Skip to main content
PLOS Genetics logoLink to PLOS Genetics
. 2019 Apr 4;15(4):e1008030. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1008030

Genes influence facial attractiveness through intricate biological relationships

Julie D White 1, David A Puts 1,2,3,*
Editor: Seth M Weinberg4
PMCID: PMC6448827  PMID: 30946742

In Greek mythology, Helen of Troy was so beautiful that her face “launched a thousand ships,” compelling King Menelaus to wage war to reclaim her from Prince Paris. Human preoccupations with beauty are enduring and now support a multibillion-dollar industry. Each day, our brains identify and catalog innumerable datapoints that bear on our impressions of beauty—those related to youth, health, adiposity, complexion, coloration, averageness, symmetry, masculinity/femininity, and personality, to name some of the best characterized (Fig 1) [1]. Congruent with the common saying that “beauty is in the eye of the beholder,” perceptions of attractiveness vary within and among individuals and across cultures. Yet when multiple individuals compare the same set of faces, clear agreement exists both within and between cultures about which faces are most attractive.

Fig 1. Some well-characterized features influencing perceptions of facial attractiveness.

Fig 1

Depicted are physical traits that influence facial attractiveness through changes in shape (e.g., adiposity, averageness, masculinity/femininity, and symmetry) or color (e.g., complexion), as well as qualities influencing facial attractiveness that might also be inferred from both shape and color information (e.g., youthfulness, health, and personality). Relationships between each of these features and attractiveness are likely mediated by a host of biological factors influenced by genotype.

Evolutionary hypotheses concerning the importance of attractiveness and its component factors in mate choice have revolved around the utility of these components in predicting the qualities of prospective mates [1]. For example, preferences for youthful appearance in female faces may function to direct courtship efforts toward those with high reproductive potential. Complexion and adiposity may reflect current health [2,3]. Other traits are purported to represent cues of underlying genes that increase the survival and reproduction of offspring by, for example, providing pathogen resistance, attractiveness, or dominance. Symmetry, masculinity, weight, and averageness have each been linked with indicators of genetic quality [35], though many of these relationships are contested [68].

Genetics of facial attractiveness

Given the importance of attractiveness across interpersonal contexts, studies that investigate the underlying genetics of facial attractiveness, such as the one reported by Hu and colleagues [9] in this issue, are invaluable but should be interpreted carefully, commensurate with the complexity of attractiveness as a phenotype. Although Hu and colleagues report considerably lower heritability estimates for facial attractiveness than a previous estimate [10], perhaps due to modest interrater reliability (S13 Fig in [9]), evidence of heritability suggests that searches for underlying loci associated with attractiveness may bear fruit. Datasets with genome-wide genetic data and rated facial attractiveness are rare and time-consuming to gather, and Hu and colleagues smartly leverage a large, pre-existing dataset. After testing 6 overlapping sets of facial attractiveness ratings, they find 1 SNP associated with rated facial attractiveness at a study-wide threshold, 1 SNP significant at genome-wide threshold, and 10 suggestively significant SNPs.

Through a series of enrichment tests, Hu and colleagues identify several correlations between attractiveness ratings and genes influencing other traits—namely, body mass index in females and lipid traits in males (Fig 4 in [9]). Indeed, this study manifests as an illustration of the ability of a large GWAS on a complex phenotype to identify genes related to its simpler component traits and correlates. The candidate genes identified for both significant results and nearly all suggestive results have entries in GWAS Catalog for traits related to attractiveness, including skin pigmentation and melanoma, body mass index (BMI), and the BMI-related phenotypes of height and waist–hip ratio (Table 1). Homogeneous skin coloration [11] and red and yellow tints [12] increase ratings of attractiveness cross-culturally, potentially due to the connection between these traits and perceptions of health and youth. The relationship between weight and attractiveness is demographically variable; for example, American men of European descent rate lower weights as more attractive, except in extremely low BMI ranges [13], whereas African American men are more likely to prefer heavier figures [14]. Hu and colleagues also identify candidate genes related to attractiveness that have been previously associated with facial morphology, possibly implicating facial traits (such as those contributing to youthful facial appearance) in perceptions of attractiveness [15]. One explanation for Hu and colleagues finding a genetic association between male-rated female attractiveness and BMI is a mediating relationship whereby the candidate gene (CDC42EP3) affects height, which directly influences BMI. Similarly, the genetic association between female-rated male attractiveness and lipid levels observed by Hu and colleagues could be explained by the previously identified impact of candidate genes CERS2 and ANXA9 on both high- and low-density lipoprotein cholesterol levels (Table 1). It is also possible that Hu and colleagues find different loci for male- and female-rated attractiveness because men and women seem to vary in the specific traits they perceive as attractive [16].

Table 1. GWAS results related to attractiveness.

Associations between genes identified in [9] and prior results related to attractiveness, found by searching GWAS Catalog (https://www.ebi.ac.uk/gwas/) for the candidate genes identified in [9] and selecting those results related to morphology, traits that influence attractiveness ratings, and the lipid traits described in [9].

Hu and colleagues GWAS Catalog
Trait Candidate Gene Trait SNP Candidate Gene P-value Study accession
MC-AS LRP1B Aging rs12474609 LRP1B 6.00 x 10-9 GCST000378
Age at menarche rs12472911 LRP1B 2.00 x 10-7 GCST000880
GCST002541
Body mass index rs12617004 LRP1B 6.00 x 10-9 GCST004904
PTPRT Facial morphology (factor 20) rs2867028 PTPRT 4.00 x 10-6 GCST004324
Eye morphology
(Left eye angle of en-ps-ex)
rs6016745 PTPRT 6.00 x 10-6 GCST006105
Obese body mass index rs7263077 PTPRT 6.00 x 10-6 GCST002828
FC-AS LY86 Obese body mass index rs4246076 LY86, LY86-AS1 6.00 x 10-6 GCST002829
Waist-hip ratio rs1294421 LOC101928004 7.00 x 10-14 GCST004064
GCST000829
GCST001954
ANTXRLP1 Melanin index rs111256285 ANTXRLP1 8.61 x 10-6 GCST004219
MC-FS CDC42EP3 Facial morphology (factor 5, width of mouth relative to central midface) rs116711337 LOC107985870 4.00 x 10-6 GCST004309
Height rs17511102 LOC105374465 2.00 x 10-18 GCST000817
GCST001956
SPON1 Facial morphology (factor 1, breadth of lateral portion of upper face) rs79756450 LOC101928132, SPON1 6.00 x 10-7 GCST004328
FC-FS MED30, EXT1 Obese body mass index status rs3115775 LOC105375721 8.00 x 10-6 GCST002828
Height rs1198912 EXT1 6.00 x 10-6 GCST000522
Cortisol secretion rs7459527 EXT1 2.00 x 10-6 GCST001762
NXN Facial morphology
(factor 15, philtrum width)
rs3851779 NXN 4.00 x 10-6 GCST004319
Mean arterial pressure rs747685 NXN 6.00 x 10-7 GCST002497
Diastolic blood pressure rs747687 NXN 2.00 x 10-7 GCST002497
MC-MS RAB11FIP4 - - - - -
FC-MS CERS2, ANXA9 High density lipoprotein cholesterol measurement rs267738 CERS2 6.00 x 10-12 GCST006611
Low density lipoprotein cholesterol measurement rs267733 ANXA9 4.00 x 10-8 GCST004233
GCST002222
Melanoma rs1722784 ANXA9 2.00 x 10-6 GCST001245
LOC285692 - - - - -
PDZRN4, GXYLT1 Height rs1405552 PDZRN4 1.00 x 10-10 GCST005951
GCST006368
Skin pigmentation rs1902910 PDZRN4 2.00 x 10-6 GCST004219
Height rs285575 PDZRN4 7.00 x 10-8 GCST002783
Overweight body mass index rs11180992 PDZRN4 3.00 x 10-6 GCST002829
Height rs11181001 PDZRN4 4.00 x 10-10 GCST005951
GCST006368
Diastolic blood pressure rs7965392 GXYLT1, YAF2 4.00 x 10-10 GCST006627

Note: Candidate genes with significant results in [9] are bolded. P-value refers to the P-value in the GWAS catalog study, represented by the GWAS catalog study accession number. AS, all samples; FC, female coders; FS, female samples; GWAS, genome-wide association study; MC, male coders; MS, male samples.

The future

The results of this study point to underlying genetic architecture mediating attractiveness. In the future, careful multivariate studies testing the relative contribution of each associated locus to the component traits of attractiveness, and to attractiveness corrected for those traits, will help researchers unravel and interpret the genetic architecture of this important and complex phenotype. Of course, replication in the few other datasets possessing both genotype and attractiveness data will aid in validation and resolution of these results, and sequencing studies will help clarify the possibly functional variants at each locus and further explore their effect on attractiveness or its related components. Hu and colleagues also briefly mention signatures of selection on alleles associated with male facial attractiveness. This result is especially intriguing and brings up several avenues for future research. Do other secondary sex traits, such as vocal characteristics, show similar signatures of selection in males, indicating sexual selection among our male ancestors [17]? Importantly, does the selection pressure driving the strong relationship between allele frequency and male attractiveness reflect pressure upon the attractiveness per se, or upon related phenotypes, such as lipid metabolism? How do potential signatures of selection fit in with previous evolutionary hypotheses? If there are causative pathways between the associated loci and attractiveness, have cross-cultural variations in preference [18] led to population-specific allele variation at these candidate attractiveness loci?

When contemplating how to depict Helen of Troy, the 5th century BC painter Zeuxis recognized the challenge of identifying the features that define beauty [19]. This challenge remains, and understanding the biological factors that influence attractiveness is equally compelling and complex. Hu and colleagues bring forth a valuable initial foray into the genetic architecture of attractiveness and emphasize the intricate relationships between attractiveness and other visible traits.

Acknowledgments

We thank Mark Shriver for his feedback on previous drafts of this paper.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Little AC, Jones BC, DeBruine LM. Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;366: 1638–59. 10.1098/rstb.2010.0404 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Stephen ID, Law Smith MJ, Stirrat MR, Perrett DI. Facial Skin Coloration Affects Perceived Health of Human Faces. Int J Primatol. 2009;30: 845–857. 10.1007/s10764-009-9380-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.de Jager S, Coetzee N, Coetzee V. Facial Adiposity, Attractiveness, and Health: A Review. Front Psychol. 2018;9: 2562 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02562 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Thornhill R, Gangestad SW. Human facial beauty—Averageness, symmetry, and parasite resistance. Hum Nat. 1993;4: 237–269. 10.1007/BF02692201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Thornhill R, Gangestad SW. Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evol Hum Behav. 2006;27: 131–144. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.06.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Van Dongen S, Gangestad SW. Human fluctuating asymmetry in relation to health and quality: A meta-analysis. Evol Hum Behav. 2011;32: 380–398. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.03.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Zaidi AA, White JD, Mattern BC, Liebowitz CR, Puts DA, Claes P, et al. Facial masculinity does not appear to be a condition-dependent male ornament and does not reflect MHC heterozygosity in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2019; 6 10.1073/pnas.1808659116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lee AJ, Mitchem DG, Wright MJ, Martin NG, Keller MC, Zietsch BP. Facial averageness and genetic quality: testing heritability, genetic correlation with attractiveness, and the paternal age effect. Evol Hum Behav. Elsevier Inc.; 2016;37: 61–66. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hu B, Shen N, Li JJ, Kang H, Hong J, Fletcher J, et al. (2019) Genome-wide association study reveals sex-specific genetic architecture of facial attractiveness. PLoS Genet 15(4): e1007973 10.1371/journal.pgen.1007973 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mitchem DG, Purkey AM, Grebe NM, Carey G, Garver-Apgar CE, Bates TC, et al. Estimating the Sex-Specific Effects of Genes on Facial Attractiveness and Sexual Dimorphism. Behav Genet. 2014;44: 270–281. 10.1007/s10519-013-9627-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Fink B, Butovskaya M, Sorokowski P, Sorokowska A, Matts PJ. Visual Perception of British Women’s Skin Color Distribution in Two Nonindustrialized Societies, the Maasai and the Tsimane’. Evol Psychol. 2017;15 10.1177/1474704917718957 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Stephen ID, Scott IML, Coetzee V, Pound N, Perrett DI, Penton-Voak IS. Cross-cultural effects of color, but not morphological masculinity, on perceived attractiveness of men’s faces. Evol Hum Behav. 2012;33: 260–267. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.10.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Wang G, Djafarian K, Egedigwe CA, El Hamdouchi A, Ojiambo R, Ramuth H, et al. The relationship of female physical attractiveness to body fatness. PeerJ. 2015;3: e1155 10.7717/peerj.1155 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Freedman REK, Carter MM, Sbrocco T, Gray JJ. Ethnic differences in preferences for female weight and waist-to-hip ratio: A comparison of African–American and White American college and community samples. Eat Behav. 2004;5: 191–198. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2004.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Jones D, Brace CL, Jankowiak W, Laland KN, Musselman LE, Langlois JH, et al. Sexual Selection, Physical Attractiveness, and Facial Neoteny: Cross-cultural Evidence and Implications [and Comments and Reply]. Curr Anthropol. 1995;36: 723–748. 10.1086/204427 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Muñoz-Reyes JA, Iglesias-Julios M, Pita M, Turiegano E. Facial Features: What Women Perceive as Attractive and What Men Consider Attractive. PLoS ONE. 2015;10 10.1371/journal.pone.0132979 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Puts DA, Hill AK, Bailey DH, Walker RS, Rendall D, Wheatley JR, et al. Sexual selection on male vocal fundamental frequency in humans and other anthropoids. Proc Biol Sci. 2016;283 10.1098/rspb.2015.2830 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Penton-Voak IS, Jacobson A, Trivers R. Populational differences in attractiveness judgments of male and female faces: Comparing British and Jamaican samples. Evol Hum Behav. 2004;25: 355–370. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.06.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mansfield E. Too Beautiful to Picture: Zeuxis, Myth, and Mimesis [Internet]. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press; 2007. Available: https://muse.jhu.edu/book/32446 [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLoS Genetics are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES