Abstract
Morality evolved within specific social contexts that are argued to shape moral choices. In turn, moral choices are hypothesized to be affected by body odors as they powerfully convey socially-relevant information. We thus investigated the neural underpinnings of the possible body odors effect on the participants’ decisions. In an fMRI study we presented to healthy individuals 64 moral dilemmas divided in incongruent (real) and congruent (fake) moral dilemmas, using different types of harm (intentional: instrumental dilemmas, or inadvertent: accidental dilemmas). Participants were required to choose deontological or utilitarian actions under the exposure to a neutral fragrance (masker) or body odors concealed by the same masker (masked body odor). Smelling the masked body odor while processing incongruent (not congruent) dilemmas activates the supramarginal gyrus, consistent with an increase in prosocial attitude. When processing accidental (not instrumental) dilemmas, smelling the masked body odor activates the angular gyrus, an area associated with the processing of people’s presence, supporting the hypothesis that body odors enhance the saliency of the social context in moral scenarios. These results suggest that masked body odors can influence moral choices by increasing the emotional experience during the decision process, and further explain how sensory unconscious biases affect human behavior.
Introduction
Moral choices are most often explained as a result of emotional and cognitive processes1–8. However, morality is primarily a social phenomenon, tightly dependent on the social context. In their Relationship Regulation theory (RR), Rai and Fiske9 highlight the role of social context in shaping moral choices and posit that people are led by moral motives to evaluate and guide one’s own and others’ judgments and behaviors, according to moral rules developed within specific social relationships. In other words, people build a particular moral motive allowing to live in a specific social context while moral transgressions are defined as the circumvention of such specific relational prescriptions9. Recent empirical evidence supports this theory: for instance, participants’ moral acceptability of tradeoff scenarios can be affected by unconscious biases, such as intergroup prejudices and stereotypes, and the perception of different social groups influences the neural systems implicated in moral choices10.
Unconscious biases can influence moral decisions based on a variety of stimuli such as attitudes (such as dispositions towards people or places)11, implicit stereotypes (such as judging a person as attractive or unintelligent because is a cheerleader)11 or somatic reactions (such as endocrine release or psychophysical reactions)12. However, this line of research has not yet considered the possibility of evaluating the effects of social information transmitted via sensory subliminal cues, such as odors. Humans transfer socially-relevant information, such as age, gender, health status, sexual availability and personal predispositions, via body (or social) odors13,14. Furthermore, odors – including people’s odors – are everywhere and we do not necessarily realize their presence consciously14,15.
The idea of using olfactory stimuli to investigate moral choices is not entirely new. Landy & Goodwin16 argued how olfactory influences on morality are greater than those mediated by vision, the sense humans mostly rely on. Additionally, Schnall et al.17 demonstrated that the presence of a disgusting odor toughens the judgment on vignettes without moral content. Also, as we have previously shown, the subliminal exposure to a neutral odor can bias moral choices towards options characterized by harm avoidance (deontological options)18. Generally speaking, a harm is justified, and to some extent forgiven, if it comes as the side-effect of a moral action carrying a greater benefit compared to an intentional harm with the same outcome19,20. All in all, odors are able to transfer social information and their effect on moral choices seems to modulate harm avoidance.
To our knowledge, previous studies have only explored the behavioral effects of olfactory contextual stimuli on moral choices17,18. However, whether and how the social context might impact moral decision making when induced via sensory subliminal stimulation, and the neural underpinnings of moral choices under the exposure of masked body odors, are still unknown. A meta-analysis showed that, in absence of odor stimulation, moral (vs. non-moral) choices were found to be associated with increased activations in primarily cognition-related areas (i.e., MTG, left and right middle temporal gyrus; rMFG, right middle frontal gyrus; rIFG, right inferior frontal gyrus) and primarily emotion-related areas (i.e., cingulate gyrus, left precuneus)21. However, the way in which moral dilemmas are formulated modulates the competition between the fast, automatic emotional response and the slow, deliberative cognitive system. As previously shown, instrumental dilemmas (Footbridge-type dilemmas)22 recruit emotion-related brain areas such as medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus, amygdala, and brain areas involved in “theory of mind” such as the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and angular gyrus1,2,4. On the other hand, accidental dilemmas (trolley-type dilemmas) are associated with activations in neural areas involved in working memory and cognitive control, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and inferior parietal lobe1,2,4.
While the impact of the exposure to body odors on the neural underpinnings of moral choices still remains unexplored, we are now aware that processing body vs. common odors23 rely on distinct neural pathways, in line with what occurs when social information is presented through other sensory modalities (e.g., Schupp et al.24 for vision and Belin et al.25 for audition). Processing body odors recruits the occipital cortex, active when either visual stimuli or socially-relevant stimuli are cross-modally presented26, the angular gyrus, responsive to human body related information27 but also involved in social cognition and multisensory integration; and the anterior and posterior cingulate cortex, previously found implicated in emotion regulation28,29 and self-reflective processes30. What still remains to be clarified is whether these regions are also involved in the perception of human body odors during a concurrent cognitively demanding task (such as making moral decisions). If this were the case, we would expect a reduction in the activation of OFC or of the higher order areas described (e.g., posterior cingulate cortex) as a result of a reduced attention for sensory analysis, in line with the reduced activation of amygdala31 or piriform cortex32,33 observed when complex judgments are performed during odor perception.
In the present work, we hereby tested whether and how introducing a social context through masked body odors impact the behavioral and the neural correlates of moral choices. The aims of the study were the following: (1) to test whether subliminally presented body odors have a selective effect on incongruent moral dilemmas (real dilemmas) or generalize to different types of decision-making scenarios (congruent or fake dilemmas); and (2) to investigate whether and how body odors impact harm avoidance decisions. In the present functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, participants were asked to answer to 64 moral dilemmas during the presentation of a fragrance neutral in pleasantness (masker) or to a body odor concealed by the same fragrance (masked body odor). The main dependent variable was the type of moral choice made, which could be utilitarian, if participants decided to execute harmful actions in order to save people, or deontological, if participants decided not to cause harm to not violate societal norms, even if the harm is meant for a greater good2,19. To explore whether the effect of the masked body odor is modulated by the dilemmatic nature of the presented scenario, half of the dilemmas were congruent, meaning that cognitive and emotional processes converged towards the same deontological action so that they were fake dilemmas, and half were incongruent dilemmas in which the two processes diverged, so they were real dilemmas34. Moreover, to clarify the modulation of the type of harm, half of the dilemmas were instrumental (dilemmas in which the harm is deliberate) and the other half were accidental (dilemmas in which the harm is a side effect).
We hypothesized that the presence of body odor would induce the participants to perceive the people involved in the scenario as more concrete, real. If that were the case, then participants are expected to be more prone to follow societal norms not to harm people. We anticipated this effect to be stronger than the increase of deontological answers shown when a neutral odor is presented18. Since it has been shown that when dealing with incongruent (compared to congruent) dilemmas, individuals were found to be more willing to provide utilitarian answers34, we expected such trend to be reduced in the presence of the masked body odor. Moreover, as in a previous study18 we observed that the presence of a neutral odor increases the number of deontological answers specifically for instrumental dilemmas, here we expected the presence of the masked body odor to result in an increment of deontological answers for such dilemmas.
With respect to the neural underpinnings, we hypothesized that the processing of incongruent (compared to the congruent) dilemmas would be associated with brain regions commonly implicated in this type of task, such as the amygdala, the ventro-medial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC1,35, the temporo-parietal junction36 and the precuneus21. Additionally, we predict that the presence of the body odor would favor activations in areas commonly associated with social information, including body odor processing, such as the angular gyrus, occipital cortex, and the anterior and posterior cingulate cortex14,23. We further hypothesized that when processing dilemmas that describe intentional harm, emotional brain areas, such as the cingulate gyrus or precuneus, would be more strongly activated. We expected that these emotional areas would be more strongly activated in the presence of the masked body odor, even when processing accidental dilemmas, usually associated with cognitive neural areas. However, given the innovative nature of this research, we had no clear predictions as to the specific neural areas to be recruited, and we therefore explore whole-brain activations with respect to this contrast.
Materials and Methods
Donors
Ten healthy, heterosexual males donated their body odors in two different days (age: 26.3 ± 3.6 years old (mean ± SD); range = 20–31). Male donors were chosen based on the greater intensity of their body odor axillary secretions37. The donors reported: (i) to be non-smokers38; (ii) not to have health issues or to undergo drug treatment known to be related to olfactory alterations; (iii) to have an age ranging from 18 to 35 years old. Informed written consent was obtained from each donor. Each donor agreed to follow behavioral, nutritional (i.e., no alcohol, smoking, food altering the natural body odor) and hygiene instructions throughout the collection session (adapted from)14. The medium of body odor collection was a t-shirt, previously washed with an odorless detergent (Liquid Detergent ECOR with no Perfume and essential oils, ECOR 27094). T-shirts were worn by donors for 12 consecutive hours during the day, right after having taken a shower using fragrance-free body wash and having dried themselves with towels washed with the same odor-free detergent used to pre-wash the t-shirts. Donors collected their body odors on separate t-shirts for each day of collection for a total of two days. Odorless plastic bags were provided to each donor to store each of their t-shirts before bringing them to the lab, the day after each collection period23,39. Samples were perceptually evaluated for odor contamination (e.g., alcohol, smoke, fragrance, food) and for body odor detectability by one to three trained experimenters. All samples were then stored in a −80 °C freezer to prevent sample deterioration40.
Participants
The original group of participants was composed of 30 women. The rationale for testing only women is based on the evidence that women show a greater preference for social emotional stimuli41, also when presented in olfactory form42. The participants followed the same criteria as the donors, and additionally, they had to score at the 16-item Sniffin’ Sticks Identification subtest of the Sniffin’ Sticks Extended test above 1043 as well as presenting a regular menstrual cycle44.
No depression or heightened sensitivity to disgust (Disgust Scale)45 was revealed. Two participants were removed from the study because of possible clinical problems. The final sample included 28 healthy, heterosexual, right-handed women aged between 19 and 34 (23.7 ± 4.2 years), who were normosmic (TDI score: 13.4 ± 1.5, range = 11–16), and whose STAI state score before the task was within the normal range (STAI state score: 33.7 ± 4.3, range = 24–42). Participants were instructed to not eat or drink anything but water one hour prior to testing, and to not wear any scented products on the day of testing. The SISSA Ethics Committee approved the study, which is in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and an informed written consent was obtained from each participant.
General procedure
At the beginning of the experiment, participants were seated in a quiet room and they were instructed about the experiment. Then participants performed the odor identification test43 and they completed the State questionnaire of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-S)46. Anxiety state data were collected because previous literature has shown that moral choices are modulated by individual variability in anxiety18,47,48. To test whether the masking procedure supposed to cover the body odor produced the expected perceptual impact to the same extent across olfactory conditions, participants were asked to rate intensity, pleasantness and familiarity of the masker, masked body odor and clean air before and after the moral decision-making task. The three tasks were all performed inside the scanner in order to override the possible confounding effects of the MRI scanner setting. The procedure of the odor-rating task and of the moral decision-making task was similar to the one applied in previous study18 (see Supplemental Information for details about the two tasks). Then, outside the scanner, participants completed again the STAI State questionnaire44. See Fig. 1 for an overview of the experimental procedure.
Odor stimuli
Two odor conditions were presented within participants. One set of dilemma alternatives (N = 32) was presented during the exposure to an emotionally neutral, rather unfamiliar odor (aka, masker odor; 200 μL of cedarwood oil, Sigma-Aldrich), as determined via pilot studies (see Supplementary Information of49 for detailed descriptions of the odor pilots) and as confirmed by previous study18. The masker odor was applied to equally-sized quadrants of cotton white t-shirt previously washed with the same detergent used for the t-shirts worn by the donors. The second set of dilemmas alternatives (N = 32) was presented during the exposure to the masked body odor. The masked body odor was prepared by including in a glass jar four donated t-shirt quadrants (supradonor) chosen from all those collected from the 10 donors and one clean t-shirt quadrant on which we applied 200 μL of masker odor50. The masking procedure was used to simulate the hygiene products usually used with the goal of making the paradigm more ecologically valid51. As customary in human body odor research14,52–55, each recipient smelled one supradonor stimulus across all dilemma trials, but in order to reduce the stimuli similarity52, the combination varied in terms of the axilla the sample came from and the day at which it was collected. The order of the two odor conditions presentations was randomized across subjects and across the four blocks of the moral decision-making task.
Odors were presented bi-rhinally in a temporally-precise, square-shaped manner using a computer-automated olfactometer56. A low bi-rhinal flow rate of 1.0 L/m (a total of 0.5 L/m per nostril) was used to prevent irritation of the nasal mucosa over time56,57. Odor stimuli were delivered directly to both participants’ nostrils from a nasal manifold, attached to the participant’s chest by means of a chest strap, connected to the olfactometer via Teflon tubing.
Odor rating task
A green fixation cross lasting for 0.5 s preceded each odor presentation. The odor presentation lasted for 4.0 s and was accompanied by a black screen. Subsequently, a white screen was presented for 6.0 ± 0.1 s (mean ± SD) during which participants were asked in succession and in a random order to answer the following questions: “How intense was the odor you just smelled?”, “How pleasant was the odor you just smelled?”, and “How familiar was the odor you just smelled?”. During question presentation, clean air was released to minimize odor residuals56. Perceptual ratings for odor intensity, pleasantness, and familiarity were collected on a 10-cm computerized Visual Analogue Scale (VAS), ranging from “not at all” to “very much”. Participants were instructed to answer even if they did not perceive any odor. The odor rating task was performed inside the scanner to reduce the time of the experimental session, but without collecting functional MRI data.
Moral decision-making task
The 4CONFiDE moral set described in Cecchetto et al.20 was reshaped for this study to include congruent and incongruent dilemmas. A total of 64 dilemmas was presented, 32 congruent and 32 incongruent. Furthermore, half of the congruent and incongruent dilemmas were accidental and the remaining instrumental. Each dilemma type was presented in 16 alternative versions to allow for the presentation of the same factor combination in both odor conditions. The order of presentation of the dilemmas was randomized across participants to exclude any presentation order effects on moral decision-making (see Fig. S1 in the Supplementary Information for a visualization of the features of the dilemma set).
Each dilemma was presented on two subsequent screens. The first screen described the scenario, in which a danger threatens to kill a group of persons, and a hypothetical action would save these people but cause the death of another person. The second screen presented the question Do you…[action verb] so that…? Participants had to choose between four options: “I certainly do it”, “I do it”, “I do not do it”, and “I certainly do not do it”. The first two choices are held to be utilitarian, as they maximize overall utility (i.e., saving more lives), whereas the latter two were non-utilitarian (deontological).
