Abstract
Methamphetamine use disorder is associated with a high likelihood of relapse. Identifying robust predictors of relapse that have explanatory power is critical to develop secondary prevention based on a mechanistic understanding of relapse. Computational approaches have the potential to identify such predictive markers of psychiatric illness, with the advantage of providing a finer mechanistic explanation of the cognitive processes underlying psychiatric vulnerability.
In this study, sixty-two recently sober methamphetamine-dependent individuals were recruited from a 28-day inpatient treatment program, and completed a Stop Signal Task (SST) while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). These individuals were prospectively followed for 1 year and assessed for relapse to methamphetamine use. Thirty-three percent of followed participants reported relapse.
We found that neural activity associated with two types of Bayesian prediction error, i.e. the difference between actual and expected need to stop on a given trial, significantly differentiated those individuals who remained abstinent and those who relapsed. Specifically, relapsed individuals exhibited smaller neural activations to such Bayesian prediction errors relative to those individuals who remained abstinent in the left temporoparietal junction (Cohen's d = 0.91), the left inferior frontal gyrus (Cohen's d = 0.57), and left anterior insula (Cohen's d = 0.63). In contrast, abstinent and relapsed participants did not differ in neural activation to non-model based task contrasts or on various self-report clinical measures.
In conclusion, Bayesian cognitive models may help identify predictive biomarkers of relapse, while providing a computational explanation of belief processing and updating deficits in individuals with methamphetamine use disorder.
Keywords: Methamphetamine dependence, Relapse, Bayesian model, Inhibitory control, Stimulant
Highlights
-
•
Methamphetamine-dependent individuals (MDI) face a high rate of relapse after treatment.
-
•
Can a Bayesian learning modeling and fMRI be used to identify markers of relapse?
-
•
MDI who relapsed within 1 year have smaller activation to Bayesian model-based prediction errors.
-
•
Such neural pattern was observed in left temporo-parietal junction, IFG, and anterior insula.
-
•
MDI more likely to relapse show weaker tracking of uncertainty and updating of their belief model.
1. Introduction
Methamphetamine use disorder is associated with a high likelihood of and short time-span to relapse (Brecht and Herbeck, 2014). Identifying precise and robust predictors of relapse is therefore essential for improving the prevention and treatment of this disorder. Generative computational models have the potential to precisely disambiguate and quantify complex cognitive processes, which may in turn be used to identify specific neuro-cognitive treatment targets for a personalized approach to treating addictive disorders (Paulus et al., 2016).
Recently, we showed that both healthy individuals (Ide et al., 2013) and stimulant users (Harlé et al., 2014; Harlé et al., 2016) continuously alter their response strategy in a standard inhibitory paradigm (stop-signal task, SST), such that dynamic fluctuations in their reaction time and performance are consistent with a Bayesian sequential adjustment of their beliefs (Yu and Cohen, 2009) and decision strategy (Shenoy and Yu, 2011). Whereas standard behavioral measures to assess performance in the SST, such as mean stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) or stop signal asynchrony dependent error rate, are relatively easy to obtain, Bayes-optimal model parameters have the advantage that they provide quantitative explanatory measures of an underlying putative cognitive process. Moreover, the model can be used to simulate data to better elucidate how behavioral dysfunctions emerge as a consequence of altered underlying cognitive processes. Here, we use a similar Bayesian approach combined with fast event-related fMRI to model methamphetamine-dependent individuals' real-time beliefs about the need to stop in the SST, and isolate neural markers of relapse one year after treatment. We aimed to determine whether those computational neural markers of inhibitory function can meaningfully distinguish methamphetamine-dependent individuals who remain abstinent from those who relapse within a year.
Based on recent studies pointing to weaker and less efficient neural encoding of Bayesian expectations of a stop response in the SST, and associated abnormalities in neural tracking of Bayesian prediction errors (i.e. difference between probability and actual occurrence of a stop signal on a given trial) in occasional stimulant users (Harlé et al., 2014; Harlé et al., 2015) and methamphetamine-dependent individuals (Harlé et al., 2016), we hypothesized that weaker or less efficient neural responses associated with Bayesian prediction errors would be observed in relapsed relative to abstinent individuals. Clusters in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG; e.g., Brodmann area 44, 47) and overlapping the interior insula bilaterally (Claus et al., 2013; Swick et al., 2011), as well in the pre-Supplementary Motor Area (SMA) (Aron, 2011; Aron et al., 2007) have been robustly implicated in guiding inhibitory function. In those regions, both structural deficits (reduced integrity of white matter fibers) (Ersche et al., 2012) and functional alterations (Hester and Garavan, 2004; Nestor et al., 2011) have been observed in stimulant addicted individuals. Larger activation patterns to unsigned Bayesian prediction errors in the SST have also been observed among occasional stimulant users progressing to problem use in a region overlapping the right anterior insula and IFG (Harlé et al., 2015). Based on these combined findings, we expected abnormal prediction error activations in the IFG/anterior insula regions to be associated with more severe addictive profiles and thus with relapse status one year after treatment.
The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and anterior insula have also been robustly linked to encoding of prediction errors and more generally expectancy violation in a wide range of cognitive task, but notably in the SST. At the computational level, both regions have been shown to encode volatility and the tracking of unexpected changes in the environment (Behrens et al., 2007; Bossaerts 2010a; Ide et al., 2013; Jiang et al., 2015; Preuschoff et al., 2008). Using a similar modeling approach as used here, Ide et al. (2013) found that a region in the dorsal ACC overlapping pre-SMA was robustly associated with neural activation to an unsigned Bayesian prediction error in the SST, while we showed that occasional stimulant use was assoviated with weaker activations to the same type of prediction error in similar, albeit more caudal region of the ACC and in the posterior insular cortex (Harlé et al., 2014). Based on this research, we hypothesized that weaker or abnormal neural signals in those regions would be associated with less efficient tracking of model-based uncertainty and thus more apparent in relapsed individuals.
Finally, the superior temporal gyrus, specifically the temporoparietal junction, has been associated with dynamic learning and anticipatory processes (Gläscher et al., 2010; Spoormaker et al., 2011), including proactive inhibition, i.e. the anticipation of the need to stop in the SST (Zandbelt et al., 2013). The left temporoparietal junction, in particular, is involved in mathematical computations (Menon et al., 2000) and has recently been implicated in Bayesian learning (d'Acremont et al., 2013). Given consistent evidence of learning deficits (Aron and Paulus, 2007; Paulus et al., 2002), and findings of hypo-activations of the superior temporal gyrus and temporoparietal junction during learning-based decision-making in methamphetamine dependence (Paulus et al., 2002), we therefore expected potential computational learning inefficiencies to be reflected in weaker recruitment of this brain region among individuals more susceptible to relapse.
Taken together, recent computational evidence suggests altered Bayesian learning during inhibitory control tasks such as the SST in stimulant users of varying clinical severity. Given that areas identified above are both critical to inhibitory control implementation and have been linked to recruitment abnormalities in stimulant users during inhibitory control, we hypothesized that among individuals diagnosed with methamphetamine dependence, those more likely to relapse would exhibit less efficient inhibitory learning as reflected by weaker neural responses associated with anticipation and belief updating. We predicted these weaker neural signals would be particularly observable in regions key to inhibitory control and inhibitory learning, including the IFG, anterior insula, ACC, and temporoparietal junction.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
The study protocol was approved by the UCSD Human Research Protections Program and all subjects gave written informed consent. Sixty-two (21% female) recently sober methamphetamine-dependent individuals were recruited from a 28-day inpatient Alcohol and Drug Treatment Program at the Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System and Scripps Green Hospital (La Jolla, CA; cross-sectional analysis of these data published elsewhere (Harlé et al., 2016)). All study procedures, including the neuroimaging session examining brain and behavior responses during the SST, occurred during the third or fourth week of treatment for all participants (i.e., all had been abstinent from methamphetamine or any other drugs, including alcohol, for 3–4 weeks). To maintain sobriety during the program, participants were screened for the presence of drugs via random urine toxicology or whenever they left the facility, and were terminated from the program if tested positive. Participants also performed the North American Adult Reading Test (Uttl, 2002) as a measure of verbal intelligence, and completed self-report measures of personality and affective measures previously associated with stimulant addiction vulnerability (Dodge et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2009; London et al., 2004), including the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) (Patton and Stanford, 1995), the Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS-V) (Zuckerman and Link, 1968), and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1961).
Lifetime DSM-IV Axis I and II diagnoses at baseline were assessed with the Semi Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (Hesselbrock et al., 1999) and based on consensus meetings with the supervising clinician and trained study personnel, including a Masters-level research assistant and postdoctoral-level psychologist (see Supplementary Text 1 for exclusion criteria). Methamphetamine dependence, and not DSM-5 diagnosis of methamphetamine use disorder, was assessed because DSM-IV was in effect at the start of the study. Participants were contacted one year later for a brief structured phone interview to assess for any use of methamphetamine, level of use, and time of relapse. At baseline, we also assessed for use and abuse/dependence criteria for other drugs (i.e., sedatives, hallucinogens, marijuana, cocaine or opiates) in the past year, as part of a larger longitudinal study, which also followed individuals with other primary drugs of abuse. For all participants in this study, methamphetamine was confirmed as participants' drug of choice. Relapse was defined as use of methamphetamine at any point during the follow-up period, while some participants may have additionally relapsed to any of these other substances (not assessed at follow-up). Based on interview responses, thirty-nine methamphetamine-dependent individuals remained abstinent from methamphetamine from the time of treatment to one-year follow-up. Nineteen individuals reported that they relapsed. Four participants could not be tracked.
2.2. Stop signal task
Participants completed a stop-signal task (6 blocks of 48 trials) while undergoing fMRI. On 216 go trials (75% of all trials), they had to press as fast as possible the left button when an ‘X' appeared or the right button when an ‘O' appeared. On 72 stop trials (25% of all trials), they heard a tone shortly after onset of the go stimulus, which instructed them not to press either button. Each trial lasted around 1300 ms, and trials were separated by a 200-ms inter-stimulus intervals (blank screen). Individuals' reaction time (time from stimulus onset to button press) provided a natural jitter. The sequence of trial types presented was pseudo-randomized. Finally, prior to scanning, participants completed the SST outside the scanner to determine their mean ‘Go’ reaction time (MRT). This measure was then used to determine the stop signal delay (SSD) for six different types stop trial of increasing difficulty, providing a subject-dependent jittered reference function. Specifically, stop signals were delivered in equal amounts at MRT-0 ms, MRT-100 ms, MRT-200 ms, MRT-300 ms, MRT-400 ms, and MRT-500 ms providing an individually customized range of difficulty (Harlé et al., 2016); see Supplementary Text 2 for task instructions).
2.3. Bayesian model of the inhibitory response expectation
To model behavior, we used a Bayes-optimal Dynamic Belief Model (Ide et al., 2013; Shenoy and Yu, 2011; Yu and Cohen, 2009), which has been robustly established to capture behavioral adjustments in the SST in both healthy individuals (Ide et al., 2013; Shenoy and Yu, 2011) and substance users (Harlé et al., 2014; Harlé et al., 2016). The model assumes that one 1) updates the prior probability of encountering stop trials, P(stop), on a trial-by-trial basis based on trial history, and 2) adjusts behavior as a function of P(stop) with higher predicted P(stop) prompting slower Go RT and higher likelihood of correctly stopping on a stop trial (Harlé et al., 2014; Ide et al., 2013). Mathematically, on each trial k, Pk(stop) is the mean of the predictive distribution p(rk|sk-1), which is a mixture of the previous posterior distribution and a fixed prior distribution, with α and 1 − α acting as the mixing coefficients, respectively, and where Sk = (s1, …,sk) is 1 on stop trials and 0 on go trials:
with the posterior distribution being updated according to Bayes' Rule:
In this study, model parameters for the beta distribution p0(r) and α were kept constant across all participants, based on prior simulations that sought to optimize behavioral fit at the group level, i.e., maximizing the goodness-of-fit of the linear regression of P(Stop) on RT across all participants (see Supplementary Text 3). A fixed setting of p0 = Beta(a = 2.5,b = 7.5; s = a + b = 10; mean = 0.25) was used, and alpha was fit to participants' data (range tested: 0.25–1.0; α = 0.5 maximized overall fit across all participants; see Fig. 1A for P(stop) sequence).
2.4. Image acquisition and preprocessing
Using a fast event-related fMRI design, six T2*-weighted EPI functional runs were collected for each participant, along with one T1-weighted anatomical image. Each scanning session was conducted on a 3 T General Electric scanner with the following parameters: T2*-weighted EPI, repetition time = 2000 ms, echo time = 40 ms, 64 × 64 matrix, 30 4-mm axial slices, FOV = 220 × 220 mm, in-plane voxel size = 3.437, and flip angle = 90o. Each run included 256 repetitions for a length of 8 min and 32 s. Functional volume acquisitions were time-locked to task onset. During the same experimental session, we collected a T1-weighted image (MPRAGE, TR = 11.4 ms, TE = 4.4 ms, flip angle = 10 degree, FOV = 256 × 256, 1 mm3 voxels) to be used for anatomical reference. All structural and functional image processing and analysis was performed with the Analysis of Functional Neuroimages (AFNI) software package (Cox, 1996), and MRI x-y slices were reconstructed into AFNI BRIK format. Echoplanar images underwent automatic coregistration to the anatomical image and each participant dataset was visually inspected to confirm successful alignment. Outlier voxels were identified in the aligned images based on whether a given time point greatly exceeded the mean number of voxel outliers for the time-series. Time points with high numbers of outlier voxels were excluded from subsequent analyses.
2.5. First-level fMRI analyses
In a first general linear model (GLM), Go, Stop Success/SS, and Stop Error/SE trials were distinguished and convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function (Go Error trials were scarce and not included in these analyses). Each was entered as both categorical and P(stop)-modulated regressor (i.e., Go, SS, SE, GoxPk(stop), SSxPk(stop), and SExPk(stop)) (Harlé et al., 2014; Ide et al., 2013). To more specifically isolate neural activity related to belief updating processes vs uncertainty tracking (Harlé et al., 2014; Yu and Cohen, 2009), a second GLM was created with two types of trial-wise Bayesian prediction errors as parametric regressors, representing the discrepancy between expected likelihood of having to stop on the upcoming trial (i.e., P(stop)) and actual trial type observed (i.e., Go = 0, Stop = 1)(Harlé et al., 2014; Ide et al., 2013). These regressors included in this order the signed prediction error (SPE = outcome-P(stop) = sk-Pk(stop)) and the unsigned prediction error (UPE = |outcome-Pk(stop)| = |sk-Pk(stop)|; see Fig. 1A, bottom), as well as trial error (0 = correct or 1 = error) to control for performance error related activity (Ide et al., 2013). Each of these regressors were orthogonalized with respect to the preceding one given the non-trivial collinearity between them. Both GLMs included the following regressors of no-interest: a baseline regressor (inter-trial intervals), three linear drift regressors (x,y,z), and three motion regressors (pitch, yaw, roll) (Harlé et al., 2014), and Go RTs (residualized with respect to P(stop)) to control for motor response confounds. Images were spatially filtered (Gaussian full width half maximum 4 mm) to account for individual anatomical differences. Automated Talairach transformations were applied to anatomical images and functional images were subsequently transformed into Talairach space. Percent signal change (%SC) was determined by dividing the signal for each regressor of interest by the baseline regressor.
2.6. Second-level analyses
At the between-subject level, two types of voxelwise mixed-effects linear models (LME) were fit to the coefficients of our first-level GLM (Pinheiro et al., 2011). In the first LME, we first tested for the interaction of clinical status (abstinent vs relapsed) and modulation of P(Stop) under each Go vs Stop trials (i.e, GoxP(stop) vs StopxPk(stop), SS and SE were averaged), with subject treated as random intercept effect. Whole brain statistical maps were obtained for the group main effect (reflecting areas tracking a clinical status group difference in prior P(stop) value irrespective of trial type) and the clinical status X P(stop) modulated trial type interaction. In order to assess for potential group difference in the modulation of trial accuracy (i.e., successful vs failed inhibition) by P(stop), a similar LME approach was used. Specifically, we obtained statistical maps for the main effect of clinical status (reflecting potential group difference in P(stop) activation on Stop trials irrespective of accuracy) and for the clinical status X P(stop) modulated trial type (SE vs SS) interaction. To correct for multiple comparisons, we used a cluster threshold adjustment based on Monte Carlo simulations (generated with AFNI's 3dClustSim program). We first calculated spatial autocorrelation function (acf) parameters from the 1st glm model residuals, using the 3dFHWMx function (which does not assume a purely Gaussian acf function, but rather a mixture of Gaussian and mono-exponential). The obtained parameters, averaged across participants, were fed to the 3dClustSim function to determine FWER corrected cluster size. Based on the simulations, a minimum cluster volume of 896 μL was found to be sufficient to correct for multiple comparisons at FWER = p < .05 with a minimum voxelwise significant threshold of p < .005. While less conservative than using a p < .001 or p < .002 voxelwise threshold, this setting provides a balance of adequate correction for multiple comparisons while ensuring a substantial minimum cluster size. We also note that this threshold is still relatively conservative in light of previous work having applied this Bayesian computational model in similar populations and experimental settings, which would provide grounds for a more focused search within the brain based on regions of interest. Finally, within the regions identified with those LMEs, we identified those that were consistent with a potential group difference in either type of Bayesian prediction errors (UPE or SPE), using our second GLM for regions of interest (ROI) analyses. Moreover, for regions consistent with a group main effect or group X trial type interaction, we specifically checked for convergence between results from the second GLM and the pattern of activation to P(Stop) as function of trial type (first GLM). Specifically, regions with non-zero P(stop) activations of opposite signs or with the same signs across Go and Stop trials, should be consistent with a significant group difference in UPE and SPE activations, respectively. This is because an SPE is equal to 0 - P(stop) = − P(stop) on Go trials, and SPE = 1 – P(stop) on Stop trials, and thus negatively correlated with P(stop) on both type of trials. In contrast, UPE is equal to |0 - P(stop)| = P(stop) on Go trials and to |1 - P(stop)| = 1 – P(stop) on Stop trials, and thus should be positively correlated on Go trials but negatively correlated with P(stop) on Stop trials (see (Harlé et al., 2014; Harlé et al., 2016; Ide et al., 2013).
3. Results
3.1. Participants characteristics and drug use
Relapsed participants did not differ from those who remained sober in terms of ethnicity, gender, age, and verbal IQ (p > .05). At baseline, groups differed neither in reported lifetime methamphetamine, cocaine, marijuana uses, nor in average number of cigarettes/week and alcoholic drinks/week over the past year (p > .05; see Table 1).
Table 1.
Relapsed individuals (n = 19) |
Abstinent individuals (n = 39) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | P value | |
Demographics | |||||
Gendera (% Female) | 22% | – | 23% | – | p = .90 |
Ethnicityb (% White, Non-Hispanic) | 63% | – | 55% | – | p = .25 |
Age | 36.9 | 9.3 | 38.2 | 11.2 | p = .65 |
Verbal IQ | 110 | 7.8 | 108 | 9.5 | p = .39 |
Drug Use (Self-Report) | |||||
Methamphetamine lifetime uses | 9777 | 12,432 | 16,525 | 36,246 | p = .41c |
Cocaine lifetime uses (usersd) | 2833 (17) | 6438 | 2898 (35) | 6496 | p = .41c |
Cannabis lifetime uses (usersd) | 5007 (19) | 9031 | 12,110 (36) | 32,540 | p = .62c |
Alcohol: typical drinks/week (userse) | 3.0 (15) | 3.0 | 3.1(8) | 2.8 | p = .75 |
Nicotine: typical cigarettes/day (userse) | 8.7 (10) | 8.9 | 13.3(31) | 9.5 | p = .08 |
Opiates lifetime uses | 77 (4) | 135 | 414 (10) | 925 | p = .47c |
Hallucinogens lifetime uses | 74 (8) | 161 | 208 (12) | 377 | p = .31c |
Drug Use (Dependence Diagnoses.)f | |||||
Cocaine | 2 | 6 | |||
Cannabis | 4 | 3 | |||
Opiates | 1 | 1 | |||
Hallucinogens | 0 | 0 | |||
Personality/Mood | |||||
Baratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS) | 78.4 | 9.6 | 76.6 | 11.8 | p = .60 |
Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS) | 23.2 | 6.0 | 23.9 | 5.4 | p = .65 |
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) | 7.8 | 6.7 | 6.7 | 6.1 | p = .61 |
Attention/Hyperactivity | |||||
ADHD Attention Symptoms | 1.8 | 3.4 | 1.9 | 2.8 | p = .92 |
ADHD Hyperactivity Symptoms | 1.9 | 3.5 | 2.3 | 3.2 | p = .76 |
Conduct Symptoms | 1.4 | 1.6 | 1.5 | 1.3 | p = .78 |
IQ = intelligence quotient (based on North American Adult Reading Test).
Chi-square test: χ2(1) = 0.5, p = .82.
Chi-square test: χ2(5) = 6.6, p = .25.
t-test computed using natural log transformed +0.5 values (due to non-normal distributions) replicated results or raw data.
Number of past and/or current users mean was calculated with (other participants denied any past/present uses).
Number of current users mean was calculated with (other participants denied any current use).
Number of participants who met criteria for dependence diagnosis for corresponding drug (in addition to methamphetamine) at baseline; note: methamphetamine was confirmed as primary drug of abuse for all participants based on clinical assessment (based on number of dependence criteria and overall lifetime uses).
3.2. Model-based behavioral adjustment
Supporting our model's assumptions (Ide et al., 2013; Shenoy and Yu, 2011), a positive linear relationship between Go RT and P(stop) was observed in all participants (B = 276 ms, t(56) = 2.8,p = .008, model omnibus test: χ2(1) = 127,p < .001; mean Pearson correlation coefficient: r = 0.17). Neither the group main effect on Go RT (χ2(1) = 0.02,p = .90); Mean RT: Relapsed = 579 ms; Abstinent = 572 ms), nor the group by P(stop) interaction (χ2(1) = 1.8,p = .18), were statistically significant (see Fig. 1B).
As expected, participants had a higher likelihood of error on trials with longer stop signal delay (χ2(1) = 106,p < .001). However, groups did not significantly differ in their average stop error rates (Group main effect: χ2(1) = 0.20,p = .66; Mean Error Rate = 0. 49). The Group by SSD interaction was not statistically significant, but a trend was observed in the direction of steeper decline in accuracy for shorter SSD among relapsed individuals (χ2(1) = 3.3,p = .07). Importantly, as predicted by our ideal observer model (Ide et al., 2013; Shenoy et al., 2011; Shenoy and Yu, 2011) and the observed RT adjustment, we found a negative relationship between error likelihood and P(stop), with higher P(stop) prompting a smaller likelihood of error (odds ratio = 0.48, Wald z = −3.04,p < .05; omnibus test: χ2(1) = 9.2, p < .05). There was no statistically significant group x P(stop) interaction (χ2(1) = 0.03,p = .85).
3.3. fMRI analyses
3.3.1. Bayesian prediction of inhibitory response (P(stop))
Testing for any group differences in brain activation associated with P(stop) irrespective of trial type, we found one area consistent with such neural pattern in the left the left temporoparietal junction including the angular gyrus (Brodmann Area 39; Volume = 23 voxels/1472 μL; Peak Voxel Coordinates:-40,-66,22; z = 3.49, p = .0005; see Fig. 2A). Specifically, statistically significantly positive P(stop) activations on both Go and Stop trials was observed in abstinent participants, whereas P(stop) activations were weaker and not different from zero in relapsed individuals (see Fig. 2B). Moreover, and consistent with this neural pattern, a group difference in SPE activation was observed based on ROI analyses with the 2nd GLM. That is, average %SC to SPE was significantly different from zero in self-reported abstinent (p < .05) but not relapsed individuals (p > .05; Mean Difference Cohen's d = 0.91; see Fig. 2C).
3.3.2. Modulation of P(stop) by action trial type (stop vs go)
Testing for any significant interaction between clinical status (abstinent vs relapsed) and P(stop) modulated trial type [StopxP(stop) vs GoxP(stop)] across all trials, we found no areas consistent with such neural pattern.
3.3.3. Modulation of P(stop) by stop trial accuracy (SS vs SE)
Activation in two neural regions were associated with a significant interaction between clinical status and P(stop) modulated stop accuracy (SS vs SE trials). In a first region, identified in the left IFG (Brodmann Area 45; Volume = 15 voxels/960 μL; Peak Voxel Coordinates: −44,21,23; z = 3.19, p = .0001; see Fig. 3A), abstinent participants exhibited a significant positive activation to P(stop) on successful stop (SS) trials (p < .05), but no significant activation or deactivation with P(stop) on stop error (SE) trials (p > .05). In contrast, relapsed individuals showed a significant positive activation to P(stop) on SE trials (p < .05) but not P(stop) activation was observed on SS trials (p > .05; see Fig. 3B). Importantly, consistent with the positive activation to P(stop) on stop trials, this was reflected by percent signal change to a Bayesian UPE. Specifically, on successful stop trials (SS), abstinent individuals showed a significant deactivation to UPE (p < .05), which was not significantly difference from zero in relapsed individuals (p > .05; Group Difference Cohen's d = 0.57, p = .03). In contrast, on SE trials, the opposite pattern was seen in that the relapsed group showed a significant deactivation to UPE (p < .05), which was not significant in abstinent individuals (p > .05; Group Difference Cohen's d = 0.72, p = .02; see Fig. 3C).
In the second region, identified in the left anterior insula (Brodmann Area 13; Volume = 15 voxels/960 μL; Peak Voxel Coordinates: −28,28,4; z = 3.70, p = .0002; see Fig. 3D), a similar pattern was observed. The abstinent group exhibited a significant positive activation to P(stop) on successful stop (SS) trials (p < .05), but no significant activation or deactivation with P(stop) on stop error (SE) trials (p > .05). In contrast, relapsed participants showed a significant positive activation to P(stop) on SE trials (p < .05) but not on SS trials (p > .05; see Fig. 3E). Again, this was reflected by the group difference in percent signal change to a Bayesian UPE. Specifically, on successful stop trials (SS), abstinent individuals showed a significant deactivation to UPE (p < .05), which was not significantly difference from zero in relapsed individuals (p > .05; Group Difference Cohen's d = 0.63, p = .02). In contrast, on SE trials, the relapsed group showed a significant deactivation to UPE (p = .05), which was not significant in abstinent individuals (p > .05; Group Difference was not significant: Cohen's d = 0.31, p = .26; see Fig. 3C).
3.3.4. Non-computational task regressors
We conducted two similar LME analyses with the categorical regressors (Go, SS, SE) to assess whether clinical groups differed in their neural responses to trial type after regressing out any variance correlated with the P(stop)-modulated predictors. We specifically looked at the interaction of clinical status with the Go - Stop contrast (SS and SE averaged), as well as with the SS – SE contrast. We found not regions consistent with either a significant group by Go-Stop contrast, or a significant group by SS-SE contrast.
4. Discussion
We aimed to identify neural markers of relapse to methamphetamine use, combining behavioral, neuroimaging, and computational approaches. We used a Bayesian ideal observer model to infer expectations of inhibitory response in a stop-signal task among fifty-eight methamphetamine-dependent individuals. We found that, relative to those MDI who remained abstinent, relapsed individuals failed to exhibit a deactivation to a signed prediction error (SPE) in the left temporoparietal junction. In addition, while abstinent individuals exhibited significant neural responses to P(stop) and an unsigned prediction error (UPE) on successfully inhibited trials in the left IFG and left anterior insula, relapsed individuals showed abnormalities in the tracking of UPE in those regions, with stronger UPE activation on failed but not successful trials. In contrast to these neural differences, no other baseline predictors, such as reported lifetime drug use or non-model based neural predictors, predicted relapse. These two types of prediction errors (SPE and UPE) carry different yet complementary relevance to behavior adjustment, as discussed below.
The SPE (outcome-P(stop)) is proportional to the degree of Dynamic Belief Model (DBM) belief updating following a given trial outcome, and quantifies how much the directional difference between expecting and observing a stop signal update prior beliefs to modify the internal model (Yu and Cohen, 2009). The fact that individuals with weaker Bayesian SPE activations in the left angular gyrus appear more likely to relapse suggests that these individuals may be less efficient at learning, i.e., updating expectations, in the task. With extensive connections to prefrontal and temporal lobes (Seghier, 2013), the left angular gyrus has been implicated in encoding prediction errors (Gläscher et al., 2010; Spoormaker et al., 2011) and event frequencies within a Bayesian learning context (d'Acremont et al., 2013), highlighting the role of this region in computationally-based learning. The observed negative as opposed to positive correlation between SPE and neural activity in this region highlights the relevance for relapse prediction of tracking this signed discrepancy specifically when observed stop frequency is lower than expectations (negative SPE). That is, because of its specific relevance to inhibitory/stop action tendency following belief adjustment, negative SPE activation may point to a specific vulnerability in flexibly implementing such action in relapsers. Interestingly, there is evidence pointing to structural abnormalities in the superior temporal region (i.e., lower grey matter volumes) as potential biological vulnerability to stimulant addiction (Ersche et al., 2012). Overall, these results are congruent with extensive evidence of difficulties learning and integrating new information to adjust behavioral performance in methamphetamine users (Aron and Paulus, 2007; Paulus et al., 2002), and with recent computational work pointing reduced prefrontal recruitment during model-based decision-making among alcohol relapsers (Sebold et al., 2017).
In contrast, the UPE (|outcome-P(stop)|) represents an overall degree of discrepancy between one's internal model prediction and actual outcome, and can be understood as a “goodness of fit” estimate of one's internal predictive model (Harlé et al., 2014). Larger UPE deactivations, as observed in the left IFG and anterior insula, may thus reflect a stronger tracking of expectancy violation as unexpected changes in the environment statistics are being monitored (Harlé et al., 2014; Yu and Dayan, 2005). In both left IFG and left anterior insula, while abstinent individuals exhibited significant deactivations UPE, relapsed participants failed to show neural tracking of UPE when successfully inhibiting a motor response, but they instead exhibited such neural responses when failing to inhibit. This could suggest two things. First, relative to individuals who remained abstinent, relapsed individuals may be less efficient at maintaining internal monitoring of unexpected uncertainty in the environment while preforming well on the task. Second, failure to successfully inhibit motor response may more specifically prompt neural tracking of uncertainty in those regions among relapsed individuals, which could point to a more selective tracking of uncertainty and higher attentional threshold to trigger strategic shifts and adapt behavior in this subset of individuals.
Indeed, several studies suggest that, while the right IFG is more robustly recruited during inhibitory control, the left IFG is also critical to response inhibition in SST and Go-NoGo tasks (Hampshire et al., 2010; Swick et al., 2011). Consistent with greater experienced expectancy violation, the left IFG has been shown to encode prediction errors (Bossaerts 2010b; d'Acremont et al., 2009), including those associated with Bayesian posterior probabilities (d'Acremont et al., 2013), and with the ability to “switch” strategy when no longer task-relevant (Aron et al., 2004; Yeung et al., 2006). Similarly, prediction error and expectancy violation signals in reinforcement learning paradigms have been robustly observed in the insular cortex (Bossaerts, 2010; Preuschoff et al., 2008), and this region has been implicated in interoception and error-based learning within complex, uncertain environment (Paulus, 2007; Paulus and Stein, 2006; Singer et al., 2009). Again, the observed deactivation vs positive activation pattern, consistent with previous evidence of negative relationships between unsigned prediction errors and activity in cingulate cortex (Hayden et al., 2011; Kennerley et al., 2011; Seo and Lee, 2007) and IFG (Harlé et al., 2014), is congruent with a downstream inhibitory role of UPE activation in this region, which may be relevant to prompt de-engagement from the current behavioral strategy. That is, those individuals prone to relapse show increased tracking of expectancy violation (i.e., appropriateness of the internal belief model guiding behavior), which is critical to guide shifts in attention away from an ineffective prediction strategy, in the face of failure, i.e., in a more reactive manner. In contrast, those more likely to remain abstinent appear to more consistently track this discrepancy between model-based beliefs and actual outcome when they are successfully performing, i.e., in a more pro-active manner. In light of recent computational work suggesting that individuals' decisions are partly guided by minimizing surprise through maximizing entropy over outcomes to “keep their options open”(Schwartenbeck et al., 2015), such belief-based process may be particularly informative/predictive in individuals vulnerable to relapse.
Overall, these neural findings suggest that methamphetamine-dependent individuals more likely to relapse one year after treatment are less efficient at tracking unexpected uncertainty and updating their internal belief model when presented with new information. This highlights the importance of distinguishing belief updating processes from expectancy violation and uncertainty monitoring, with growing computational evidence of distinct neural substrates (Schwartenbeck et al., 2016; Schwartenbeck et al., 2015). Interestingly, this pattern echoes a recent meta-analysis of reward prediction in substance abusers, pointing to weaker anticipation of reward and higher prediction error signals when receiving unexpected rewards (Luijten et al., 2017). The similarity between this meta-analysis and our results further supports a learning impairment hypothesis, i.e. a problem of adapting behavior to a changing environment, generally consistent with a shift from model-based, goal-directed behavior to habitual decision-making associated with inflexible learning (Heinz et al., 2017; Sebold et al., 2017). We note, however, that this study has several limitations, including a small sample size, the fact that relapse/abstinence status was based on self-report, and that alcohol, prescription drugs, and other secondary drugs of abuse were not monitored after treatment. In addition, medical status, hospitalizations, and psychiatric comorbidities were not assessed during the follow-up interval. The latter limits our ability to evaluate the specificity of the neurocognitive markers identified to methamphetamine dependence and the role of psychiatric comorbidities in this predictive model. Future research should include a more in-depth clinical assessment to tease apart those factors. In addition, we acknowledge the group differences in Bayesian model-based neural responses were observed in the absence of statistically significant group difference in behavioral performance. However, the group by SSD interaction contrast predicting accuracy was marginally significant and pointing to more marked decline in performance as SSD shortens (i.e., more challenging) among relapsed individuals. Moreover, this lack of strong behavioral findings does not preclude the relevance of such subtle neural differences to inhibitory learning and/or the worsening of behavioral inhibitory performance as individuals progress towards relapse. It would thus be critical to assess behavioral performance at follow-up in future studies to assess the evolution of neural changes in anticipatory processes as it impacts performance.
In conclusion, the combination of functional neuroimaging and Bayesian cognitive modeling may be helpful in identifying finer neural markers for methamphetamine use disorder relapse, which can provide a mechanistic explanation of more subtle processing deficits. We note, however, that a group model fitting approach was adopted in this study to identify potential neurocognitive predictors of relapse at the individual level, and thus a more in-depth study of clinical prediction utility is warranted. Nonetheless, such generative embedding approach is a first step to help bridge the clinical predictive utility and the scientific requirement of mechanistic insight into cognition, which for instance is not realistically attainable with standard multivariate classification algorithms (Brodersen et al., 2011). Specifically, if robust to replication, and with the hope to refine such neurocomputational test to be more subpopulation- and individual-specific, this cognitive modeling approach may be useful to predict long-term relapse status at the individual level. Such approach may further be able to pinpoint specific predisposing deficits in a) the adequacy of individuals' internal predictive model, and/or b) the efficiency of experience-based learning. Given their relevance to addictive behavior and recent developments in computational psychiatry, such Bayesian computational framework could be further strengthened by considering: the degree of precision/confidence about goal attainment, which may further modulate the influence of expectancies on choice (Rigoli et al., 2017), and the balance between model-free vs model-based/goal-directed control in relation to Pavlovian-instrumental transfer effects (Heinz et al., 2017; Sebold et al., 2017). The observed computational markers may provide new avenues in the prevention of stimulant addiction and the development of personalized treatments. For instance, cognitive training and biofeedback may be used to specifically target the computational learning processes showing evidence of neural inefficiencies in combination with standard addiction rehabilitation programs for those individuals identified at risk of relapse.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Drs. Wesley Thompson and Alan Simmons for their helpful statistical guidance and comments. This study was supported by grants from the Veterans Health Administration (grant# IK2-CX001584); and from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) to Martin Paulus, including grants # R01-DA016663 and #R01-DA018307. The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication. The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Footnotes
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101794.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
References
- Aron A.R. From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;69:e55–e68. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron J.L., Paulus M.P. Location, location: using functional magnetic resonance imaging to pinpoint brain differences relevant to stimulant use. Addiction. 2007;102:33–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01778.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron A.R., Monsell S., Sahakian B.J., Robbins T.W. A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. Brain. 2004;127:1561–1573. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron A.R., Behrens T.E., Smith S., Frank M.J., Poldrack R.A. Triangulating a cognitive control network using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:3743–3752. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0519-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck A.T., Ward C.H., Mendelson M., Mock J., Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behrens T.E.J., Woolrich M.W., Walton M.E., Rushworth M.F.S. Learning the value of information in an uncertain world. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:1214–1221. doi: 10.1038/nn1954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bossaerts P. Risk and risk prediction error signals in anterior insula. Brain Struct. Funct. 2010;214:645–653. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0253-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brecht M.-L., Herbeck D. Time to relapse following treatment for methamphetamine use: a long-term perspective on patterns and predictors. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;139:18–25. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.02.702. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brodersen K.H., Schofield T.M., Leff A.P., Ong C.S., Lomakina E.I., Buhmann J.M., Stephan K.E. Generative embedding for model-based classification of fMRI data. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2011;7 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Claus E.D., Feldstein Ewing S.W., Filbey F.M., Hutchison K.E. Behavioral control in alcohol use disorders: relationships with severity. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs. 2013;74:141–151. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2013.74.141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox R.W. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 1996;29:162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- d'Acremont M., Lu Z.L., Li X., Van der Linden M., Bechara A. Neural correlates of risk prediction error during reinforcement learning in humans. Neuroimage. 2009;47:1929–1939. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- d'Acremont M., Schultz W., Bossaerts P. The human brain encodes event frequencies while forming subjective beliefs. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:10887–10897. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5829-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge R., Sindelar J., Sinha R. The role of depression symptoms in predicting drug abstinence in outpatient substance abuse treatment. J. Subst. Abus. Treat. 2005;28:189–196. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2004.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ersche K.D., Jones P.S., Williams G.B., Turton A.J., Robbins T.W., Bullmore E.T. Abnormal brain structure implicated in stimulant drug addiction. Science. 2012;335:601–604. doi: 10.1126/science.1214463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gläscher J., Daw N., Dayan P., O'Doherty J.P. States versus rewards: dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying model-based and model-free reinforcement learning. Neuron. 2010;66:585–595. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampshire A., Chamberlain S.R., Monti M.M., Duncan J., Owen A.M. The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control. Neuroimage. 2010;50:1313–1319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harlé K.M., Shenoy P., Stewart J.L., Tapert S.F., Yu A.J., Paulus M.P. Altered neural processing of the need to stop in young adults at risk for stimulant dependence. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:4567–4580. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2297-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harlé K.M., Stewart J.L., Zhang S., Tapert S.F., Yu A.J., Paulus M.P. Bayesian neural adjustment of inhibitory control predicts emergence of problem stimulant use. Brain. 2015;138:3413–3426. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harlé K.M., Zhang S., Ma N., Yu A.J., Paulus M.P. Reduced neural recruitment for Bayesian adjustment of inhibitory control in methamphetamine dependence. Biol. Psychiatry. 2016;1:448–459. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayden B.Y., Heilbronner S.R., Pearson J.M., Platt M.L. Surprise signals in anterior cingulate cortex: neuronal encoding of unsigned reward prediction errors driving adjustment in behavior. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:4178–4187. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4652-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinz A., Deserno L., Zimmermann U.S., Smolka M.N., Beck A., Schlagenhauf F. Targeted intervention: computational approaches to elucidate and predict relapse in alcoholism. Neuroimage. 2017;151:33–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hesselbrock M., Easton C., Bucholz K.K., Schuckit M., Hesselbrock V. A validity study of the SSAGA--a comparison with the SCAN. Addiction. 1999;94:1361–1370. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.94913618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hester R., Garavan H. Executive dysfunction in cocaine addiction: evidence for discordant frontal, cingulate, and cerebellar activity. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:11017–11022. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3321-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ide J.S., Shenoy P., Yu A.J., Li C.S. Bayesian prediction and evaluation in the anterior cingulate cortex. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:2039–2047. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2201-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiang J., Beck J., Heller K., Egner T. An insula-frontostriatal network mediates flexible cognitive control by adaptively predicting changing control demands. Nat. Commun. 2015;6:8165. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennerley S.W., Behrens T.E., Wallis J.D. Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:1581–1589. doi: 10.1038/nn.2961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee B., London E.D., Poldrack R.A., Farahi J., Nacca A., Monterosso J.R., Mumford J.A., Bokarius A.V., Dahlbom M., Mukherjee J. Striatal dopamine d2/d3 receptor availability is reduced in methamphetamine dependence and is linked to impulsivity. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:14734–14740. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3765-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- London E.D., Simon S.L., Berman S.M., Mandelkern M.A., Lichtman A.M., Bramen J., Shinn A.K., Miotto K., Learn J., Dong Y. Mood disturbances and regional cerebral metabolic abnormalities in recently abstinent methamphetamine abusers. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2004;61:73. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.1.73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luijten M., Schellekens A.F., Kühn S., Machielse M.W.J., Sescousse G. Disruption of reward processing in addiction: an image-based meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Jama Psychiatry. 2017;74:387–398. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V., Rivera S.M., White C.D., Glover G.H., Reiss A.L. Dissociating prefrontal and parietal cortex activation during arithmetic processing. Neuroimage. 2000;12:357–365. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nestor L.J., Ghahremani D.G., Monterosso J., London E.D. Prefrontal hypoactivation during cognitive control in early abstinent methamphetamine-dependent subjects. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging. 2011;194:287–295. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patton J.H., Stanford M.S. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1995;51:768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus M.P. Decision-making dysfunctions in psychiatry--altered homeostatic processing? Science. 2007;318:602–606. doi: 10.1126/science.1142997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus M.P., Stein M.B. An insular view of anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry. 2006;60:383–387. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus M.P., Hozack N.E., Zauscher B.E., Frank L., Brown G.G., Braff D.L., Schuckit M.A. Behavioral and functional neuroimaging evidence for prefrontal dysfunction in methamphetamine-dependent subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:53–63. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00334-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus M.P., Huys Q.J.M., Maia T.V. A roadmap for the development of applied computational psychiatry. Biol. Psychiatry. 2016;1:386–392. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinheiro J., Bates D., DebRoy S., Sarkar D. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna, Austria: 2011. The R Development Core Team 2011 nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R Package Version 3.1–102.http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/nlme/index.html Available at: [Google Scholar]
- Preuschoff K., Quartz S.R., Bossaerts P. Human insula activation reflects risk prediction errors as well as risk. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:2745–2752. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4286-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rigoli F., Mathys C., Friston K.J., Dolan R.J. A unifying Bayesian account of contextual effects in value-based choice. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2017;13 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartenbeck P., FitzGerald T.H.B., Mathys C., Dolan R., Kronbichler M., Friston K. Evidence for surprise minimization over value maximization in choice behavior. Sci. Rep. 2015;5 doi: 10.1038/srep16575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartenbeck P., FitzGerald T.H.B., Dolan R. Neural signals encoding shifts in beliefs. Neuroimage. 2016;125:578–586. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sebold M., Nebe S., Garbusow M., Guggenmos M., Schad D., Beck A., Kuitunen-Paul S., Sommer C., Frank R., Neu P. When habits are dangerous-alcohol expectancies and habitual decision-making predict relapse in alcohol dependence. Biol. Psychiatry. 2017;82(11):847–856. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seghier M.L. The angular gyrus multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. Neuroscientist. 2013;19:43–61. doi: 10.1177/1073858412440596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seo H., Lee D. Temporal filtering of reward signals in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex during a mixed-strategy game. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:8366–8377. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2369-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shenoy P., Yu A.J. Rational decision-making in inhibitory control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2011;5 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shenoy P., Rao R.P.N., Yu A. A rational decision making framework for inhibitory control. Adv. Neural Inf. Proces. Syst. 2011;23 http://papers.nips.cc/paper/3937-a-rational-decision-making-framework-for-inhibitory-control [Google Scholar]
- Singer T., Critchley H.D., Preuschoff K. A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009;13:334–340. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spoormaker V.I., Andrade K.C., Schröter M.S., Sturm A., Goya-Maldonado R., Sämann P.G., Czisch M. The neural correlates of negative prediction error signaling in human fear conditioning. Neuroimage. 2011;54:2250–2256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swick D., Ashley V., Turken U. Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks. Neuroimage. 2011;56:1655–1665. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uttl B. North American adult Reading test: age norms, reliability, and validity. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2002;24:1123–1137. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.8.1123.8375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeung N., Nystrom L.E., Aronson J.A., Cohen J.D. Between-task competition and cognitive control in task switching. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:1429–1438. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3109-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu A.J., Cohen J.D. Sequential effects: superstition or rational behavior. Adv. Neural Inf. Proces. Syst. 2009;21:1873–1880. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu A.J., Dayan P. Uncertainty, neuromodulation, and attention. Neuron. 2005;46:681–692. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zandbelt B.B., Bloemendaal M., Neggers S.F.W., Kahn R.S., Vink M. Expectations and violations: delineating the neural network of proactive inhibitory control. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013;34:2015–2024. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman M., Link K. Construct validity for the sensation-seeking scale. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1968;32:420. doi: 10.1037/h0026047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.