: 10.3758/BF03210947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fanselow MS, Bolles RC. Triggering of the endorphin analgesic reaction by a cue previously associated with shock: reversal by naloxone. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society. 1979;14:88–90. doi: 10.3758/BF03329408. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fanselow MS, LeDoux JE. Why we think plasticity underlying pavlovian fear conditioning occurs in the basolateral amygdala. Neuron. 1999;23:229–232. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80775-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Faull OK, Jenkinson M, Ezra M, Pattinson KTs. Conditioned respiratory threat in the subdivisions of the human periaqueductal gray. eLife. 2016;5:e12047. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gallego J, Nsegbe E, Durand E. Learning in respiratory control. Behavior Modification. 2001;25:495–512. doi: 10.1177/0145445501254002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Greiner EM, Norris MR, Müller I, Ng KH, Sangha S. Sex differences in fear regulation and reward seeking behaviors in a fear-safety-reward discrimination task. bioRxiv. 2018 doi: 10.1101/390377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  35. Gruene TM, Flick K, Stefano A, Shea SD, Shansky RM. Sexually divergent expression of active and passive conditioned fear responses in rats. eLife. 2015;4:e11352. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Helmstetter FJ, Tershner SA. Lesions of the periaqueductal gray and rostral ventromedial medulla disrupt antinociceptive but not cardiovascular aversive conditional responses. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1994;14:7099–7108. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-11-07099.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Insanally MN, Carcea I, Field RE, Rodgers CC, DePasquale B, Rajan K, DeWeese MR, Albanna BF, Froemke RC. Spike-timing-dependent ensemble encoding by non-classically responsive cortical neurons. eLife. 2019;8:e42409. doi: 10.7554/eLife.42409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Iwata J, LeDoux JE. Dissociation of associative and nonassociative concomitants of classical fear conditioning in the freely behaving rat. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1988;102:66–76. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.102.1.66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Johansen JP, Tarpley JW, LeDoux JE, Blair HT. Neural substrates for expectation-modulated fear learning in the amygdala and periaqueductal gray. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:979–986. doi: 10.1038/nn.2594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Johnstone T, Somerville LH, Alexander AL, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH, Whalen PJ. Stability of amygdala BOLD response to fearful faces over multiple scan sessions. NeuroImage. 2005;25:1112–1123. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jones LM, Fontanini A, Sadacca BF, Miller P, Katz DB. Natural stimuli evoke dynamic sequences of states in sensory cortical ensembles. PNAS. 2007;104:18772–18777. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705546104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kim JJ, Rison RA, Fanselow MS. Effects of Amygdala, Hippocampus, and periaqueductal gray lesions on short- and long-term contextual fear. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1993;107:1093–1098. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.6.1093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kim EJ, Kim ES, Covey E, Kim JJ. Social transmission of fear in rats: the role of 22-kHz ultrasonic distress vocalization. PLOS ONE. 2010;5:e15077. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Koutsikou S, Crook JJ, Earl EV, Leith JL, Watson TC, Lumb BM, Apps R. Neural substrates underlying fear-evoked freezing: the periaqueductal grey-cerebellar link. The Journal of Physiology. 2014;592:2197–2213. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.268714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Krout KE, Loewy AD. Periaqueductal gray matter projections to midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei of the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2000;424:111–141. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20000814)424:1&#x0003c;111::AID-CNE9&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. LeDoux JE, Iwata J, Cicchetti P, Reis DJ. Different projections of the central amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral correlates of conditioned fear. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1988;8:2517–2529. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-07-02517.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lee JL, Dickinson A, Everitt BJ. Conditioned suppression and freezing as measures of aversive pavlovian conditioning: effects of discrete amygdala lesions and overtraining. Behavioural Brain Research. 2005;159:221–233. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Liddell BJ, Brown KJ, Kemp AH, Barton MJ, Das P, Peduto A, Gordon E, Williams LM. A direct brainstem-amygdala-cortical 'alarm' system for subliminal signals of fear. NeuroImage. 2005;24:235–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Liebman JM, Mayer DJ, Liebeskind JC. Mesencephalic central gray lesions and fear-motivated behavior in rats. Brain Research. 