Abstract
Background
The aim of this study was to explore perceived fatigue, experienced functional limitations due to fatigue and clinical correlates in patients with Myotonic Dystrophy type 1 (DM1).
Methods
In total, 32 consecutive patients with DM1 (14 women and 18 men) and 30 sex, age and education matched healthy control subjects participated. Perceived fatigue was rated on the Fatigue Impact Scale (FIS). Patients also completed a set of assessments aimed to characterize CTG-repeat size, muscle impairment, depression and cognitive functions. Non-parametric analysis were performed as appropriate, including Mann-Whitney U-test and Spearman correlation test.
Results
DM1 patients had higher FIS total score than healthy controls, suggesting higher fatigue levels. More specifically, DM1 patients scored higher on the FIS physical and psychosocial subscales than controls but not on the FIS cognitive scale. Scores on fatigue correlated significantly with muscle impairment and depression.
Conclusions
Perceived fatigue is significantly more common in patients with DM1 than in healthy controls. Higher ratings on depression and muscle impairment were associated with the condition. This indicates that both depression and muscle impairment may contribute to the experience of fatigue in DM1.
Keywords: Myotonic dystrophy, Fatigue, Neuromuscular, Depression, Cognition
Background
Myotonic dystrophy type 1 (DM1) is a slowly progressive inherited multisystem disorder, caused by the expansion of an unstable CTG trinucleotide repeat in the DMPK gene on chromosome 19q13.3 [1, 2]. The disorder negatively affects skeletal and smooth muscles, eyes, the heart and the endocrine- and central nervous systems. A high proportion of DM1 patients report fatigue [3]. Although a comprehensive and widely accepted definition is lacking, fatigue is commonly defined as an overwhelming sense of tiredness, lack of energy and feeling of exhaustion associated with difficulties in initiating or sustaining voluntary activities [4]. In many muscle dystrophies, muscle weakness and abnormal fatigability occurs after physical exertion [5]. This is the case in DM1; however, sleep disturbances and depression are also associated with the condition [6, 7]. Thus, in DM1 the cause of fatigue is multifactorial and may be associated with both central and peripheral nervous system abnormalities [8].
Since the first study on experienced fatigue in DM1 [9], several studies have explored the condition. These studies generally show a significantly higher prevalence of self-rated fatigue in patients with DM1, than as seen in normative data. However, to the best of our knowledge, only few case-control studies are available [10–14]. These studies have in most cases included small control samples, variable measures and with one exception [13] there is a general lack of information on effect sizes, making conclusions about the prevalence of fatigue, difficult to draw. Furthermore, few studies have presented information on the perceived impact of fatigue on different life domains. Available data indicate a positive correlation between higher fatigue levels and disrupted social participation (mobility, housing, employment and recreation) [15]. Patients scoring high on fatigue also describe reduced physical and social functioning, poorer mental health, less vitality and more physical pain. They also show lower levels of physical activity and less optimistic impression about their general health and quality of life [15, 16]. Few studies have explored fatigue and its relation to cognitive functions, except one that showed no correlation between fatigue and intelligence as measured on IQ tests [6].
The aim of the present study was to explore perceived fatigue in patients with DM1, using a standardized rating scale and to compare the patient’s results with those of matched healthy control subjects. We also analyzed whether patients age, disease duration, sex, degree of muscle impairment, CTG-repeat size, depression and performance on neuropsychological tests were associated with ratings on fatigue.
Methods
Subjects
Thirty-two patients (age range 23–61 years) with genetically confirmed adult-onset/classical DM1 [1] who consecutively attended the Neuromuscular Centre at the Sahlgrenska University hospital agreed to participate. The inclusion criterion was being 15 to 65 years old. In all cases, symptoms had first appeared after 10 years of age. All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision. Exclusion criteria included acquired brain injury, alcohol/drug misuse, and major psychiatric or somatic illness. No participant used psychostimulants during assessment. The control group consisted of 30 healthy individuals recruited from schools and workplaces in Gothenburg, matched with patients for sex, age and education. None of the control persons had any disease or was on any medication or drug. All participants gave informed consent and the regional ethical board in Gothenburg approved the study. Data on demographic and clinical characteristics are presented in Table 1. The subjects were recruited between 2010 and 2011.