Before starting the moral decision-making task, participants performed two practice trials. An Italian version of the instructions suggested by Christensen et al.19 and previously used in Cecchetto et al.20 was administered.
See Fig. 1 for an overview of the moral decision-making task. Each trial began with a black cross that was displayed for 5.0 ± 0.3 s. Then, a green cross was presented for 1.2 ± 0.2 s and the odor delivery started. Subsequently, the scenario was presented for 22.0 s. The scenario presentation was combined with the odor presentation. Afterwards, the question slide was presented together with the releasing of clean air to minimize odor residuals56. The four choices were displayed below the question. Participants had maximum 5.0 s to answer. After the answer a black cross was presented for 5.0 s.
The 64 dilemmas were divided into four blocks that corresponded to four scanning runs. During each block, 16 trials balanced for moral dilemmas types and odor conditions, were presented in randomized order. Participants were allowed to take a short break at the end of each run while lying in the scanner. Dilemmas were presented using a black font color (font: Calibri, size: 24) against a white background. Stimulus presentation was delivered with E-prime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA).
Behavioral data analysis
Frequency analysis was performed on the four response options to see whether the number of each option changed based on the odor condition. Since no significant differences were found among the four response options in relation to odour condition, we collapsed them for the subsequent analyses.
Behavioral data were analyzed with linear mixed-effects models (LMMs)58 using R (version 2.10.1; http://www.r-project.org/) and in particular using the lme function (nlme package; https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/nlme/nlme.pdf) for continuous variables and the glmer function (lme4 package; http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html) for binary variables (deontological or utilitarian answer). To account for individual differences (e.g., some people are more “deontological” than others), participants were included in the models as random factors. To avoid a warning of non-convergence, an optimizer (bobyqa) was applied59. Results with and without the optimizer are not significantly different (https://github.com/lme4/lme4/blob/master/misc/notes/release_notes.md). Estimates on the choice between utilitarian and deontological responses were based on an adaptive Gaussian Hermite approximation of the likelihood with 10 integration points. For odor ratings, models with odor and session were tested. For moral choice, two models were performed: the first included odor, as the main variable of interest of our analysis, and congruency. The second model included odor and intentionality and it was performed considering only incongruent dilemmas.
Outliers in reaction times were determined by means of the outliers-labelling rule60. From a sample of 1792, 127 trials were removed for no response (N = 127/1792, 7.08%), and 43 trials were removed because of extremely long choice reaction times (>2.6 s; N = 43/1665, 2.58%; mean of reaction times is 820.8 ± 507.2 s). Conditions have equivalent final samples of trials (Masker odor = 810, Masked Body odor = 812; X21 = 0.0025, p = 0.96).
MRI data acquisition and pre-processing
A 3 Tesla Philips Achieva whole-body MR Scanner at the University Hospital of Udine (ASUI Udine, Italy), equipped with an 8-channel head coil, was used for MRI scanning. Head movement was minimized through cushioning within the coil. Functional volumes were obtained using a whole-head T2*-weighted echoplanar image (EPI) sequence (repetition time [TR] = 2.5 s, echo time = 35 ms, flip angle = 90°, 28 transverse axial slices with interleaved acquisition, 3.50 × 3.59 × 4.00 mm3 voxel resolution, field of view = 230 × 230 mm2, acquisition matrix = 68 × 62, SENSE factors: 2 in the anterior–posterior direction). The number of volumes acquired varied for each participant and run given the task duration based on participants’ reaction times (mean number of volumes per run = 260 ± 4.6, range = 153–270). Anatomical images were acquired during the final odor rating task as 180 T1-weighted images (0.75 mm slice thickness). Stimuli were viewed through VisuaStim Goggles system (Resonance Technology) mounted to the head coil, which was adjusted on each participant’s vision. Responses were made and recorded through one MR-compatible response pads (Lumitouch, Lightwave Medical Industries, Coldswitch technologies, Richmond, CA) using the right hand. To minimize influences of breathing effects, participants were instructed and trained to maintain a constant and normal breathing rate. Due to technical problems, images from the first session of one participant and the second session of another participant were removed from the analysis.
Data were analyzed with SPM12 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK). All functional volumes were spatially realigned to the first volume, slice- time corrected, segmented in gray matter, white matter and cerebrospinal fluid tissues, spatially normalized to the standard EPI template, and smoothed using a Gaussian kernel with full width at half maximum (FWHM) of 8 mm3. Movement-related variance was analyzed using the Art toolbox (www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect). For each run, outlier scans were identified based on the TR-to-TR composite motion more than 2 mm and/or considering whether the scan-to-scan global BOLD signal normalized to z-scores deviated from mean more than z = 3. The time-points identified as outliers were regressed out as separate nuisance covariates in the first-level design matrix. All participants displayed a percentage of outlier scan inferior to the cutoff (25%), therefore no one was excluded from the analyses and all trials were retained.
fMRI data analysis
Two separated fMRI data analyses were carried out: in the first analysis, odor conditions and congruency of dilemmas were considered to explore whether the effect of masked body odor was modulated by the dilemmatic nature of the presented scenario; in the second analysis, which was performed only on incongruent dilemmas, odor conditions and intentionality as the type of dilemmas were considered to investigate the effects of masker body odor on the processing of different types of harm.
Statistical analyses were performed using a general linear model (GLM) approach. In the first-level analysis, data were analyzed separately for each participant. In each trial, four events were modelled: the presentation of clean air, of an odor, of the scenario combined with an odor and, of the slide including the question. The duration of each of these events was set to 0 except for the scenario presentation, which was set to a fixed time of 22.0 s. The combination of these four event types with dilemma congruency (congruent vs incongruent) or dilemma intentionality (accidental vs instrumental) and the two odor conditions (masker vs masked body odor) led to a total of 16 regressors for each run. The six motion parameters were also included as regressors of no interest in the design matrix. All regressors were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function. Low-frequency signal drifts were filtered using a cutoff period of 128.0 s. As a next step, at the individual level, contrast parameters were estimated for all the 16 regressors of interest, averaged across the four runs. Subsequently, at the second-level analysis, 4 contrast images of the event scenario presentations from the combination odor with congruency or intentionality of each participant were submitted to a flexible factorial design, with subject as random factor, odor conditions and congruency or intentionality as fixed factors, to assess neural activations of the dilemma processing during the exposure to the odor. Later, the 4 contrast images were entered to linear contrasts of the repeated measure ANOVA with two within-subject factors to investigate main effects and interactions. To identify the neuronal substrates of single odor condition or single dilemma type, simple main effects (i.e., [masker odor – masked body odor] for each odor condition and each dilemma type separately) were analyzed. To investigate whether odor conditions affect neural activity related to moral dilemma processing, we performed a dilemma type (i.e. congruent/incongruent or accidental/instrumental) by odor condition (masker odor/masked body odor) interaction at group level. Moreover, to clarify whether the neural underpinnings involved in the masker body odor effects for one dilemma type (i.e. incongruent or accidental) are shared by the opposing dilemma type (i.e. congruent or instrumental), exclusive and inclusive conjunction analyses were performed between the neural areas recruited for the interactions [odor × congruency or intentionality].
Finally, in order to investigate the relationship between brain activations and moral choices, the mean beta values of the activated clusters were extracted using the REX toolbox (Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, MA) and simple correlation analyses were performed with the percentage of utilitarian responses.
Whole-brain analyses were thresholded at p < 0.05, family-wise error (FWE) cluster-level corrected for multiple comparisons across the whole brain. The AAL2 toolbox61,62 was used to guide the labelling of the activated clusters.
Results
Masked body odor and masker are perceptually similar
We first tested whether the masking procedure applied to cover the masked body odor had the expected perceptual impact and rendered the olfactory conditions equivalent in their basic perceptual dimensions. The LMM on intensity ratings (clean air: 2.45 ± 0.13 points; masker: 5.83 ± 0.18 points; masked body odor: 6.10 ± 0.16 points; see Fig. 2A and Table 1) revealed that both the masker and the masked body odor were perceived as significantly more intense than clean air (p < 0.001; reference factor: clean air), but no significant difference was found between the masker and the masked body odor (p = 0.48; reference factor: masker). A difference emerged when looking at the effect of session (pre moral decision-making task: 5.23 ± 0.21 points; post moral decision-making task: 4.56 ± 0.20 points; p = 0.024): odors were rated as less intense during the second session compared to the first session suggesting that participants might have adapted during the task (Dalton, 2000).
Table 1.
Intensity Ratings | β | SE | t value | p value | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fixed effects | Lower | Upper | ||||
Intercept | 6.18 | 0.32 | 19.20 | <0.001 | 5.546 | 6.807 |
Clean Air | −3.31 | 0.35 | −9.28 | <0.001 | −4.016 | −2.615 |
Masked body odor | 0.24 | 0.34 | 0.70 | 0.484 | −0.432 | 0.914 |
Session (Post) | −0.66 | 0.29 | −2.28 | 0.024 | −1.233 | −0.093 |
Familiarity Ratings | ||||||
Fixed effects | ||||||
Intercept | 5.76 | 0.37 | 15.61 | <0.001 | 5.035 | 6.480 |
Clean Air | −1.85 | 0.33 | −5.61 | <0.001 | −2.502 | −1.207 |
Masked body odor | 0.27 | 0.33 | 0.83 | 0.410 | −0.370 | 0.910 |
Session (Post) | −0.27 | 0.27 | −0.99 | 0.323 | −0.814 | 0.267 |
Pleasantness Ratings | ||||||
Fixed effects | ||||||
Intercept | 5.05 | 0.32 | 15.55 | <0.001 | 4.416 | 5.689 |
Clean Air | −0.47 | 0.31 | −1.51 | 0.132 | −1.079 | 0.138 |
Masked body odor | −0.38 | 0.30 | −1.26 | 0.210 | −0.976 | 0.212 |
Session (Post) | −0.20 | 0.25 | −0.78 | 0.436 | −0.695 | 0.298 |
Note: β = estimate; SE = standard error; 95% CI = confidence interval. Significant p values are reported in bold. Table shows model with masker condition set as reference.
The LMM on familiarity ratings (clean air: 3.78 ± 0.14 points; masker: 5.61 ± 0.20 points; masked body odor: 5.91 ± 0.17 points; see Fig. 2B and Table 1) showed that both the masker and the masked body odor were perceived as significantly more familiar than clean air (p < 0.001; reference factor: clean air), but no significant difference was found between the masker and the masked body odor (p = 0.40; reference factor: masker). No significant differences were found between the ratings performed before and after the task (p = 0.32; reference factor: pre).
The LMM on pleasantness ratings (clean air: 4.48 ± 0.12 points; masker: 4.89 ± 0.16 points; masked body odor: 4.58 ± 0.18 points; Fig. 2C) showed no significant differences across the three odor conditions. Moreover, no significant differences were found between sessions. Please, refer to Table 1 for descriptive data.
State anxiety is increased at the end of the task
A Wilcoxon test (W = 148801, p < 0.0001) determined that participants’ state anxiety was increased at the end of the task (34.36 ± 6.45 points, range = 22–48) as compared to its beginning (33.67 ± 4.33 points, range = 24–42; see Fig. S2 in the Supplementary Information).
Irrespective of odor condition, incongruent dilemmas produce more utilitarian responses
First, the model including odor conditions, congruency and the interaction between them was performed (see Table 2 for descriptive data of single parameters). There was a significant effect of congruency on moral choice: the likelihood of choosing the utilitarian option increased when dilemmas were incongruent (z = 10.08, p < 0.001). In other words, when cognitive and emotional processes diverge (real dilemmas), more utilitarian answers are produced than when cognitive and emotional processes converge (fake dilemmas). No significant effects were found for the main effect of odor condition or for the interaction odor × congruency.
Table 2.
Moral choices | β | SE | z value | p value | βexp | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fixed effects | Lower | Upper | |||||
Intercept | −1.19 | 0.18 | −6.37 | <0.001 | 0.302 | 0.209 | 0.436 |
Masked body odor | 0.25 | 0.16 | 1.50 | 0.13 | 1.282 | 0.926 | 1.775 |
Incongruency (Incongruent) | 1.65 | 0.16 | 10.08 | <0.001 | 5.223 | 3.788 | 7.202 |
Masked body odor * Incongruency (Incongruent) | −0.32 | 0.22 | −1.42 | 0.15 | 0.726 | 0.466 | 1.129 |
Note: β = estimate; SE = standard error; β exp = exponential of β coefficient; 95% CI = confidence interval. Significant p values are reported in bold. Table shows model with deontological choice and masker odor condition set as references. The contrast condition from the reference for categorical factors is reported in italic in brackets.
Irrespective of odor condition, accidental dilemmas produce more utilitarian responses
Considering the results of the previous model, we tested the effect of odor conditions, intentionality and the interaction between these two factors on incongruent dilemmas only (see Table 3 for descriptive data of single parameters). A significant effect of intentionality emerged (z = −0.43, p < 0.001): in incongruent dilemmas, the likelihood of choosing the utilitarian option increased when dilemmas were accidental (vs instrumental). The odor condition, alone or in interaction, did not affect the type of moral choice made.
Table 3.
Moral choices | β | SE | z value | p value | βexp | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fixed effects | Lower | Upper | |||||
Intercept | 0.004 | 0.17 | 0.02 | 0.98 | 1.003 | 0.717 | 1.405 |
Masked body odor | 0.24 | 0.15 | 1.58 | 0.11 | 1.268 | 0.945 | 1.704 |
Intentionality (Instrumental) | −0.67 | 0.15 | −0.43 | <0.001 | 0.512 | 0.380 | 0.690 |
Masked body odor * Intentionality (Instrumental) | −0.34 | 0.22 | −1.56 | 0.12 | 0.714 | 0.468 | 1.090 |
Note: β = estimate; SE = standard error; β exp = exponential of β coefficient; 95% CI = confidence interval. Significant p values are reported in bold. Table shows model with utilitarian choice and masker odor condition set as references. The contrast condition from the reference for categorical factors is reported in italic in brackets.
fMRI brain activations
Areas involved in moral cognition are selectively activated by incongruent dilemmas
The processing of real dilemmas (contrast incongruent vs congruent dilemmas) revealed activations in the left middle frontal gyrus, left inferior parietal gyrus and bilateral precuneus (see Table 4 and Fig. 3A). The correlation analysis performed between beta values and percentage of utilitarian answers did not show significant results. No significant activations emerged when considering the processing of fake dilemmas (congruent vs incongruent dilemmas).
Table 4.
Brain region | Side | Cluster size | peak MNI coordinates | p (FWE-corr) | T | Z score | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | ||||||
Incongruent >Congruent | ||||||||
Middle frontal gyrus | L | 57 | −27 | 11 | 56 | 0.037 | 4.98 | 4.64 |
Middle frontal gyrus | L | −31 | 21 | 39 | 3.41 | 3.29 | ||
Inferior parietal gyrus | L | 177 | −52 | −42 | 39 | <0.001 | 4.41 | 4.16 |
Inferior parietal gyrus | L | −34 | −46 | 39 | 4.17 | 3.96 | ||
Precuneus | L | 78 | −6 | −67 | 49 | 0.012 | 4.21 | 4 |
Precuneus | L | −13 | −49 | 53 | 3.81 | 3.65 | ||
Precuneus | R | 5 | −60 | 49 | 3.8 | 3.64 | ||
Masked body odor >Masker | ||||||||
Supramarginal gyrus | L | 72 | −59 | −35 | 42 | 0.017 | 5.39 | 4.97 |
Supramarginal gyrus | L | −62 | −32 | 28 | 4.6 | 4.32 | ||
Masker >Masked body odor | ||||||||
Calcarine cortex | L | 259 | −10 | −91 | −11 | <0.001 | 4.37 | 4.13 |
Middle occipital gyrus | L | −17 | −91 | 7 | 4.08 | 3.88 | ||
Calcarine cortex | R | 8 | −88 | 7 | 4.08 | 3.88 | ||
Precuneus | R | 164 | 1 | −53 | 28 | <0.001 | 4.14 | 3.94 |
Lingual gyrus | L | −20 | −53 | 4 | 3.97 | 3.79 | ||
Posterior cingulum | L | −6 | −49 | 32 | 3.81 | 3.65 | ||
Incongruent (masked body odor) >Congruent (masker) | ||||||||
Inferior parietal gyrus | L | 244 | −59 | −35 | 42 | <0.001 | 6.26 | 5.64 |
Inferior parietal gyrus | L | −45 | −46 | 56 | 4.11 | 3.91 | ||
Inferior parietal gyrus | L | −27 | −42 | 35 | 4.03 | 3.83 | ||
Congruent (masker) >Incongruent (masked body odor) | ||||||||
Calcarine cortex | L | 109 | −10 | −95 | −7 | 0.001 | 4.11 | 3.91 |
Calcarine cortex | R | 5 | −91 | 0 | 4.05 | 3.86 | ||
Calcarine cortex | L | −6 | −95 | 7 | 3.91 | 3.73 | ||
Incongruent (masked body odor >masker) >Congruent (masked body odor >masker) | ||||||||
Supramarginal gyrus | L | 233 | −59 | −35 | 42 | <0.001 | 6.26 | 5.64 |
Supramarginal gyrus | L | −45 | −46 | 56 | 4.11 | 3.91 |
Note: Anatomical labels follow the nomenclature of the Automated Anatomical Labelling (AAL2). Peak locations are expressed in MNI coordinates. Voxelwise threshold, p < 0.001. FWE cluster level corrected p < 0.05.
Activation in visual areas tracks the utilitarian responses to incongruent dilemmas when exposed to the masker odor only
The presence of the masked body odor (vs the masker odor) during the presentation of both incongruent and congruent dilemmas was accompanied by activations in the left supramarginal gyrus (see Table 4 and Fig. 3B). In contrast, the presence of the masker odor (vs the masked body odor) activates the bilateral calcarine cortex, the left middle occipital gyrus, the right precuneus, the left lingual gyrus and the left posterior cingulum (see Table 4). The beta values extracted from the cluster including the bilateral calcarine cortex and the left middle occipital gyrus significantly correlate with the number of utilitarian responses to incongruent dilemmas when exposed to the masker odor (r = 0.48, p = 0.009). No other significant correlation between behavioral responses and neural activations emerged.
The masked body odor during incongruent dilemmas is associated with activations in the left supramarginal gyrus
To identify whether the brain regions that are active components in the masked body odor effect for the incongruent dilemmas are shared also for the masked body odor effect in congruent dilemmas, exclusion and inclusion conjunction analyses were performed between the areas recruited for the interaction “masked body odor and incongruent dilemmas” and for the interaction “masked body odor and congruent dilemmas”. The exclusion conjunction analysis for [incongruent (masked body odor >masker) >congruent dilemmas (masked body odor >masker)] showed that the left supramarginal gyrus was significantly recruited only when the masked body odor was presented during the processing of incongruent dilemmas (see Table 4 and Fig. 3C). No significant correlations were found between the extracted beta values and percentage of utilitarian answers in this contrast. The inclusion conjunction analysis and the opposite exclusion conjunction analysis did not reveal any significant results.
Emotional areas are involved in instrumental vs accidental incongruent dilemmas
To evaluate the effect of intentionality, only incongruent dilemmas were considered. Processing accidental (vs instrumental) dilemmas significantly activated the left lingual gyrus, left fusiform gyrus, the left inferior occipital gyrus and the left middle occipital gyrus (see Table 5 and Fig. 4A), whereas processing instrumental (vs accidental dilemmas) was related to significant activation in the bilateral precuneus (see Table 5 and Fig. 4B). No significant correlations with behavioral responses were retrieved.
Table 5.
Brain region | Side | Cluster size | peak MNI coordinates | p (FWE-corr) | T | Z score | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | ||||||
Accidental >Instrumental | ||||||||
Lingual gyrus | L | 165 | −10 | −81 | −4 | <0.001 | 4.74 | 4.44 |
Fusiform gyrus | L | −24 | −74 | −7 | 3.95 | 3.77 | ||
Inferior occipital gyrus | L | −24 | −95 | −11 | 3.68 | 3.53 | ||
Middle occipital gyrus | L | 163 | −27 | −70 | 32 | <0.001 | 4.57 | 4.3 |
Middle occipital gyrus | L | −34 | −88 | 14 | 4.29 | 4.07 | ||
Middle occipital gyrus | L | −27 | −88 | 25 | 4.27 | 4.05 | ||
Instrumental >Accidental | ||||||||
Precuneus | L | 88 | −10 | −53 | 32 | 0.006 | 5 | 4.66 |
Precuneus | R | 5 | −56 | 32 | 3.92 | 3.74 | ||
Accidental (masker odor) >Instrumental (masked body odor) | ||||||||
Calcarine | L | 1252 | −6 | −88 | 7 | <0.001 | 5.43 | 5.01 |
Lingual gyrus | L | −6 | −84 | −7 | 5.36 | 4.94 | ||
Calcarine | L | −20 | −63 | 7 | 5.16 | 4.79 | ||
Accidental (masked body odor) >Instrumental (masker odor) | ||||||||
superior parietal gyrus | L | 141 | −24 | −63 | 46 | <0.001 | 4.52 | 4.25 |
Inferior parietal gyrus | L | −34 | −53 | 46 | 4.03 | 3.84 | ||
Inferior parietal gyrus | L | −45 | −42 | 42 | 3.81 | 3.65 | ||
Angular gyrus | R | 56 | 33 | −67 | 46 | 0.032 | 4.22 | 4 |
Instrumental (masker odor) >Accidental (masked body odor) | ||||||||
Precuneus | L | 257 | −10 | −53 | 32 | <0.001 | 7.1 | 6.25 |
Precuneus | R | 1 | −53 | 28 | 6.46 | 5.79 | ||
Angular gyrus | L | 106 | −48 | −63 | 32 | 0.002 | 5.09 | 4.73 |
Accidental (masked body odor >masker) >Instrumental (masked body odor >masker) | ||||||||
Superior parietal gyrus | L | 140 | −24 | −63 | 46 | 0.001 | 4.52 | 4.25 |
Inferior parietal gyrus | L | −34 | −53 | 46 | 4.03 | 3.84 | ||
Inferior parietal gyrus | L | −45 | −42 | 42 | 3.81 | 3.65 | ||
Angular gyrus | R | 56 | 33 | −67 | 46 | 0.032 | 4.22 | 4 |
Note: Anatomical labels follow the nomenclature of the Automated Anatomical Labelling (AAL2). Peak locations are expressed in MNI coordinates. Voxelwise threshold, p < 0.001. FWE cluster level corrected p < 0.05.
The masked body odor during accidental dilemmas is associated with activations in the left parietal and right angular gyri
To clarify whether the brain regions that are active components in the masked body odor effect for the accidental dilemmas are shared also for the masked body odor effect in instrumental dilemmas, conjunction analyses were performed between the neural areas recruited for the interaction “masked body odor and accidental dilemmas” and for the interaction “masked body odor and instrumental dilemmas”. The exclusive conjunction analysis for accidental (masked body odor >masker) >instrumental (masked body odor >masker) showed significant activations in the left superior and inferior parietal gyrus and in the right angular gyrus. The inclusive conjunction analysis and the opposite exclusion conjunction analysis did not reveal any significant results suggesting that the masked body odor modulated only the processing of accidental dilemmas (see Table 5 and Fig. 4C). The correlation analysis between the beta values extracted and the percentage of utilitarian responses did not reveal any significant results.
Discussion
Previous research suggests that moral rules are developed within specific social-relational contexts that, in turn, play a critical role in shaping moral choices9,10. As human body odors are powerful messengers for socially-relevant information63, able to modulate the behavior and neural processing of the receiver13,14,23,39,53–55, we hypothesized that body odors might affect moral choices through the modulation of the perceived social context (i.e., by inducing the perception of the real presence of a person). With this in mind, we asked participants to decide their course of action to moral scenarios while exposed to a neutral fragrance (masker) or to a body odor hidden by the same masker odor (masked body odor). The analysis of the neural correlates revealed that the exposure to the masked body odor: a) modulates the activity in the brain areas involved in the processing of incongruent (real) dilemmas, but not in those involved in the processing of congruent (fake) dilemmas; and b) increases the activations in areas processing sensory and emotional information when incongruent accidental dilemmas are presented.
In our study, we investigated whether masked body odors influence any decision-making task or whether the influence is specific to moral dilemmas, as we had anticipated. The analysis we performed revealed that the masked body odors moderate the neural responses only related to incongruent (but not congruent) dilemmas, increasing the involvement of the left supramarginal gyrus. While presented with a real (incongruent) moral dilemma, the participants immediately experience a negative emotional reaction at the thought of provoking harm: the final decision will be deontological providing that this emotional reaction is sufficiently influential, and that participants have limited time and cognitive resources to make their decision. On the other hand, if participants have enough time, motivation and cognitive resources, they will have the possibility to engage in cognitive deliberation about costs and benefits, in which case the emotional response may be overshadowed, resulting in an utilitarian response to the dilemma2,34. The information of the masked body odor might interfere with this conflict enhancing the neural pathways that promote prosocial behavior64, therefore emphasizing the emotional processing of the sensory information, and facilitate the emergence of deontological responses. This multisensory integration of the social and sensory information provided by the masked body odor, and the emotional information provided by the moral dilemmas involve the left supramarginal gyrus, close to the angular gyrus1, one of the neural areas previously found to be associated with the processing of body odors2,3. The supramarginal gyrus has been often considered as part of the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) - a neural area typically associated with self-awareness and body-related information processing4,5 and, as such, often involved in tasks of theory of mind5,6, empathy for pain7 and in the perception of anxiety body odors8. Importantly, these aspects become relevant when considering a body odor in the context of moral dilemmas. Indeed, the centrality of the left supramarginal gyrus in multisensory integration processes has recently been supported in a study that identifies this brain area as an important node for the olfactory-visual processing9.
Since in the fake (congruent) dilemmas there is no conflict between emotional and cognitive aspects of the decision (i.e., the benefits do not balance the costs), the social information about the presence of a person becomes irrelevant for the decision itself.
Moreover, in the present study, we clarified whether the masked body odor effect is modulated by the type of harm, being it deliberate (instrumental dilemmas) or an inadvertent effect (accidental dilemmas). Previous studies showed that the accidental harm is judged as being more morally acceptable, it receives higher percentage of utilitarian answers, and it engages lower emotional reactions compared to intentional harming4,64. Our results are in line with this literature: instrumental dilemmas presented higher percentage of deontological answers and recruited neural areas involved in emotional processing (e.g., precuneus) when compared to accidental dilemmas. Interestingly, the masked body odor seems to moderate the processing of the accidental dilemmas by enhancing the activation of the angular gyrus, which is usually associated with social cognition, multisensory integration and “theory of mind”27, and the inferior parietal gyrus, which is important for self-other discrimination65. This result seems to support our hypothesis that the presence of a body odor can induce the participant to perceive the social context of the dilemmas as more concrete, as if the odor signaled the presence of a real person, and not just of a hypothetical context. The reason why the masked body odor seems to selectively affect the processing of the accidental and not of the instrumental dilemmas may be due to the higher emotional involvement of the instrumental dilemmas, which prevents the participants to consider the additional emotional information provided by the odor.
The present fMRI data replicate and extend previous findings concerning the neural networks recruited by social odor processing14,15,23. Besides replicating the enrollment of the left supramarginal gyrus, as discussed above, we also showed major activity in the left hemisphere areas. This result, in line with previous studies23,66, supports the hypothesis that olfactory-mediated affective processes are lateralized in the left hemisphere67.
In our study, the body odor was masked by a neutral odor. This masker was applied to simulate the hygiene products usually used to cover the natural body odors we produce and to make the paradigm more ecologically valid51. Additionally, it allowed studying the effects of the body odor when they are unconsciously perceived. As seen in previous work18, odor effects can emerge irrespective of perceiving the presence of an odor; moreover, masking the body odor limited the inter-individual differences in odor intensity and pleasantness. Such differences can significantly affect decisions, as it seemed in the previous cases when intensity and pleasantness differences across odor conditions were evident17,18. Here we succeeded in making these conditions perceptually similar for intensity, pleasantness and familiarity, removing the possible confounding effect of these factors on the differences in the moral decisions.
To our knowledge, this is the first study that tests the effects of masked body odors on the neural underpinning of moral decision-making. The present study has some limitations for which future studies are necessary. First, it was designed around a moral decision-making paradigm based on the presentation of moral dilemmas, which felt dilemmatic as the participants’ anxiety levels raised at the end of the task. The use of this sort of dilemmas has been previously criticized (e.g.)55,64,68: (i) dilemmas are described in lengthily written texts, which increase the time needed by the participants to process each stimulus; (ii) to make dilemmas credible they cannot be repeated; (iii) the conceptual factors cannot be analyzed separately, but have to be intermingled in each dilemma. These aspects reduce the possibility to present large numbers of trials, therefore limiting the power of the study. To overcome these issues, we have used here a standardized, culturally-equivalent moral set, specifically designed for imaging experiments, that shows high consistency across the different dilemmas20. Moreover, to increase the power of our observations, we based the investigation on a theoretically-motivated interest for one conceptual factor (Intentionality). Despite these efforts, the behavioral analysis failed to reveal any significant mean effects of the odor conditions or significant interactions with odor and dilemma congruency or intentionality. One proposed explanation is that the dilemmas were designed to simultaneously assess also other conceptual factors, such as personal force, benefit recipient and evitability. It is for future studies to clarify whether the masked body odor elicits different effects on moral choices when different conceptual factors are considered (see for example)69. Second, the participants’ respiratory patterns were not recorded and incorporated in the fMRI data processing. Although this is common practice in many olfactory neuroimaging studies70,71, we invite future studies to investigate whether the breathing patterns to human body odor can have an impact on the moral decisions made. Third, only one common odor (cedarwood oil) has been used as masker, and the results cannot be generalizable to all common odors. Fourth, future studies should also increase the sample size to allow the comparison of masked body odors effects in women and men and evaluate potential sex-related effects. Lastly, it would be interesting if future investigations would be extended to clinical populations with a deficit in emotion processing, such as patients with lesions in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex72, or non-clinical populations with emotional deficiencies, such as those with a lack of empathy or with high levels of alexithymia73,74, to examine whether the masked body odor effects on moral decision-making can overcome the usual tendency in this population to give higher percentage of utilitarian answers74.
To conclude, the value of these results is highlighted by the consideration that most of the moral decisions, from everyday choices to choices that we are forced to make under unexpected circumstances, are made in the presence of other people. Starting from the theory proposed by Rai and Fiske9, which advanced the hypothesis that actions and outcomes should be considered in the context of specific social relationships, indeed any action - including violence and impure acts - can be perceived as morally acceptable depending on the social relationships it takes place in9, body odors were used as a means for triggering the social context and for making the social norms more salient. Our results indicate that body odors could effectively mediate moral decisions, possibly increasing the emotional experience during the decision process, and this effect is possible even when the perceiver cannot appreciate the presence of the body odors. Moreover, the current results suggest that, as Cikara et al.10 posited, the context in which the decisions are made is relevant for understanding which decision is made.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Luigi Alberto Gozzi for his help in the body odors collection, Carlotta Cogoni and Michele Furlan for their input regarding the neuroimaging analysis. Financial support has been provided by the European Research Council -ERC (Grant Agreement No 682117 BiT-ERC-2015-CoG) to D.B.
Author Contributions
C.C., V.P. and R.I.R. developed the study concept and the study design; C.C., V.P. and E.L. collected the data; C.C., V.P. and D.B. conceptualized data analyses; C.C. performed data analyses under the supervision of D.B. and V.P., C.C and V.P. interpreted the data and drafted the manuscript. E.L., D.B., R.I.R. provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of manuscript submission.
Data Availability
The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-019-41937-0.
References
- 1.Greene JD, Nystrom LE, Engell AD, Darley JM, Cohen JD. The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment. Neuron. 2004;44:389–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Greene JD, Sommerville RB, Nystrom LE, Darley JM, Cohen JD. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science. 2001;293:2105–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1062872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hutcherson, C. A., Montaser-Kouhsari, L., Woodward, J. & Rangel, A. Emotional and Utilitarian Appraisals of Moral Dilemmas Are Encoded in Separate Areas and Integrated in Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. J. Neurosci. 35 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 4.Sarlo M, et al. Temporal Dynamics of Cognitive–Emotional Interplay in Moral Decision-making. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012;24:1018–1029. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Carmona-Perera M, Martí-García C, Pérez-García M, Verdejo-García A. Valence of emotions and moral decision-making: increased pleasantness to pleasant images and decreased unpleasantness to unpleasant images are associated with utilitarian choices in healthy adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:626. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Białek M, De Neys W. Conflict detection during moral decision-making: evidence for deontic reasoners’ utilitarian sensitivity. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2016;28:631–639. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2016.1156118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Greene, J. D. Why are VMPFC patients more utilitarian? A dual-process theory of moral judgment explains. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 322-3; author reply, 323-4 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 8.Van Bavel JJ, FeldmanHall O, Mende-Siedlecki P. The neuroscience of moral cognition: from dual processes to dynamic systems. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2015;6:167–172. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rai TS, Fiske AP. Moral psychology is relationship regulation: Moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychol. Rev. 2011;118:57–75. doi: 10.1037/a0021867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cikara M, Farnsworth RA, Harris LT, Fiske ST. On the wrong side of the trolley track: neural correlates of relative social valuation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2010;5:404–413. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol. Rev. 1995;102:4. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bechara A, Damasio AR. The somatic marker hypothesis: A neural theory of economic decision. Games Econ. Behav. 2005;52:336–372. doi: 10.1016/j.geb.2004.06.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.McClintock MK, et al. Human Body Scents: Conscious Perceptions and Biological Effects. Chem. Senses. 2005;30:i135–i137. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Parma, V. et al. Processing of Human Body Odors. in Springer Handbook of Odor 127–128 (Springer International Publishing), 10.1007/978-3-319-26932-0_51 (2017).
- 15.Lundström JN, Olsson MJ. Functional Neuronal Processing of Human Body Odors. Vitam. Horm. 2010;83:1–23. doi: 10.1016/S0083-6729(10)83001-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Landy JF, Goodwin GP. Does Incidental Disgust Amplify Moral Judgment? A Meta-Analytic Review of Experimental Evidence. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2015;10:518–536. doi: 10.1177/1745691615583128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Schnall S, Haidt J, Clore GL, Jordan AH. Disgust as Embodied Moral Judgment. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2008;34:1096–1109. doi: 10.1177/0146167208317771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Cecchetto C, Rumiati RI, Parma V. Relative contribution of odour intensity and valence to moral decisions. Perception. 2017;46:447–474. doi: 10.1177/0301006616689279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Christensen JF, Flexas A, Calabrese M, Gut NK, Gomila A. Moral judgment reloaded: a moral dilemma validation study. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:607. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Cecchetto C, Rumiati RI, Parma V. Promoting cross-culture research on moral decision-making with standardized, culturally- equivalent dilemmas: Th e 4CONFiDe set. J. Heal. Soc. Sci. 2017;22:173–194. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Garrigan B, Adlam ALR, Langdon PE. The neural correlates of moral decision-making: A systematic review and meta-analysis of moral evaluations and response decision judgements. Brain Cogn. 2016;108:88–97. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lotto L, Manfrinati A, Sarlo M. A New Set of Moral Dilemmas: Norms for Moral Acceptability, Decision Times, and Emotional Salience. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2014;27:57–65. doi: 10.1002/bdm.1782. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lundström JN, Boyle JA, Zatorre RJ, Jones-Gotman M. Functional Neuronal Processing of Body Odors Differs from that of Similar Common Odors. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:1466–1474. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Schupp HT, et al. The Facilitated Processing of Threatening Faces: An ERP Analysis. Emotion. 2004;4:189–200. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Belin P, Fecteau S, Bédard C. Thinking the voice: neural correlates of voice perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004;8:129–135. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000;4:223–232. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01482-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Seghier ML. The Angular Gyrus. Neurosci. 2013;19:43–61. doi: 10.1177/1073858412440596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cato MA, et al. Processing Words with Emotional Connotation: An fMRI Study of Time Course and Laterality in Rostral Frontal and Retrosplenial Cortices. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004;16:167–177. doi: 10.1162/089892904322984481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Johns M, Inzlicht M, Schmader T. Stereotype threat and executive resource depletion: examining the influence of emotion regulation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2008;137:691–705. doi: 10.1037/a0013834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.van der Meer L, Costafreda S, Aleman A, David AS. Self-reflection and the brain: A theoretical review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies with implications for schizophrenia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010;34:935–946. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Seubert J, Freiherr J, Djordjevic J, Lundström JN. Statistical localization of human olfactory cortex. Neuroimage. 2013;66:333–342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kareken DA, Mosnik DM, Doty RL, Dzemidzic M, Hutchins GD. Functional anatomy of human odor sensation, discrimination, and identification in health and aging. Neuropsychology. 2003;17:482. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.17.3.482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Savic I, Gulyas B, Larsson M, Roland P. Olfactory functions are mediated by parallel and hierarchical processing. Neuron. 2000;26:735–745. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81209-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Conway P, Gawronski B. Deontological and Utilitarian Inclinations in Moral Decision Making: A Process Dissociation Approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2013;104:216–235. doi: 10.1037/a0031021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Shenhav, A. & Greene, J. D. Integrative Moral Judgment: Dissociating the Roles of the Amygdala and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. J. Neurosci. 34 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 36.Decety J, Lamm C. The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition. Neurosci. 2007;13:580–593. doi: 10.1177/1073858407304654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Prokop-Prigge KA, Greene K, Varallo L, Wysocki CJ, Preti G. The Effect of Ethnicity on Human Axillary Odorant Production. J. Chem. Ecol. 2016;42:33–39. doi: 10.1007/s10886-015-0657-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Boesveldt S, et al. Gustatory and olfactory dysfunction in older adults: a national probability study. Rhinology. 2011;49:324–30. doi: 10.4193/rhino10.155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Lundström JN, Jones-Gotman M. Romantic love modulates women’s identification of men’s body odors. Horm. Behav. 2009;55:280–284. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lenochova P, Roberts SC, Havlicek J. Methods of Human Body Odor Sampling: The Effect of Freezing. Chem. Senses. 2008;34:127–138. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjn067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Proverbio AM, Zani A, Adorni R. Neural markers of a greater female responsiveness to social stimuli. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:56. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Pause BM, Lübke K, Laudien JH, Ferstl R, Murphy C. Intensified Neuronal Investment in the Processing of Chemosensory Anxiety Signals in Non-Socially Anxious and Socially Anxious Individuals. PLoS One. 2010;5:e10342. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hummel T, Sekinger B, Wolf SR, Pauli E, Kobal G. ‘Sniffin’ sticks’: olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem. Senses. 1997;22:39–52. doi: 10.1093/chemse/22.1.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lundström JN, McClintock MK, Olsson MJ. Effects of reproductive state on olfactory sensitivity suggest odor specificity. Biol. Psychol. 2006;71:244–247. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Rozin P, Haidt J, McCauley C, Dunlop L, Ashmore M. Individual Differences in Disgust Sensitivity: Comparisons and Evaluations of Paper-and-Pencil versus Behavioral Measures. J. Res. Pers. 1999;33:330–351. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.1999.2251. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Spielberger, C. The State-trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI): Test Manual for Form X (1968).
- 47.Starcke K, Ludwig A-C, Brand M. Anticipatory stress interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2012;7:61. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Youssef FF, et al. Stress alters personal moral decision making. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:491–498. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cecchetto C, Rumiati RI, Aiello M. Alexithymia and emotional reactions to odors. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:14097. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-14404-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Martins Y, et al. Preference for Human Body Odors Is Influenced by Gender and Sexual Orientation. Psychol. Sci. 2005;16:694–701. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01598.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Saxton TK, Lyndon A, Little A, Craig Roberts S. Evidence that androstadienone, a putative human chemosignal, modulates women’s attributions of men’s attractiveness. Horm. Behav. 2008;54:597–601. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mitro S, Gordon AR, Olsson MJ, Lundström JN, Vainius A. The Smell of Age: Perception and Discrimination of Body Odors of Different Ages. PLoS One. 2012;7:e38110. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Mujica-Parodi LR, et al. Chemosensory cues to conspecific emotional stress activate amygdala in humans. PLoS One. 2009;4:e6415. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Wudarczyk OA, et al. Chemosensory anxiety cues moderate the experience of social exclusion – an fMRI investigation with Cyberball. Front. Psychol. 2015;6:1475. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Wudarczyk OA, et al. Chemosensory anxiety cues enhance the perception of fearful faces - An fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2016;143:214–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Lundström, J. N., Gordon, A. R., Alden, E. C. & Boesveldt, S. Methods for building an inexepensive computer-controlled olfactometer for temporally-precise experiments. 78, 179–189 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 57.Lötsch J, Ahne G, Kunder J, Kobal G, Hummel T. Factors affecting pain intensity in a pain model based upon tonic intranasal stimulation in humans. Inflamm. Res. 1998;47:446–450. doi: 10.1007/s000110050359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Winter, B. Linear models and linear mixed effects models in R with linguistic applications. arXiv Prepr. arXiv1308.5499 (2013).
- 59.Powell, M. J. D. The BOBYQA algorithm for bound constrained optimization without derivatives. Cambridge NA Rep. NA2009/06, Univ. Cambridge, Cambridge 26–46 (2009).
- 60.Hoaglin DC, Iglewicz B, Tukey JW. Performance of some resistant rules for outlier labeling. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1986;81:991–999. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1986.10478363. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Tzourio-Mazoyer N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15:273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Rolls ET, Joliot M, Tzourio-Mazoyerc N. Implementation of a new parcellation of the orbitofrontal cortex in the automated anatomical labeling atlas. Neuroimage. 2015;122:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Wyatt, T. D. Pheromones and animal behavior: chemical signals and signatures (2014).
- 64.Sarlo M, lorella L, Rumiati R, Palomba D. If it makes you feel bad, don’t do it! Egoistic rather than altruistic empathy modulates neural and behavioral responses in moral dilemmas. Physiol. Behav. 2014;130:127–134. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Uddin LQ, Molnar-Szakacs I, Zaidel E, Iacoboni M. rTMS to the right inferior parietal lobule disrupts self–other discrimination. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2006;1:65–71. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Lundström JN, Boyle JA, Zatorre RJ, Jones-Gotman M. The neuronal substrates of human olfactory based kin recognition. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:2571–2580. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Royet J-P, Plailly J. Lateralization of olfactory processes. Chem. Senses. 2004;29:731–745. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Carter RM, Huettel SA. A nexus model of the temporal–parietal junction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013;17:328–336. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Cecchetto, C., Lancini, E., Rumiati, R. I. & Parma, V. Women smelling men’s masked body odors show enhanced harm aversion in moral dilemmas. Physiol. Behav. 201, 212–220 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 70.Ripp, I. et al. Multisensory integration processing during olfactory-visual stimulation—An fMRI graph theoretical network analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 71.Reichert JL, et al. Functional brain networks during picture encoding and recognition in different odor contexts. Behav. Brain Res. 2017;333:98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.06.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Ciaramelli E, Braghittoni D, di Pellegrino G. It is the outcome that counts! Damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex disrupts the integration of outcome and belief information for moral judgment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2012;18:962–71. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Patil I, Silani G. Reduced empathic concern leads to utilitarian moral judgments in trait alexithymia. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:501. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Cecchetto C, Korb S, Rumiati RI, Aiello M. Emotional reactions in moral decision-making are influenced by empathy and alexithymia. Soc. Neurosci. 2017;13:226–240. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2017.1288656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.