Abstract
One of the primary goals of the Healthy Parks Healthy People (HPHP) program, located in the San Francisco Bay Area, is to offer group-based physical activities in natural settings. These activities arefor racially and ethnically diverse groups of individuals as an “upstream” strategy for improving health. This study investigated the health impact of selected two-hour HPHP Bay Area events that targeted low-income racial and ethnic minority groups using physiological and self-reported measures of stress and related variables.
Study participants (N=52) in the selected HPHP Bay Area events donated saliva and filled out psychological measures of perceived stress (PSS-4; Cohen et al.) and mood state (I-PANAS-SF; Thompson, 2007) at the beginning and the end ofa two-hour guided walk in green spaces. Moreover, a measure of perceived restoration (SRRS; Han, 2007) was completed at the end ofthe walk. Study participants wore a physical activity self-monitoring device (Garmin Vivofit 2) to capture their step count and heart rate during the event.
Stress, both measured by the analysis of salivary cortisol and self-reported perceived stress, significantly decreased over the course of the event (p<.0$) and there was a significant increase in positive mood (p<.05). The monitoring device also indicated that individuals were engaged in moderate levels of physical activity during the guided walks (<di>x</di> =8,990 steps, HR 95 bpm).
The results encourage further development of nature-based health interventions to mitigate stress. Such interventions may be especially appropriate for low-income, urban, racial and ethnic minority groups that likely experience increased levels of stress due to social inequities and poor living conditions.
Keywords: stress, mood, underserved population, Healthy Parks Healthy People
Introduction
Seven of the top ten leading causes of death in the United States are due to chronic illnesses; including, heart disease, cancer, and diabetes mellitus type two (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012). These chronic diseases disproportionately impact racial and ethnic minority populations. For example, African Americans have the highest rate of death due to heart disease, and non-Black Hispanics have the greatest number of “years of life lost” due to diabetes (Mensah et al. 2005; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2013).
Health equity research necessitates the engagement of transdisciplinary approaches to gathering comprehensive evidence about how environmental and social stress gets under our skin and affects people at a biological level (Warnecke et al., 2008). This evidence is necessary in developing meaningful strategies to address “upstream” factors, such as social inequities and poor living conditions, which can lead to chronic illness and other health disparities (Bay Area Regional Health Inequities Initiative, 2015). A potential strategy is to increase the exposure of socially disadvantaged groups to the natural environment (Ward Thompson et al., 2012).
Healthy Parks Healthy People (HPHP) is an international movement that enacts an agenda based on the beliefin a reciprocal relationship between people’s mental, physical, spiritual health, and social well-being and the sustainability of the environment. Although the importance of the HPHP activities and events is often presumed apart of the the field of parks and recreation, this is based on general evidence regarding the impact of, and the exposure to, recreation in natural environments (Maller, Townsend, Pryor, Brown, & St Leger, 2006). There is limited empirical data indicating the impacts of HPHP activities. This is the first study to use both physiological and self-report measures to investigate the health outcomes of participating in a HPHP event.
The purpose of this study was to examine the health impacts of selected Healthy Parks Healthy People Bay Area (HPHP) events that included primarily urban, low-income individuals that identified as belonging to a racial and/or ethnic minority group. The study explored the effect of a two-hour nature- based walk on stress levels while monitoring the participants’ physical activity levels during the HPHP event.
Residential environments have an important impact on people’s health (Roe et al., 2013). Individuals residing in impoverished neighborhoods, especially racial and ethnic minority individuals, have less access to safe environments that promote physical activity, which can then lead to poor health outcomes (Huston, et al., 2003; Lovasi, et al., 2009). Furthermore, individuals who live in neighborhoods with higher rates of poverty tend to suffer from overcrowding, pollution, discrimination, and experience more stressful life events (Baum et al., 1999; Matheson et al., 2006; Roosa, 2010). The aggregation of life stressors also coincides with elevated levels of psychological stress in these populations (Boardman et al., 2001).
Elevated levels of stress act through a multitude of biological pathways (Merkin et al, 2009). In response to a perceived threat, our body releases various hormones, including cortisol. Cortisol has a wide range of physiological effects (e.g., metabolism, gluconeogenesis and immune system suppression), but its most vital function is to regulate the body’s stress response system. The stress response system functions appropriately in instances of acute stress, but chronic stress (i.e., stress experienced over an extended period of time) can cause cortisol dysregulation leading to negative health consequences. The biological consequences of chronic stress are modulated by the interplay of multiple factors, this includes, an individual’s coping mechanisms, available support/stress buffers, and access to resources enabling coping with or minimizing stressors (Braveman et al., 2011; Chen & Miller, 2013; Hertzman & Boyce 2010; Salleh, 2008).
One potential resource for reducing the impact of stressors, that is often available to members ofimpoverished communities, is physical activity in parks and other green spaces (Abercrombie et al., 2008; Boone, Buckley, Grove, & Sister, 2009). Spending time in green spaces has long been thought to improve physical and psychological health (van den Berg, Hartig, & Staats, 2007). More recently, there is a growing body of evidence showing that exposure to natural environments improves physical and mental health (e.g., Mitchell & Popham, 2007, 2008; White, Alcock, Wheeler, & Depledge, 2013). More specific to this study, the amelioration of stress has been a guiding principle of park design since at least the era of American landscape architect Frederick Law Olmstead where there was construction of grand parks, such as Central Park (Dustin, Bricker, & Schwab, 2010). Additionally, there is enduring common knowledge that leisure is associated with a reduction in stress (Hull IV & Michael, 1995; Ulrich, Dimberg, & Driver, 1990).
The current study that leisure is associated with a reduction in stress is grounded in one of the two primary restoration theories, Ulrich’s (1981, 1983, 1984) Stress Reduction Theory (SRT). This theory contends that natural environments reduce psychological and physiological stress and restore individuals to equilibrium, due to humans’ evolutionary adaptations to natural environments. Ulrich’s theory assumes that the initial response to the environment is affective rather than cognitive (Ulrich, 1983). Support for the theory can be found in previous studies where associations between an increase in green space near individuals’ residences and significantly lower levels of stress (Gidlöf-Gunnarsson & Öhrström, 2007) results in decreased mortality (Takano, Nakamura, & Watanabe, 2002). Although the exact mechanism for how nature can reduce stress is still being debated, it has been documented that nature buffers the negative impact of stressors (e.g., Brown, Barton, & Gladwell, 2013) and decreases recovery time following exposure to a stressor (e.g., van den Berg, Jorgensen, & Wilson, 2014).
Findings about the impacts of the environment on cortisol are mixed, but largely support a positive association between natural environments and lower cortisol levels in humans. Numerous studies, including an extensive series of shinrin-yoku (forest bathing) field experiments (Lee et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2015; Mao, et al., 2012; Miyazaki, Lee, Park, Tsunetsugu, & Matsunaga, 2011; Park, Tsunetsugu, Kasetani, Kagawa, & Miyazaki, 2010), found that exposure to the natural environment was significantly associated with lower cortisol levels in comparison to exposure to an urban environment. Additional field experiments showed significantly lower levels of cortisol while gardening (Rodiek, 2002; van den Berg & Custers, 2010) and running (Harte & Eifert, 1995) in outdoor settings. The impact of the level of naturalness (from pristine to polluted) on restoration is still being explored; however, Lee et al. (2015) found that exposure to agricultural settings also led to significantly lower levels of cortisol.
Despite these repeated findings in the literature, that exposure to natural settings reduces cortisol, some studies have failed to identify significant differences in stress-reduction between natural and artificial settings. These studies speculate that their lack of significance was due to low levels of participant stress before environmental exposure (Beil & Hanes, 2013), a lack of environmental pristineness (Tyrväinen, et al., 2014), and the duration of exposure to the nature environment that was too brief (Beil & Hanes, 2013; Hartig, et al., 1996).
Although studies, such as the one cited above, have failed to find significant cortisol reduction as a result of exposure and recreation in a natural settings, the larger body of evidence currently supports the claim that nature exposure reduces stress, as measured by cortisol. Given this evidence, the current study provides exploratory empirical evidence from a population of primarily racial and ethnic minority individuals living in low-income communities. Furthermore, this study provides the first evaluation of HPHP events using physiological and self-report measures to investigate the health outcomes of participating in a HPHP event. Given SRT’s assumption of an initial affective response, through the restorative quality of natural environment (Ulrich, 1983), mood and the perceived quality of the event sites were also measured.
Methods
Participant Recruitment
This study recruited racial and ethnic minority individuals from low-income communities in the San Francisco Bay Area in the fall of 2016. This population was chosen to examine if their stress and mood changed after a guided walk.The sample was a convenience sample of participants in three Bay Area HPHP events. The researchers chose to collect data at HPHP events located in a rural regional park and within a large urban park. This choice was due to previous studies’ (e.g., Tyrvainen, et al., 2014) speculations that “natural pristineness” (e.g., influence of visual, sound and air pollution) may have an association with stress reduction. A total of 142 Bay Area residents and family members joined one of the three HPHP events. All three data collection days had similar weather conditions with moderate temperatures. Any participant over 18 that could respond to a written survey in English, Spanish or Chinese were recruited for participation in the study. A total of 99 adults were invited to participate and over half of them (N=52) agreed to participate in the study (response rate 53%).
Researchers partnered with two program providers, the East Bay Regional Park District and the San Francisco Recreation and Parks Department. These agencies had been working closely with underserved communities and had provided Healthy Parks Healthy People Bay Area (HPHP) programs once a month or more since 2014 (Institute at the Golden Gate, 2016). Their recruitment efforts were facilitated by a community outreach specialist that invited members of marginalized communities to HPHP programs, and formed ongoing, positive relationships with the communities. Additionally, one of the agencies provided a chartered bus to bring a group of primarily Latino residents from one of the most impoverished regions of the San Francisco Bay Area to participate at an event in a regional park. All the programs were free of charge and open to local residents; however, the agencies primarily targeted underserved communities. Program providers promoted their HPHP programs through websites, email solicitation with flyers, and word-of-mouth.
The research team intentionally included culturally diverse graduate and undergraduate students that represented the racial and ethnic diversity of the San Francisco Bay Area to minimize participant anxiety through cultural sensitivity during data collection (Napoles-Springer & Stewart, 2006; Yancy, 2006). All members of the research team spoke at least one other language than English fluently (e.g., Spanish, Chinese [Mandarin and Cantonese], Japanese). The research team received training in how to effectively build trust with potential study participants, articulate the importance of this study to target communities, and respond to any concerns or misconceptions pertaining to the nature of biospecimen (i.e., saliva) collection process. Throughout the study, members of the research team provided feedback to improve the recruitment procedure (Napoles-Springer et al., 2017) and biospecimen collection protocol (Ramirez et al., 2017) to accommodate the range of cultural norms in the targeted communities. Members of the research team and program facilitators modeled every step of the collection process to decrease participant anxiety and increase participation.
Measurements
A mix of biomarker assays and validated surveys were used, with the impacts of the HPHP events being measured using salivary cortisol, perceived stress, and the positive and negative affect. Other variables, such as time (prepost event), perceived restoration (perception of the natural environment experienced during the HPHP event), and physical activity (heart rates and step counts), were measured to examine if they significantly influenced outcome variables.
Physiological Stress (salivary cortisol levels).
Similar to other studies investigating the link between the environment and stress, salivary cortisol was used to measure stress levels. While some studies have used Alpha-amylase (Razani et al., 2016) or both Alpha-amylase and cortisol (Beil & Hanes, 2013), salivary cortisol is the primary biomarker for measuring physiological stress (El-Farhan, Rees, & Evans, 2017) and is more resilient to ambient temperature fluctuations prior to analysis. Additionally, it can be stored at room temperature for three weeks without significant reductions in measured cortisol (Djuric et al., 2008).
Saliva was collected before and after the guided nature walk. To obtain the cleanest samples possible, participants were asked to not eat 30 minutes prior to donating saliva. Participants were asked to pool saliva in their mouth for transfer into a 2 mL collection tube via a straw. After each 1 mL (1/5 teaspoon) of saliva was collected pre and post-event, the researchers stored the samples at −4°C until they were placed into a laboratory freezer immediately following each event.
Perceived Stress.
Stress was measured pre and post-event using the four-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-4; see Table 1; Cohen et al., 1983). PSS is one of the most commonly used psychological instrument for measuring the perception of stress (Cohen, 1994). The PSS has a strong body of published psychometric data supporting the validity and reliability of the scale (e.g., Lee, 2012; Roberti, Harrington, & Storch, 2006). Many investigators studying the impact of nature on health in the U.S. and other countries have also used this scale (e.g., Beil & Hanes, 2013; Fan, Das, & Chen, 2011; de Vries, van Dillen, Groenewegen, & Spreeuwenberg, 2013; Herzog & Strevey, 2008; Irvine, & Warber, 2013; Marselle, Roe et al., 2013; Ward Thompson et al., 2012). The short version PSS-4 was selected for this study since it is recommended as useful and feasible in situations where a short questionnaire is required (Cohen, et al, 1983). Although the original questionnaire asks feelings and thoughts during the previous month (e.g., In the last month, how often have you felt that things were going your way?), this study slightly modified the response question stem (e.g., How often do you feel . . .?) and response options using a 7-point Likert Scale from Never to Very often (see Table 1).
Table 1:
Measurements | Pre | During | Post |
---|---|---|---|
Stress hormone (cortisol levels) | X | X | |
Saliva Sample | |||
Perceived Stress (PSS-4; Cohen, et al., 1983) | X | X | |
Question: How often do you feel . . . | |||
1. confident about your ability to handle your personal problems? | |||
2. that you are unable to control the important things in your life? | |||
3. that things are going your way? | |||
4. difficulties are piling up so high that you cannot overcome them? | |||
Response Options: Never 1..2..3..4..5..6..7 Very Often | |||
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (I-PANAS-SF; Thompson, 2007) | X | X | |
How do you feel now? | |||
1. Upset | |||
2. Hostile | |||
3. Alert | |||
4. Ashamed | |||
5. Inspired | |||
6. Nervous | |||
7. Determined | |||
8. Attentive | |||
9. Afraid | |||
10. Active | |||
Response Options: Not at all 1..2..3..4..5..6..7.. | |||
Very much so | |||
Restoration (SRRS; Han, 2003) | X | ||
Question: How would you describe the effect of the landscape had on you during the program? | |||
1. Grouchy vs. good natured | |||
2. Anxious us. relaxed | |||
3. My breathing becamefaster | |||
4. My hands began sweating | |||
5. I was interested in the scenery | |||
6. I felt attentive to the scene | |||
7. I would like to visit this place more often | |||
8. I would have liked to stay there longer | |||
Response Options: Not at all 1..2..3..4..5..6..7 | |||
Very much so | |||
Physical Activity (Garmit Vivofit 2.0) | X | ||
1. Step Count | |||
2. Heart Rate |
Perceived Mood.
Perceived mood was measured using the International Positive and Negative Affect Short-Form (I-PANAS-SF; Thompson, 2007). I-PANAS-SF is a 10-item scale. This validated measure asks participants, “how do you feel now?” using a 7-point Likert scale anchored at the ends with Not at all and Very much so. Negative affect (NA) consisted of five items including Upset, Hostile, Ashamed, Nervous, and Afraid, and positive affect (PA) included Alert, Inspired, Determined, Attentive, and Active. This instrument has also been used in a number of health and nature studies (e.g., Brown, Barton, & Gladwell, 2013; Herzog & Strevey, 2008; Tyrväinen et al., 2014). Previous research found positive and negative affects to be consistent across cultures (Russel, 1983; Russel, Lewicka, & Niit, 1989).
Perceived Restoration.
Perceived restoration was measured in the post period using Han’s (2003) 8-item Short-version Revised Restoration Scale (SRRS) that is grounded in Ulrich’s (1983) Stress Reduction Theory (SRT). SRRS was used to assess whether participants perceived the restorative qualities of the natural environments at the two sites as different. Furthermore, this instrument was used to evaluate whether observed changes in stress and mood levels varied with perceived restoration. The instrument has previously been used in both laboratory (e.g., Han, 2007; Vassiljev et al., 2010) and field experiments (e.g., Wilson, et al., 2016).
Physical Activity.
The level of physical activity during the HPHP events was measured by two indicators, heart rate and step count, using a wearable activity monitorGarmin Vivofit 2.0. After reviewing over a dozen commercially available physical activity self-monitoring devices, Garmin Vivofit was selected due to its accuracy of heart rate and step count measurement as well as the relative ease of use for research participants. Garmin Vivofit is reported as being just as good or even better than other devices measuring step count (Alsubheen, George, Baker, Rohr, & Basset, 2016; Huang, Xu, Yu, & Shull, 2016) and appears to have the lowest systematic error (i.e., −0.2% at 3.5 km /h walk test; Leth, Nielsen Nielsen, & Dinesen, 2017). Participants were asked to wear a wrist-worn Garmin Vivofit and associated chest-strap heart rate sensor. The chest-strap was placed directly on the participants’ skin. Participants had access to a tall nylon tent (6’3” x 4’ x 4’) to lift their clothing up and fit the heart band. Not all participants took advantage of the tents. In some cases, such as women wearing traditional Muslim dresses, participants may have chosen to not wear a heart band if a discreet location to fit the heart bands had not been provided.
Data Analysis
Salivary cortisol was used to measure physiological levels of stress using Enzyme-Linked Immunosorbent Assay (ELISA) kits from ALPCO. Samples were prepared for analysis in accordance with the Expanded Range High Sensitivity Salivary Cortisol Enzyme Immunoassay protocol (Salimetrics, 2016). Saliva samples were frozen and stored at −20°C. After two days of storage, the samples were thawed at room temperature for analysis. Once the samples reached room temperature (23°C), they were spun at 4,000 rpm for 20-minutes. Supernatant was extracted and then analyzed using a competitive ELISA technique.
Three subsamples of each saliva sample were analyzed. Saliva was loaded on an antibody-coated 96 well plate. The plate was then read using a Synergy HTX multi-mode reader from BioTek. Five-parameter linear regression analysis (Gen 5 version 2.04) was used to determine the concentration (nanogram per milliliter, ng/ml) of cortisol. The detection kit is limited to measuring a minimum of 1 ng/ ml and maximum of ioong/ml. Samples with high levels ofvariation between the three subsamples suggest that the saliva was polluted with food or some other substance that decreased the reliability of the cortisol concentration estimation process. Therefore, only samples with a coefficient of variation (CV) of less than 20% between the three subsamples were included in the data set.
The responses to the demographic questionnaire and psychological instruments (4-PSS; Cohen et al., 1983, I-PANAS-SF; Thompson, 2007, SRRS; Han, 2007), were manually entered into a Microsoft Excel worksheet, then analyzed using SPSS version 24. Descriptive statistics were calculated and paired samples t-tests compared changes in the levels of cortisol, perceived stress, and positive and negative mood before and after the nature walk. Bivariate correlations and regression analyses were used to explore relationships.
The step count and heart rate from the wearable Garmin Vivofit activity tracker were subsequently uploaded to the online Garmin Connect website. The peak and average heart rate values and step count data were then extracted and analyzed in SPSS. Additionally, the duration and the type of activities observed were manually recorded. The average duration of the walk was calculated during each of the nature walks.
Results
The study participants (N=52) represented a similar age distribution with the San Francisco Bay Area, yet they were more likely to be female, Hispanic/Latino, and lower-income compared to the overall population of the Bay Area (U.S. Census Bureau, 2018; U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development, 2018; see Table 2). While half (50%) of Bay Area residents were female, nearly two-thirds (63%) of the participants were female. Whereas 22% of the Bay Area population identified as Hispanic or Latino, almost half (43%) of the research participants identified as Hispanic or Latino. While the educational attainment rates of participants were similar, the research participants showed 11% lower level ofemployment than Bay Area residents in general.
Table 2:
Study Participants | 2017 Bay Area Data★ | |
---|---|---|
Sex | ||
Female | 63% | 50% |
Male | 37% | 50% |
Age (vears) | ||
18–64 | 91% | 79% |
65 and older | 9% | 21% |
Race/Ethnicity | ||
Asian | 31% | 26% |
Black or African American | 9% | 6% |
Hispanic or Latino (any race) | 43% | 22% |
White (not Hispanic or Latino) | 13% | 44% |
Employment Status | ||
Employed (Full and Part Time) | 66% | 77% |
Educational Attainment | ||
High school graduate or higher | 87% | 89% |
Bachelor’s degree or higher | 47% | 49% |
Pre-tax household income (USD) | ||
Median | $25,ooo-$49,ooo | $109,009 |
Note.-The information was calculated using the data provided by the U.S. Census Bureau and the U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development
The San Francisco Bay Area is an expensive place to live. The cost of living in San Francisco is 62.6% higher than the U.S. average and housing is nearly three times more expensive than in other U.S. cities (America: Cost of Living Index by City 2017 Mid-Year, 2018; Sciacca, 2017; Wallace, 2018). The median Bay Area household income in 2017 was $109,000 (U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development, 2018), whereas 88% of the participants of the study had a household income below $100,000. Assuming a conservative average family size of three (estimated at 3.28 in 2000; Association of Bay Area Governments, 2017), then the low income limit for the six Bay Area counties would be an average of $84,575 (U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development, 2018). Using this measure of poverty, approximately 80% of the study participants had a low income or less, and at least 63% had a very low income.
There were no significant differences for perceived restoration between the participants at the two program sites. Similarly, no significant differences were apparent between the two program sites in terms of pre-post changes in salivary cortisol, change in perceived stress, or changes in positive or negative affect. The number of average steps was significantly lower (p < .05) at the event provided by one program provider ( = 5,490) compared to the average at the other two events provided by another program provider ( = 11,232). However, there were no differences in average or maximum heart rate.
Demographics
Physical Activity
As a whole, participants averaged 8,990 steps during the two-hour nature walk. Therefore, participants nearly averaged the 10,000 steps per day that is recommended for an adult to be considered active (Tudor-Locke, & Bassett Jr, 2004). The minimum number of steps recorded for any participant was 1,752 steps per event and the maximum was 14,567 steps per event. This large variability is likely attributable to some participants choosing to walk only part of the route, differences in the number of steps needed to cover the same distance, and differences in the length of the walk in different events.
Due to the requirement for the heart tracker to be placed directly on the skin of participants’ chest as well as the limited time to synchronize a chest monitor with a wrist device, only 17 sets of valid heart rate data were collected and analyzed. The average heart rate for these participants was 95 beats per minute. The mean of the maximum heart rate was 132 beats per minute.
Positive and Negative Affect
The level of positive affect significantly increased from before- to after- the nature walk (Mbefore = 19.52 vs. Mafter = 22.29, P < .05; Table 3). Cohen’s effect size value (d = 0.35) suggested a low to moderate practical significance (Richardson, 2011). In contrast, although the mean for negative affect did decrease, this change was not significant (Mbefore = 7.39 vs. Mafter = 6.59, p > .05). Participants who were employed full-time had significantly higher levels of positive affect before the event (Mfull-time = 23.20 vs. Mnon full-time = 15.96, p < .05); however, there were no significant differences after the event or for the change in positive affect, suggesting participation led to a equalizing of positive affect that benefitted individuals that were not employed full-time.
Table 3:
Before Nature Walk |
After Nature Walk |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | t | Cohen’s d | df | |
Positive Affect | 19.52 | 8.23 | 22.29 | 8.93 | 2.24⋆ | 0.35 | 41 |
Negative Affect | 7.39 | 3.8 | 6.59 | 4.95 | ns | 0.14 | 40 |
Perceived Stress (PSS-1o) | 14–24 | 4.25 | 12.02 | 4.78 | 3.63⋆⋆ | 0.54 | 41 |
Cortisol (ug/dL) | 14.2 | 7.75 | 11..05 | 7.2 | 2.52⋆ | 0.39 | 40 |
Notes: p<.05,
p<.01
Salivary Cortisol
Out of the 52 saliva samples provided by research participants, 41 were included in this analysis. Saliva samples were excluded due to high rates of variation between the three subsamples that were assayed or insufficient saliva was provided.
There was a significant decrease in salivary cortisol (Mbefore = 14.20 ug/dL vs. Mafter = 11.05 ug/dL, p < .05). Additionally, the value of Cohen’s effect size (d = 0.54) suggested a moderate practical significance (Richardson, 2011). Although bivariate correlations suggested that age was significantly correlated with lower levels of salivary concentration before and after the nature walk (p < .05), a regression analysis with change in salivary cortisol as the dependent variable and demographic variables (income, education, employment, age, and gender) as independent variables was not significant (p < .05).
Although the number of heart rate observations is relatively small, significant bivariate correlations between maximum heart rate with pre-event salivary cortisol concentrations (r = .58, p < .05) and change in salivary cortisol concentrations from pre- to post-event (r = −.69, p < .05) suggest areas for future research. There were no significant gender differences in the average cortisol levels or changes in cortisol before and after the guided walk in the current study (p > .05).
Three multiple regression analyses were conducted with intake time and wake-up time as independent variables and pre-event cortisol, post-event cortisol, and change in cortisol as the dependent variables. The analysis explained a significant amount of variance in pre-event cortisol (F(2,35) = 4.523, p < .05, R2 = .205); however, only wake-up time and not intake added statistical significance to the prediction (p < .05). This finding that a later wake-up time was significantly correlated with pre-event cortisol levels was expected based on normal diurnal decreases in salivary cortisol (DeSantis, Adam, Hawkley, Kudielka, & Cacioppo, 2015). The other analyses did not explain a significant portion of post-event cortisol nor change in cortisol concentrations (p > .05).
Perceived Stress
Perceived stress significantly decreased from before to after nature walk (M = 14.24, M = 12.02 retrospectively; p < .05). Cohen’s effect size value (d = .39) suggested low to moderate practical significance (Richardson, 2011). Participants’ perceived stress before the nature walk did not significantly differ by employment status; however, after the nature walk, participants with fulltime employment showed significantly lower levels of perceived stress than the participants without full-time employment (MFT = 9.52; Mnone FT = 14.21; p < .05). Similarly, the changes in pre- and post-perceived stress levels were significantly different by employment status (MFT = −4.43; Mnone FT = −.29; p < .05). Participants with at least a bachelor’s degree had significantly lower levels of perceived stress before the event (M4-year degree or higher =12.81;M less than a 4-year degree, = 15.46; p < .05), but the post-event difference nor the change significantly differed by educational attainment.
Discussion
The results of this study indicate that participants’ stress decreased and their positive mood increased after participating in the Healthy Parks Healthy People programs that included group walks in natural environments. Concentrations of salivary cortisol and perceived stress decreased significantly (p < .05) from the start to the end of the event. Salivary cortisol concentrations decreased 22%, and self-reported perceived stress average decreased 16%. The decrease in salivary cortisol is similar to or exceeds decreases in salivary cortisol previously reported in shinrin-yoku studies (e.g., Park, Tsunetsugu, Kasetani, Kagawa, & Miyazaki, 2010). Furthermore, self-reports of positive affect increased 14%. This aligns with the Stress Reduction Theory’s assumption that individuals’ initial reaction to natural environments is affective (Ulrich, 1983). In summary, our findings from a biomarker of stress test, self-reported stress measures, and positive affect indicators support the thesis that participants were less stressed and felt better after engaging in physical activity in a natural environment.
Implications
This work documents that racial and ethnic minority individuals from low- income communities can benefit from guided walks in nature. Previously, most studies that examine the impact of nature on stress that included cortisol measurements have not reported the race, ethnicity, or income of study participants (Harte & Eifert, 1995; Hartig, Böök, Garvill, Olsson, & Gärling, 1996; Rodiek, 2002; Roe et al., 2013; Tyrväinen et al., 2014; van den Berg & Custers, 2010; Ward Thompson et al., 2012). In studies that have included racial demographics, all participants were reported as members ofpredominant racial and/or ethnic groups. For example, a U.S. study included all White participants (Beil & Hanes, 2013) and shinrin-yoku studies exclusively used young healthy Japanese (Lee et al., 2015; Lee et al., 2009; Miyazaki et al., 2011; Park et al., 2010) or Chinese (Mao et al., 2012) male university students as research participants. Teas and colleagues’ (2007) study of post-menopausal women is one of the few studies that used salivary cortisol to measure the impact of environment on stress that included study participants that were not part of the predominant racial or ethnic group (6 of the 19 subjects were Black women). Given this lack ofinclusion of racial or ethnic minority groups in such research, coupled with research about how urban populations of color may have fears or other constraints that may prevent them from positively experiencing nature (Bixler & Floyd, 1997; Virden & Walker, 1999), it could be falsely concluded that urban populations may not benefit from such nature-based interventions.
In contrast, this study’s findings suggest that racial and ethnic minority urban populations from low-income communities can benefit from public health interventions that include physical activity in nature. This is an important finding for marginalized populations in urban areas who may be the most in need of such nature-based interventions because they are more likely to experience social inequities and poor living condition that are “upstream” determinants of health disparities (Bay Area Regional Health Inequities Initiative, 2015). Because of these life stressors, low-income racial and ethnic minority individuals may have the most to benefit from the stress buffering or restorative properties of experiences in nature (Hynes & Lopez, 2007; Jennings, Johnson Gaither, & Gragg, 2012; Kabisch, Qureshi, & Haase, 2015).
Related literature on health impacts of nature has found that lower socioeconomic status groups benefit more from increases in nearby residential green space compared to other groups (e.g., Maas, Verheij, Groenewegen, De Vries, & Spreeuwenberg, 2006; Mitchell & Popham, 2008). For example, a couple of studies in Scotland specifically investigated the association between low-income communities and nearby residential green space by measuring cortisol levels as a biomarker of stress (Roe et al., 2013; Ward Thompson et al., 2012). Both Ward Thompson and colleagues (2012) and Roe and colleagues (2013) found significantly lower levels of cortisol for low-income people residing near green spaces compared to matched comparison groups residing farther from these spaces. The current study reinforces these previous findings and shows that exposure to natural environments is associated with decreased levels of cortisol for racial and ethnic minority members of low-income urban communities.
Although both the measured physiological indicator of stress (cortisol) and self-reported level of stress decreased, the two measures of stress were not significantly correlated to one another. This is not the first study to find low or no levels of correlation between physiological and self-report measures of stress (e.g., Tyrvainen et al., 2014; Ward Thompson et al., 2012).
Limitations
This study was a pilot study with multiple issues limiting its generalizability. The sample was a convenience sample ofpeople that self-selected to participate in the event and in the study. A control group that did not participate in the nature-based intervention was not included and measures collected from the participants before and after the nature walk were not administered on days when they did not participate in this activity. Additionally, the analysis has limited information about the daily diurnal fluctuation of cortisol levels. Although the sample of participants was less White and poorer than residents from the local area, it did not exclusively contain racial and ethnic minority individuals living in low-income households.
Future Research
This study focused on a group-based intervention in nature. Future research is needed to understand if such group-based events are more effective than individual activities for the population investigated in this study. In contrast to the group experience in the current study, findings from previous studies with different participant characteristics showed greater benefits for people recreating in natural environments individually rather than with a group. In a study that used the related construct of revitalization, investigators found that participants were more revitalized after a walk in a park when they walked alone, and relatively more revitalized after a walk along a street when they walked with a friend (Johansson, Hartig, & Staats, 2011). Moreover, other Scandinavian research found that individuals with high levels of stress preferred to avoid activities that involved socializing (Stigsdotter & Grahn, 2011). However, different cultures have varying orientations to solitude or socializing in natural settings (Loukaitou-Sideris, 1995). In their Taiwanese study, Weng and Chiang (2014) found the largest reductions in anxiety and increases in perceived restoration occurred after walking outdoors and chatting with friends. Moreover, urban residents may appreciate the increased perceived security of visiting a natural area in a group (Bixler & Floyd, 1997). However, more research is needed to understand if group programs in nature, such as the Healthy Parks Health People activities studied in this work, are more or less effective compared to individual interventions for urban populations.
Conclusion
Healthy Parks Healthy People (HPHP) is an international movement that contends that parks and other green spaces can benefit people’s physical and mental health (Maller et. al, 2006). Although a previous study looked at how HPHP activities increased the perceived value of parks and frequency of visits (de Kievit, 2001), this is the first study to use both physiological and self-report measures to examine the health outcomes of participating in a HPHP related event. The findings of this study suggest that these types of programs, which include group walks in nature, can benefit participant health, including low- income racial and ethnic minority participants.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the partnering agencies, community members, and SF State University students and staff who participated and supported this study. In particular we thank Mona Koh at East Bay Regional Park District, Jim Wheeler at San Francisco Recreation and Parks, and Martha Arriaga, Andy Chen, Maria Cruz, Sandy Jang, Rebecca Mendez, Cathy Samayoa, and Anthony Zhu in the Health Equity Research Lab at SF State. This study was supported by the Common Fund of the National Institutes of Health via award number UL1GM118985.
Contributor Information
Aiko Yoshino, Department of Recreation, Parks & Tourism at San Francisco State University San Francisco State University, San Francisco CA.
Jackson Wilson, Department of Recreation, Parks & Tourism at San Francisco State University San Francisco State University, San Francisco CA.
Edgar J. Velazquez, Department of Biology at San Francisco State University San Francisco State University, San Francisco CA.
Eric Johnson, Graduate Student at San Francisco State University San Francisco State University, San Francisco CA.
Leticia Márquez-Magaña, Department of Biology at San Francisco State University San Francisco State University, San Francisco CA.
References
- Abercrombie LC, Sallis JF, Conway TL, Frank LD, Saelens BE, & Chapman JE (2008). Income and racial disparities in access to public parks and private recreation facilities. American Journal ofPreventive Medicine, 34(1), 9–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alsubheen SA, George AM, Baker A, Rohr LE, & Basset FA (2016). Accuracy of the vivofit activity tracker. Journal of Medical Engineering & Technology, 40(6), 298–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- America: Cost of Living Index by City 2017 Mid-Year. (2018). Retrieved June 12, 2018, from https://www.numbeo.com/cost-of-living/region_rankings.jsp?title=2017-mid®ion=019
- Andersen RM, Davidson PL, & Baumeister SE (2011). Improving access to care In Andersen RM, Rice TH & Kominski GF (Eds.), Changing the US health care system: Key issues in health services policy and management (3rd ed., pp. 33–69). San Francisco: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Association of Bay Area Governments. (2017). Bay Area census: San Francisco Bay Area population, 2017, from http://www.bayareacensus.ca.gov/bayarea.htm.
- Baum A, Garofalo JP, & Yali AM (1999). Socioeconomic status and chronic stress: Does stress account for SES effects on health? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,896, 131–144. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08111.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bay Area Census. (2017). May 22, 2017, from http://www.bayareacensus.ca.gov/bayarea.htm
- Bay Area Regional Health Inequities Initiative. (2015). May 22, 2017, from http://barhii.org/framework/
- Beil K, & Hanes D (2013). The influence of urban natural and built environments on physiological and psychological measures of stress—A pilot study. International Journal ofEnvironmental Research and Public Health, 10(4), 1250–1267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bixler RD, & Floyd MF (1997). Nature is scary, disgusting, and uncomfortable. Environment and Behavior, 29(4), 443–467. [Google Scholar]
- Boardman JD, Finch BK, Ellison CG, Williams DR, & Jackson JS (2001). Neighborhood disadvantage, stress, and drug use among adults. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42(2), 151–165. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boone CG, Buckley GL, Grove JM, & Sister C (2009). Parks and people: An environmental justice inquiry in Baltimore, Maryland. Annals of the Association of American Geographers, 99(4), 767–787. doi: 10.1080/00045600903102949 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Braveman P, Egerter S, & Williams DR (2011) The social determinants of health: Coming of age. >Annual Review of Public Health, 32, 381–398. 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031210-101218 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown DK, Barton JL, & Gladwell VF (2013). Viewing nature scenes positively affects recovery of autonomic function following acute-mental stress. Environmental Science & Technology, 47(11), 5562–5569. 10.1021/es305019p [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2012) Chronic Disease Overview. Retrieved from: https://www.cdc.gov/chronicdisease/overview/
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2013) Health disparities experienced by racial/ethnic minority populations Morbidity and Mortality Weeky Report. Department of Health and Human Services. [Google Scholar]
- Chen E & Miller GE (2013) Socioeconomic status and health: Mediating and moderating factors. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 723–749. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185634 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S, Kamarck T, & Mermelstein R (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal ofHealth & Social Behavior, 24, 386–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S (1994). Perceived Stress Scale. Mind Garden. http://www.mindgarden.com/documents/PerceivedStressScale.pdf [Google Scholar]
- de Kievit J (2001). Healthy parks healthy people - A natural link. Australian Parks and Leisure, 4(3), 19–20. [Google Scholar]
- DeSantis AS, Adam EK, Hawkley LC, Kudielka BM, & Cacioppo JT (2015). Racial and ethnic differences in diurnal cortisol rhythms: are they consistent over time?. Psychosomatic medicine, 77(1), 6–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Vries S, van Dillen SM, Groenewegen PP, & Spreeuwenberg P (2013). Streetscape greenery and health: Stress, social cohesion and physical activity as mediators. Social Science & Medicine, 94, 26–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Djuric Z, Bird CE, Furumoto-Dawson A, Rauscher GH, Ruffin MT, Stowe RP, Tucker KL, & Masi CM (2008). Biomarkers of psychological stress in health disparities research. Open BiomarkersJournal, 1(1), 7–19. 10.2174/1875318300801010007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dustin DL, Bricker KS, & Schwab KA (2010). People and nature: Toward an ecological model of health promotion. Leisure Sciences, 32(1), 3–14. [Google Scholar]
- El-Farhan N, Rees DA, & Evans C (2017). Measuring cortisol in serum, urine and saliva-are our assays good enough?. Annals ofclinical biochemistry, 54(3), 308–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fan Y, Das KV, & Chen Q (2011). Neighborhood green, social support, physical activity, and stress: Assessing the cumulative impact. Health & Place, 17(6), 1202–1211. 10.1016/j.healthplace.2011.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gidlöf-Gunnarsson A, & Öhrström E (2007). Noise and well-being in urban residential environments: The potential role of perceived availability to nearby green areas. Landscape and Urban Planning, 83(2–3), 115–126. 10.1016Zj.landurbplan.2007.03.003 [Google Scholar]
- Han K-T (2003). A reliable and valid self-rating measure of the restorative quality of natural environments. Landscape and Urban Planning, 64(4), 209–232. 10.10146/S0169-2046(02)00241-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Han K-T (2007). Responses to six major terrestial biomes in terms of scenic beauty, preference, and restorativeness. Environment and Behavior, 39(4), 529–556. [Google Scholar]
- Harte JL, & Eifert GH (1995). The effects of running, environment, and attentional focus on athletes’ catecholamine and cortisol levels and mood. Psychophysiology, 32(1) 49–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartig T, Böök A, Garvill J, Olsson T, & Gärling T (1996). Environmental influences on psychological restoration. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 37(4), 378–393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hertzman C & Boyce T (2010). How experience gets under the skin to create gradients in developmental health. Annual Review ofPublic Health, 31, 329–347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog TR, & Strevey SJ (2008). Contact with nature, sense of humor, and psychological well-being. Environment and Behavior, 40(6), 747–776. [Google Scholar]
- Huang Y, Xu J, Yu B, & Shull PB (2016). Validity of FitBit, Jawbone UP, Nike+ and other wearable devices for level and stair walking. Gait & Posture, 48, 36–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hull IV RB, & Michael SE (1995). Nature-based recreation, mood change, and stress restoration. Leisure Sciences, 17(1), 1–14. [Google Scholar]
- Huston SL, Evenson KR, Bors P, & Gizlice Z (2003). Neighborhood environment, access to places for activity, and leisure-time physical activity in a diverse North Carolina population. American Journal ofHealth Promotion, 18(1), 58–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hynes H, & Lopez R (2007). Cumulative risk and a call for action in environmental justice communities. Journal ofHealth Disparities Research and Practice, 1(2), 29–57. [Google Scholar]
- California Department of Education. (2018). Income Eligibility Scales for 2017–18— Rates, Eligibility Scales, & Funding. Retrieved May 8, 2018, from https://www.cde.ca.gov/ls/nu/rs/scales1718.asp
- Institute at the Golden Gate. (2016). May 22, 2017, from http://www.hphpbayarea.org/
- Jennings V, Johnson Gaither C, & Gragg RS (2012). Promoting environmental justice through urban green space access: A synopsis. Environmental Justice, 5(1), 1–7. [Google Scholar]
- Johansson M, Hartig T, & Staats H (2011). Psychological benefits of walking: Moderation by company and outdoor environment. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 3(3), 261–280. [Google Scholar]
- Kabisch N, Qureshi S, & Haase D (2015). Human-environment interactions in urban green spaces—A systematic review of contemporary issues and prospects for future research. Environmental Impact Assessment Review, 50, 25–34. [Google Scholar]
- Larson NI, Story MT, & Nelson MC (2009). Neighborhood environments. Disparities in access to healthy foods in the U.S. AmericanJournal ofPreventive Medicine, 36(1), 74–81. 10.i0i6/j.amepre.2008.09.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee EH (2012). Review of the psychometric evidence of the Perceived Stress Scale. Asian Nursing Research, 6(4), 121–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J, Park BJ, Ohira T, Kagawa T, & Miyazaki Y (2015). Acute effects of exposure to a traditional rural environment on urban dwellers: A crossover field study in terraced farmland. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 12(2), 1874–1893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J, Park BJ, Tsunetsugu Y, Kagawa T, & Miyazaki Y (2009). Restorative effects of viewing real forest landscapes, based on a comparison with urban landscapes. Scandinavian Journal ofForest Research, 24(3), 227–234. [Google Scholar]
- Leth S, Hansen J, Nielsen OW, & Dinesen B (2017). Evaluation of commercial self-monitoring devices for clinical purposes: results from the future patient trial, phase I. Sensors, 17(1), 211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loukaitou-Sideris A (1995). Urban form and social context: Cultural differention in the uses of urban parks. Journal of Planning Education and Research, 14(2), 89–102. 10.1177/0739456X9501400202 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lovasi GS, Hutson MA, Guerra M, & Neckerman KM (2009). Built environments and obesity in disadvantaged populations. Epidemiologic reviews, 31(1), 7–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maas J, Verheij RA, Groenewegen PP, De Vries S, & Spreeuwenberg P (2006). Green space, urbanity, and health: How strong is the relation? Journal ofEpidemiology and Community Health, 60(7), 587–592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maller C, Townsend M, Pryor A, Brown P, & St. Leger L. (2006). Healthy nature healthy people: ‘Contact with nature’ as an upstream health promotion intervention for populations. Health Promotion International, 21(1), 45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mao GX, Lan XG, Cao YB, Chen ZM, He ZH, Lv YD, Wang YZ, Hu XL, Wang GF, & Jing Y (2012). Effects of short-term forest bathing on human health in a broad-leaved evergreen forest in Zhejiang Province, China. Biomedical and Environmental Sciences, 25(3), 317–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marselle MR, Irvine KN, & Warber SL (2013). Walking for well-being: Are group walks in certain types of natural environments better for well-being than group walks in urban environments? International Journal ofEnvironmental Research and Public Health, 10(11), 5603–5628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matheson FI, Moineddin R, Dunn JR, Creatore MI, Gozdyra P, & Glazier RH (2006). Urban neighborhoods, chronic stress, gender and depression. Social Science & Medicine,63(10), 2604–2616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mensah GA, Mokdad AH, Ford ES, Greenlund KJ, & Croft JB (2005). State of disparities in cardiovascular health in the United States. Circulation, 111(10), 1233–1241. 10.1161/01.CIR.0000158136.76824.04 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merkin SS, Basurto-Dávila R, Karlamangla A, Bird C, Lurie N, Escarce J, & Seeman T (2009). Neighborhoods and cumulative biological risk profiles by race/ ethnicity in a national sample of U.S. adults: NHANES III. Annals of Epidemiology, 19(3), 194–201. 10.i0i6/j.annepidem.2008.i2.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell R, & Popham F (2007). Greenspace, urbanity and health: Relationships in England. Journal ofEpidemiology and Community Health, 61(8), 681–683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell R, & Popham F (2008). Effect of exposure to natural environment on health inequalities: An observational population study. The Lancet, 372(9650), 1655–1660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyazaki Y, Lee J, Park B, Tsunetsugu Y, & Matsunaga K (2011). Preventive medical effects of nature therapy. Nihon Eiseigaku Zasshi. Japanese Journal of Hygiene, 66(4), 651–656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nápoles-Springer AM, Cook E, Ginossar T, Knight KD, Ford ME (2017). Chapter four- Applying a conceptual framework to maximize the participation of diverse populations in cancer clinical trials. Advances in Cancer Research, 133, 77–94. 10.i0i6/bs.acr.2016.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nápoles-Springer AM, & Stewart AL (2006). Overview of qualitative methods in research with diverse populations: Making research reflect the population. Medical Care, 44(ii), S5–S9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park B-J, Tsunetsugu Y, Kasetani T, Kagawa T, & Miyazaki Y (2010). The physiological effects of shinrin-yoku (taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing): Evidence from field experiments in 24 forests across Japan. Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine, 15(1), 18–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramirez J, Elmofty M, Castillo E, DeRouen M, Shariff-Marco S, Allen L, Lin- Gomez S, Napoles AM, & Marquez-Magana L (2017). Evaluation of cortisol and telomere length measurements in ethnically diverse women with breast cancer using culturally sensitive methods. Journal of Community Genetics, 8(29), 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Razani N, Kohn MA, Wells NM, Thompson D, Flores HH, & Rutherford GW (2016). Design and evaluation of a park prescription program for stress reduction and health promotion in low-income families: The Stay Healthy in Nature Everyday (SHINE) study protocol. Contemporary Clinical Trials, 51, 8–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richardson JTE (2011). Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educational Research Review, 5(2), 135–147. doi: https://doi.org/i0.i0i6/j.edurev.20i0.i2.00i [Google Scholar]
- Roberti JW, Harrington LN, & Storch EA (2006). Further psychometric support for the 10-item version of the Perceived Stress Scale. Journal ofCollege Counseling, 9(2), 135–147. [Google Scholar]
- Rodiek S (2002). Influence of an outdoor garden on mood and stress in older persons. Journal ofTherapeutic Horticulture, 13, 13–21. [Google Scholar]
- Roe JJ, Ward Thompson C, Aspinall PA, Brewer MJ, Duff EI, Miller D, Mitchell R, & Clow A (2013). Green space and stress: Evidence from cortisol measures in deprived urban communities. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 10(9), 4086–4103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roosa MW, Burrell GL, Nair RL, Coxe S, Tein J-Y, & Knight GP (2010). Neighborhood disadvantage, stressful life events, and adjustment among Mexican American early adolescents. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 30(4), 567–592. 10.1177/0272431609338177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell JA (1983). Pancultural aspects of the human conceptual organization of emotions. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 45, 1281–1288. [Google Scholar]
- Russell JA, Lewicka M, & Niit T (1989). A cross-cultural study of a circumplex model of affect. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 57, 848–856. [Google Scholar]
- Salimetrics. (2016) Expanded range high sensitivity salivary cortisol enzyme immunoassay kit. State College, PA.
- Salleh R (2008). Life event, stress and illness. Malaysian Journal of Medical Sciences, 15(4), 9–18. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sciacca A (2017, April 22). In costly Bay Area, even six-figure salaries are considered ‘low income.’ Retrieved May 12, 2018, from https://www.mercurynews.com/2017/04/22/in-costly-bay-area-even-six-figure-salaries-are-considered-low-income/
- Stigsdotter UK, & Grahn P (2011). Stressed individuals’ preferences for activities and environmental characteristics in green spaces. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 10(4), 295–304. 10.1016/j.ufjg.2011.07.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Takano T, Nakamura K, & Watanabe M (2002). Urban residential environments and senior citizens’ longevity in megacity areas: The importance of walkable green spaces. Journal ofEpidemiology and Community Health, 56(12), 913–918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teas J, Hurley T, Ghumare S, & Ogoussan K (2007). Walking outside improves mood for healthy postmenopausal women. Clinical Medicine: Oncology, 1, 35–43. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson ER (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242. [Google Scholar]
- Tudor-Locke C, & Bassett DR Jr (2004). How many steps/day are enough? Preliminary pedometer indices for public health. Sports Medicine, 34(1), 1–9. 10.2165/00007256-200434010-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tyrvainen L, Ojala A, Korpela KM, Lanki T, Tsunetsugu Y, & Kagawa T (2014). The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment. Journal ofEnvironmental Psychology, 38(0), 1–9. 10.1016Zj.jenvp.2013.12.005 [Google Scholar]
- Ulrich RS (1981). Natural versus urban scenes some psychophysiological effects. Environment and Behavior, 13(5), 523–556. [Google Scholar]
- Ulrich RS (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment In Altman I & Wohlwill J (Eds.), Human Behavior & Environment (Vol. 6, pp. 85–125). New York: Plenum. [Google Scholar]
- Ulrich RS (1984). View through a window may influence recovery. Science (224), 420–421. 10.1126/science.6i43402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulrich RS, Dimberg U, & Driver BL (1990). Psychophysiological indicators of leisure consequences. Journal ofLeisure Research, 22(2), 154–166. [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Census Bureau. (2018). U.S. Census Bureau QuickFacts: Marin County, California; San Mateo County, California; Santa Clara County, California; Alameda County, California; Contra Costa County, California; San Francisco County, California. Retrieved May 8, 2018, from https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/fact/table/marincountycalifornia,sanmateocountycalifomia,santadaracountycalifomia,alamedacountycalifornia,contracostacountycalifornia,sanfranciscocountycalifornia/PST045217
- U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development. (2018). Income Limits. Retrieved May 8, 2018, from https://www.huduser.gov/portal/datasets/il.html#20i7
- van den Berg AE, & Custers M (2010). Gardening promotes neuroendocrine and affective restoration from stress. Journal of Health Psychology, 16(1), 3–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Berg AE, Hartig T, & Staats H (2007). Preference for nature in urbanized societies: Stress, restoration, and the pursuit of sustainability. Journal of Social Issues, 63(1), 79–96. 10.nn/j.1540-4560.2007.00497.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- van den Berg AE, Jorgensen A, & Wilson ER (2014). Evaluating restoration in urban green spaces: Does setting type make a difference? Landscape and Urban Planning, 127(0), 173–181. 10.i0i6/j.landurbplan.2014.04.012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vassiljev P, Palo T, Kull A, Kulvik M, Bell S, Kull A, & Mander U (2010). Forest landscape assessment for cross country skiing in declining snow conditions: The case of Haanja Upland, Estonia. Baltic Forestry, 16(2), 280–295. [Google Scholar]
- Virden RJ, & Walker GJ (1999). Ethnic/racial and gender variations among meanings given to, and preferences for, the natural environment. Leisure Sciences, 21(3), 219–239. [Google Scholar]
- Wallace N (2018, May 15). What is the true cost of living in San Francisco? Retrieved May 12, 2018, from https://smartasset.com/mortgage/what-is-the-cost-of-living-ln-san-francisco
- Ward Thompson C, Roe J, Aspinall P, Mitchell R, Clow A, & Miller D (2012). More green space is linked to less stress in deprived communities: Evidence from salivary cortisol patterns. Landscape and Urban Planning, 105(3), 221–229. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.12.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Warnecke RB, Oh A, Breen N, Gehlert S, Paskett E, Tucker KL, Lurie N, Rebbeck T, Goodwin J, Flack J, Srinivasan S, Kerner J, Heurtin-Roberts S, Abeles R, Tyson FL, Patmios G, & Hiatt RA (2008). Approaching health disparities from a population perspective: The National Institutes of Health Centers for Population Health and Health Disparities. American Journal ofPublic Health, 98(9), 1608–1615. 10.2105/AJPH.2006.102525 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weng PY, & Chiang YC (2014). Psychological restoration through indoor and outdoor leisure activities. Journal ofLeisure Research, 46(2), 203–217. [Google Scholar]
- White MP, Alcock I, Wheeler BW, & Depledge MH (2013). Would you be happier living in a greener urban area? A fixed-effects analysis of panel data. Psychological Science, 24(6), 920–928. 10.1177/0956797612464659 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams DR, Neighbors HW, & Jackson JS (2003). Racial/ethnic discrimination and health: findings from community studies. American Journal ofPublic Health, 93(2), 200–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson JD, McGinnis N, Latkova P, Tierney P, & Yoshino A (2016). Urban park soundscapes: Association of noise and danger with perceived restoration. Journal of Park and Recreation Administration, 34(3). 10.18666/JPRA-2016-V34-I3-6927 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yancy AK, Ortega AN, Kumanyika SK, 2006. Effective recruitment and retention of minority research participants. Annual Review ofPublic Health 271–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]