1970;23:353–370. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(70)90062-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. MacLennan AJ, Jackson RL, Maier SF. Conditioned analgesia in the rat. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society. 1980;15:387–390. doi: 10.3758/BF03334567. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mahan AL, Ressler KJ. Fear conditioning, synaptic plasticity and the amygdala: implications for posttraumatic stress disorder. Trends in Neurosciences. 2012;35:24–35. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Maren S, Phan KL, Liberzon I. The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14:417–428. doi: 10.1038/nrn3492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. McDannald MA. Contributions of the amygdala central nucleus and ventrolateral periaqueductal grey to freezing and instrumental suppression in pavlovian fear conditioning. Behavioural Brain Research. 2010;211:111–117. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.03.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. McDannald MA, Galarce EM. Measuring pavlovian fear with conditioned freezing and conditioned suppression reveals different roles for the basolateral amygdala. Brain Research. 2011;1374:82–89. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.12.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. McEchron MD, Bouwmeester H, Tseng W, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF. Hippocampectomy disrupts auditory trace fear conditioning and contextual fear conditioning in the rat. Hippocampus. 1998;8:638–646. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:6&#x0003c;638::AID-HIPO6&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. McNally GP, Johansen JP, Blair HT. Placing prediction into the fear circuit. Trends in Neurosciences. 2011;34:283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mobbs D, Petrovic P, Marchant JL, Hassabis D, Weiskopf N, Seymour B, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. When fear is near: threat imminence elicits prefrontal-periaqueductal gray shifts in humans. Science. 2007;317:1079–1083. doi: 10.1126/science.1144298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Mobbs D, Yu R, Rowe JB, Eich H, FeldmanHall O, Dalgleish T. Neural activity associated with monitoring the oscillating threat value of a tarantula. PNAS. 2010;107:20582–20586. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009076107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ozawa T, Ycu EA, Kumar A, Yeh LF, Ahmed T, Koivumaa J, Johansen JP. A feedback neural circuit for calibrating aversive memory strength. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:90–97. doi: 10.1038/nn.4439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Parsons RG, Gafford GM, Helmstetter FJ. Regulation of extinction-related plasticity by opioid receptors in the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray matter. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2010;4:44. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. 6th Edition. Boston, Amsterdam: Academic Press/Elsevier; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  62. Perusini JN, Fanselow MS. Neurobehavioral perspectives on the distinction between fear and anxiety. Learning & Memory. 2015;22:417–425. doi: 10.1101/lm.039180.115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Ingvar M. Placebo and opioid analgesia-- imaging a shared neuronal network. Science. 2002;295:1737–1740. doi: 10.1126/science.1067176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pickens CL, Golden SA, Adams-Deutsch T, Nair SG, Shaham Y. Long-lasting incubation of conditioned fear in rats. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;65:881–886. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Rauch SL, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK. Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;47:769–776. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00828-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Ray MH, Hanlon E, McDannald MA. Lateral orbitofrontal cortex partitions mechanisms for fear regulation and alcohol consumption. PLOS ONE. 2018;13:e0198043. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0198043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Rescorla RA. Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychological Review. 1967;74:71–80. doi: 10.1037/h0024109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Rescorla RA. Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1968;66:1–5. doi: 10.1037/h0025984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Roesch MR, Calu DJ, Schoenbaum G. Dopamine neurons encode the better option in rats deciding between differently delayed or sized rewards. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10:1615–1624. doi: 10.1038/nn2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rozeske RR, Jercog D, Karalis N, Chaudun F, Khoder S, Girard D, Winke N, Herry C. Prefrontal-Periaqueductal Gray-Projecting neurons mediate context fear discrimination. Neuron. 2018;97:898–910. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.12.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Satpute AB, Wager TD, Cohen-Adad J, Bianciardi M, Choi JK, Buhle JT, Wald LL, Barrett LF. Identification of discrete functional subregions of the human periaqueductal gray. PNAS. 2013;110:17101–17106. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1306095110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sengupta A, McNally GP. A role for midline and intralaminar thalamus in the associative blocking of pavlovian fear conditioning. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;8:148. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Tovote P, Fadok JP, Lüthi A. Neuronal circuits for fear and anxiety. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2015;16:317–331. doi: 10.1038/nrn3945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tovote P, Esposito MS, Botta P, Chaudun F, Fadok JP, Markovic M, Wolff SB, Ramakrishnan C, Fenno L, Deisseroth K, Herry C, Arber S, Lüthi A. Midbrain circuits for defensive behaviour. Nature. 2016;534:206–212. doi: 10.1038/nature17996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tracey I. Getting the pain you expect: mechanisms of placebo, nocebo and reappraisal effects in humans. Nature Medicine. 2010;16:1277–1283. doi: 10.1038/nm.2229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Vertes RP, Linley SB, Hoover WB. Limbic circuitry of the midline thalamus. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2015;54:89–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Vianna DM, Graeff FG, Brandão ML, Landeira-Fernandez J. Defensive freezing evoked by electrical stimulation of the periaqueductal gray: comparison between dorsolateral and ventrolateral regions. Neuroreport. 2001;12:4109–4112. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200112210-00049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Vianna DM, Allen C, Carrive P. Cardiovascular and behavioral responses to conditioned fear after medullary raphe neuronal blockade. Neuroscience. 2008;153:1344–1353. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.03.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Vianna DM, Carrive P. Changes in cutaneous and body temperature during and after conditioned fear to context in the rat. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;21:2505–2512. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04073.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Von Krosigk M, Smith AD. Descending projections from the substantia nigra and retrorubral field to the medullary and pontomedullary reticular formation. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1991;3:260–273. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1991.tb00088.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Wager TD, Atlas LY. The neuroscience of placebo effects: connecting context, learning and health. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2015;16:403–418. doi: 10.1038/nrn3976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Walker DL, Cassella JV, Lee Y, De Lima TC, Davis M. Opposing roles of the amygdala and dorsolateral periaqueductal gray in fear-potentiated startle. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 1997;21:743–753. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(96)00061-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Walker RA, Andreansky C, Ray MH, McDannald MA. Early adolescent adversity inflates threat estimation in females and promotes alcohol use initiation in both sexes. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2018;132:171–182. doi: 10.1037/bne0000239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Watson TC, Cerminara NL, Lumb BM, Apps R. Neural correlates of fear in the periaqueductal gray. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:12707–12719. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1100-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Wilson A, Kapp BS. Effect of lesions of the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray on the pavlovian conditioned heart rate response in the rabbit. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 1994;62:73–76. doi: 10.1016/S0163-1047(05)80061-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Wright KM, DiLeo A, McDannald MA. Early adversity disrupts the adult use of aversive prediction errors to reduce fear in uncertainty. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2015;9:227. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Yeh LF, Watanabe M, Sulkes-Cuevas J, Johansen JP. Dysregulation of aversive signaling pathways: a novel circuit endophenotype for pain and anxiety disorders. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2018;48:37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Zahm DS, Trimble M. The dopaminergic projection system, basal forebrain macrosystems, and conditioned stimuli. CNS Spectrums. 2008;13:32–40. doi: 10.1017/S1092852900016138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Zhao Z, Yang Y, Walker DL, Davis M. Effects of substance P in the Amygdala, Ventromedial Hypothalamus, and periaqueductal gray on fear-potentiated startle. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:331–340. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Zhou J, Gardner MPH, Stalnaker TA, Ramus SJ, Wikenheiser AM, Niv Y, Schoenbaum G. Rat orbitofrontal ensemble activity contains a multiplexed but value-invariant representation of task structure in an odor sequence task. bioRxiv. 2018 doi: 10.1101/507376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Decision letter

Editor: Geoffrey Schoenbaum1
Reviewed by: Satoshi Ikemoto2, Laura Bradfield3

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Ventrolateral periaqueductal gray neurons prioritize threat probability over fear output" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Geoffrey Schoenbaum as the Reviewing Editor, and the evaluation has been overseen by Laura Colgin as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Satoshi Ikemoto (Reviewer #2) and Laura Bradfield (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

In this study the authors recorded from PAG neurons during a fear discrimination task. Three cues predicted certain, uncertain, and no shock. They measured fear during the 10s cues as suppression behavior, demonstrating discriminative fear responding. However the degree of the difference between the certain and uncertain shock was larger than the difference in suppression, especially early, which was quite similar for these two cues. Subsequent analyses of unit firing revealed populations of onset and ramping neurons that seemed to reflect the fear response as shown previously. However a more careful analysis regressing firing on probability of shock versus behavior showed that onset neurons were primarily reflecting probability and ramping neurons reflected a combination of the two. These data nicely show that contrary to current proposals in simpler tasks, the firing of PAG neurons is not simply reflective of behavioral or fear output and instead reflects a more careful assessment of the actual probability of shock or danger.

Essential revisions:

Overall the reviewers were very positive. The report provides novel information on how PAG responses correlate with shock probability versus behavioral output, information somewhat contradictory to current ideas that PAG drives the response. However all three reviewers shared the general concern that the conclusion did not consider some possible alternatives or problems in the approach. Reviewer #1 suggests that it assumes a linear relationship between shock probability and behavior; reviewer #2 worries that there is not a sufficient exploration of the probability space for a regression analysis, and reviewer #3 raises the possibility that other factors, like stress, might add to the fear from shock probability. These concerns might be addressed with additional analyses (population comparisons or analyses of specific units showing that many discriminate different probability cues when behavior does not). However, to some extent they mostly require some mention in the Discussion. Being clear that suppression is what is measured (in Abstract and elsewhere) as fear may also be helpful. In addition to this, reviewer #3 also made the point that the recording study is correlational. Some discussion regarding whether these signals are generated in PAG or received from upstream areas like amygdala, and whether they are prominent enough to guarantee output downstream would be a useful addition.

Minor points:

Please look through the paper and try to smooth out any complex writing like that identified by reviewer 2. Please see the individual reviews, which are provided in their entirety below, for other suggestions.

Reviewer #1:

In this study the authors recorded from PAG neurons during a fear discrimination task. Three cues predicted certain, uncertain, and no shock. They measured fear during the 10s cues as suppression behavior, demonstrating discriminative fear responding. However the degree of the difference between the certain and uncertain shock was larger than the difference in suppression, especially early, which was quite similar for these two cues. Subsequent analyses of unit firing revealed populations of onset and ramping neurons that seemed to reflect the fear response as shown previously. However a more careful analysis regressing firing on probability of shock versus behavior showed that onset neurons were primarily reflecting probability and ramping neurons reflected a combination of the two. These data nicely show that contrary to current proposals in simpler tasks, the firing of PAG neurons is not simply reflective of behavioral or fear output and instead reflects a more careful assessment of the actual probability of shock or danger. Overall I thought the study was creative and the analysis was careful and the results clear. I have only a few suggestions.

1) The initial screen seemed to identify neurons that had heightened activity to any of the cues. Were all the neurons selective for the proper order or was there variability in preferred cues?

I think the supplementary figure showing the variance captured by different regressors was convincing. Some display of this might be in the main text.

One alternative or clarification that I think might be helpful also is for the authors to more clearly explain how their task allows them to dissociate the two ideas about what PAG neurons are encoding. Assuming I am correct this is both simple as well as an elegant solution to the problem. It should be made clear I think to help the reader understand how it is done. Relatedly a small caveat may need to be put in the Discussion to the effect that their conclusion (I think) assumes a linear relationship between firing and behavior. Since they have another variable – threat – that provides a much better correlation I accept this as a reasonable assumption here. But I think if I am correct that it should be mentioned.

Reviewer #2:

The authors investigated neural activity of the vlPAG with respect to behavioral suppression induced by shock-associated cues. They focused on two types of neurons: ones that responded to the onset of cue associated with shock and those that displayed ramping responses to shock. They found that activities of both types of neurons were better correlated with the probability of shock delivery rather than cue-induced suppression.

My concern is that a key finding of the study depends on a linear regression analysis containing a measurement variable with only three values: threat probabilities of 0, 0.375, and 1.0. I would have felt more confident if the experiment had more probability values.

Minor Comments:

Writing is hard. It sometimes requires effort to understand. Here are examples:

“ramping activity prioritized threat probability over fear output.”

“vlPAG signals for fear output may reside in other functional populations, such has neurons showing inhibition of firing to danger cues [Tovote, Fadok and Luthi, 2015].”

“Yet, this would still require that adjacent populations for threat probability and fear output exist within the vlPAG.”

Abstract

The key dependent measure, conditioned suppression, should be mentioned in the Abstract section. Remember that this is a primary scientific report. Although theoretical constructs such as "fear" can be mentioned, the concrete measure of fear should be identified in Abstract.

Results

Throughout the Results section, t-tests were conducted as post-hoc comparisons after ANOVAs. Are the familywise error rates controlled?

Discussion

An observation with statistical data is described for the first time in the Discussion section. Such information should be reported in the result section. In addition, the authors discussion is not coherent. The authors first state that their electrophysiological observation was performed in the caudal vlPAG. Then, they state that the neural activity of the caudal vlPAG is not different from those of the rostral subregion. Explain how they can tell subregional difference when they focused on the caudal subregion.

Perhaps, it is important to discuss the present findings in light of the findings that selective lesions of vlPAG is sufficient to disrupt freezing. It does not seem to be difficult to explain how the loss of neurons coding threat probability can disrupt behavioral expression of fear.

The authors state "We did not observe a substantial population of vlPAG neurons with these temporal firing characteristics (Figure 1—figure supplement 3), making the vlPAG a poor candidate for signaling most forms of sustained fear output". This statement is based on the assumption that much larger neural populations are needed to control behavior. However, research on neural ensemble suggests that the loss of only a handful neurons is sufficient to affect behavior. For example, Koya et al., 2009, found that only 2 – 3% of neurons are responsible for mediating context-specific sensitization induced by cocaine administration (Nat Neurosci, 12(8), 1069-1073. doi: http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v12/n8/suppinfo/nn.2364_S1.html).

Reviewer #3:

Wright and McDannald present a comprehensive and elegant set of experiments showing that, contrary to the currently prevailing view, single-unit signalling in the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray (vlPAG) appears to reflect threat probability independently of fear output. They used three distinct auditory cues that predicted footshock with certainty (p = 1), uncertainty (p =.375) or not at all (i.e. safety, p = 0). They found that two distinct populations of vlPAG neurons: onset and ramping neurons, primarily reflected threat probability rather than fear output (as measured by conditioned suppression of an appetitively-trained nose-poke behaviour). Overall I think this article is superb, extremely well-written and easy to follow, a very interesting topic, and a thoughtful discussion of results.

Minor Comments:

There are a couple of very minor points I would like to suggest for improvement. None of the concerns are substantive.

1) The most pressing issue I would like to see addressed is the fact that although electrophysiological recording of single-units is extremely informative, it does not actually provide us with causal data, only correlational. Indeed, the authors acknowledge and deal with this in the discussion, there addressing most of the notes I had made to myself whilst reading the manuscript, which was very satisfying. However, there's one more possibility I would like to see addressed, and that is that the firing patterns they are observing in vlPAG neurons represent information being received from the amygdala, rather than a signal that is being transferred to downstream targets. In other words, does the amygdala code for threat probability, send this information to vlPAG where it then translates it into the necessity for a particular fear response. Unfortunately this is impossible to test with ephys, but I would like to see it mentioned as a possibility, and possibly argued against if the authors see it as unlikely.

2) Further, it is worth noting that uncertainty in and of itself is a stressor, which could explain the fact that the fear output towards the uncertainty cue is higher than might be expected. It could be that the amygdala is encoding the threat probability and that information is being conveyed to the vlPAG, but that the additional stress from uncertainty is being encoded elsewhere in the brain and added to the fear output response at some unknown mediating output structure.

3) In the Results it would be really helpful to see how many rats/neurons etc. were included in the study as I had to search through to find this information.

4) In Figure 1—figure supplement 3, the authors suggest (at least as I understand it) that there is no relation between normalised firing rate and suppression ratio in the overall population of neurons, but from looking at the graph it appears that there might be at least a weak negative correlation? That is, when normalised firing rate was lowest, in the danger condition, suppression ratio appears to be at its highest (indicating high fear), and vice versa for the safety conditions. Is that correct or am I missing something?

5) In the last paragraph of the subsection “Ramping neurons signal threat probability through delay until shock receipt”, Figures 1E and 1F are referred to, I believe this should say Figures 6E and 6F.

eLife. 2019 Mar 7;8:e45013. doi: 10.7554/eLife.45013.024

Author response


Reviewer #1:

[…] 1) The initial screen seemed to identify neurons that had heightened activity to any of the cues. Were all the neurons selective for the proper order or was there variability in preferred cues?

This is an excellent idea. To address this, we have performed a new analysis to address this question and the results are reported in Figure 4D/H (subsection “Population biases are evident in vlPAG single-units”). Separately for Onset and Ramping populations, we counted the number of neurons showing the ordered firing pattern (danger > uncertainty > safety). We also counted the number of neurons showing the next 3 most common firing patterns. We shuffled each neuron’s normalized firing rate to danger, uncertainty and safety and then counted the number of neurons for each pattern type. This process was repeated 1000 times to generate a distribution from the shuffled firing data. This would tell how often we would expect each pattern to occur by chance. The actual values were then compared to the distributions. The number of Onset and Ramping neurons showing the ordered firing pattern (danger > uncertainty > safety) fell entirely outside of the shuffled distribution, while neurons with all other firing patterns fell within the distribution. The results reveal that the ordered firing pattern was greater than would have been expected by chance.

I think the supplementary figure showing the variance captured by different regressors was convincing. Some display of this might be in the main text.

In order to address a concern of reviewer 2, we performed a new analysis in which we asked if the relative firing pattern to danger, uncertainty and safety by Onset (subsection “Onset neurons signal threat probability”) and Ramping neurons (subsection “Ramping neurons prioritize threat probability over fear output”) was better captured by a probability other than 0.375. The result of this analysis was a ‘tuning curve’ showing that Onset neurons were best captured by the actual shock probability while Ramping neurons were slightly shifted right (now reported in Figure 5). As a part of this analysis, one can visualize the result of the ‘binary’ regression result in which safety and uncertainty were both assigned probabilities of 0.000. We have made sure to point this out in the Results section and have kept the full description of the binary regression in the supplement.

One alternative or clarification that I think might be helpful also is for the authors to more clearly explain how their task allows them to dissociate the two ideas about what PAG neurons are encoding. Assuming I am correct this is both simple as well as an elegant solution to the problem. It should be made clear I think to help the reader understand how it is done. Relatedly a small caveat may need to be put in the Discussion to the effect that their conclusion (I think) assumes a linear relationship between firing and behavior. Since they have another variable – threat – that provides a much better correlation I accept this as a reasonable assumption here. But I think if I am correct that it should be mentioned.

We agree that the non-linear behavior output we observed must be reconciled with the linearly scaled activity we observe in the vlPAG. We have now devoted a significant portion of the Discussion to this problem (tenth-twelfth paragraphs). Briefly, we think that a vlPAG-derived threat signal is but one of multiple signals that ultimately control fear output. We have evidence that a threat signal originating from the retrorubral field weights uncertainty much more like danger. Summing these two threat signals would better approximate the non-linear behavior output we see in our discrimination procedure.

We also agree that understanding this non-linearity is key to understanding the results. For this reason we now also describe the non-linearity of fear output in the Introduction to better set up the reader for the analyses we perform in the manuscript.

Reviewer #2:

[…] My concern is that a key finding of the study depends on a linear regression analysis containing a measurement variable with only three values: threat probabilities of 0, 0.375, and 1.0. I would have felt more confident if the experiment had more probability values.

This is a valid concern. To address this, we have performed a new regression analysis in which we incremented the probability assigned to uncertainty from 0.000 to 1.000 in 0.125. We report the β coefficient for threat probability for each increment in Figure 5E/J (subsection “Ramping neurons prioritize threat probability over fear output”). The result is a ‘tuning curve’ showing the uncertainty probability that best captures the relative firing pattern of each population. For Onset neurons, this analysis revealed the actual probability of 0.375 to be the ‘peak’ of the tuning curve. This provides further evidence that Onset neurons are tuned to shock probability. For Ramping neurons, the peak was shifted right, consistent with more mixed signaling of threat probability and fear output. We are grateful for this suggested analysis as we feel it confirms and extends the regression findings.

Minor Comments:

Writing is hard. It sometimes requires effort to understand. Here are examples:

“ramping activity prioritized threat probability over fear output.”

“vlPAG signals for fear output may reside in other functional populations, such has neurons showing inhibition of firing to danger cues [Tovote, Fadok and Luthi, 2015].”

“Yet, this would still require that adjacent populations for threat probability and fear output exist within the vlPAG.”

We agree that those sections, and others, were difficult to read. We have revised them and have carefully read through the remainder of the manuscript. In addition, we have solicited the help of our new postdoc, Jasmin Strickland – someone with extensive knowledge of behavior but not involved in writing this manuscript – to read all sections for clarity. We have revised sections she found difficult to understand. She is acknowledged for her contribution.

Abstract

The key dependent measure, conditioned suppression, should be mentioned in the Abstract section. Remember that this is a primary scientific report. Although theoretical constructs such as "fear" can be mentioned, the concrete measure of fear should be identified in Abstract.

We completely agree. We now describe conditioned suppression as our measure of fear in the Abstract. We also note the experiment was performed with male, Long Evans rats.

Results

Throughout the Results section, t-tests were conducted as post-hoc comparisons after ANOVAs. Are the familywise error rates controlled?

For the initial submission, familywise error rates were controlled for the majority of analyses. In revising the manuscript, we found that we had not performed these corrections for the paired t-tests in the interval suppression ratio figure (Figure 1D) as well as the single-sample t-tests in the interval regression figures (Figure 5D/I and Figure 6C/D). We have now corrected for multiple tests in all of these figures. The statistical patterns did not change.

Discussion

An observation with statistical data is described for the first time in the Discussion section. Such information should be reported in the result section. In addition, the authors discussion is not coherent. The authors first state that their electrophysiological observation was performed in the caudal vlPAG. Then, they state that the neural activity of the caudal vlPAG is not different from those of the rostral subregion. Explain how they can tell subregional difference when they focused on the caudal subregion.

This is a fair point. We have moved all stats reported in the Discussion to the Results. We apologize for the poor wording re: rostral and caudal. The vlPAG has an extensive anterior/posterior extent. In comparison to the full extent, all of our recordings came from the caudal portion. Our recordings centered on bregma levels -7.62 and -7.98. We meant to state that there was no difference in the signals obtained from these two recording levels. We also note that the vlPAG extends nearly to -8.70, and that because we did not record from this most extreme caudal extent, it is possible that neurons signaling fear output reside there. We have clarified this point in the Discussion.

Perhaps, it is important to discuss the present findings in light of the findings that selective lesions of vlPAG is sufficient to disrupt freezing. It does not seem to be difficult to explain how the loss of neurons coding threat probability can disrupt behavioral expression of fear.

We definitely agree and have added this to the manuscript (Discussion).

The authors state "We did not observe a substantial population of vlPAG neurons with these temporal firing characteristics (Figure 1—figure supplement 3), making the vlPAG a poor candidate for signaling most forms of sustained fear output". This statement is based on the assumption that much larger neural populations are needed to control behavior. However, research on neural ensemble suggests that the loss of only a handful neurons is sufficient to affect behavior. For example, Koya et al., 2009, found that only 2 – 3% of neurons are responsible for mediating context-specific sensitization induced by cocaine administration (Nat Neurosci, 12(8), 1069-1073. doi: http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v12/n8/suppinfo/nn.2364_S1.html).

We agree and have removed all language suggesting the small number of high firing rate cue-responsive neurons diminishes their possible role in behavior.

Minor Comments:

There are a couple of very minor points I would like to suggest for improvement. None of the concerns are substantive.

1) The most pressing issue I would like to see addressed is the fact that although electrophysiological recording of single-units is extremely informative, it does not actually provide us with causal data, only correlational. Indeed, the authors acknowledge and deal with this in the discussion, there addressing most of the notes I had made to myself whilst reading the manuscript, which was very satisfying. However, there's one more possibility I would like to see addressed, and that is that the firing patterns they are observing in vlPAG neurons represent information being received from the amygdala, rather than a signal that is being transferred to downstream targets. In other words, does the amygdala code for threat probability, send this information to vlPAG where it then translates it into the necessity for a particular fear response. Unfortunately this is impossible to test with ephys, but I would like to see it mentioned as a possibility, and possibly argued against if the authors see it as unlikely.

All of these observations are valid and are now addressed in the Discussion. We are only reporting a correlate, and we have not determined whether our observed neurons contribute to fear output. The question concerning the amygdala-vlPAG relationship is a valid one. We favor an interpretation in which central amygdala inputs train up the vlPAG, and with sufficient training vlPAG activity becomes independent. In support, Ozawa and colleagues have shown that inhibiting CeA inputs to the vlPAG has no impact on activity at cue onset and only partially reducing vlPAG ramping activity (Ozawa et al., 2017). Further, Lee and colleagues have shown that the CeA is required for the acquisition of conditioned suppression with minimal training trials, but that with more extensive fear conditioning, the CeA is no longer required (Lee et al., 2005). I observed a similar pattern for the CeA in one of my dissertation experiments (McDannald, 2010). This discussion is now included in the Discussion.

2) Further, it is worth noting that uncertainty in and of itself is a stressor, which could explain the fact that the fear output towards the uncertainty cue is higher than might be expected. It could be that the amygdala is encoding the threat probability and that information is being conveyed to the vlPAG, but that the additional stress from uncertainty is being encoded elsewhere in the brain and added to the fear output response at some unknown mediating output structure.

We agree and data from our laboratory may have identified such a signal that reflects threat probability plus additional stress from uncertainty: the retrorubral field. We discuss this possibility but keep it brief, as it is highly speculative (Discussion).

3) In the Results it would be really helpful to see how many rats/neurons etc. were included in the study as I had to search through to find this information.

We have gone through the manuscript to include more instances of specifically stating the number of rats recorded from and the numbers included in each population. We have also explicitly stated that the number of Onset and Ramping neurons from each subject is shown in full in the supplementary figures that include all waveforms.

4) In Figure 1—figure supplement 3, the authors suggest (at least as I understand it) that there is no relation between normalised firing rate and suppression ratio in the overall population of neurons, but from looking at the graph it appears that there might be at least a weak negative correlation? That is, when normalised firing rate was lowest, in the danger condition, suppression ratio appears to be at its highest (indicating high fear), and vice versa for the safety conditions. Is that correct or am I missing something?

We included Figure 1—figure supplement 3 so that neural activity, and two indicators of fear output (nose poke rate and suppression ratio), could be visualized in once place. From our view, the main takeaways are (1) the two measures of fear output strongly resemble one another. This means that by using a ratio, we weren’t fundamentally changing the data. (2) Behavior was very consistent across all recording sessions. That is, the behavior in sessions from which we recorded Onset neurons (neurons 1-29 on y axis) was virtually identical to the other recording sessions. This means that the correlate we observed was not due to sampling odd or outlier behavioral sessions. (3) It is also apparent from this figure that almost no neurons are uniquely safety-responsive.

5) In the last paragraph of the subsection “Ramping neurons signal threat probability through delay until shock receipt”, Figures 1E and 1F are referred to, I believe this should say Figures 6E and 6F.

The error has been corrected.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Wright KM, McDannald MA. 2019. Data from: Ventrolateral periaqueductal gray neurons prioritize threat probability over fear output. CRCNS. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.45013.019

    Data Availability Statement

    Full electrophysiology data set will be uploaded to http://crcns.org/ upon acceptance for publication.

    Single-unit data are publicly available on CRCNS (http://crcns.org/data-sets/brainstem/pag-1), under the doi: 10.6080/K0R49P0V. Users must first create a free account (https://crcns.org/register) before they can download the datasets from the site.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Wright KM, McDannald MA. 2019. Data from: Ventrolateral periaqueductal gray neurons prioritize threat probability over fear output. CRCNS.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES