Abstract
Objective
Work on longevity has found protective social, cognitive, and emotional factors, but to date we have little understanding of the impact of motivational dynamics. Autonomy orientation, or stable patterns of self‐regulation, is theorized to be a protective factor for long‐term mental and physical health (Ryan & Deci, 2017), and it is therefore a prime candidate for examining how stable psychosocial factors are linked to longevity, or life expectancy.
Method
Essays written in the 1930s by participants in the Nun Study were coded for indicators of an autonomy orientation. These were selected in line with an extensive theoretical literature based in self‐determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985). Essays were coded for the propensity for choice in action, susceptibility to pressure, self‐reflection, integration of experiences, and parental support for autonomy. These coded variables were used to predict age of death.
Results
Using 176 codable essays provided by now‐deceased participants, linear regression analyses revealed that choiceful behavior, self‐reflection, and parent autonomy support predicted age of death. Participants who demonstrated these stable and beneficial motivational characteristics lived longer.
Conclusions
Personality constructs reflecting a healthy form of self‐regulation are associated with long‐term health. Implications for health interventions are discussed.
Keywords: autonomy, longevity, motivation, Nun Study, self-determination theory
1. INTRODUCTION
Across disciplines, researchers have found strong evidence for cognitive, social, emotional, and personality factors predicting longevity, or life expectancy; yet, the role of motivation has been ignored. Among other factors, higher intelligence (Gottfredson & Deary, 2004); personality traits such as conscientiousness, openness, and extraversion (e.g., Fry & Debats, 2009); expressing more positive emotions (Danner, Snowdon, & Friesen, 2001); and providing social support (Brown, Nesse, Vinokur, & Smith, 2003) have predicted longevity. There is reason to believe that individuals' motivational orientation could impact longevity as well, as motivation governs behavior over a life span in ways that can either protect long‐term mental and physical health or undermine it (for a review, see Ryan & Deci, 2017; Weinstein & Ryan, 2011). Herein, we explore the characteristics of an autonomous motivational orientation that may be impactful for long‐term health, considered in terms of longevity. Data were collected as part of the Nun Study, a longitudinal study of aging in Catholic sisters, which has paired early life essays written by the sisters at the point of entry to the North American School Sisters of Notre Dame (SSND) congregation with age of death, collected decades later. Essays were autobiographical and often centered around reasons for becoming a sister, providing us with a unique and rich opportunity to explore whether tendencies characteristic of an autonomous motivational orientation were related to longevity.
1.1. Autonomous motivational orientation, stress, and health
Defined by the theoretical framework of self‐determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 2008a), an autonomous motivational orientation is a tendency for one's behavior to be driven by interests, goals, and values, and less so by pressure from others (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2008b). Autonomously oriented individuals take more interest in the world, and in their own internal experiences and emotions, and they experience a greater sense of choice around their behaviors (Weinstein, Przybylski, & Ryan, 2012). This pattern of behaving benefits mental and physical health across the life span (see reviews in Deci & Ryan, 2008a; Weinstein & Ryan, 2011).
A body of work reveals that a more autonomous orientation is associated with effective coping with stressful situations, and a greater likelihood of demonstrating resilience in the face of adversity (Ryan & Deci, 2017; Weinstein & Hodgins, 2009; Weinstein & Ryan, 2011). Over time, this has cumulative effects on physical health, leading to lower risk for mortality and illness in old age (e.g., Surtees, Wainright, & Knaw, 2006). Those who are higher in autonomy have also been shown to exhibit fewer type A personality patterns (Deci & Ryan, 1985), less rageful behaviors (Knee, Nanayakkara, Vietor, Neighbors, & Patrick, 2001), and more psychological growth following stressful events (e.g., Lumb, Beaudry, & Blanchard, 2016), indicating they respond more adaptively to both daily and major life stressors. Further, those who regulate autonomously experience more vitality, a positive arousal state that reflects a high level of functioning, after a stressful encounter (Weinstein & Hodgins, 2009). Moreover, these patterns of resilient responding are evident across the life span (Chang & Yu, 2013). Beyond looking at an autonomous motivational orientation globally, its specific components each have been found to protect health, giving more insight into how it might relate to longevity.
1.1.1. Autonomous motivation for behavior
According to self‐determination theory, as individuals are increasingly autonomously oriented, they tend to behave from a sense of choice—acting volitionally and fully endorsing behavior—rather than behaving because of pressure—being coerced or pushed into behaving by others (Ryan & Deci, 2008a). Researchers have argued experiencing choice (and not pressure) around important life decisions and even on a daily basis is a key reason that autonomously oriented individuals tend to cope better with stress (Weinstein & Ryan, 2011). For example, research suggests that the experience of choice is related to stressors being seen as challenging rather than overwhelming (De Cuyper & De Witte, 2006), with implications for mortality and health (Seery, 2011). Complementing work shows that a greater sense of pressure is linked to higher salivary cortisol responses (Quested et al., 2011) and high blood pressure (Weinstein, Legate, Kumashiro, & Ryan, 2016).
1.1.2. Autonomous processing of experiences
A second aspect of an autonomous orientation is processing emotional experiences fully and openly. Specifically, those who are autonomously oriented self‐reflect, or actively consider their reactions and feelings about emotion‐laden experiences (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Turner & Silvia, 2006), and they demonstrate a curiosity or willingness to think more about their own past and present experiences (Hodgins et al., 2010; Weinstein et al., 2012). This way of self‐reflecting is also indicative of a mindful orientation, or an open receptivity to experiences as they occur, which has been closely linked to autonomy (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Mindfulness has also been linked to lower stress (e.g., Brown & Ryan, 2003; Weinstein et al., 2012), as indicated by lower salivary cortisol after stressful events (Brown, Weinstein, & Creswell, 2012) and lower autonomic arousal generally (Ditto, Eclache, & Goldman, 2006).
Complementing self‐reflective processes, the other aspect of autonomously processing affective experiences is integration, or bringing new experiences, both positive and negative, into harmony with one's emotions, sense of self, and values (for a review, see Weinstein, Przybylski, & Ryan, 2013); the result is an increasingly coherent and developing sense of self. An emerging line of research has identified that such integrative processing of experiences promotes successful coping (Roth & Assor, 2012) and results in lower autonomic arousal when individuals are emotionally challenged (Roth et al., 2014; Weinstein & Hodgins, 2009), suggesting they learn from and adapt to stressors.
Importantly, seminal work coding content in personal narratives has found that narratives demonstrating integration and awareness (self‐reflection) correlate with wellness (Bauer & McAdams, 2004; Bauer, McAdams, & Sakaeda, 2005). Even more immediate to the current work, coding of identity processes that show integration has been shown to be predictive of the health of older adults 10 years later, as indicated by subjective coding (Pals, 2006). Further, constructs related to autonomous functioning such as self‐integration have been coded in narratives of older and younger adults and relate to wellness in both (Bauer et al., 2005; Pasupathi & Mansour, 2006).
1.1.3. Parent autonomy support
Finally, a precursor to an autonomous orientation is parent autonomy support, or parents' acceptance and encouragement for self‐expression (e.g., Wong, Wiest, & Cusick, 2002). Autonomy support from parents and close others has been shown to reduce ill health, stress reactivity (Quested et al., 2011), and physical health symptoms on a daily basis (Legate, Ryan, & Rogge, 2017); in fact, support that does not promote autonomy has been shown to increase cardiovascular reactivity (Zniva, Pauli, & Schulz, 2017). Given its implications for health, we explored essays for themes of parents as autonomy supportive or not; in this case, parent support was considered by evaluating how sisters described their parents' reaction to the life‐altering decision to join the congregation, one that can lead to conflict and withdrawal of support within families (Trzebiatowska, 2008).
1.2. The Nun Study sample
It is important to consider the historical and cultural context from which these essays emerged, as narratives are colored by the culture of their writer (Schuman, Rieger, & Gaidys, 1994; Thorne & McLean, 2003). The sisters in this study entered the SSND order and wrote these autobiographical essays between 1930 and 1942. Their decisions were likely influenced by less access to education and occupational opportunities for girls and women at the time (primarily domestic service, factories, and clerical work, with fewer in professional roles as teachers or nurses; Baughman, Bondi, Layman, McConnell, & Tomkins, 2001) and by economic hardship (these young women came primarily from working‐class and rural farming community backgrounds; McNamara, 1996). This was also a period of peak activity for religious orders and their influence in schools, led by Pope Pius XI (the “social‐action pope”), who emphasized the importance of Catholics shaping society (O'Donoghue & O'Donoghue, 2004). During this time, young women were often encouraged or persuaded by Catholic teachers or religious leaders to join for their own salvation as well as that of society (Cummings, 2009; Lee & Holland, 1993). Thus, joining a religious congregation was more common for young women at the time than it is today, and it was often seen as a prestigious, and dutiful, contribution to the church from large Catholic families. Sisters in the United States, who typically held professions (teachers, nurses) and were involved in the community, likely had more in common with women in the general population than they did with secluded orders of nuns residing in nunneries. Though the latter show lower psychosocial stress levels than the general population (Timio et al., 1997), the same may not have been true for the population of sisters living and working within theUnited States; indeed, religious leaders in the United States show substantial variability of stress levels (Rayburn, Richmond, & Rogers, 1986).
Also important is that previous studies have relied on the Nun Study sample to study health and longevity. Most research with this sample has focused on dementia, longevity, and the link between these two (e.g., Tyas, Snowdon, Desrosiers, Riley, & Markesbery, 2007). Work on dementia finds that factors like brain infarcts (Snowdon et al., 1997), low educational attainment (Mortimer, Snowdon, & Markesbery, 2003), and low linguistic ability in early life (Riley, Snowdon, Derosiers, & Markesbury, 2005) and in later life (Kemper, Greiner, Marquis, Prenovost, & Mitzner, 2001) are predictors. Research on longevity using this sample has found that self‐rated functioning and self‐rated global health (Greiner, Snowdon, & Greiner, 1996), as well as low linguistic ability in early life (Snowdon, Griener, Kemper, Nanayakkara, & Mortimer, 1999), predicted all‐cause mortality. Most immediate to the present research, Danner and colleagues (2001) linked the presence of positive emotions in the sisters' narratives to their longevity, using mortality data available at the time from 42% of the sample.
1.3. Present research
The present research draws on autobiographical essays written at a pivotal moment in these young women's lives to examine the relation between an autonomous orientation and longevity. These accounts were drafted when participants had made the impactful life decision to join a religious congregation and thus provide rich insights into the motivational orientations of these young women at a formative life stage. Informed by the literature in self‐determination theory, essays were coded for these characteristics of an autonomous motivational orientation and linked to age of mortality. If such a link were present, it may be that these motivational characteristics already known to protect short‐term health might extend to greater longevity.
Focusing on core constructs indicative of an autonomous motivation orientation, hypotheses were as follows:
-
Hypothesis 1:
Experiencing more choice and less pressure for joining the congregation would relate to greater longevity. As essays centered around reasons for joining the congregation, both types of motivation were expected to be highly salient.
-
Hypothesis 2:
Demonstrating a tendency for self‐reflection and the ability to more fully integrate hardships (exhibiting learning and growth from these experiences) would be linked to greater longevity.
-
Hypothesis 3:
Describing parents as being autonomy supportive would be linked to greater longevity.
2. METHOD
2.1. Participants
Data from 678 women who were members of the North American School Sisters of Notre Dame (SSND) congregation who participated in the Nun Study were analyzed for the present study. The Nun Study cohort was followed closely from 1991 until the present with the aim of evaluating the cognitive, functional, and neuropathologic correlates of aging (see more in Danner et al., 2001; Snowdon et al., 1996). Informed consent for release of SSND records, study evaluations, and brain autopsy was obtained from all individual participants at the time of enrollment and periodically throughout follow‐up. Procedures were approved by the institutional review boards of the University of Kentucky and the University of Minnesota. Archived essays were included for 180 individuals from the Baltimore (79 women) and Milwaukee (101 women) SSND regions (though the women originated from the midwestern, eastern, and southern United States). The North American SSND requested that each incoming sister provide an autobiography. Of the full sample, a total of 180 essays from the Baltimore and Milwaukee regions met the criteria of being handwritten so they could be authenticated as unaltered, and written by native English speakers. Of the 180 women who wrote these essays, ages ranged from 18 to 32 years (M = 22 years). Data on age of death were available for 176 individuals, as four were still alive at time of analysis and were therefore excluded from our analyses.
2.2. Procedures for coding constructs
As a first step, the article's authors, researchers well versed in self‐determination theory, examined a subset of essays to ensure that essays were suitable to code for content indicative of an autonomous motivational orientation. Indicators of autonomous motivation, processing of experience, and themes of parental autonomy support emerged, and coders decided on ratings for each before coding commenced.
Coding of the essays comprised two phases. In a first phase, the two raters consulted with a third, who was well trained in the theoretical approach employed in this project. Fifty‐four essays were coded in this way, with discussions to resolve discrepancies between the three coders. Having achieved satisfying inter‐rater reliability at this early stage, two raters continued to evaluate essays, at this point making separate evaluations, but using a round‐robin procedure with overlaps in coding to allow continued checks of reliability that reflect agreement throughout the coding process. Major discrepancies were resolved through discussion. This approach resulted in scores from two raters for just over 50% of the essays (98 essays); the remaining essays were evaluated by one rater.
2.3. Coded constructs (predictors)
2.3.1. Choice
Choice was operationalized as expressing a willful decision to enter the congregation. Essays were coded 0 if there was no choiceful behavior in them, 1 if there was indication of choiceful behavior (i.e., participants expressed that they willfully entered the congregation), and 2 if they showed persistent or enthusiastic choiceful behavior around entering the congregation. A coding of 2 was further defined if choice was a recurrent theme in the essay, or if they expressed persistence in choosing to enter the congregation despite obstacles in making the choice. Inter‐rater reliability between coders for this construct was acceptable. To ensure ongoing agreement on these complex topics, disagreements were resolved through ongoing discussions after, approximately, each set of eight participants, and discussions were mediated by a third, independent researcher. As a result, reliability improved for codings derived during the training session, from κ = .79, to very high after the training period, κ = .96. See Table 1 for excerpts from essays illustrating how this construct (choice) and other constructs were coded.
Table 1.
Codes | ||
---|---|---|
1 | 2 | |
Choice | “The ardent desire to live for Christ Crucified could not be resisted. More thoroughly than ever convinced that the world was never to know me I departed it with joy on {month} {date}, {year} when I received the Bonnet in {congregation} Monastery Convent Chapel.” | “Just when I received the desire to become a sister I do not know, for as long as I can remember, even as a child, I always expressed the desire to my parents. … When I finished high school I was not able to enter due to my father's unemployment. … During this time I had not only helped my parents, but also saved enough to embrace my heart's fondest desire. In the meantime my father had received steady employment, so I felt free to enter as I had the consent of my parents and approval of my confessor, Father {last name}.” |
Pressure | “At the end of my second year at highschool, I was persuaded to join three other girls who were going to the Aspiranture in {city}.” | “The Sisters urged me to return with her, but I was as still in doubt. I prayed to the blessed Virgin to help me and to show me the way, for I had great confidence in her. The blessed Virgin did help me and led me here. How? I do not know, but in a week's time I was ready to enter with the new class.” |
Self‐reflection | “I heard this statement from my aunt. ‘Those who always say they are going to be Sisters are the very ones who get married.’ From the day I listened to my aunt's speech I seldom again ever said I would be a Sister.” | “Our visits with him each Thanksgiving and summer made a deep impression and turned my mind to the great question of ‘what doth it profit?’ His growth in physical health and spiritual peace made me reflect and it is to his example of courage and perseverance that I gratefully attribute my own following of Christ's call. Of course the annual retreats at school, especially those of my last college years, prepared the way but his ready sacrifice of all that was near and dear gave me something concrete to ponder over and imitate.” |
Integrating experience | “This first break from home was hard, very hard. I was an only child and I know my mother felt my leaving keenly, but the beautiful spirit in which she made the sacrifice, helped to lessen the pain of parting.” | “The doctor confirmed my mother's fears—it was scarlet fever. The doctor fought for four months what he termed would be a ‘losing battle,’ but faith and prayers triumphed and I was spared. … [T]hen Mother said, ‘If you think you will like that life; be happy with the choice, I will not stand in your way. Perhaps God spared you to me years ago—so that I could give you back to Him.’ Joy and sorrow filled my heart—joy at the consent of my dearest one on earth and sorrow at the thought of leaving so tender a mother.” |
Parent autonomy support | (–1) “My Mother cannot be reconciled to my Vocation, and the knowledge of the suffering which I have caused her, has been the one unhappy obstacle, to an otherwise perfectly happy year.” | (1) “Though this was a sudden shock to my parents, they consented cheerfully, allowing me to finish highschool at {institution}.” |
2.3.2. Pressure
Pressure was operationalized in terms of being compelled, forced, or coerced into entering the congregation by others. For example, such influences might come from teachers, parents, or other influential religious figures in one's life (a priest). Pressure was coded 0 when absent, 1 when present but subtle (i.e., when text suggested a pressuring influence but pressure was not explicitly expressed), and 2 when present and explicit. Decisions made by the two coders for this construct were highly related during and after training (κ = .82–1.00).
2.3.3. Self‐reflection
Self‐reflection was operationalized in terms of demonstrating having thought about one's own psychological experiences and emotions, also reflected in showing insight and awareness about experiences. Essays were coded 0 when absent, 1 when present but subtle (e.g., when this was apparent in one or two meaningful sentences), and 2 when self‐reflection was a strong and consistent theme across text in an essay (κ training = .91, κ post training = .97).
2.3.4. Integration of hardships
Integration of hardships was operationalized in terms of learning and growth from challenges and difficulties in one's life. Such growth from hardships, in particular, is a powerful indicator of psychosocial adjustment (McAdams, Reynolds, Lewis, Patten, & Bowman, 2001). Essays were coded 0 when absent, 1 when present but subtle (e.g., when expressed through one or two meaningful sentences), and 2 when learning and growth were profound (κs = .89–.99).
2.3.5. Parent autonomy support
Parent autonomy support was operationalized in terms of parents' being perceived as for, against, or neutral in terms of the decision to enter the congregation. Essays were coded 0 when parents were explicitly against the decision, 1 when neutral (or absent), and 2 when explicitly supportive. In some cases, participants reported their parents struggled with the decision (for economic or personal reasons), but over time they embraced and supported the decision; parents' willingness to support was coded 2 even if initial conflict was present, given the willingness to be open and responsive is characteristic of being autonomy supportive (Ryan & Deci, 2017; κs = .95–.93).
2.4. Outcome and control variables from the longitudinal data set
2.4.1. Outcome variable
Age of death
Data were available for 176 out of 180 participants, as four were still living at the time of analyses; age of death ranged from 78 to 107 years (M = 89.36, SD = 5.63). Descriptive analyses showed that age of death was normally distributed across the sample (skewness = .34, kurtosis = −.27).
2.4.2. Control variables
Parent age of death
Data were available for both fathers' and mothers' age of death in 156 and 160 cases, respectively. Parents' age of death ranged from 22 to 110 years (fathers' M = 71.42, SD = 15.52; mothers' M = 73.46, SD = 15.95).
Education
As a measure of educational achievement, the number of years in school was recorded at the time of entry into the Nun Study (age 75 or older) and ranged from 8 to 18 years (M = 16.50, SD = 2.31).
Age when writing essays
Women ranged in age from 18 to 32 years when writing the autobiographical essays coded in this article (M = 21.84 years, SD = 2.67).
Positive emotions
Positive emotion sentences were coded for all essays and data published in Danner et al. (2001), and the coding procedures that derived this variable are further described in the article. The authors also coded negative and neutral emotion words, but found these did not relate to age of death; as such, we only control for positive emotions in our analyses.1
3. RESULTS
3.1. Preliminary correlations
Zero‐order correlations between our predictor, outcome, and control variables of interest are presented in Table 2. Findings showed no relations between sisters' parents' age of death and sisters' own age of death, mother r(156) = −.03, p = .73, father r(152) = .06, p = .46, and parents' age of death did not relate to any of our coded constructs (rs < .12, ps > .14). Additional correlations examined relations of our constructs of interest (coded scores and age of death) with education levels, operationalized as the number of years of education, given that more educated individuals may have a higher quality of life (and longer life span). Pearson correlation analyses showed no relations with years of education for all but the parent support construct, in which case more education related to more parent support, r(180) = .24, p = .001. In addition, those who received more parental autonomy support were younger at age of writing, r(180) = –.25, p = .001. Finally, relations were present with coding of positive emotions, with rs ranging from .21–.62 (except for perceived pressure, which did not relate p = .51). All other relations were rs < .09, ps > .24.
Table 2.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Major study constructs | ||||||||||
1. Age of death | ||||||||||
2. Choice | .27** | |||||||||
3. Pressure | −.05 | −.05 | ||||||||
4. Self‐reflection | .19* | .40** | .16* | |||||||
5. Integration | .12 | .33** | .16* | .62** | ||||||
6. Parent support | .19* | .06 | .13 | .10 | .08 | |||||
Covariates | ||||||||||
7. Father's age of death | .06 | .07 | .03 | −.12 | −.07 | .11 | ||||
8. Mother's age of death | −.03 | .06 | −.05 | .01 | −.00 | −.07 | .08 | |||
9. Years of education | .06 | −.04 | .02 | .04 | .00 | .24** | −.06 | −.09 | ||
10. Age at writing | .15* | .10 | .04 | .04 | .14 | −.25** | .09 | .09 | –.23** | |
11. Positive emotions | .10 | .37** | .05 | .53** | .64** | .21** | .16* | .10 | .04 | –.03 |
Note. *p < .05. **p < .01.
3.2. Analytic strategy
In previous work, the percent of those who had died by the time data analysis took place was used as the primary outcome (Danner et al., 2001), but in this case, age of death was available for all but four participants. Given that the primary benefit of using a hazard model over regression analyses is that it allows for estimates in instances where much of the sample is still alive, the more parsimonious approach was employed, excluding the small minority of participants who were still alive in this case. Data were analyzed, first, using linear regression analyses not adjusting for controls, and second, adjusting for (a) years of education, (b) age at writing, and (c) positive emotions coding, because these potential confounds correlated with our coding in the preliminary analysis.
3.3. Primary Analyses
Multiple regression analyses were used to predict age of death from our motivational constructs as reflected in the essays.
3.3.1. Hypothesis 1: Motivation would link to longevity
Findings showed those who expressed choice around joining the SSND lived longer, on average 2.04 years longer for every unit coded, b = 2.04, 95% CI [0.94, 3.14], β = .27, t(174) = 3.67, p < .001, though there was no relation with perceived pressure, b = −0.75; 95% CI [–3.03, 1.53], β = −.05, t(174) = –0.65, p = .52. For illustrative purposes, Figure 1 shows the percentage alive for high and low choice (above and below the median) at each age.
3.3.2. Hypothesis 2: Fuller processing would link to longevity
Examining autonomous emotional processing, those who demonstrated more self‐reflection lived longer, b = 1.47 (years per unit coded), 95% CI [0.33, 2.61], β = .19, t(174) = 2.55, p = .01 (see Figure 2). On the other hand, there was no relation between integration and longevity, b = 0.95, 95% CI [–0.22, 2.16], β = .12, t(174) = 1.61, p = .11.
3.3.3. Hypothesis 3: Parental autonomy support would link to longevity
Perceived support from parents was related to a longer life span, with the average unit of coding linking to an additional 1.90 years, 95% CI for b [0.40, 3.40], β = .19, t(174) = 2.49, p = .01 (Figure 3 illustrates this relation).
3.4. Supplementary analyses
Given our variables of interest were related to years of education, age at writing essays, and positive emotions, an additional series of analyses was conducted regressing age of death onto these constructs as well as our own. The analyses showed a weaker, but still significant, effect of choice, b = 1.93, 95% CI [0.73, 3.12], β = .25, t(171) = 3.19, p = .002, self‐reflection, b = 1.37, 95% CI [0.04, 2.70], β = .18, t(171) = 2.03, p = .04, and parental support, b = 2.08, 95% CI [0.49, 3.68], β = .20, t(171) = 2.58, p = .01, on age of death.
Additionally, given previous research has identified differences between the two SSND regions (more in Danner et al., 2001), further tests examined whether relations of interest would be moderated by location. To achieve this, a series of hierarchical linear regressions tested moderation for each of our constructs of interest by SSND region. Effects (or absence of effects) did not vary as a function of location for choice, t(172) = 0.97, p = .33, pressure, t(172) = –0.82, p = .41, self‐reflection, t(172) = 0.12, p = .90, self‐integration, t(172) = –0.57, p = .57, or perceived parent support, t(172) = 1.59, p = .11.
4. DISCUSSION
Our findings expand the existing literature on psychological determinants of physical health by suggesting that one's motivational orientation may be associated with long‐term health benefits to the extent of impacting longevity. This study showed three major findings in line with this view: Sisters who demonstrated choicefulness in their behavior, who showed the propensity for self‐reflection, and who perceived parents as being autonomy supportive lived longer.
Findings build on previous research showing lower cardiovascular reactivity and salivary cortisol as a function of these three indicators of an autonomous motivational orientation (e.g., Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, Cuevas, & Lonsdale, 2014; Ditto et al., 2006; Hodgins et al., 2010; Weinstein et al., 2016), and they suggest these indicators may have cumulative effects across the life span. These benefits may be explained by a psychological resilience that buffers against bigger life stressors (Weinstein & Ryan, 2011), or by lower physiological and stress reactivity on a daily basis that have cumulative effects on disease prevalence and recovery in late life (Legate et al., 2017; Surtees et al., 2006). While this study cannot address these potential mechanisms, future work building on these findings would greatly inform how motivation seems to impact health in later life. Behavioral mechanisms may also explain links of present study constructs with health. For example, those who are reflective or experience choice and autonomy support may be more likely to disclose difficult experiences in appropriate ways (Weinstein & Hodgins, 2009), leading to a multitude of health benefits (e.g., Pennebaker, 1993). In addition, those who are more autonomously oriented or supported (all three of our predictive indicators) may be more resilient because they perceive stressful situations as less threatening or overwhelming; but it is equally plausible that autonomous individuals may perceive a stressful situation as threatening but cope in more adaptive ways (Ntoumanis, Edmunds, & Duda, 2009).
In addition, our findings complement existing research suggesting psychological factors can play a role in longevity or mortality. For example, previous work has suggested a link between childhood attachment and mortality (Smith, Williamson, Walsh, & McCartney, 2016), and it might be that autonomy‐supportive parenting predicts longevity through promoting a secure attachment (Whipple, Bernier, & Mageau, 2011). Further, while previous research identified that positive emotions link to longevity (Danner et al., 2011), this study suggests that the way that emotions, whether positive or negative, are approached and understood—namely, in a thoughtful and reflective manner—is also important for longevity.
These findings can also be considered in light of Marcia's (1966) Identity Status Paradigm, which further defines identity processes in terms of exploration, that is, the weighing of identity alternatives before choosing a certain direction, and commitment, the extent to which one is then committed to the chosen identity. Though we did not address these identity‐relevant constructs in the present article, previous research has suggested that autonomy‐supportive parenting increases identity commitment and positive forms of exploration (Luyckx, Goossens, Soenens, & Byers, 2006), revealing that identity processes like exploration and commitment may perhaps be related to long‐term health. Indeed, additional work has coded for commitment themes in written narratives and found these relate to generative and pro‐social lives (McAdams, Diamond, De St. Aubin, & Mansfield, 1997), a fascinating link given the pro‐social nature of religious vocations (Daga & Madnawat, 2016).
Importantly, we explored a narrative approach, which builds on previous work that seeks to define, and understand the impacts of, personal stories. For example, seminal work by Bauer and colleagues (2005) found that narratives showing integration and learning (using a coding procedure similar to ours for integration and self‐reflection) relate to higher well‐being. Similar approaches were taken by King and Smith (2004) and Pals (2006), the latter linking processes similar to our self‐reflection and integration constructs to participants' subjective perceptions of physical health years later. These approaches, which are examined in Pasupathi, Mansour, and Brubaker (2007) and further reviewed in Adler, Lodi‐Smith, Philippe, and Houle (2016), provide options for rich and varied codings of narrative data, and they offer a broad base of understanding of constructs reflected in personal narratives.
We also explore an interesting and nuanced topic, that of choicefulness in entering into a way of life that is perceived by some to have more restrictions or less freedom (see discussion of such perceptions in relation to Catholic religious figures in McNamara, 1996). In fact, the writings of participants in this study illustrated that feeling a sense of choice is important even when choosing a path that may hold certain restrictions. This distinction—between the concept of autonomy and that of freedom—highlights that people can volitionally enter into roles in which they are asked to sacrifice certain freedoms (Ryan & Deci, 2006) or benefit from choice even in restricted environments (Van der Kaap‐Deeder et al., 2017). Notably, the strict lifestyle this sample observed (e.g., women who do not smoke, who abstain from sexual activity) may limit the generalizability of these findings to the general population. For example, work comparing this sample to age‐matched women in the general population found that the sisters tended to have lower mortality rates, especially in mortality from smoking‐related diseases (Butler & Snowdon, 1996). It will therefore be important for future research to test whether motivation orientation and associated constructs, such as choicefulness, predict longevity in other more representative samples.
Finally, the present findings can be viewed in light of existing research on introjected (guilty and pressured) and identified (coming from a place of valuing and interest) motivation for religion, more broadly. An existing literature suggests that Catholics are particularly vulnerable to pressure and guilt as compared to other Christian religions; notably, pressure was not linked to well‐being in some past research (Sheldon, 2006), whereas in other studies the experience has, albeit small, links with anxiety, depression, and somatization (Ryan, Rigby, & King, 1993). Informing this nascent literature examining religious motivation, the current findings also failed to find an effect of pressure to join the congregation on longevity. While there was no relation with pressure, choice linked to a longer life span. This finding—that having an autonomous form of religious motivation is beneficial to one's well‐being—is conceptually replicated in samples of Belgian Christians (Neyrinck, Vansteenkiste, Lens, Duriez, & Hutsebaut, 2006), Israeli Jews (Assor, 2012), and American Christians (Ryan et al., 1993).
The study has some notable limitations. Most importantly, findings relied on coding of one written essay, which might not be well‐representative of overall experiences or stable tendencies of the sisters. In addition, this research did not investigate why an autonomous orientation would predict longevity, which represents a critical direction of future research. Potential mechanisms could include common genetic determinants of motivation and health, as have been shown in relation to intelligence (Davies et al., 2011), physiological reactions to psychological stress (Savolainen, Eriksson, Kajantie, Pesonen, & Räikkönen, 2015), and behavioral health habits (Martin, Friedman, & Schwartz, 2007). Finally, the current set of findings should be seen as exploratory in nature as they were not preregistered; future studies should replicate motivational links with longevity before conclusions can be confidently drawn.
Further, our sample consisted of “old adults”; that is, there were no recorded deaths before the age of 79 years, and all Nun Study participants were a minimum of age 75 when the study was begun. Although many psychosocial predictors of functioning relate to physical health similarly in young and old adults (e.g., self‐esteem, Pruessner et al., 2005; Big Five personality traits, Costa & McCrae, 1980), certain causes of death at midlife and early old age, such as malignant neoplasms (Heron, 2012), ischemic heart disease and stroke (Lloyd‐Jones et al., 2009), injury (Krug, Sharma, & Lozano, 2000), and infections (Chaturvedi, Satoskar, Khare, & Kalgutkar, 2011) may have been less apparent in those individuals we evaluated, who all lived to an old age. Indeed, it may have been that this quality of the sample may have lent more sensitivity to analyses relying on subtle psychosocial indicators of resilience. In this case, our coding may have more immediately predicted variability due to long‐term cardiovascular and autoimmune health due to resilient coping styles (Ong & Bergeman, 2004). It is therefore imperative that future work explore whether motivational constructs continue to predict life expectancy across different age ranges.
Yet the findings that choiceful motivation, self‐reflection, and parent autonomy support have links with longevity decades later demonstrate the importance of these factors in shaping long‐term health. Importantly, these results held when controlling for the presence of positive emotions, as emotional expression as conveyed in narratives plays a critical role in resilience and long‐term health (Danner et al., 2001). Interestingly, previous analyses based on the coded content of narratives indicate that more important than either positive or negative emotions is the capacity to experience them concurrently (Adler & Hershfield, 2012), which points to the possibility that a healthy psychological approach to one's emotions and self‐relevant experiences is more important than the emotions themselves. Here, we begin to examine motivation orientation as a style of relating to the self, and to important experiences.
Our findings suggest that modifying one's motivation orientation to be more autonomous might reduce risk for poor health in older age. Motivational characteristics are likely modifiable at different developmental stages (Guay, Boggiano, & Vallerand 2001), as may also be the case for the hypothesized constructs that comprise orientation leading to greater health and longevity (e.g., capacity for self‐reflection; Ryan & Deci, 2017). Given this, it suggests fruitful avenues for intervening at different developmental stages to promote a more autonomous motivational orientation, especially by encouraging choiceful decision making, providing opportunities for self‐reflection, and intervening with parents to enhance their provision of autonomy support. It is thus critical that work continue to investigate these and other motivational factors that may offer more clues on extending longevity.
CONFLICT OF INTERESTS
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: Data collection for the Nun Study was supported by Grants R01AG09862, K04AG00553, and R03AG045476 from the National Institute on Aging.
Weinstein N, Legate N, Ryan WS, Hemmy L. Autonomous orientation predicts longevity: New findings from the Nun Study. Journal of Personality. 2019;87:181–193. 10.1111/jopy.12379
Funding information
Grants R01AG09862, K04AG00553, R03AG045476 from the National Institute on Aging
ENDNOTE
REFERENCES
- Adler, J. M. , & Hershfield, H. E. (2012). Mixed emotional experience is associated with and precedes improvements in psychological well‐being. PLoS ONE, 7, e35633 10.1371/journal.pone.0035633 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adler, J. M. , Lodi‐Smith, J. , Philippe, F. L. , & Houle, I. (2016). The incremental validity of narrative identity in predicting well‐being: A review of the field and recommendations for the future. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20, 142–175. 10.1177/1088868315585068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allison, P. D. (1995). Survival Analysis Using the SAS System: A Practical Guide. Biometrics, 67, 1177–1183. 10.1111/j.1541-0420.2011.01658_7.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Assor, A. (2012). Allowing choice and nurturing an inner compass: Educational practices supporting students' need for autonomy In Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 421–439). Boston, MA: Springer; 10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7_20 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bartholomew, K. J. , Ntoumanis, N. , Cuevas, R. , & Lonsdale, C. (2014). Job pressure and ill‐health in physical education teachers: The mediating role of psychological need thwarting. Teaching and Teacher Education, 37, 101–107. 10.1016/j.tate.2013.10.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer, J. J. , & McAdams, D. P. (2004). Personal growth in adults' stories of life transitions. Journal of Personality, 72, 573–602. 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00273.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer, J. J. , McAdams, D. P. , & Sakaeda, A. R. (2005). Interpreting the good life: Growth memories in the lives of mature, happy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 203–217. 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baughman, J. S. , Bondi, V. , Layman, R. , McConnell, T. , & Tompkins, V. (2001) Working women in the 1930s In V. Bondi (Ed.), American Decades (Vol. 4). Detroit, MI: Gale; Retrieved from http://link.galegroup.com/apps/doc/CX3468301237/UHIC?u=sand55832&xid=4a83ab53 [Google Scholar]
- Brown, S. L. , Nesse, R. M. , Vinokur, A. D. , & Smith, D. M. (2003). Providing social support may be more beneficial than receiving it: Results from a prospective study of mortality. Psychological Science, 14, 320–327. 10.1111/1467-9280.14461 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown, K. W. , & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well‐being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848. 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown, K. W. , Weinstein, N. , & Creswell, D. (2012). Trait mindfulness modulates neuroendocrine and affective responses to social evaluative threat. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 2037–2041. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler, S. M. , & Snowdon, D. A. (1996). Trends in mortality in older women: Findings from the Nun Study. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 51, S201–S208. 10.1093/geronb/51b.4.s201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang, L. C. , & Yu, P. (2013). Relationships between leisure factors and health‐related stress among older adults. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 18, 79–88. 10.1080/13548506.2012.686621 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaturvedi, M. , Satoskar, M. , Khare, M. S. , & Kalgutkar, A. D. (2011). Sudden, unexpected and natural death in young adults of age between 18 and 35 years: A clinicopathological study. Indian Journal of Pathology and Microbiology, 54(1), 47–50. 10.4103/0377-4929.77323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa, B. T. , & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Still stable after all these years: Personality as a key to some issues in adulthood and old age. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim (Eds.), Life span development and behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 5‐102). New York, NY: Academic Press.
- Cummings, K. S. (2009). The wageless work of paradise: Integrating women into American religious history. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press; 10.5149/9780807889848_cummings.7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Daga, P. , & Madnawat, A. V. S. (2016). Spirituality and compassionate love in nuns and philanthropists. Indian Journal of Positive Psychology, 7, 376–379. [Google Scholar]
- Danner, D. D. , Snowdon, D. A. , & Friesen, W. V. (2001). Positive emotions in early life and longevity: Findings from the Nun Study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 804–813. 10.1037//0022-3514.80.5.804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davies, G. , Tenesa, A. , Payton, A. , Yang, J. , Harris, S. E. , Liewald, D. , … McGhee, K. (2011). Genome‐wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and polygenic. Molecular Psychiatry, 16, 996–1005. 10.1038/mp.2011.85 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deci, E. L. , & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The General Causality Orientations Scale: Self‐determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 109–134. 10.1016/0092-6566(85)90023-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Deci, E. L. , & Ryan, R. M. (2008a). Self‐determination theory: A macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 49, 182–185. 10.1037/a0012801 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Deci, E. L. , & Ryan, R. M. (2008b). Facilitating optimal motivation and psychological well‐being across life's domains. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 49, 14–23. 10.1037/0708-5591.49.1.14 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- De Cuyper, N. , & De Witte, H. (2006). Autonomy and workload among temporary workers: Their effects on job satisfaction, organizational commitment, life satisfaction, and self‐rated performance. International Journal of Stress Management, 13, 441–459. 10.1037/1072-5245.13.4.441 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ditto, B. , Eclache, M. , & Goldman, N. (2006). Short‐term autonomic and cardiovascular effects of mindfulness body scan meditation. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 32, 227–234. 10.1207/s15324796abm3203_9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fry, P. S. , & Debats, D. L. (2009). Perfectionism and the five‐factor personality traits as predictors of mortality in older adults. Journal of Health Psychology, 14, 513–524. 10.1177/1359105309103571 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gottfredson, L. S. , & Deary, I. J. (2004). Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 1–4. 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301001.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Greiner, P. A. , Snowdon, D. A. , & Greiner, L. H. (1996). The relationship of self‐rated function and self‐rated health to concurrent functional ability, functional decline, and mortality: findings from the Nun Study. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 51, S234–S241. 10.1093/geronb/51b.5.s234 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guay, F. , Boggiano, A. K. , & Vallerand, R. J. (2001). Autonomy support, intrinsic motivation, and perceived competence: Conceptual and empirical linkages. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 643–650. 10.1177/0146167201276001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Heron, M. (2012). Deaths: Leading causes for 2008. National Vital Statistics Reports: From the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics, National Vital Statistics System, 60, 1–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodgins, H. S. , Weibust, K. S. , Weinstein, N. , Shiffman, S. , Miller, A. , Coombs, G. , & Adair, K. C. (2010). The cost of self‐protection: Threat response and performance as a function of autonomous and controlled motivations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(8), 1101–1114. https://doi.10.1177/0146167210375618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemper, S. , Greiner, L. H. , Marquis, J. G. , Prenovost, K. , & Mitzner, T. L. (2001). Language decline across the life span: Findings from the Nun Study. Psychology and Aging, 16, 227–239. 10.1037//0882-7974.16.2.227 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King, L. A. , & Smith, N. G. (2004). Gay and straight possible selves: Goals, identity, subjective well‐being, and personality development. Journal of Personality, 72, 967–994. 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00287.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knee, C. R. , Nanayakkara, A. , Vietor, N. A. , Neighbors, C. , & Patrick, H. (2001). Implicit theories of relationships: Who cares if romantic partners are less than ideal? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(7), 808–819. 10.1177/0146167201277004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Krug, E. G. , Sharma, G. K. , & Lozano, R. (2000). The global burden of injuries. American Journal of Public Health, 90, 523–526. 10.2105/ajph.90.4.523 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee, V. E. , & Holland, P. B. (1993). Catholic schools and the common good. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Legate, N. , Ryan, R. M. , & Rogge, R. D. (2017). Daily autonomy support and sexual identity disclosure predicts daily mental and physical health outcomes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43, 860–873. 10.1177/0146167217700399 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lloyd‐Jones, D. , Adams, R. , Carnethon, M. , De Simone, G. , Ferguson, T. B. , Flegal, K. , … Haase, N. (2009). Heart disease and stroke statistics—2009 update. Circulation, 119, e21–e181. 10.1161/circulationaha.108.191259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luyckx, K. , Goossens, L. , Soenens, B. , & Beyers, W. (2006). Unpacking commitment and exploration: Preliminary validation of an integrative model of late adolescent identity formation. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 361–378. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lumb, A. B. , Beaudry, M. , & Blanchard, C. (2016). Posttraumatic growth and bereavement: The contribution of self‐determination theory. OMEGA‐Journal of Death and Dying, 75, 311–366. 10.1177/0030222816652971 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego‐identity status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3(5), 551–558. 10.1037/h0023281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin, L. R. , Friedman, H. S. , & Schwartz, J. E. (2007). Personality and mortality risk across the life span: The importance of conscientiousness as a biopsychosocial attribute. Health Psychology, 26, 428–436. 10.1037/0278-6133.26.4.428 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McAdams, D. P. , Diamond, A. , . De St Aubin, E. , & Mansfield, E. (1997). Stories of commitment: The psychosocial construction of generative lives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 678–694. 10.1037//0022-3514.72.3.678 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McAdams, D. P. , Reynolds, J. , Lewis, M. , Patten, A. H. , & Bowman, P. J. (2001). When bad things turn good and good things turn bad: Sequences of redemption and contamination in life narrative and their relation to psychosocial adaptation in midlife adults and in students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 474–485. 10.1177/0146167201274008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McNamara, J. A. (1996). Sisters in arms: Catholic nuns through two millennia. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 10.3138/cjh.32.1.144 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mortimer, J. A. , Snowdon, D. A. , & Markesbery, W. R. (2003). Head circumference, education and risk of dementia: Findings from the Nun Study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 671–679. 10.1076/jcen.25.5.671.14584 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neyrinck, B. , Vansteenkiste, M. , Lens, W. , Duriez, B. , & Hutsebaut, D. (2006). Cognitive, affective and behavioral correlates of internalization of regulations for religious activities. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 321–332. 10.1007/s11031-006-9048-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ntoumanis, N. , Edmunds, J. , & Duda, J. L. (2009). Understanding the coping process from a self‐determination theory perspective. British Journal of Health Psychology, 14, 249–260. 10.1348/135910708x349352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Donoghue, T. A. , & O'Donoghue, T. (2004). Come follow me and forsake temptation: Catholic schooling and the recruitment and retention of teachers for religious teaching orders, 1922–1965 (Vol. 45). New York, NY: Peter Lang; 10.1111/j.1748-5959.2009.01174.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ong, A. D. , & Bergeman, C. S. (2004). The complexity of emotions in later life. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59(3), 117–122. 10.1093/geronb/59.3.p117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pals, J. L. (2006). Narrative identity processing of difficult life experiences: Pathways of personality development and positive self‐transformation in adulthood. Journal of Personality, 74, 1079–1110. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00403.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pasupathi, M. , & Mansour, E. (2006). Adult age differences in autobiographical reasoning in narratives. Developmental Psychology, 42, 798–808. 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.798 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pasupathi, M. , Mansour, E. , & Brubaker, J. R. (2007). Developing a life story: Constructing relations between self and experience in autobiographical narratives. Human Development, 50, 85–110. 10.1159/000100939 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pennebaker, J. W. (1993). Putting stress into words: Health, linguistic, and therapeutic implications. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 539–548. 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90105-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pruessner, J. C. , Baldwin, M. W. , Dedovic, K. , Renwick, R. , Mahani, N. K. , Lord, C. , … Lupien, S. (2005). Self‐esteem, locus of control, hippocampal volume, and cortisol regulation in young and old adulthood. Neuroimage, 28, 815–826. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quested, E. , Bosch, J. A. , Burns, V. E. , Cumming, J. , Ntoumanis, N. , & Duda, J. L. (2011). Basic psychological need satisfaction, stress‐related appraisals, and dancers' cortisol and anxiety responses. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 33, 828–846. 10.1123/jsep.33.6.828 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rayburn, C. A. , Richmond, L. J. , & Rogers, L. (1986). Men, women, and religion: Stress within leadership roles. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 540–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riley, K. P. , Snowdon, D. A. , Desrosiers, M. F. , & Markesbery, W. R. (2005). Early life linguistic ability, late life cognitive function, and neuropathology: findings from the Nun Study. Neurobiology of Aging, 26, 341–347. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.06.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roth, G. , & Assor, A. (2012). The costs of parental pressure to express emotions: Conditional regard and autonomy support as predictors of emotion regulation and intimacy. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 799–808. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roth, G. , Benita, M. , Amrani, C. , Shachar, B. H. , Asoulin, H. , Moed, A. , … Kanat‐Maymon, Y. (2014). Integration of negative emotional experience versus suppression: Addressing the question of adaptive functioning. Emotion, 14, 908–919. 10.1037/a0037051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan, R. M. , & Deci, E. L. (2006). Self‐regulation and the problem of human autonomy: Does psychology need choice, self‐determination, and will? Journal of Personality, 74, 1557–1586. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00420.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan, R. M. , & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self‐determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 10.7202/1041847ar [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan, R. M. , Rigby, S. , & King, K. (1993). Two types of religious internalization and their relations to religious orientations and mental health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 586–596. 10.1037/0022-3514.65.3.586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savolainen, K. , Eriksson, J. G. , Kajantie, E. , Pesonen, A. K. , & Räikkönen, K. (2015). Associations between the five‐factor model of personality and leukocyte telomere length in elderly men and women: The Helsinki Birth Cohort Study (HBCS). Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 79, 233–238. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.04.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuman, H. , Rieger, C. , & Gaidys, V. (1994). Collective memories in the United States and Lithuania In N. Schwarz & S. Sudman (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the validity of retrospective reports (pp. 313–333). New York, NY: Springer; 10.1007/978-1-4612-2624-6_21 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Seery, M. D. (2011). Challenge or threat? Cardiovascular indexes of resilience and vulnerability to potential stress in humans. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1603–1610. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheldon, K. M. (2006). Catholic guilt? Comparing Catholics' and Protestants' religious motivations. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 16, 209–223. 10.1207/s15327582ijpr1603_5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Smith, M. , Williamson, A. E. , Walsh, D. , & McCartney, G. (2016). Is there a link between childhood adversity, attachment style and Scotland's excess mortality? Evidence, challenges and potential research. BMC Public Health, 16, 655–658. 10.1186/s12889-016-3201-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowdon, D. A. , Greiner, L. H. , Kemper, S. J. , Nanayakkara, N. , & Mortimer, J. A. (1999). Linguistic ability in early life and longevity: Findings from the Nun Study In The paradoxes of longevity (pp. 103–113). Berlin, Germany; Springer; 10.1007/978-3-642-60100-2_9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Snowdon, D. A. , Greiner, L. H. , Mortimer, J. A. , Riley, K. P. , Greiner, P. A. , & Markesbery, W. R. (1997). Brain infarction and the clinical expression of Alzheimer disease: The Nun Study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 277, 813–817. 10.1001/jama.1997.03540340047031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowdon, D. A. , Kemper, S. J. , Mortimer, J. A. , Greiner, L. H. , Wekstein, D. R. , & Markesbery, W. R. (1996). Linguistic ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer's disease in late life: Findings from the Nun Study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 275, 528–532. 10.1001/jama.1996.03530310034029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Surtees, P. G. , Wainwright, N. W. , & Khaw, K. T. (2006). Resilience, misfortune, and mortality: Evidence that sense of coherence is a marker of social stress adaptive capacity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 61, 221–227. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.02.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorne, A. , & McLean, K. C. (2003). Telling traumatic events in adolescence: A study of master narrative positioning. In R. Fivush & C. A. Haden (Eds.), Connecting culture and memory: The development of an autobiographical self (pp. 169–185). New York, NY: Peter Lang.
- Timio, M. , Lippi, G. , Venanzi, S. , Gentili, S. , Quintaliani, G. , Verdura, C. , … Timio, F. (1997). Blood pressure trend and cardiovascular events in nuns in a secluded order: a 30‐year follow‐up study. Blood pressure, 6(2), 81–87. 10.3109/08037059709061804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trzebiatowska, M. (2008). Vocational habit(u)s: Catholic nuns in contemporary Poland In Vincett G., Aune K., & Sharma S. (Eds.), Women and religion in the West: Challenging secularization (pp. 83–89). Theology and Religion in Interdisciplinary Perspective Series in Association With the BSA Sociology of Religion Study Group. Aldershot, United Kingdom: Ashgate. [Google Scholar]
- Turner, S. A., Jr. , & Silvia, P. J. (2006). Must interesting things be pleasant? A test of competing appraisal structures. Emotion, 6, 670–674. 10.1037/1528-3542.6.4.670 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tyas, S. L. , Snowdon, D. A. , Desrosiers, M. F. , Riley, K. P. , & Markesbery, W. R. (2007). Healthy ageing in the Nun Study: Definition and neuropathologic correlates. Age and Ageing, 36, 650–655. 10.1093/ageing/afm120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Kaap‐Deeder, J. , Audenaert, E. , Vandevelde, S. , Soenens, B. , Van Mastrigt, S. , Mabbe, E. , & Vansteenkiste, M. (2017). Choosing when choices are limited: The role of perceived afforded choice and autonomy in prisoners' well‐being. Law and Human Behavior, 41, 567–578. 10.1037/lhb0000259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinstein, N. , & Hodgins, H. S. (2009). The moderating role of autonomy and control on the benefits of written emotion expression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 351–364. 10.1177/0146167208328165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinstein, N. , Przybylski, A. K. , & Ryan, R. M. (2012). The index of autonomous functioning: Development of a scale of human autonomy. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 397–413. 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.03.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Weinstein, N. , Przybylski, A. K. , & Ryan, R. M. (2013). The integrative process: New research and future directions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 69–74. 10.1177/0963721412468001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Weinstein, N. , Legate, N. , Kumashiro, M. , & Ryan, R. M. (2016). Autonomy support and diastolic blood pressure: Long term effects and conflict navigation in romantic relationships. Motivation and Emotion, 40, 212–225. 10.1007/s11031-015-9526-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinstein, N. , & Ryan, R. M. (2011). A self‐determination theory approach to understanding stress incursion and responses. Stress and Health, 27, 4–17. 10.1002/smi.1368 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Whipple, N. , Bernier, A. , & Mageau, G. A. (2011). Broadening the study of infant security of attachment: Maternal autonomy‐support in the context of infant exploration. Social Development, 20, 17–32. 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00574.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wong, E. H. , Wiest, D. J. , & Cusick, L. B. (2002). Perceptions of autonomy support, parent attachment, competence and self‐worth as predictors of motivational orientation and academic achievement: An examination of sixth‐ and ninth‐grade regular education students. Adolescence, 37, 255–266. https://search.proquest.com/openview/5ab76a880a794e74eecd97f92b362445/1?pq-origsite=gscholar&cbl=41539 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zniva, R. , Pauli, P. , & Schulz, S. M. (2017). Overprotective social support leads to increased cardiovascular and subjective stress reactivity. Biological Psychology, 123, 226–234. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]