Table 1.
DM1 (n = 32) | HC (n = 30) | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|
Age | 40.1 (10); [39.5] | 40 (11.1) [37.5] | .92 |
Proportion women | 56.3% | 53.3% | .82 |
Education, years | 11.2 (2.3) [12] | 11.6 (1.9) [12] | .96 |
Disease duration, years | 16.2 (9.2) [15.5] | ||
CTG-repeat sizeb | 712 (274) [750] | ||
MIRS | 4.1 (1); [4.0] | ||
BDI-II | 9.2 (6.3); [7.5] |
Note: MIRS = Muscular Impairment Rating Scale, BDI = Beck Depression Inventory, HC = healthy control subjects. aResults are presented as mean (sd); [median]. bn = 29
Self-rating of fatigue
We used a Swedish version of the Fatigue Impact Scale (FIS), a self-rated questionnaire on fatigue with good psychometric properties [17, 18]. FIS includes 40 items, each of which is scored from 0 (no problem) to 4 (extreme problem), providing a continuous total score range from 0 to 160. High scores reflect functional limitation due to fatigue within the previous.
month in three different domains: physical (10 items), psychosocial (20 items) and cognitive (10 items). Physical functioning involves motivation, effort, coordination and stamina. Psychosocial functioning is associated with isolation, coping, workload and emotions. Cognitive functioning reflects memory, concentration, thinking and organizing one’s thoughts.
Self-rating on depression
Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI II) was used to collect self-ratings of depression. BDI II is a widely used 21-item standardized self-report questionnaire measuring depression on a 4-point scale from 0 to 3 [19]. Proposed cut-off scores is as follows: 0–13, minimal depression; 14–19, mild depression; 20–28, moderate depression and 29–63, severe depression [19]. We also performed an item-analysis on two separable dimensions of depression: a cognitive affective (item 1, 3–14 and 17) and a somatic (item 15, 16, 18–20). The cognitive-affective dimension represents items such as lack of joy, guilt and suicidal thoughts and the somatic dimension includes a lack of energy, daytime sleepiness and exhaustion [19].
Neuropsychological assessment
Patients with DM1 were given a standardized neuropsychological test battery measuring various cognitive functions. Tests included measured verbal ability (Vocabulary [20]), verbal fluency (F-A-S [21]), verbal memory (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) [22]), visual construction ability (Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT) [23]), visual memory (RCFT [23]), speed (Trail Making Test A (TMT A) [24] and Digit symbol [20]), attention (TMT A, B [24] and Digit Span [20]) and executive functions (Stroop Color Word Test B [25], TMT B [24] and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [26]). The tests were presented in a previous study by the present authors [27].
Procedure
An experienced neuropsychologist (the first author, SW) examined the patients at Sahlgrenska University hospital. The test procedure was performed during a two-hour session (with a break to avoid exhaustion) in the same order by all patients. A master student in clinical psychology under the supervision of SW performed the assessment on healthy controls at schools and workplaces. In all cases, participants completed testing in a quiet environment with adequate lighting.
Rating on muscle impairment
Ratings on muscle impairments were performed by an experienced neurologist (CL) using the Muscular Impairment Rating Scale (MIRS) [28]. MIRS is an ordinal five-point rating scale, where 1 implies normal function/minimal symptoms and higher values indicates increasing levels of muscle impairment. MIRS 4 indicate mild to moderate proximal muscle weakness. The scale is a reliable measurement of muscular impairment in DM1 [28].
Genetic analysis
DNA was extracted from peripheral blood lymphocytes and analyzed for the expansion of the CTG repeat in the DMPK gene. The analysis was performed using polymerase-chain-reaction and southern blot using the probe PM1 0 M6 [2]. The size of the CTG expansions was assessed visually from exposed x-ray films.
Statistical analysis
Due to significant deviations from the normal distribution on several variables, non-parametric analysis were performed as appropriate, including Mann-Whitney U-test and Spearman correlation test with a Bonferroni-Holm correction for multiple tests [29]. We used Cohen’s guidelines [30] to interpret effect sizes as follows: small size <.30, medium effect size >.30 and large effect size >.50. The alpha level was set at .05. We compared total FIS score with normative data on Swedish control persons [31]. Data were analyzed using PASW base 18 (Chicago, IL).
Results
Figure 1 shows FIS scores for the patients with DM1 and the healthy controls. A Mann-Whitney test for independent measures revealed significant between group differences in total FIS scores (U = 276, p = .004, r = −.37), FIS physical scores (U = 151.5, p = < .001, r = −.57), and FIS psychosocial scores (U = 318, p = .022, r = −.34) with medium to large effect sizes. With a total score of 44 used as a cut off (the 3rd quartile in a sample of 194 randomly selected Swedish control persons) [31] 50% of DM1 patients score above cut off while only 20% of the control sample did.
The correlation between DM1 patient’s scores on the FIS and depression, muscle function, disease duration and CTG repeat size was analyzed using Spearman’s correlation test. As shown in Table 2 there were positive correlations between scores on BDI-II and FIS total scores (rs (30) = .68, p < .001), FIS cognitive scores (rs (30) = .64, p < .001), FIS psychosocial scores (rs (30) = .68, p < .001) and FIS physical scores (rs (30) = .65, p < .001) with large effect sizes. We also analyzed the correlation between FIS and separate somatic and cognitive-affective BDI dimensions (see Methods section for details). Significant correlations (< .05) emerged between the two BDI dimensions, FIS total score and all FIS subscales. Muscle function as rated by MIRS correlated significantly with FIS total score (rs (30) 0 .37, p < .05) and FIS psychosocial score (rs (30) = .40, p < .05). To explore the association between perceived fatigue and results on the neuropsychological tests, we analyzed correlations between FIS scores and neuropsychological data (see Table 3). The FIS psychosocial scale correlated significantly with Trail Making Test A (rs (30) = .37, p < .05). However, when corrected for mass significance using the Bonferroni-Holm test [29] with alpha set to .05, no significance remained. We found no differences between men and women in their ratings of fatigue, nor did we find any correlation between ratings of fatigue and duration of disease, age or CTG repeat expansion size.
Table 2.
FIS-T | FIS-PS | FIS-PHY | FI-C | |
---|---|---|---|---|
BDI-II | .68 ** | .68** | .65** | .64 ** |
BDI-S | .43* | .41* | .47** | .46** |
BDI-C | .49** | .50** | .42* | .46** |
MIRS | .37* | .40 * | .34 | .26 |
Disease duration (years) | .11 | .14 | .13 | .10 |
CTG-repeats † | .06 | .08 | .09 | −.11 |
Note: FIS = Fatigue Impact scale, BDI = Beck Depression Inventory, MIRS = Muscle Impairment Rating Scale. FIS-T = total score, FIS-C = cognitive score, FIS-PS = psychosocial score, FIS-PHY = physical score. BDI-S = BDI somatic dimension, BDI-C = BDI cognitive-affective dimension. * = p < .05, ** = p < .001, † = n = 29
Table 3.
FIS-T | FIS-PS | FIS-PHY | FIS-C | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Vocabulary | .17 | .18 | .08 | .10 |
FAS | .00 | −.03 | .02 | .01 |
RAVLT | −.01 | −.04 | .03 | .04 |
RCFT copy | −.34 | −.34 | −.34 | −.29 |
RCFT recall | −.30 | −.32 | −.28 | −.23 |
TMT A | .32 | .37* | .31 | .08 |
TMT B | .20 | .22 | .20 | .11 |
Digit symbol | −.06 | −.12 | .01 | .02 |
Digit span | .05 | .00 | .06 | .02 |
Stroop CWT B | .16 | .17 | −.22 | .07 |
WCST (categories) | −.14 | −.11 | −.08 | .17 |
Note: FIS = Fatigue Impact Scale, FAS = FAS Controlled Oral Word Association Test, RCFT = Rey Complex Figure Test, RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, TMT = Trail Making Test, Stroop CWT = Stroop Color Word Test, WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. FIS-T = total score, FIS-C = cognitive score, FIS-PS = psychosocial score, FIS-PHY = physical score. * = p < .05
Discussion
Ratings of perceived fatigue were significantly higher in patients with DM1 than in matched healthy controls. This finding confirms significant between group differences [10–14]. We also found a moderate effect size when comparing FIS total scores. We found significant between group differences on the FIS psychosocial and physical domain with medium to large effect sizes. This indicates that more patients with DM1 experienced lower psychosocial functioning, workload and higher social isolation because of fatigue. These results are in line with an earlier report on disrupted social participation in DM1 [15]. More patients than controls also experienced limitations due to fatigue on psychological functioning, including emotional lability, irritability and stress and physical functioning, such as reduced motivation, poorer effort, and worse coordination and stamina. These results indicate that DM1 patients experience limitations due to fatigue, which is consistent with measurements of fatigue, psychological [6] and physical functioning [16]. Furthermore, there was a positive relationship between ratings on higher fatigue level and greater muscular impairment, which reinforces findings on fatigue and physical functioning [16].
Patients with higher scores on the FIS also scored higher on depression. Depression may be the result of general stress associated with disability in a chronic disease, where fatigue may be one factor leading to reduced activity, social isolation- and ultimately depression [32]. However, a depressive attitude may also negatively influence a person’s perception of their disability, including their perceived level of fatigue. When the association between perceived fatigue and two subdomains of the BDI II (a cognitive and a somatic) were analyzed we found that ratings on fatigue were related to both domains. This means that a depressive attitude may influence perceived fatigue, but also that fatigue may have an impact on ratings of depression. It is notable that although the ratings mainly showed signs of mild to moderate depression, only a few scores indicated severe depression, which replicates the results of an earlier study by our research group [33]. However, the correlation indicates that even small variations in experienced depression may influence fatigue, and vice versa.
We did not find any difference between groups regarding the FIS cognitive score and there were no significant correlations between perceived fatigue and any neuropsychological measure. These results complement earlier findings on fatigue and IQ [6] measured on demarcated cognitive functions. In other disorders, fatigue has been associated with higher physiological costs such as more widespread cerebral activation [34]. This physiological cost was related to the subjective experience of fatigue rather than to actual cognitive performance. Similarly, in DM1 fatigue and cognitive performance may operate through different mechanisms.
These findings are best understood in light of the study’s limitations. First, the assessment of fatigue were performed before the publication of an instrument specifically validated in patients with DM1 [35]. It is our impression that patients could understand and fill in the FIS satisfactorily, however the use of validated instruments are warranted in future studies. Second, excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is associated with DM1 [35]. Fatigue and EDS are strongly interrelated with muscle function and depression. Therefore, EDS may, indirectly influence correlations between fatigue, muscle function and depression. There was no rating on EDS in this study and it should be included in future studies as a covariate. Lastly, we did not find any correlation between ratings of perceived fatigue and measures of cognition. In other disorders [36, 37] ratings of fatigue have been associated in computerized tests over longer time interval with reduced attention and vigilance. The assessments in this study used only traditional paper-and-pencil tests over a shorter time span. We therefore propose that computerized tests should be included in future studies to increase sensitivity for detecting possible correlations.
Conclusions
In summary, these data shows a significantly higher prevalence of perceived fatigue in patients with DM1 than in matched healthy control subjects, with an experienced impact on physical and psychosocial domains. These findings support the inclusion of fatigue as a main target for treatment interventions aimed to reduce fatigue through cognitive behavioral therapy and graded exercise. Such an intervention was recently applied and shown to reduce fatigue, increase activity and improve social participation [38].
Acknowledgements
We gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Kristina Wennerstrand, Lic Psychologist for the neuropsychological assessment of healthy controls and Lena Samuelsson, PhD, Department of clinical genetics, Sahlgrenska University hospital for the genetic analysis.
Funding
This study was supported by the West Sweden Muscle Foundation. The funding agency was not involved in the study design, collection of data, analysis, interpretation or preparation of the manuscript.
Availability of data and materials
The dataset generated and analyzed during the current study are not publicly available, but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- BDI-II
Beck Depression Inventory II
- DM1
Myotonic Dystrophy type 1
- FIS
Fatigue Impact Scale
- MIRS
Muscle Impairment Rating Scale
- RAVLT
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
- RCFT
Rey Complex Figure Test
- TMT
Trail Making Test
- WCST
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
Authors’ contributions
Study concept and design: SW, CL Acquisition of data: SW, CL Analysis and interpretation of the data: SW Drafting of the manuscript: SW, CL Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: SW, CL Obtaining funding: SW Study supervision: SW SW had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for its integrity and the accuracy of the data analysis. Both authors approved the submitted version of the paper.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All participants gave informed written consent and the regional ethical board in Gothenburg approved the study.
The regional ethical board in Gothenburg located at Sahlgrenska University hospital approved the study.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Stefan Winblad, Phone: +46 317861692, Email: stefan.winblad@psy.gu.se.
Christopher Lindberg, Email: christopher.lindberg@vgregion.se.
References
- 1.Thornton CA. Myotonic dystrophy. Neurol Clin. 2014;2:705–719. doi: 10.1016/j.ncl.2014.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Brook JD, Mccurrach ME, Harley HG, et al. Molecular basis of myotonic dystrophy: expansion of a trinucleotid (CTG) repeat at the 3’end of a transcript encoding a protein kinase family member. Cell. 1992;68:799–800. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(92)90154-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Heatwole C, Bode R, Johnson N, et al. Patient-reported impact of symptoms in myotonic dystrophy type 1 (PRISM-1) Neurology. 2012;79:348–357. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318260cbe6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Finsterer J, Zarrouk Mahjoub S. Fatigue in healthy and diseased individuals. Am J Hosp Pall Med. 2014;31:562–575. doi: 10.1177/1049909113494748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Angelini C, Tasca E. Fatigue in muscular dystrophies. Neuromuscul Disord. 2012;22:214–220. doi: 10.1016/j.nmd.2012.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Laberge L, Dauvilliers Y, Bégin P, Richer L, Jean S, Mathieu J. Fatigue and daytime sleepiness in patients with myotonic dystrophy type 1: to lump or split? Neuromuscul Disord. 2009;19:397–402. doi: 10.1016/j.nmd.2009.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kalkman JS, Schillings ML, Zwarts MJ, van Engelen BG, Bleijenberg G. The development of a model of fatigue in neuromuscular disorders: a longitudinal study. J Psychosom Res. 2007;5:571–579. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Baldanzi S, Ricci G, Bottari M, Chico L, Simoncini C, Gabriele S. The proposal of a clinical protocol to assess central and peripheral fatigue in myotonic dystrophy type 1. Arch Ital Biol. 2017;155:43–53. doi: 10.12871/000398292017125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.van der Werf S, Kalkman J, Bleijenberg G, van Engelen B, Schillings M, Zwarts M. The relation between daytime sleepiness, fatigue, and reduced motivation in patients with adult onset myotonic dystrophy. JNNP. 2003;74:134–139. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.1.138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Schillings ML, Kalkman JS, Janssen HMHA, van Engelen BGM, Bleijenberg G, Zwarts MJ. Experienced and physiological fatigue in neuromuscular disorders. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007;118:292–300. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Minnerop M, Weber B, Schoene-Bake J-C, et al. The brain in myotonic dystrophy 1 and 2: evidence for a predominant white matter disease. Brain. 2011;134:3527–3543. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gallais B, Montreuil M, Gargiulo M, Eymard B, Gagnon C, Laberge L. Prevalence and correlates of apathy in myotonic dystrophy type 1. BMC Neurol. 2015;15:148. doi: 10.1186/s12883-015-0401-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hamilton MJ, MacLean J, Cumming S, et al. Outcome measures for central nervous system evaluation in myotonic dystrophy type 1 may be confounded by deficits in motor function or insight. Front Neurol. 2018;9:780. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Yu H, Laberge L, Jaussent I, et al. Daytime sleepiness and REM sleep characteristics in myotonic dystrophy: a case-control study. Sleep. 2011;2:165–170. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.2.165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Gagnon C, Mathieu J, Jean S, et al. Predictors of disrupted social participation in myotonic dystrophy type 1. Arch Phys Med Rehab. 2008;89:1246–1255. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.10.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kalkman JS, Zwarts MJ, Schillings ML, van Engelen BGM, Bleijenberg G. Different types of fatigue in patients with facioscaphumeral dystrophy, myotonic dystrophy and HMSN-I. Experienced fatigue and physiological fatigue. Neurol Sc. 2008;29:238–240. doi: 10.1007/s10072-008-0949-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Fisk JD, Ritvo PG, Ross L, Haase DA, Marrie TJ, Schlech WF. Measuring the functional impact of fatigue: initial validation of the fatigue impact scale. Clin Inf Dis. 1994;18:79–83. doi: 10.1093/clinids/18.Supplement_1.S79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Flensner G, Ek A-C, Söderhamn O. Reliability and validity of the Swedish version of the fatigue impact scale (FIS) Scand J Occup Ther. 2005;12:170–180. doi: 10.1080/11038120510031806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Beck A, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck depression inventory. Swedish manual. 2. Stockholm: Psykologiförlaget; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bartfai A, Nyman H, Stegmann B. Wechsler adult intelligence scale – revised. Swedish manual: Psykologiförlaget; 1992.
- 21.Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;2:167–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Schmidt M. Rey auditory verbal learning test. A handbook. Torrance: Western Psychol Serv; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Fastenau PS, Denburg NL, Hufford BJ. Adult norms for the Rey-Osterieth complex figure test and for supplemental recognition and matching trials from the extended complex figure test. Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;1:30–47. doi: 10.1076/clin.13.1.30.1976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tombaugh T. Trail making test a and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19:203–214. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mitrushina M, Boone KB, Razani J, D'Elia LF. Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 108–133. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Grant DA, Berg EA, Card NHW. Sorting test. WCST -revised and expanded. Swedish manual. Stockholm: Psykologiförlaget; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Winblad S, Lindberg C, Hansen S. Cognitive deficits and CTG repeat expansion size in classical myotonic dystrophy type 1. Behav Brain Funct. 2006;2:16. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-2-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Mathieu J, Boivin H, Meunier D, Gaudreault M, Bégin P. Assessment of a disease-specific muscular impairment rating scale in myotonic dystrophy. Neurology. 2001;56:336–340. doi: 10.1212/WNL.56.3.336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat. 1979;6:65–70. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Cohen J. A power primer. Psych Bull. 1992;112:155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Flensner G, Ek A-C, Landtbom A-M, Söderhamn O. Fatigue in relation to perceived health: people with multiple sclerosis compared with people in the general population. Scand J Car Sci. 2008;22:390–400. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2007.00542.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Swain MG. Fatigue in chronic disease. Clin Sci (Lond) 2000;1:1–8. doi: 10.1042/cs0990001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Winblad S, Jensen C, Mansson J-E, Samuelsson L, Lindberg C. Depression in myotonic dystrophy type 1: clinical and neuronal correlates. Behav Brain Funct. 2010;6:25. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.DeLuca JF. Cognition and mental effort. In: DeLuca J, editor. Fatigue as a window to the brain. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Gallais B, Gagnon C, Forgues G, Côté I, Laberge L. Further evidence for the reliability and validity of the fatigue and daytime sleepiness scale. J Neurol Sci. 2017;375:23–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2017.01.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Krupp LB, Elkins LE. Fatigue and declines in cognitive functioning in multiple sclerosis. Neurology. 2000;55:934–939. doi: 10.1212/WNL.55.7.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Aldughmi M, Bruce J, Siengsukon CF. Relationship between fatigability and perceived fatigue measured using the neurological fatigue index in people with multiple sclerosis. Int J MS Care. 2017;19:232–239. doi: 10.7224/1537-2073.2016-059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Okkersen K, Jimenez Moreno C, Wenninger S, et al. Cognitive behavioural therapy with optional graded exercise therapy in patients with severe fatigue with myotonic dystrophy type 1: a multicentre, single blind, randomized trial. Lancet Neurol. 2018;8:671–680. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30203-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The dataset generated and analyzed during the current study are not publicly available, but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.